Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Development of the Bristol Rabbit Pain Scale (BRPS): A multidimensional composite pain scale specific to rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus)

  • Livia Benato ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    livia.benato@bristol.ac.uk

    Affiliation Animal Welfare and Behaviour, School of Veterinary Sciences, University of Bristol, Langford, United Kingdom

  • Joanna Murrell ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Joanna Murrell, Nicola J. Rooney

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Highcroft Veterinary Referrals, Whitchurch, Bristol, United Kingdom

  • Toby G. Knowles,

    Roles Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Animal Welfare and Behaviour, School of Veterinary Sciences, University of Bristol, Langford, United Kingdom

  • Nicola J. Rooney

    Contributed equally to this work with: Joanna Murrell, Nicola J. Rooney

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Animal Welfare and Behaviour, School of Veterinary Sciences, University of Bristol, Langford, United Kingdom

Abstract

A species-specific composite pain scale is a prerequisite for adequate pain assessment. The aim of this study was to develop a multidimensional pain scale specific to rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) called the Bristol Rabbit Pain Scale (BRPS). The scale was developed over five phases using a unique combination of methods: focus groups and behavioural observation. The first two phases aimed at identifying descriptors to describe a rabbit in pain, and then reducing their number, both using focus groups. A total of 72 pain descriptors were grouped under six categories (Demeanour, Posture, Facial expression, Attention to the painful area, Audible and Other) and ‘No pain’ descriptors were added. The third phase aimed to confirm, through video observation of rabbits, the categories and descriptors previously described, to reject those terms that were ambiguous, and identify any new descriptors that had not been included in the previous list of descriptors. This led to the rejection of the categories Audible and Attention to the painful area and of 34 descriptors. Seven new descriptors were identified. The last two phases constructed the final format of the BRPS by refining the categories, ranking the descriptors on an ordinal scale and testing the internal reliability of the scale using Cronbach’s alpha test. This led to a composite pain scale of six categories (Demeanour, Posture, Locomotion, Ears, Eyes and Grooming) with four intensities of pain (0, 1, 2, and 3), a total score of 0–18, and a high Cronbach’s alpha coefficient (alpha = 0.843). This BRPS fills an important gap in the field of rabbit medicine and has the potential to improve the assessment and management of pain in rabbits providing veterinary professionals with a novel multidimensional pain assessment tool. Further studies will investigate the clinical utility, validity and reliability of the BRPS.

Introduction

In veterinary clinics in the UK, many surgical procedures such as dental treatment, lump and abscess removal and elective surgeries such as ovariohysterectomy and orchiectomy are regularly performed on rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) [1, 2]. More than 11,000 experimental procedures are also carried out on this species in laboratory settings (Home Office, Annual statistics of scientific procedures on living animals Great Britain 2018). Adequate management of perioperative pain is therefore essential. The ability to ameliorate pain relies on the capacity to detect and quantify it adequately [3].

The Rabbit Grimace Scale (RbtGS) is a pain scale specifically designed for rabbits [4]. The RbtGS relies on five facial indicators (Orbital tightening, Cheek flattening, Nostril shape, Whisker shape and position, and Ear shape and position) each on a 0–2 scale; where 0 is no pain and 2 is extreme pain. The RbtGS is easy to use, although some of the facial indicators such as whisker position may be difficult to assess [5]. Moreover, it has been developed based on a breed of straight-eared rabbits, common in laboratories. This can be a limiting factor when used in a clinical setting as lop-eared rabbits currently comprise the majority of the cases treated in UK veterinary practices [6].

Grimace pain scales are very popular pain assessment tools, because they are reliable and quick to use in a busy clinical environment [7]. However, the experience of pain is multi-dimensional and composite pain scales are considered the gold standard when assessing pain [8]. More recently, a new composite pain scale specific for rabbits (CANCRS) has been designed, merging the Rabbit Grimace Scale (RbtGS) with a clinical pain scale (CPS) comprising physiologic parameters (pupil dilation, respiratory rate, respiratory pattern, heart rate) and behavioural responses (response to palpation, mental status and vocalization) [9]. The scale rates four levels of pain intensity (No pain, Discomfort, Moderate pain and Severe pain). The overall scale (CANCRS), and the two component elements: (RbtGS and CPS) were each tested for reliability and validity. The results showed that both elements are reliable assessment tools in a clinical environment while the CPS did not differentiate between the different levels of pain. Physiological parameters can present limitations when assessing pain, as they can be altered by concurrent problems such as stress and infection [1012].

In 1985, Morton and Griffiths [13] developed a multidimensional pain scale for use in a laboratory context. This pain scale took in consideration changes due to pain in a group of laboratory animals: rabbits, monkeys, dogs, cats, guinea pigs, and rats [13]. The scale was based on the clinical signs seen most commonly by the animal carers such as veterinary surgeons, veterinary nurses or research workers and described five categories (body weight, appearance, clinical signs, unprovoked behaviour, and response to an appropriate stimulus). A score of 0 to 3; where 0 is normal and 3 is severe; was then assigned to each category. This composite pain scale has not been tested for reliability and validity, and it is not specific to rabbits, possibly leading to a misinterpretation of the clinical signs of pain in this species.

The method used by Morton and Griffiths (1985) based on focus group discussion was also used to develop species-specific multidimensional pain scales such as the Glasgow composite measure pain scale (CMPS) for dogs [14] and cats [15] and the Colorado State University Feline and Canine Acute Pain Scale (CSU-FAPS; CSU-CAPS) (Colorado State University Cancer Centre, Veterinary Medical Centre 2006). To date a similar approach has not been used in rabbits.

