Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The effect of colour on reading performance in children, measured by a sensor hub: From the perspective of gender

  • Tamara Jakovljević ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft

    tamara.jakovljevic@hotmail.com

    Affiliation Sensor Technologies, Jožef Stefan International Postgraduate School, Ljubljana, Slovenia

  • Milica M. Janković,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Electrical Engineering, University of Belgrade, Belgrade, Serbia

  • Andrej M. Savić,

    Roles Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Electrical Engineering, University of Belgrade, Belgrade, Serbia

  • Ivan Soldatović,

    Roles Formal analysis, Supervision

    Affiliation Institute of Medical Statistics and Informatics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Belgrade, Belgrade, Serbia

  • Ivan Mačužić,

    Roles Resources

    Affiliation Faculty of Engineering, University of Kragujevac, Kragujevac, Serbia

  • Tadeja Jere Jakulin,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision

    Affiliation FTŠ Turistica, UP, Portorož, Slovenia

  • Gregor Papa,

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Jožef Stefan Institute, Ljubljana, Slovenia

  • Vanja Ković

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Laboratory for Neurocognition and Applied Cognition, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Belgrade, Belgrade, Serbia

Abstract

In recent decades reported findings regarding gender differences in reading achievement, cognitive abilities and maturation process in boys and girls are conflicting. As reading is one of the most important processes in the maturation of an individual, the aim of the study was to better understand gender differences between primary school students. The study evaluates differences in Heart Rate Variability (HRV), Electroencephalography (EEG), Electrodermal Activities (EDA) and eye movement of participants during the reading task. Taking into account that colour may affect reading skills, in that it affects the emotional and physiological state of the body, the research attempts to provide a better understanding of gender differences in reading through examining the effect of colour, as applied to reading content. The physiological responses of 50 children (25 boys and 25 girls) to 12 different background and overlay colours of reading content were measured and summarised during the reading process. Our findings show that boys have shorter reading duration scores and a longer Saccade Count, Saccade Duration Total, and Saccade Duration Average when reading on a coloured background, especially purple, which could be caused by their motivation and by the type of reading task. Also, the boys had higher values for the Delta band and the Whole Range of EEG measurements in comparison to the girls when reading on coloured backgrounds, which could reflect the faster maturation of the girls. Regarding EDA measurements we did not find systematic differences between groups either on white or on coloured/overlay background. We found the most significant differences arose in the HRV parameters, namely (SDNN (ms), STD HR (beats/min), RMSSD (ms), NN50 (beats), pNN50 (%), CVRR) when children read the text on coloured/overlay backgrounds, where the girls showed systematically higher values on HRV measurements in comparison to the boys, mostly with yellow, red, and orange overlay colours.

Introduction

In children reading skills progress over developmental stages [1], while learning to read is one of the most important achievements of the early school years [2]. The complex process of reading skill acquisition involves perception and cognition, via integration of auditory and visual information processing, and memory, attention, and language skills [3]. Reading skills depend upon a range of cognitive abilities and perceptual processes that affect learning during development [46]. Gender differences in cognitive abilities during development have been widely analysed in neuropsychological and psychological research. However, these differences are still subject to debate [711]. Some studies have reported such differences [12, 13], while others report that they are not able to isolate them [1418]. This implies the need for additional research on gender differences during cognitive development. More generally, in the past decade, the question of whether males and females differ in cognitive ability has been the focus of significant research [19]. While males and females do not differ in general intelligence, which is a general consensus [20], gender differences are commonly observed for more specific cognitive abilities such as visual-spatial ability [21] and language [22]. However, most gender differences are small or trivial (close to zero) in magnitude, explained by the Hyde gender similarities hypothesis (GSH) [15]. The gender gap in reading achievement, which is found cross-culturally, may be one exception to this hypothesis [19, 23, 24]. Hyde [25] concluded that it is “difficult to reconcile” the magnitude of the gender gap observed in reading with that in other domains of verbal ability, which is typically much smaller, as is claimed by Linn [26].

In the developmental context, girls tend to be superior to boys in verbal abilities and linguistic function from infancy through to adulthood, and for this reason gender difference has been investigated in previous studies, reporting greater reading achievements in girls [24, 27], with boys being better in visuospatial tasks involving memory [28]. Female students read more frequently and had a more positive attitude towards reading, resulting in better reading comprehension [29]. Recent research reports a larger Scan Path Length and Saccade Amplitude in female subjects [30]. Also, in tests of early reading ability Harper and Pelletier [31] found no gender differences in children’s performance. However, the study employing eye-tracing methodology revealed gender differences in reading abilities indicated by Saccade Duration, Regression Rate, and Blink Rate [27].

Currently there are very few sources that explore the impact of colour on the reading process, specifically with early school-age children [32, 33]. The influence of text-, background-, or overlay-colour on the reading process in children is reported in literature [3436] but there is no clear consensus regarding this. A recent study reported that colour does not influence the reading process [37], while conversely another has found that colour may be particularly effective for early readers such as school-age children [38]. In the study of visual stress, it is found that male subjects prefer blue and green, and females prefer pink and purple overlay colours for reading [39]. Therefore, the effect of colour on the reading process from the perspective of gender is interesting for further investigation.