Two recent surveys on the attitudes of veterinary surgeons [1] and veterinary nurses [16] towards pain and analgesia in rabbits highlighted that a large number of veterinary professionals did not use any pain assessment tool in rabbits (77% veterinary surgeons and 71.1% veterinary nurses). The majority of the respondents using a pain assessment tool stated they used the RbtGS regularly (12% and 20.5% of the overall population of veterinary surgeons and veterinary nurses respectively). None of the respondents mentioned the pain scale from Morton and Griffiths. The reason could be that, despite being a multidimensional pain scale, it is not specific to rabbits, and it is not currently used in other species such as cats and dogs as other composite pain scales are now available. The CANCRS was not mentioned as it was developed after the surveys were carried out. Other pain assessment tools were mentioned such as unidimensional pain scales, behavioural changes and multidimensional pain scales designed for other companion animals such as cats and dogs. However, all these methods either are not specific to rabbits, or they can be time consuming, and are less reliable than species-specific pain scales as they can be subjective to the observer’s experience [17, 18], leading to potential risks of undertreating rabbits in pain. Approximately half of veterinary nurses (50.35%) believed that a validated composite pain scale specifically designed for rabbits would improve pain assessment in this species [16].

The aim of this study was to develop a multidimensional pain scale specific for rabbits intended to assess acute pain in rabbit patients using a unique combination of methods: focus groups and behavioural observation. The purpose of the focus groups was to identify descriptors of a rabbit in pain using stakeholders in the field of rabbit medicine, and analgesia and anaesthesia following the methods described by Wong et al. [19] and Kinalski et al. [20]. Focus group discussion allows the determination of the best descriptors that can be used to construct a pain assessment tool that is specific to rabbits [21]. However, prey species like rabbits tend to hide signs of pain and therefore can be challenging for a practitioner to describe their behaviours properly [14]. Behavioural observation of the animals may be used to confirm pain descriptors and to identify additional behaviours that a rabbit in pain might exhibit [22] and this can be carried out by observation of video clips of rabbits recorded during the perioperative period [22]. Another important aspect when developing any scale, is to test its internal consistency, which can be assessed using the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient [23]. The coefficient is a measure ranging between 0 and 1, where results closer to 1 signify that there is good level of correlation between the categories measuring pain [24, 25]. It can also be used to assess whether the score given to each category by different respondents is consistent and free from random error [24].

Whilst previous rabbit pain scales have used facial action units and a combination of physiological and behavioural parameters extrapolated from cat and dog pain scales, a careful selection of the rabbit pain indicators based on focus group discussion and observation have not been previously carried out. Therefore, here we used a series of progressive steps to devise the new Bristol Rabbit Pain Scale.

Methods and results

This study’s protocol and the written consent forms were approved by the Faculty of Health Sciences Research Ethics Committee (FREC-66205) of the University of Bristol.

The overall development of the BRPS involved five phases as described below (Fig 1).

Phase one: Collection of descriptors that describe a rabbit in pain

Methods.

The aim of this phase was to identify words and short sentences (descriptors) that described rabbits in pain and to group them into categories. This was achieved with stakeholders in the fields of rabbit management and medicine.

A total of nine focus groups (total of 34 participants; 28 females and 6 males) were carried out during November 2018: One of veterinary nurses (6 people), two of rabbit owners (5 and 4 people respectively), two of rehoming centre workers (5 and 3 people respectively), one of scientists (3 people) and one of veterinary surgeons (8 people). The participants were contacted based on their knowledge and interest in rabbits and were recruited via email and were all based in the South-West of the United Kingdom (UK). They all gave written consent. The inclusion criteria for the veterinary surgeons, veterinary nurses, rehoming centre workers and rabbit owners were that they all had experience either treating, and nursing or looking after rabbits at the time the focus group was carried out. The inclusion criterion for the scientists was that they all had worked with rabbits during their scientific career.

Each focus group met once at the University of Bristol for two hours, during which a facilitator (LB) led the participants through three exercises. The aim of having different focus groups and exercises was to allow participants with the same background to discuss freely the topic with minimal intervention from the facilitator. The first exercise encouraged the participants to list a series of words and short phrases (descriptors) that described pain in rabbits. No limitation to the number of descriptors was applied. Descriptors were not prioritised or rejected during this phase. During the second exercise, descriptors from other research studies that used behaviour-based measures to assess pain in cats, rabbits and guinea pigs [15, 22, 26] were shown to the participants. The participants were then asked to select the descriptors that best applied to a rabbit in pain and add them to the previous list. In the third exercise, nine categories were provided (Posture, Comfort, Demeanour, Mobility, Response to touch, Facial expression, Attention to the painful area, Response to people, and Other). The category Facial expression was taken from the RbtGS [4] while the rest of the categories were taken from cat and dog pain scales [15, 27]. The participants were asked to group the descriptors collected during the first two exercises under these categories. The participants were encouraged to change the title of the categories or to add more categories if they wished to.

Results.

When the descriptors were pooled together from the nine different focus groups, some descriptors were repeated in more than one category. A total of 276 descriptors and 12 categories were collected. None of the nine categories originally suggested were rejected. None of the groups changed the title of any of the categories including ‘Other’. However, three new categories, Response to other rabbits, Food and drinking, and Audible, were added (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Categories and descriptors collected during the nine focus groups in phase one of the study.

Nine categories were provided (Posture, Comfort, Demeanour, Mobility, Response to touch, Facial expression, Attention to the painful area, Response to people, and Other) and three new categories were added by the participants (Response to other rabbits, Food and drinking, and Audible)*. Some descriptors were reported in more than one category.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252417.t001

Phase two: Reduction of the descriptors and categories

Methods.

The aim of this phase was to reduce the descriptors and categories collected during the previous phase to a smaller more practical number. This was carried out during one single specialist focus group attended by four participants (4 males): two specialist veterinary surgeons in rabbit and small mammal medicine (DZooMed) and two in small animal anaesthesia and analgesia (Dip.ECVAA). None of the participants of this focus group had attended the first focus group. All the participants gave a written consent. The participants were contacted based on their expertise in the field of rabbit medicine and small animal pain and analgesia. They were recruited via email and were all based in the South-West of the United Kingdom (UK). Due to their extensive knowledge, the specialists were considered the best judges to determine which categories and descriptors from the previous list should be used in the novel pain scale. However, descriptors and categories that were reported by the majority of the groups (≥ 6) during the previous phase were automatically included in the new list.

The specialists focus group met once at the University of Bristol in March 2019 for two hours and conducted three exercises facilitated by LB. During the first exercise, the specialists were provided with the list of categories from Phase 1 and asked, based on their clinical experience, to choose the categories that they thought should be included in the novel pain scale. Secondly, the specialists were provided with the list of descriptors from Phase 1 and were asked to select the descriptors for each of the previously named categories. During the third exercise, the specialists were asked to review the categories and descriptors that had so far been selected, and to add one or more descriptors describing ‘No-pain’ for each category. They were also requested to review the name of the categories.

Results.

During the first exercise, the categories Demeanour and Posture were reported by the majority of the first focus groups and included in the new list. The categories Facial expression, Attention to the painful area, Audible and Other, were chosen by the specialists. During the second exercise, fifteen descriptors were reported by the majority of the first focus groups and included in the new list. Fifty-seven descriptors were selected by the specialists and added to the new list. The total of 72 descriptors were then grouped under the six categories. During the third exercise, the category ‘Other’ was renamed ‘Food intake/toileting’. The category Posture was renamed Posture/Locomotion. A total of 10 ‘No pain’ descriptors were added (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Final descriptors and categories chosen during the specialist focus group in phase two.

The ‘No Pain’ descriptors added during the focus group are presented in Italic.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252417.t002

Phase three: Video observation

Methods.

The aim of this phase was to confirm through direct observation of rabbits undergoing surgery that the descriptors identified in Phases 1 and 2 as denoting pain, did occur more post-surgery, and those denoting no pain were more likely to occur pre-operatively and, as a consequence, to reject those terms that were ambiguous. A second aim was to identify any additional behaviours exhibited by rabbits undergoing surgery that could be used to construct the novel pain scale but that had not been included in the previous list of descriptors.

Thirty-one rabbits of any age, gender and breed undergoing ovariohysterectomy (OVH) and orchiectomy (OR) from four veterinary clinics in the South-West of the United Kingdom (UK) were video and audio-recorded using GoPro Hero7 Black® Cameras during the perioperative period from the moment they were hospitalised until the time they were discharged on the same day. The cameras were securely fitted on the door of the kennel, facing the rabbits with a maximum distance of 700 mm to the back of the kennel. The rabbits were hospitalised in the wards either alone or with other animals, and veterinary staff would enter the room intermittently. The owners had given consent to inclusion in the study and analgesia was provided to the rabbits according to the normal practice protocol.

The videos from three rabbits were rejected (one was considered too poor quality and two showed two rabbits housed together), so 28 individually-housed animals were included in this study. Four video clips of 5-minute-length each were selected per rabbit; two before (At 30- and 60-minutes post admission) and two from after surgery (At 120- and 150-minutes of the recovery time after tracheal extubation). A total of 112 video clips were analysed (n = 56 Before surgery-B; n = 56 After surgery-A). The video clips were randomised, and the observer (LB) was blinded as to whether each video clip was ‘B’ or ‘A’. The presence of each descriptor listed during Phase 2, including the no-pain ones (Table 2), was assessed in each video clip. If the descriptor was observed, it was recorded as one (1) if it was not observed it was recorded as zero (0). If any additional behavioural descriptors were observed in the video clips, they were added to the list, and the presence or absence recorded in each video clip.

Results.

In this phase, 27 descriptors were rejected because their definition was ambiguous and difficult to interpret e.g. grumpy, furrowed brows (n = 11), they described changes over time and therefore could not be quantified during a short assessment period e.g. eating less, reduced faeces (n = 6), or they implied a relationship or action towards another rabbit or a person and therefore could not be assessed during the observation as the animals were hospitalised individually, e.g. lashing out to a person, flinching as response to palpation (n = 10).

None of the descriptors in the categories Attention to the painful area (n = 5) and Audible (n = 7) were recorded in any of the video clips and therefore these categories were rejected. The descriptors that occurred both before and after surgery and did not show any difference between the two periods were also rejected (n = 7; Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Results from the analysis of the video clips.

The number in each column (Before; After) corresponds to the number of video clips in which each descriptor was observed. Some of the descriptors were rejected because a they were ambiguous (n = 11); b they described changes over time (n = 6); c they implied relationships with or action towards another rabbit or a person (n = 10); d they were not seen in any of the video clips (n = 12); e they did not differentiate between before and after surgery (n = 7). The new descriptors were recorded under the categories “New descriptors”.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252417.t003

The rest of the descriptors were confirmed as potential descriptors for the novel pain scale. The no-pain descriptors of four categories (Demeanour, Posture/Locomotion, Facial expression and Food intake/toileting) were recorded predominantly before surgery. The rest of the descriptors were recorded predominantly in the after-surgery video clips. Additional descriptors were identified and grouped under a category called “New descriptors”: semi-closed and closed eyes, moving and non-moving ears, exploring/being active, mobility, laying on a flank, and grooming (Table 3).

Phase four: Ranking descriptors of the BRPS

Methods.

The aim of Phase 4 was to refine the categories and rank the descriptors in order of pain intensity using a 4-point scale. The categories from the previous phase, were reviewed by the authors and additional changes made in order to better define them and increase clarity. The new descriptors identified from the video observation were also allocated within the refined categories. Finally, the descriptors within each category were ranked in an ordinal scale with four levels of pain intensity. To ensure correct ranking, descriptors from previous studies on pain in rabbits were used as a guideline [22, 28, 29] by the authors (LB, JM, NJR). Video observation of the rabbits was also used to adjust the descriptors for accuracy and to transform them into short sentences to better describe the differences that a rabbit would exhibit depending on the intensity of pain.