Reading involves attention, memory, and sensory integration, which may be reflected in the psycho-physiological state of the individual engaged in the reading task. These processes are a result of fundamental physiological and neural processes, which are measurable by different BioSignal modalities such as Electrocardiography (ECG), Electroencephalography (EEG), Electrodermal Activity (EDA), and eye movement. The goal of the recent study was to incorporate multimodal sensor measurements to investigate the effect of colour on the content within the reading task in children from the perspective of gender differences. We have included measurements of heart rate variability (HRV), EEG, EDA, and eye-tracking to assess the influence of background and overlay colour on reading performance in boys and girls attending the early years of primary school. We aimed to address the mechanisms of colour effect on the reading process through electrophysiological correlates of the reader’s state while taking into account the gender aspect of the reading acquisition.

The current research is an attempt to present new evidence regarding gender differences in reading skill. This research aims to contribute to the existing body of knowledge on the effect of the text, background and overlay colour according to gender.

We aimed to investigate the effects of colour on the content as a stressor during the reading task. The present study aims to further illuminate underlying physiological and behavioural processes accompanying the reading task in children from the gender perspective.

Materials and methods

Participants

The study was carried out with fifty healthy participants, boys and girls (25 plus 25) randomly chosen from students in the second and third years (aged 8–10) of primary school “Drinka Pavlović” in Belgrade. Inclusion criteria were that children have normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and no reading and learning disabilities or attention disorders. A typical case for exclusion would be the presence of large artefacts in the acquired signals. However, no such cases were observed in our sample. According to these criteria, no participants were excluded from the statistical analysis.

Children individually participated in the experiment, each under the same experimental conditions: during the school day and in the same small classroom. They received a short instruction about the experiment setup. After they finished the reading test, participants received a certificate and a small present from the researcher. The research process was anonymous and the collected data were anonymised. Only the data regarding the gender and age of the participants were available to the research team. Before starting the experiment, researchers received oral informed consent about the students’ participation from the parents at a school class meeting, organised by the school director and the class teacher, which was summarized in the school director’s note and delivered to researchers.

The ethical committee of the Psychology Department of the University of Niš (a branch of the Serbian Psychology Association) approved the experimental procedure No 9/2019.

Procedure

A computer screen and keyboard were placed in front of every child. The participants read the text in silence from the computer screen, as per the instructions from the researcher at the beginning of the experiment. Each of the participants read the story from the stimuli presentation on the screen, pressing the space button to receive the next paragraph on the next slide. The experiment started with the presentation of black text on white background (the referent slide) as children would typically see in daily life. After that, a pseudo-randomised background colour with black text was presented to the children along with an overlay version (marked by O in the further text, e.g. “red O is for red overlay”) of the presented slides. The text on each colour/slide was different but was kept in a logical order. Except for the referent slide, no other colour was fixed to any particular stimuli presentation. There are no other factors (semantic or affective content, syntax, vocabulary, or text complexity) that could impact the reading process apart from the actual colour, given that the colours were randomly presented to the participants, rather than being selected.

The experiment design was exactly the same as in [40].

Fig 1 shows the sensor hub which consists of a portable multimodal ECG/EEG/EDA and eye tracking system for physiological data acquisition during the reading task. For data real-time monitoring and storage two laptops were used, one for the ECG/EEG/EDA system, and one for eye movement signal monitoring (connected with an external keyboard and the screen positioned in front of the child).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Portable multimodal ECG/EEG/EDA system and eye-tracking system.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252622.g001

A portable remote eye tracker (SMI RED-m 120-Hz, https://www.smivision.com) was mounted in front of the participants and fixed to the screen to secure its stability. In order to ensure that each participant was the same distance from the screen, an adjustable chin-rest was mounted on the table (it was placed 16 cm above the table and 57 cm from the eye-tracking sensor) [41]. For the stimuli presentation SMI Experiment Centre 3.7 was used. An iView RED-m was used for data storage and collection.

The Smarting (mBrainTrain, Belgrade, Serbia) mobile system with a 24-channel EEG amplifier was used for recording EEG and ECG signals, which were communicating wirelessly with a laptop via Bluetooth. In the experiment a Greentek Gelfree-S3 cap with twenty-two monopolar EEG channels was used (10/20 locations: Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, O1, O2, F7, F8, T7, T8, P7, P8, Fz, Cz, Pz, AFz, CPz, POz). The FPz electrode was used as the ground site and for the reference site the FCz electrode was used. The ECG signal was recorded by one channel of the Smarting amplifier using a surface SKINTACT ECG electrode placed at the left chest region above the heart. The signals of EEG and ECG were acquired with 250 Hz sampling rate and 24-bit resolution. Prior to the test, the skin-electrode impendence was below the manufacturer’s recommended value of 1 kOhm.

For the synchronisation of the multimodal ECG/EEG/EDA and the eye tracking system, one channel of the Smarting amplifier with a small photosensitive sensor was used. This sensor registered the colour changes between two subsequent slides (one black and one white slide lasting for 200 ms each, positioned between two presented slides) indicating the colour changes of the presented slides.

A custom-made galvanic skin response device [41] that sends data via Bluetooth to a laptop was used (sampling rate 40 Hz) for electrodermal activity (EDA) acquisition. EDA data were recorded on the laptop using the Smarting application.

Data processing

BeGaze 3.7 software was used to monitor eye tracking data. Eye tracking analysis included the following parameters: a) Fixation Count, b) Fixation Frequency (count/second), c) Fixation Duration Total (ms), d) Fixation Duration Average (ms), e) Saccade Count, f) Saccade Frequency (count/second), g) Saccade Duration Total (ms) and i) Saccade Duration Average (ms).

EEG/ECG/EDA data were analysed using Matlab ver. 8.5 (Mathworks, USA) in the following manner for each presented slide:

  1. EEG data of all subjects was processed offline. EEG signals were band-pass filtered using 4th order Butterworth filter to extract the activity in the following frequency bands: Delta (0.5–4 Hz), b) Theta (4–7 Hz), c) Alpha (7–13 Hz), d) Beta (15–40 Hz) and e) broadband EEG activity (0.5–40 Hz).
    Filtered signals of all subjects/channels were squared and segmented according to the event markers while each epoch was associated with the reading task of one slide. The median value of data associated with each epoch was calculated for obtaining a single band-power value. Median calculation is used to remove impulse-noises associated with movements, blinks and other artefacts that may occur in the EEG during reading within each epoch. Additional visual inspection of power epochs was conducted to ensure that the median values represent the valid quantification of the band-power activity of each epoch.
  2. Heart activity beats were extracted using Kubios HRV Premium 3.3.1. software [42, 43]. The beats are detected using the Kubios built-in algorithm based on the Pan–Tompkins algorithm [44]. The period between two beats, so called beat-to-beat interval (BBI), and time domain heart rate variability (HRV) parameters [45], Table 1, were extracted by the same software. Also, the Kubios built-in threshold based artefact correction algorithm was performed (a local average interval of 0.35 s was selected and the detected artefacts were automatically replaced by cubic spline interpolated values within the software).
  3. The average value of EDA data was calculated for each slide.

Statistical methodology

Here we present results as percentages, means ± standard deviation or taking into account data type and distribution. We compared groups (boys vs. girls) using a parametric test, an independent samples t-test. All p-values which were less than 0.05 were considered significant. The data were analysed within the SPSS 20.0 software (IBM Corp. Released 2011. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.). The Bonferroni corrections were applied in all the statistical analysis where necessary as a control for multiple comparisons.

Results

Reading results on white (default) background with black text

Gender comparisons (girl vs. boys) regarding the examined parameters for white background only are presented in Table 2. A significant difference has been obtained regarding a single HRV parameter for pNN50 (%), where girls have higher scores in comparison to boys. In all other parameters of EEG frequency bands (Alpha, Beta, Theta, Delta), ECG parameters and Eye tracking measurements, we observed no significant difference between girls and boys.

thumbnail
Table 2. Reading duration, EEG, eye tracking, EDA and HRV parameters in girls and boys—significant p values are marked as bold.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252622.t002

Background and overlay colours

In Table 3 a comparison between girls and boys, based on the t-test for independent samples, was obtained on each of the parameters measured in the study, namely: Reading duration, EEG, Eye tracking, EDA and HRV. As is obvious from the table, the girls (coloured in red) scored systematically higher in many of the HRV measurements. In particular for SDNN (ms) they scored higher on yellow, red O, orange O, and purple O; for CVRR they scored higher on yellow, red O, yellow O, orange O and purple O; for STD HR girls scored higher on red, yellow, orange, turquoise, red O, blue O, yellow O, orange O and purple O; for RMSSD they scored higher on yellow, turquoise, red O, yellow O, orange O and purple O; for NN50 they scored higher on red, yellow, orange, turquoise, red O, blue O, yellow O, orange O and turquoise O; and for pNN50 they scored higher on yellow, orange, purple, turquoise, red O, yellow O, orange O and purple O. Boys only scored higher when reading on a purple background for the following eye-tracking parameters: Saccade Count, Saccade Duration Total and Saccade Duration Average (coloured in blue).

thumbnail
Table 3. Differences between girls (marked with red colour) and boys (marked with blue colour) on reading duration, EEG, eye tracking, EDA and HRV parameters (p < .05).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252622.t003

Girls vs. boys across all of the examined parameters over averaged scores aggregated for all tested colours are presented in Table 4. Boys achieved higher scores on a few EEG and eye-tracking measurements, namely: Delta and Whole range EEG band measurements and Fixation Count, Saccade Count and Saccade Duration Total. The girls, on the other hand, scored higher on the Reading Duration and on a few HRV measures, namely: SDNN, STDHR, RMSSD, NN50, PNN50 and CVRR.

thumbnail
Table 4. Reading duration, EEG, eye tracking and EDA parameters in girls and boys across all colours together—significant p values are marked in bold.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252622.t004

Discussion

The Reading Duration, EEG, eye tracking, EDA and HRV parameters were evaluated in 50 children (25 female and 25 male second and third year students (aged 8–10) of primary school) using a multimodal sensor hub. As reading process involves attention, memory, and sensory integration, which may be reflected in the psychophysiological state of the individual engaged in the reading task, the study aim was investigating different BioSignal modalities such as ECG, EEG, EDA, and eye movement during the reading task.