Results.

The category Posture/locomotion comprised five descriptors: three describing posture [Relaxed/Comfortable, Hunched, Tense posture) and two locomotion (Still, Inactive), and was split into two new categories: Posture and Locomotion. The category Facial expression contained three descriptors (Open eyes, Flattened ears and Eyes tightened) and was changed into two new categories “Ears” (Flattened ears) and “Eyes” (Open eyes, Eyes tightened). The category Food intake/toileting was changed into “Food” as the descriptors describing the faecal output had been previously rejected. A new category “Grooming” was added. The new descriptors identified from the video observation were allocated within the new categories: Semi-closed and Closed eyes (Eyes); Moving and Not moving ears (Ears), Exploring/being active, Mobility (Locomotion), Laying on a flank (Posture) and Grooming (Grooming).

The descriptors within each of the final seven categories (Demeanour, Locomotion, Posture, Ears, Eyes, Food and Grooming) were then ranked on an ordinal scale. The no-pain descriptors were listed under the intensity pain “0” (No Pain). The remaining descriptors were ranked from 1 to 3. The categories Posture, Ears, Grooming and Food listed fewer descriptors than the four levels of pain intensity so the gaps were filled with new descriptors (Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 4. Ranking of descriptors on an ordinal scale.

The descriptors in bold were added in order to fill the gaps due to a small number of descriptors within the category.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252417.t004

Phase five: Internal consistency

Methods.

The aim of this phase was to measure the internal consistency of the BRPS using the Cronbach alpha coefficient, which describes the extent to which all the categories measure the same concept [25]. A total of 21 respondents, veterinary surgeons and veterinary nurses working with rabbits, were contacted by email and provided with the novel BRSP in the format of a one single page form and were asked to assess up to four rabbits in acute pain using the scale. They were also asked to give general comments regarding the use of the novel scale. The internal consistency of the BRPS was calculated using the Cronbach alpha coefficient and analysed using SPSS (Version 20, IBM). The coefficient is generally measured between 0 and 1 and a coefficient ≥ 0.7 is considered good internal correlation [24, 25]. The impact of removing each category from the scale was also quantified.

Results.

A total of 61 rabbits in acute pain were assessed. The rabbits had been admitted for a variety of conditions such as mild trauma, intestinal hypomotility and surgical procedures such as OVH and OR. All four levels of pain were represented with a median score (and range) of 8 [0–18]. Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the BRSP was 0.846. The removal of each category had similar impact on the internal consistency (Table 5). In view of the comments expressed by some of the respondents regarding food being a limitation within a clinical setting, the category Food was removed from the final version of the scale. This change did not affect the overall internal consistency of the BRPS (alpha = 0.843) (Table 5). The BRPS was then finalised and instructions on how to use it added to the ultimate version (Table 6).

thumbnail
Table 5. The internal consistency of the BRPS was analysed using the Cronbach’s alpha test.

The table shows the Cronbach’s alpha of the scale if each item would be deleted.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252417.t005

thumbnail
Table 6. Final version of the Bristol Rabbit Pain Scale (BRPS).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252417.t006

Discussion

The current study describes the development of the Bristol Rabbit Pain Scale (BRPS), a multidimensional pain scale specific to rabbits. The development process consisted of a total of five phases, using a unique combination of methods: focus groups and behavioural observation. During the first two phases, focus groups were used to obtain a pool of descriptors of rabbits in pain that could be added to the novel pain scale. The third phase consisted of video observations of rabbits during the perioperative period to confirm the descriptors included in the novel pain scale and to reject those that were ambiguous. The last two phases focused on the construction of the final format of the novel pain scale, formatting it, and testing its internal consistency. This led to a composite pain scale of six categories (Demeanour, Posture, Locomotion, Ears, Eyes and Grooming) and four intensities of pain (0, 1, 2, and 3) with a total score from 0 to 18.

Similar, to the RbtGS [4] and the CANCRS [9], the BRPS is a pain assessment tool specific for rabbits, and like the CANCRS is a composite pain scale. The RbtGS followed the method used to develop the rat [30] and mouse [31] grimace scales, which obtained still images of the head and face of rabbits before and after painful procedures such as ear tattooing, and to score the facial expressions on a 3-point- scale. The CANCRS was developed by merging the RbtGS with physiological and behavioural responses (CPS) selected from pain scales used to assess cats and dogs [9]. In contrast, the BRPS was developed following two distinct methods, the method used to develop the Glasgow composite measure pain scale (CMPS) for cats and dogs [15, 27, 32] that comprises collection and expert validation of the descriptors [33], and video observation of the animals to confirm the descriptors more commonly exhibited by animals in pain during the perioperative period.

Focus groups

The descriptor collection and expert validation were carried out by focus groups. The use of focus group discussion is a well-recognised method described in both human and veterinary medicine to gather information about a specific topic [19, 34, 35]. In human medicine, it is often incorporated into the development of pain scales for subjects that are unable to self-report pain such as children [36, 37] and individuals with difficulties in verbal communication [38]. This is generally carried out during several sessions when the indicators are carefully discussed amongst the content experts such as clinicians and nurses specialised in the specific area. Studies have shown that observers are also capable of describing behavioural changes seen in animals due to pain [33]. In veterinary medicine, this method was first used by Morton and Griffiths in 1985 [13] and then consolidated during the develop of the CMPS for dogs [32] and cats [15]. Here, uniquely, we used focus groups with experts from a variety of disciplines, owners and researchers as well as veterinary professionals which we believed served to ensure diverse potential indicators of pain behaviours were all considered.