Gender differences in reading are widely reported [2, 19, 24, 27, 4649], and it was found that motivation, attitudes, and the type of reading task could impact on reading skills in boys more closely than in girls [47]. It was speculated that the boys’ reading performance could depend more on their motivation and attitude. The results of the present study showed that boys had shorter reading duration parameters than girls, but at the same time, they scored higher in some eye- tracking measures, and had longer Fixation Count, Saccade Count, and Saccade Duration Total measurements than the girls, irrespective of background/overlay colour. They also had a longer Saccade Count, Saccade Count Total and Saccade Count Average when reading text on a purple background. Additionally it is reported that males have a more positive emotional response than females during competitive game play [50]. Therefore, here we have taken into account findings that suggest that boys are more motivated to read in new conditions, without teachers’ grades/assessments, and have more competitive attitudes in comparison to female students. It has also been reported that male students have poor reading abilities in comparison to girls [27, 51], and therefore make more exploratory eye movements, which consequently result in larger Saccade Amplitudes as we have also demonstrated in the present research.

Regarding the normal maturation processes reflected in the EEG, McCarthy reported [52] that gender differences are equally distributed, while other researchers [53, 54] found that EEG differences between boys and girls suggest earlier maturation in girls [55]. On the other hand, Cohn [56] and Gasser [57] found no differences between boys and girls measurable by EEG. It is also reported that the amount of activity in the lower frequency EEG bands decreases with age, and in higher frequency bands, it increases [58, 59]. The gender differences in EEG are also reported in context of the task performance and cognitive activity. During the task and rest phase, females have a higher EEG power than males [60, 61]. Also, gender differences are frequently reflected in numerous factors, like task and age [62]. Some research shows that in most cognitive tasks including language, there exist inappreciable differences in behavioural output between the genders [1618]. EEG power and its distribution over a lifetime also varies between the genders [63]. EEG maturation markers increase in faster band activity (alpha, beta) and decrease in slower band activity (theta, delta) [55, 57, 6466]. In the present study it is shown that boys have higher values of delta band and whole range of EEG in comparison to girls. Clarke et al. report that boys’ EEG matures faster than girls’ in childhood, but that these differences are eliminated during adolescence. Conversely Gasser et al. [57] declare that there are no gender differences, mostly because of high interindividual variability in the EEG power spectrum. Other authors have found that until the age of 16 the alpha rhythm does not mature [67]. In previous research the delta band was found to be higher in young individuals than in adults because of incomplete cortical maturation, and is typically even higher in children with learning disabilities [68]. These findings are in line with our results, which showed increased Delta and Whole Range EEG bands to be more prominent in boys than in girls, which could be as a result of the faster maturation process in girls. This is in keeping with previously mentioned results showing that boys had a less mature pattern of eye-movements in comparison to girls.

Likewise, electrodermal activity has been used in several studies with the objective of clarifying markers of psychophysiological functioning and children’s developmental processes [69, 70].

Several studies supported higher levels of baseline SCL (Skin Conductance Level) [68] and SCL reactivity to stressors [71] in girls in comparison to boys. In the present study we found no evidence of systematic differences between boys and girls for this measurement when reading text on either white or coloured/overlay background.

Physiological mechanisms during adolescence actively and progressively undergo changes. It has been reported that HRV progressively reduces with age, and development during adolescence can be assessed using the heart rate variability (HRV) [72]. HRV can be used to ascertain the evolution of the ontogenetic maturation [7376]. Moreover, the gender influence measured by HRV parameters was manifested only in young adults and younger adolescents and our study group belongs to the same age category. Research study shows that measurements of HRV depend on the age but not on the gender of healthy children [77]. When different colours of background, text, and overlay were included in the reading process we found significant differences between girls and boys, whereby girls scored higher on HRV parameters. This indicates higher emotional reactions in girls when they read the text on the coloured/overlay background in comparison to the boys. Regarding this result, our findings are compatible with previously reported results showing that girls have higher values on SDNN and RMSSD measurements.

Conclusion

Primarily this research aimed to assess gender differences in the reading process and to contribute to existing research on the effect of text, background and overlay colour according to gender.

Secondly, the aim was also to investigate the effects of colour on the content as a stressor in the reading task. In order to shed light on contradictory reports regarding gender differences in reading skills, present study illuminates underlying physiological and behavioural processes in the reading task in children from the gender perspective. It evaluates differences in reading duration, EEG, ECG, EDA and eye movement measures on both white and 12 different background/text/overlay colours. It was found that boys show shorter reading duration parameters than girls, and at the same time longer eye-tracking measures such as Fixation Count, Saccade Count, and Saccade Duration Total while reading on a coloured background/overlay, whereas they had a Longer Saccade Duration, Saccade Duration Total, and Saccade Duration Average on a purple background. These results partially support our expectation that boys would have more difficulties reading the text when displayed on background/overlay colours. However, they did not have more issues reading on the coloured background in comparison to the white/default background.

Comparing EEG parameters in girls and boys during reading on white background we did not find systematic differences. Observing all the colours together, it is shown that boys have higher values in Delta and Whole Range bands in comparison to the girls. As the Delta Range is higher in young adults, the findings are aligned with previous research where it is shown that boys will have more difficulties in reading tasks because the reading process is still not automated in comparison to the girls. In fact, they have also demonstrated longer Saccade Count and Saccade Duration measurements in comparison to the girls when reading on a purple background. It seems that the colour can really increase the task difficulty for less proficient readers. We did not find systematic differences for EDA measures between boys and girls while reading on white or coloured background/overlay content. However, regarding ECG measures, girls scored significantly higher on HRV measures (SDNN (ms), STD HR (beats/min), RMSSD (ms), NN50 (beats), pNN50 (%), CVRR), in particular on yellow, orange O and red O colours. These findings are also contributable to studies where it is shown that girls have higher values on HRV measures than boys, which is particularly evident from results including the additional effect of colour on the reading process.

Finally, we can underline that colours used as a stressor in a particular reading task could illuminate gender differences, especially in eye-tracking and ECG measures. Boys have shown longer Saccade Count and Saccade Duration in comparison to girls while reading on the purple colour. Boys have shown shorter reading duration than girls on all coloured background/overlay, and longer eye-tracking measures such as Fixation Count, Saccade Count, and Saccade Duration Total. Regarding the ECG (SDNN, STD HR, RMSSD, NN50, pNN50, CVRR) measures, girls scored higher than boys while reading on yellow, orange O, and red O colours. These findings show that colours could be contributing to a better understanding of gender differences and their relation to the context of the reading processes.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge Elementary School “Drinka Pavlović” (Belgrade) and IPS Jozef Stefan.

References

  1. 1. Ziegler JC, Goswami U. Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory [Internet]. Vol. 131, Psychological Bulletin. 2005 [cited 2020 Dec 13]. p. 3–29. Available from: pmid:15631549
  2. 2. Vlachos F, Papadimitriou A. Effect of age and gender on children’s reading performance: The possible neural underpinnings. Cogent Psychology [Internet]. 2015 Dec 31 [cited 2020 Dec 13];2(1):1045224. Available from: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/23311908.2015.1045224
  3. 3. Ehri LC. Phases of development in learning to read words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading [Internet]. 1995 [cited 2020 Dec 13];18(2):116–25. Available from: /record/1996-07654-001
  4. 4. Korneev AA, Matveeva EY, Akhutina T v. What We Can Learn about Reading Development from the Analysis of Eye Movements. Human Physiology. 2018 Mar 1;44(2):183–90.
  5. 5. Lobier M, Dubois M, Valdois S. The Role of Visual Processing Speed in Reading Speed Development. Barton JJS, editor. PLoS ONE [Internet]. 2013 Apr 4 [cited 2020 Mar 30];8(4):e58097. Available from: pmid:23593117
  6. 6. Lerkkanen MK, Rasku-Puttonen H, Aunola K, Nurmi JE. Reading performance and its developmental trajectories during the first and the second grade. Learning and Instruction. 2004 Apr 1;14(2):111–30.
  7. 7. Weiss EM, Kemmler G, Deisenhammer EA, Fleischhacker WW, Delazer M. Sex differences in cognitive functions. Personality and Individual Differences. 2003;35(4):863–75.
  8. 8. Hedges L v., Nowell A. Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science. 1995;269(5220):41–5. pmid:7604277
  9. 9. Sex and Cognition | The MIT Press [Internet]. [cited 2020 Dec 13]. Available from: https://mitpress.mit.edu/books/sex-and-cognition
  10. 10. Harris P. Book reviews: Sex differences in cognitive abilities D. F. Halpern Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1986. x + 189 pp. Child Language Teaching and Therapy [Internet]. 1988 Jun 25 [cited 2020 Dec 13];4(2):230–1. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/026565908800400212
  11. 11. Ardila A, Rosselli M, Matute E, Inozemtseva O. Gender Differences in Cognitive Development. Developmental Psychology [Internet]. 2011 Jul [cited 2020 Dec 13];47(4):984–90. Available from: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0023819 pmid:21744957
  12. 12. The psychology of sex differences.—PsycNET [Internet]. [cited 2020 Dec 21]. Available from: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1975-09417-000
  13. 13. Maccoby E. The development of sex differences, Stanford Calif.: Stanford University Press; 1966.
  14. 14. Joel D, Berman Z, Tavor I, Wexler N, Gaber O, Stein Y, et al. Sex beyond the genitalia: The human brain mosaic. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America [Internet]. 2015 Dec 15 [cited 2020 Dec 21];112(50):15468–73. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26621705/ pmid:26621705
  15. 15. Hyde JS. The gender similarities hypothesis. Vol. 60, American Psychologist. 2005. p. 581–92. pmid:16173891
  16. 16. Hyde JS. Sex and cognition: Gender and cognitive functions. Vol. 38, Current Opinion in Neurobiology. Elsevier Ltd; 2016. p. 53–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2016.02.007 pmid:26953847
  17. 17. Lindberg SM, Hyde JS, Petersen JL, Linn MC. New Trends in Gender and Mathematics Performance: A Meta-Analysis. Psychological Bulletin [Internet]. 2010 Nov [cited 2020 Dec 18];136(6):1123–35. Available from: /pmc/articles/PMC3057475/?report = abstract pmid:21038941
  18. 18. Zell E, Krizan Z, Teeter SR. Evaluating gender similarities and differences using metasynthesis. American Psychologist [Internet]. 2015 Jan [cited 2020 Dec 18];70(1):10–20. Available from: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0038208 pmid:25581005
  19. 19. Reilly D, Neumann DL, Andrews G. Gender Differences in Reading and Writing Achievement: Evidence From the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP). 2018 [cited 2020 Dec 11]; Available from: pmid:30234314
  20. 20. Halpern DF, Straight CA, Stephenson CL. Beliefs About Cognitive Gender Differences: Accurate for Direction, Underestimated for Size. Sex Roles [Internet]. 2011 Mar [cited 2020 Dec 13];64(5–6):336–47. Available from: /record/2011-03859-004