Video observation

In the current study, video observation of rabbits was used to further assess the descriptors generated during the focus groups; to confirm viability of those included in the novel pain scale and to reject those that were ambiguous. Similarly, in human medicine, novel pain scales developed to assess non-verbal patients are often tested through direct observation of the patient in order to find the descriptors that best describe the response of the subject to pain [37, 39, 40]. In veterinary medicine, direct observation using video recording is generally reported in studies to assess post-operative pain in animals such as rats [41], guinea pigs [26], rabbits [42]and horses [43]. In these studies, ethograms, created using information from the literature review, were used to assess the most common pain behaviours exhibited by the animals.

The video analysis of the rabbits led, amongst other changes, to the rejection of all the descriptors in the category ‘Audible’ that described an animal vocalising, and the category ‘Attention to the painful area”. This was because none of these descriptors were recorded in any of the video clips suggesting that their inclusion added no value.

In a study on the use of local anaesthetic cream for pain-free venepuncture in laboratory animals [44], vocalisation was used to assess pain in dogs and rats, but not in rabbits. In contrast, Morton and Griffiths (1985) [13] described vocalisation as a sign of pain, distress or discomfort not only in laboratory animals such as dogs, cats, guinea pigs and rats but also in rabbits. Similarly, vocalisation was observed in rabbits experiencing pain in a study evaluating EMLA® cream use during tattooing of rabbits [4], and when tested for inter-rater reliability, the results showed a good level of agreement (k = 0.88) [9]. In the current study, none of the rabbits vocalised during the perioperative period. One explanation could be, that rabbits tend to vocalise when short and sharp acute pain is inflicted such as ear tattooing, and they do not vocalise when suffering acute post-operative pain. It is possible that more research needs to be done to better describe vocalisations when assessing pain in rabbits, and to determine the use of this category in pain scales depending on their specific purpose.

The category ‘Attention to the painful area’ is frequently used to assess pain in cats [15], dogs [14] and horses [45]. In these species, if the animals are pain-free they tend not to pay attention to a wound while if they experience severe pain, depending on the species, they can chew the wound, growl or become aggressive when the wound is palpated. Rabbits respond differently. In a study that evaluated EMLA cream attention to the painful area was identified by the rabbit grooming the head and ears following ear tattooing [4]. During the development of the CANCRS, neither the category ‘Attention to the painful area’ nor ‘Grooming’ were used as indicators of pain but ‘Response to palpation’ was included as a category. This was chosen based on other pain scales for small mammals and rated from 0 to 2 where 0 was no reaction, 1 was reaction during palpation and 2 was reaction before palpation [9]. In the current study, both categories ‘Attention to the painful area’ and ‘Response to touch’ were initially highlighted by the first focus group but ‘Attention to the painful area’ was then favoured over ‘Response to touch’ by the specialist focus group. However, in the video analysis, none of the rabbits paid attention to the surgical wound and therefore the category ‘Attention to the painful area’ was rejected.

Grooming was seen to be a more common response of the animal to pain: the grooming of the head and body was observed more frequently in rabbits with no or mild pain, but it was not exhibited by the animals suffering more severe pain. These findings were confirmed by a recent study on post-operative behaviour in 28 rabbits undergoing orthopaedic surgery where the duration of body self-cleaning was found to be shorter during pain (1 hour post recovery) than at 24 h post recovery [42]. This study also assessed the influence of the presence of an observer on pain behaviours and found that no significant difference was found in body cleaning while ear scratching and head self-cleaning did not change between the presence and absence of an observers. This is an important consideration as the presence of veterinary staff assessing the animals could have altered this behaviour. Grooming is a normal behaviour exhibited by rabbits as part of their daily routine [46, 47] but non-essential behaviours such as this normally cease in cases of stress or danger [48]. In addition, previous studies suggest that overgrooming of the painful area can be seen following sharp and acute pain [4]. Therefore, ‘Grooming’ was added as category.

The categories Demeanour, Posture and Locomotion were included in the final version of the BRPS. These categories were previously reported in studies assessing pain in rabbits [28], cats [15] and dogs [32]. An animal in pain usually tends to be less responsive and to exhibit a tense posture. However, while predator species such as cats and dogs are described as restless or reluctant to move depending of the intensity of the pain [15, 32], prey species such as guinea pigs and rabbits are generally less active and less mobile when in pain [4, 26]. The CANCRS includes the response of the animal to the environment and external stimuli under the category Mental status, it does not include Posture or Locomotion [9]. The BRSP reflects these species differences in pain response and takes into consideration changes that are specific to rabbits.

The BRPS also includes the categories ‘Ears’ and ‘Eyes’.; two facial action units previously reported in the RbtGS, in other grimace scales such as those used on rats and mice [30, 31] and in behaviour-based studies on the perioperative pain in rabbits [28]. In the CANCRS, these two facial units were found to have substantial inter-rater agreement (Cohen’s kappa: ear position = 0.78, and orbital tightening = 0.68). When describing orbital tightening, the current study confirmed findings of the previous studies, that an animal suffering some degree of discomfort and pain keeps their eyes semi-closed or closed whilst ones pain-free keeps their eyes open.

Generally, when assessing the ears to measure pain, the position is taken into consideration and a rabbit in pain holds the ears flat against the body [4]. During the current study, the ear position was considered alongside ear movement as, like other animals rabbits tend to move their ears towards a sound or a noise [49]. During the preoperative period, rabbits were more likely to orient their ears in response to a noise, while after surgery, they kept the ears still. Similarly, responsiveness to sounds was taken in consideration when developing a pain scale for acute colic in horses, where the ear orientation towards sounds was considered 0 (no pain) and no response to sounds and backward position was scored 2 (intense pain) [50]. One advantage of evaluating ear position and movement together, is that it also allows assessment of lop-eared rabbits. Lop-eared, like straight-eared rabbits, tend to orientate the ears towards sounds although to a smaller degree. During the direct observations, it was noticed that mini-lop and dwarf lop rabbits moved the lopped pinna backwards and forwards, while rabbits with heavier ears such as French lop rabbits exhibited subtle, but obvious movements of the base of the ears and of the head but were unable to move the pinna. The BRPS takes into consideration changes including these two facial action units.