  21. 21. Voyer D, Voyer Susan D. D. Gender differences in scholastic achievement: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin [Internet]. 2014 [cited 2020 Dec 13];140(4):1174–204. Available from: /record/2014-15035-001 pmid:24773502
  22. 22. Miller DI, Halpern DF. The new science of cognitive sex differences [Internet]. Vol. 18, Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2014 [cited 2020 Dec 13]. p. 37–45. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1364661313002325 pmid:24246136
  23. 23. Gender Reilly D., culture, and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoS ONE. 2012 Jul 10;7(7).
  24. 24. Lynn R, Mikk J. Sex differences in reading achievement. Trames. 2009;13(1):3–13.
  25. 25. Hyde JS. Gender similarities and differences [Internet]. Vol. 65, Annual Review of Psychology. Annual Reviews Inc.; 2014 [cited 2020 Dec 13]. p. 373–98. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/23808917/ https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115057 pmid:23808917
  26. 26. Hyde JS, Linn MC. Gender Differences in Verbal Ability: A Meta-Analysis. Psychological Bulletin [Internet]. 1988 [cited 2020 Dec 13];104(1):53–69. Available from: /record/1988-35304-001
  27. 27. Zhan Z, Wu J, Mei H, Wu Q, Fong PSW. Individual difference on reading ability tested by eye-tracking: from perspective of gender. Interactive Technology and Smart Education [Internet]. 2020 Mar 4 [cited 2020 Dec 11];17(3):267–83. Available from: https://www.emerald.com/insight/content/doi/10.1108/ITSE-12-2019-0082/full/html
  28. 28. Andreano JM, Cahill L. Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learning and Memory [Internet]. 2009 Apr [cited 2020 Dec 13];16(4):248–66. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19318467/ pmid:19318467
  29. 29. Devetak I, Glažar SA. The Influence of 16‐year‐old Students’ Gender, Mental Abilities, and Motivation on their Reading and Drawing Submicrorepresentations Achievements. International Journal of Science Education [Internet]. 2010 Aug 8 [cited 2020 Dec 13];32(12):1561–93. Available from: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/09500690903150609
  30. 30. Abdi Sargezeh B, Tavakoli N, Daliri MR. Gender-based eye movement differences in passive indoor picture viewing: An eye-tracking study. Physiology and Behavior. 2019 Jul 1;206:43–50. pmid:30922820
  31. 31. McTigue EM, Schwippert K, Uppstad PH, Lundetræ K, Solheim OJ. Gender Differences in Early Literacy: Boys’ Response to Formal Instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology [Internet]. 2020 [cited 2020 Dec 13]; Available from: /record/2020-72586-001
  32. 32. van Bommel WJM, van den Beld GJ. Lighting for work: A review of visual and biological effects. Lighting Research and Technology [Internet]. 2004 Dec 19 [cited 2020 Mar 4];36(4):255–69. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1191/1365782804li122oa
  33. 33. de Jong PF, van der Leij A. Effects of Phonological Abilities and Linguistic Comprehension on the Development of Reading. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2002 Jan;6(1):51–77.
  34. 34. Conway ML, Evans BJW, Evans JC, Suttle CM, Engel FL, Child PBASIC, et al. Colors, colored overlays, and reading skills. Allen P, editor. Frontiers in Psychology [Internet]. 2014 Sep 20 [cited 2019 Jun 10];5(7):9–21. Available from: www.frontiersin.org
  35. 35. Wilkins AJ, Evans BJW. Visual stress, its treatment with spectral filters, and its relationship to visually induced motion sickness. Applied Ergonomics [Internet]. 2010 Jul [cited 2020 Mar 3];41(4):509–15. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0003687009000325 pmid:19286164
  36. 36. Pinna B, Deiana K. On the Role of Color in Reading and Comprehension Tasks in Dyslexic Children and Adults. i-Perception [Internet]. 2018 May 1 [cited 2020 Dec 11];9(3):204166951877909. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/2041669518779098
  37. 37. Denton TF, Meindl JN. The Effect of Colored Overlays on Reading Fluency in Individuals with Dyslexia. Behavior Analysis in Practice [Internet]. 2016 Sep [cited 2020 Dec 12];9(3):191–8. Available from: /pmc/articles/PMC4999357/?report = abstract pmid:27622123
  38. 38. Veszeli J, Shepherd AJ. A comparison of the effects of the colour and size of coloured overlays on young children’s reading. Vision Research. 2019 Mar 1;156:73–83. pmid:30664883
  39. 39. Conway ML, Evans BJW, Evans JC, Suttle CM. Does gender influence colour choice in the treatment of visual stress? PLoS ONE [Internet]. 2016 Sep 1 [cited 2020 Dec 11];11(9). Available from: /pmc/articles/PMC5029909/?report = abstract pmid:27648842
  40. 40. Jakovljević T, Janković M, Savić A, Soldatović I, Todorović P, Jakulin TJ, et al. The Sensor Hub for Detecting the Developmental Characteristics in Reading in Children on a White vs. Coloured Background/Coloured Overlays. 2020 Dec 2 [cited 2020 Dec 21]; Available from: www.preprints.org
  41. 41. Giagloglou E, Radenkovic M, Brankovic S, Antoniou P, Zivanovic-Macuzic I. Pushing, pulling and manoeuvring an industrial cart: A psychophysiological study. Int. J. Occup. Saf. Ergon. 2019, 25, 296–304. pmid:28849989
  42. 42. Sachin S, Netaji G N. Pattern analysis of different ECG signal using Pan-Tompkin’s algorithm. (IJCSE) International Journal on Computer Science and Engineering. 2010.
  43. 43. Kubios HRV Premium– Kubios [Internet]. [cited 2021 Apr 4]. Available from: https://www.kubios.com/hrv-premium/
  44. 44. Tarvainen MP, Niskanen JP, Lipponen JA, Ranta-aho PO, Karjalainen PA. Kubios HRV—Heart rate variability analysis software. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine [Internet]. 2014 Jan [cited 2021 Apr 4];113(1):210–20. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24054542/ pmid:24054542
  45. 45. Shaffer F, Ginsberg JP. An Overview of Heart Rate Variability Metrics and Norms. Frontiers in Public Health [Internet]. 2017 Sep 28 [cited 2020 Jul 11];5:258. Available from: /pmc/articles/PMC5624990/?report = abstract pmid:29034226
  46. 46. Sam W. The use of eye tracking with infants and children. Practical Research With Children. Routledge. 2016; pp.24–45.
  47. 47. Logan S, Johnston R. Investigating gender differences in reading. Educational Review [Internet]. 2010 May [cited 2020 Dec 16];62(2):175–87. Available from: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00131911003637006
  48. 48. Spironelli C, Penolazzi B, Angrilli A. Gender differences in reading in school-aged children: An early ERP study. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2010 Jul;35(4):357–75. pmid:20614355
  49. 49. Eilers S, Tiffin-Richards SP, Schroeder S. Gender Cue Effects in Children’s Pronoun Processing: A Longitudinal Eye Tracking Study. Scientific Studies of Reading [Internet]. 2019 Nov 2 [cited 2020 Dec 12];23(6):509–22. Available from: /record/2019-30089-001
  50. 50. Kivikangas JM, Kätsyri J, Järvelä S, Ravaja N. Gender differences in emotional responses to cooperative and competitive game play. Pavlova M, editor. PLoS ONE [Internet]. 2014 Jul 1 [cited 2020 Dec 11];9(7):e100318. Available from: pmid:24983952
  51. 51. Rayner K, Yang J, Schuett S, Slattery TJ. Eye movements of older and younger readers when reading unspaced text. Experimental psychology [Internet]. 2013 [cited 2020 Apr 2];60(5):354–61. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23681016 pmid:23681016
  52. 52. McCarthy MM, Konkle ATM. When is a sex difference not a sex difference? Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology [Internet]. 2005 [cited 2021 Jun 3];26(2):85–102. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16083951/ pmid:16083951
  53. 53. Eeg-Olofsson O, Petersén I, Selldén U. The development of the electroencephalogram in normal children from the age of 1 through 15 years. Paroxysmal activity. Neuropädiatrie [Internet]. 1971 [cited 2020 Dec 15];2(4):375–404. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/5171489/ pmid:5171489
  54. 54. Matousek M. and Petersén I. (1973) Frequency analysis of the EEG in normal children and adolescents. In Kellaway P. and Petersén I., Eds., Automation of Clinical Electroencephalography. Raven Press, New York, pp. 75102.—References—Scientific Research Publishing [Internet]. [cited 2020 Dec 13]. Available from: https://www.scirp.org/(S(vtj3fa45qm1ean45vvffcz55))/reference/ReferencesPapers.aspx?ReferenceID=852753
  55. 55. Clarke AR, Barry RJ, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M. Age and sex effects in the EEG: Development of the normal child. Clinical Neurophysiology [Internet]. 2001 May [cited 2020 Dec 11];112(5):806–14. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1388245701004886 pmid:11336896
  56. 56. Cohn NB, Kircher J, Emmerson RY, Dustman RE. Pattern reversal evoked potentials: Age, sex and hemispheric asymmetry. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/ Evoked Potentials. 1985 Nov 1;62(6):399–405. pmid:2415335
  57. 57. Gasser T, Verleger R, Bächer P, Sroka L. Development of the EEG of school-age children and adolescents. I. Analysis of band power. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology [Internet]. 1988 [cited 2020 Dec 13];69(2):91–9. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/2446839/ pmid:2446839
  58. 58. Smith JR. The Electroencephalogram During Normal Infancy and Childhood: II. The Nature of the Growth of the Alpha Waves. Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology [Internet]. 1938 [cited 2020 Dec 13];53(2):455–69. Available from: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/08856559.1938.10533821
  59. 59. Lindsley DB. A Longitudinal Study of the Occipital Alpha Rhythm in Normal Children: Frequency and Amplitude Standards. Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology [Internet]. 1939 [cited 2020 Dec 13];55(1):197–213. Available from: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/08856559.1939.10533190
  60. 60. Corsi-Cabrera M, Ramos J, Guevara MA, Arce C, Gutierrez S. Gender differencesm in the eeg during cognitive activity. International Journal of Neuroscience. 1993;72(3–4):257–64. pmid:8138380