Internal consistency

The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the BRPS was good at 0.843. This is similar to the multidimensional pain scale developed to assess acute postoperative pain in cats (alpha = 0.867) [51] and higher than the Glasgow scale for assessing pain in dogs (alpha = 0.632) [32]. Neither the RbtGS nor the CANCRS were tested for internal consistency. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the BRPS showed that all the categories had good level of correlation between them. The test also showed that none of the categories, if deleted, would have substantially changed the alpha coefficient and therefore the internal reliability of the scale.

Limitations

One limitation of the current study is that the rabbits observed during the direct analysis of Phase 3 underwent either ovariohysterectomy or orchiectomy, two surgical procedures considered mildly to moderately painful [1]. Therefore, it is possible that during the direct observations, changes associated with more severe pain were not seen in the videos. However, although the use of analgesia and multimodal analgesia has increased over the years in rabbits [1], many analgesic protocols have not been tested in this species and many studies have shown that an optimal analgesic protocol has not yet been found [5, 52, 53]. Based on this, it is reasonable to assume that the rabbits in the current study might have experienced some post-operative pain, and some severe pain.

Moreover, it is reasonable to assume that due to the administration of analgesia, during the perioperative period following the normal protocol of the veterinary practice, the rabbits may have not exhibited some pain behaviours limiting the pain descriptors analysed and used in the development of this novel pain scale.

To be able to adequately describe the different level of pain during the ranking of the descriptors, the choice of the descriptors for each category was guided by the experience of the authors (LB, JM and NJR), similarly to other studies [51]. The choice of the descriptors was also guided by a literature review of studies describing pain in rabbits [22, 28, 29] and confirmed by comparing the video clips before and after surgery. This method was previously reported during the development of a tool to measure pain in children in an emergency department [39] in order to select words and descriptors for inclusion in their pain scale for children, analysed and compared scales previously validated for the youngest patients. Current paucity of multidimensional pain scales in rabbits meant this was not possible and therefore we relied entirely on reviewing the literature [21].

Another limitation of the current study is that the rabbits that were video recorded during the peri-operative period had interaction with veterinary staff. This could have contributed to some changes of the pain behaviours. Pinho et al. (2020) assessed pain-related behaviours in rabbits following orthopaedic surgery and the effects of the presence of an observer. They found that, during the pre-operative period, the rabbits were less active and explored less if the observer was present while during the post-operative period, behaviours such as e.g. winching increased and flinching decreased. They concluded that the influence of an observer on the rabbit’s behaviour could lead to false negative results when assessing pain. In a busy veterinary clinic, the interaction between the animals and the veterinary staff during the peri-operative period is normal practice. Veterinary staff is constantly responsible for the care of the inpatients while hospitalised. The aim of the current study was to develop a pain assessment tool that could be used in such environment. For this reason, the rabbits recorded in the videos were hospitalised in wards according to the normal practice protocol. Moreover, the video clips selected during the current study were solely time-based (Before and After surgery) to highlight those behaviours that were seen more frequently perioperatively irrespective of whether there was interaction or not between the rabbits and the veterinary staff.

The rabbits recorded in the videos during the pre-operative period could also have exhibited signs of stress due to the new environment leading to a misinterpretation of the “no-pain” descriptors. To reduce this eventuality, the “no pain” descriptors assessed during the video observation were based on the results of the two focus groups where specific pain behaviours were thoroughly considered. However, at the present, no stress scales such as those for horses [54] and cats [55] are available for rabbits, and therefore specific stress descriptors could not be confirmed or rejected during the analysis. The influence of stress on pain and the consequent rabbit behavioural response is not yet well understood, and further research is necessary.

Conclusions

Adequate pain assessment is best achieved by the use of a species-specific, multidimensional pain scale. This study has filled a gap in rabbit medicine by developing the BRPS: a multidimensional pain scale similar to those currently available for cats and dogs, but specific to rabbits. The BRPS was developed using a unique approach based on focus group discussions and direct observation of the animals. It is comprised of six categories each rated on a 4-point scale and shows high internal consistency. The BRPS includes the category ‘Ears’ acknowledging that not only the position, but also the movement of the rabbit’s ears are important, and allowing the assessment of lop-eared as well as straight-eared rabbits, a limiting factor in other pain assessment tools already available for this species. The BRPS has the potential to improve the assessment and management of pain in rabbits by providing veterinary professionals with a novel, multidimensional pain assessment tool. Further studies will investigate the clinical utility, validity and reliability of the BRPS.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank The Alumni Association of the University of Bristol who supported this work, and all the participants of the focus groups and the respondents of the survey for their time and for sharing their expertise and experience.

References

  1. 1. Benato L, Murrell JC, Blackwell EJ, Saunders R, Rooney N. Analgesia in pet rabbits: a survey study on how pain is assessed and ameliorated by veterinary surgeons. The Veterinary record. 2020;186(18):603. pmid:32303663
  2. 2. PDSA. Paw Report 2019. https://www.pdsa.org.uk/media/7420/2019-paw-report_downloadable.pdf2019.
  3. 3. Mathews K, Kronen PW, Lascelles D, Nolan A, Robertson S, Steagall PV, et al. Guidelines for Recognition, Assessment and Treatment of Pain. J Small Anim Pract. 2014;55(6):E10–E68. pmid:24841489
  4. 4. Keating SCJ, Thomas AA, Flecknell PA, Leach MC. Evaluation of EMLA Cream for Preventing Pain during Tattooing of Rabbits: Changes in Physiological, Behavioural and Facial Expression Responses. Plos One. 2012;7(9):11. pmid:22970216
  5. 5. Hedenqvist P, Trbakovic A, Thor A, Ley C, Ekman S, Jensen-Waern M. Carprofen neither reduces postoperative facial expression scores in rabbits treated with buprenorphine nor alters long term bone formation after maxillary sinus grafting. Research in Veterinary Science. 2016;107:123–31. pmid:27473985
  6. 6. O’Neill DG, Craven HC, Brodbelt DC, Church DB, Hedley J. Morbidity and mortality of domestic rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) under primary veterinary care in England. Veterinary Record. 2020;186(14):8. pmid:31594841
  7. 7. Mogil JS, Silva Dutra GG, Pang DSJ, Chambers CT. The development and use of facial grimace scales for pain measurement in animals. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2020;116:480–93. pmid:32682741
  8. 8. Duhn LJ, Medves JM. A systematic integrative review of infant pain assessment tools. Advances in neonatal care. 2004;4(3):126–40. pmid:15273943
  9. 9. Banchi P, Quaranta G, Ricci A, Mauthe von Degerfeld M. Reliability and construct validity of a composite pain scale for rabbit (CANCRS) in a clinical environment. PloS one. 2020;15(4):e0221377. pmid:32352960
  10. 10. Cooper CS, Metcalf-Pate KA, Barat CE, Cook JA, Scorpio DG. Comparison of Side Effects between Buprenorphine and Meloxicam Used Postoperatively in Dutch Belted Rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 2009;48(3):279–85. pmid:19476717
  11. 11. Varga M. Textbook of rabbit medicine. 2nd ed: Butterworth Heinemann; 2014.
  12. 12. Wenger S. Anesthesia and analgesia in rabbits and rodents. Journal of Exotic Pet Medicine. 2012;21(1):7–16.
  13. 13. Morton DB, Griffiths PHM. Guidelines on the recognition of pain, distress and discomfort in experimental-animals and an hypothesis for assessment. Veterinary Record. 1985;116(16):431–6. pmid:3923690
  14. 14. Reid J, Nolan AM, Hughes JML, Lascelles D, Pawson P, Scott EM. Development of the short-form Glasgow Composite Measure Pain Scale (CMPS-SF) and derivation of an analgesic intervention score. Animal Welfare. 2007;16:97–104.
  15. 15. Calvo G, Holden E, Reid J, Scott EM, Firth A, Bell A, et al. Development of a behaviour-based measurement tool with defined intervention level for assessing acute pain in cats. The Journal of small animal practice. 2014;55(12):622–9. pmid:25470416
  16. 16. Benato L, Murrell J, Blackwell E, Saunders R, Rooney N. Pain and analgesia in pet rabbits: a survey of the attitude of veterinary nurses. J Small Anim Pract. 2020;61(9):576–81. pmid:32841364
  17. 17. Kohn DF, Martin TE, Foley PL, Morris TH, Swindle MM, Vogler GA, et al. Guidelines for the assessment and management of pain in rodents and rabbits. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 2007;46(2):97–108. pmid:17427317
  18. 18. Benato L, Rooney NJ, Murrell JC. Pain and analgesia in pet rabbits within the veterinary environment: a review. Vet Anaesth Analg. 2019;46(2):151–62. pmid:30737017
  19. 19. Wong LP. Focus group discussion: a tool for health and medical research. Singapore medical Journal 2008 49(3):256–61. pmid:18363011
  20. 20. Kinalski DD, Paula CC, Padoin SM, Neves ET, Kleinubing RE, Cortes LF. Focus group on qualitative research: experience report. Revista brasileira de enfermagem. 2017;70(2):424–9. pmid:28403311
  21. 21. Boateng GO, Neilands TB, Frongillo EA, Melgar-Quiñonez HR, Young SL. Best Practices for Developing and Validating Scales for Health, Social, and Behavioral Research: A Primer. Frontiers in public health. 2018;6:149. pmid:29942800
  22. 22. Farnworth MJ, Walker JK, Schweizer KA, Chuang CL, Guild SJ, Barrett CJ, et al. Potential behavioural indicators of post-operative pain in male laboratory rabbits following abdominal surgery. Animal Welfare. 2011;20(2):225–37.
  23. 23. Cronbach LJ. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika 1951:297–334.
  24. 24. Streiner DL. Starting at the beginning: an introduction to coefficient alpha and internal consistency. Journal of personality assessment. 2003;80(1):99–103. pmid:12584072
  25. 25. Mohsen Tavakol DR. Making sense of Cronbach’s alpha. International Journal of Medical Education. 2011;2:53–5. pmid:28029643
  26. 26. Ellen Y, Flecknell P, Leach M. Evaluation of Using Behavioural Changes to Assess Post-Operative Pain in the Guinea Pig (Cavia porcellus). Plos One. 2016;11(9):20. pmid:27583446
  27. 27. Morton CM, Reid J, Scott EM, Holton LL, Nolan AM. Application of a scaling model to establish and validate an interval level pain scale for assessment of acute pain in dogs. Am J Vet Res. 2005;66(12):2154–66. pmid:16379662