  61. 61. M M, K Y, H F, Y O, H U, M M, et al. Age development and sex differences of various EEG elements in healthy children and adults—quantification by a computerized wave form recognition method. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology [Internet]. 1985 [cited 2020 Dec 18];60(5). Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/2580690/ pmid:2580690
  62. 62. Etchell A, Adhikari A, Weinberg LS, Choo AL, Garnett EO, Chow HM, et al. A systematic literature review of sex differences in childhood language and brain development [Internet]. Vol. 114, Neuropsychologia. Elsevier Ltd; 2018 [cited 2020 Dec 18]. p. 19–31. Available from: /pmc/articles/PMC5988993/?report = abstract https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.011 pmid:29654881
  63. 63. Langrová J, Kremláček J, Kuba M, Kubová Z, Szanyi J, Langrová J. Gender Impact on Electrophysiological Activity of the Brain. Physiol Res [Internet]. 2012 [cited 2020 Dec 18];61:119–27. Available from: www.biomed.cas.cz/physiolres pmid:23130897
  64. 64. Somsen RJM, Van’t Klooster BJ, van der Molen MW, van Leeuwen HMP, Licht R. Growth spurts in brain maturation during middle childhood as indexed by EEG power spectra. Biological Psychology [Internet]. 1997 Jan 31 [cited 2020 Dec 18];44(3):187–209. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9043653/ pmid:9043653
  65. 65. Martinović Z, Jovanović V, Ristanović D. EEG power spectra of normal preadolescent twins. Gender differences of quantitative EEG maturation. Neurophysiologie Clinique. 1998 Jun 1;28(3):231–48. pmid:9686399
  66. 66. Segalowitz SJ, Santesso DL, Jetha MK. Electrophysiological changes during adolescence: A review [Internet]. Vol. 72, Brain and Cognition. Brain Cogn; 2010 [cited 2020 Dec 18]. p. 86–100. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19914761/ pmid:19914761
  67. 67. Marcuse L v., Schneider M, Mortati KA, Donnelly KM, Arnedo V, Grant AC. Quantitative analysis of the EEG posterior-dominant rhythm in healthy adolescents. Clinical Neurophysiology [Internet]. 2008 Aug [cited 2020 Dec 18];119(8):1778–81. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18486545/ pmid:18486545
  68. 68. Penolazzi B, Spironelli C, Angrilli A. Delta EEG activity as a marker of dysfunctional linguistic processing in developmental dyslexia. Psychophysiology [Internet]. 2008 [cited 2020 Dec 13];45(6):1025–33. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18803600/ pmid:18803600
  69. 69. El-Sheikh M, Keiley M, Hinnant JB. Developmental trajectories of skin conductance level in middle childhood: Sex, race, and externalizing behavior problems as predictors of growth. Biological Psychology. 2010 Feb;83(2):116–24. pmid:19945501
  70. 70. El-Sheikh M. Children’s skin conductance level and reactivity: Are these measures stable over time and across tasks? Developmental Psychobiology [Internet]. 2007 Mar 1 [cited 2020 Dec 13];49(2):180–6. Available from: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/dev.20171 pmid:17299790
  71. 71. McManis MH, Bradley MM, Keith Berg W, Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ. Emotional reactions in children: Verbal, physiological, and behavioral responses to affective pictures. Psychophysiology [Internet]. 2001 [cited 2020 Dec 13];38(2):222–31. Available from: /record/2001-14778-008 pmid:11347868
  72. 72. Estévez-Báez M, Carricarte-Naranjo C, Jas-García JD, Rodríguez-Ríos E, Machado C, Montes-Brown J, et al. Influence of Heart Rate, Age, and Gender on Heart Rate Variability in Adolescents and Young Adults. In: Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology. Springer New York LLC; 2019. p. 19–33.
  73. 73. Wolf T, Tsenkova V, Ryff CD, Davidson RJ, Willette AA. Neural, hormonal, and cognitive correlates of metabolic dysfunction and emotional reactivity. Psychosomatic Medicine [Internet]. 2018 Jun 1 [cited 2020 Dec 13];80(5):452–9. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29595709/
  74. 74. Tolga Doğru M, Murad Başar M, Yuvanç E, Şimşek V. The relationship between serum sex steroid levels and heart rate variability parameters in males and the effect of age Erkeklerde serum cinsiyet steroidleri ile kalp hızı değişkenliği verileri arasındaki ilişki ve yaşın etkileri. Vol. 38, Türk Kardiyol Dern Arş-Arch Turk Soc Cardiol. 2010.
  75. 75. Moodithaya S, Avadhany ST. Gender differences in age-related changes in cardiac autonomic nervous function. Journal of Aging Research [Internet]. 2012 [cited 2020 Dec 13];2012:1–7. Available from: http://www.hindawi.com/journals/jar/2012/679345/ pmid:22187649
  76. 76. Evans BE, Stam J, Huizink AC, Willemen AM, Westenberg PM, Branje S, et al. Neuroticism and extraversion in relation to physiological stress reactivity during adolescence. Biological Psychology [Internet]. 2016 May 1 [cited 2020 Dec 13];117:67–79. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26956979/ pmid:26956979
  77. 77. Bobkowski W, Stefaniak ME, Krauze T, Gendera K, Wykretowicz A, Piskorski J, et al. Measures of heart rate variability in 24-h ECGs depend on age but not gender of healthy children. Frontiers in Physiology [Internet]. 2017 May 18 [cited 2020 Dec 11];8(MAY). Available from: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fphys.2017.00311/full