  28. 28. Leach MC, Allweiler S, Richardson C, Roughan JV, Narbe R, Flecknell PA. Behavioural effects of ovariohysterectomy and oral administration of meloxicam in laboratory housed rabbits. Research in Veterinary Science. 2009;87(2):336–47. pmid:19303122
  29. 29. Weaver LA, Blaze CA, Linder DE, Andrutis KA, Karas AZ. A Model for Clinical Evaluation of Perioperative Analgesia in Rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 2010;49(6):845–51. pmid:21205451
  30. 30. Sotocinal SG, Sorge RE, Zaloum A, Tuttle AH, Martin LJ, Wieskopf JS, et al. The Rat Grimace Scale: A partially automated method for quantifying pain in the laboratory rat via facial expressions. Molecular Pain. 2011;7. pmid:21241462
  31. 31. Langford DJ, Bailey AL, Chanda ML, Clarke SE, Drummond TE, Echols S, et al. Coding of facial expressions of pain in the laboratory mouse. Nature methods. 2010;7(6):447–9. pmid:20453868
  32. 32. Holton L, Reid J, Scott EM, Pawson P, Nolan A. Development of a behaviour-based scale to measure acute pain in dogs. Veterinary Record. 2001;148(17):525–31. pmid:11354645
  33. 33. Reid J, Nolan AM, Scott EM. Measuring pain in dogs and cats using structured behavioural observation. The Veterinary Journal. 2018;236:72–9. pmid:29871754
  34. 34. Ivone C-L, Amy M, Andrew B, Matthew L. Validation of mouse welfare indicators: a Delphi consultation survey. Scientific Reports [Internet]. 2019; 9(1):[1–11 pp.].
  35. 35. Rioja-Lang F, Bacon H, Connor M, Dwyer CM. Rabbit welfare: determining priority welfare issues for pet rabbits using a modified Delphi method. Veterinary Record Open. 2019;6(1):e000363. pmid:31903189
  36. 36. Voepel-Lewis T, Malviya S, Tait AR, Merkel S, Foster R, Krane EJ, et al. A comparison of the clinical utility of pain assessment tools for children with cognitive impairment. Anesthesia and analgesia. 2008;106(1):72–8. pmid:18165556
  37. 37. Pölkki , Korhonen A, Axelin A, Saarela T, Laukkala H. Development and preliminary validation of the Neonatal Infant Acute Pain Assessment Scale (NIAPAS). International Journal of Nursing Studies. 2014;51(12):1585–94. pmid:24815773
  38. 38. Gelinas C, Puntillo KA, Joffe AM, Barr J. A Validated Approach to Evaluating Psychometric Properties of Pain Assessment Tools for Use in Nonverbal Critically Ill Adults. Semin Respir Crit Care Med. 2013;34(2):153–68. pmid:23716307
  39. 39. Fournier-Charrière E, Tourniaire B, Carbajal R, Cimerman P, Lassauge F, Ricard C, et al. EVENDOL, a new behavioral pain scale for children ages 0 to 7 years in the emergency department: design and validation. Pain. 2012;153(8):1573–82. pmid:22609430
  40. 40. Stevens B, Johnston C, Petryshen P, Taddio A. Premature Infant Pain Profile: development and initial validation. The Clinical journal of pain. 1996;12(1):13–22. pmid:8722730
  41. 41. Klune CB, Larkin AE, Leung VSY, Pang D. Comparing the Rat Grimace Scale and a composite behaviour score in rats. PloS one. 2019;14(5):e0209467. pmid:31150408
  42. 42. Pinho RH, Leach MC, Minto BW, Rocha FDL, Luna SPL. Postoperative pain behaviours in rabbits following orthopaedic surgery and effect of observer presence. PloS one. 2020;15(10):e0240605. pmid:33091089
  43. 43. Price J, Catriona S, Welsh EM, Waran NKDoVCSRSoVSEBRMEHRGUK. Preliminary evaluation of a behaviour-based system for assessment of post-operative pain in horses following arthroscopic surgery. Vet Anaesth Analg. 2003;30(3):124–37. pmid:14498844
  44. 44. Flecknell PA, Liles JH, Williamson HA. The use of lignocaine-prilocaine local anaesthetic cream for pain-free venepuncture in laboratory animals. Laboratory animals (London). 1990;24(2):142. pmid:2366511
  45. 45. Bussieres G, Jacques C, Lainay O, Beauchamp G, Leblond A, Cadore JL, et al. Development of a composite orthopaedic pain scale in horses. Research in Veterinary Science. 2008;85(2):294–306. pmid:18061637
  46. 46. Gunn D, Morton DB. INVENTORY OF THE BEHAVIOR OF NEW-ZEALAND WHITE-RABBITS IN LABORATORY CAGES. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 1995;45(3–4):277–92.
  47. 47. Trocino A, Majolini D, Tazzoli M, Filiou E, Xiccato G. Housing of growing rabbits in individual, bicellular and collective cages: fear level and behavioural patterns. Animal. 2013;7(4):633–9. pmid:23153589
  48. 48. Verwer CM, van Amerongen G, van den Bos R, Hendriksen CFM. Handling effects on body weight and behaviour of group-housed male rabbits in a laboratory setting. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 2009;117(1–2):93–102.
  49. 49. Siniscalchi M, Laddago S, Quaranta A. Auditory lateralization of conspecific and heterospecific vocalizations in cats. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition. 2016;21(3):215–27.
  50. 50. van Loon J, Back W, Hellebrekers LJ, van Weeren PR. Application of a Composite Pain Scale to Objectively Monitor Horses with Somatic and Visceral Pain under Hospital Conditions. J Equine Vet Sci. 2010;30(11):641–9.
  51. 51. Brondani JT, Luna SPL, Padovani CR. Refinement and initial validation of a multidimensional composite scale for use in assessing acute postoperative pain in cats. Am J Vet Res. 2011;72(2):174–83. pmid:21281191
  52. 52. Goldschlager GB, Gillespie VL, Palme R, Baxter MG. Effects of Multimodal Analgesia with Low-Dose Buprenorphine and Meloxicam on Fecal Glucocorticoid Metabolites after Surgery in New Zealand White Rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 2013;52(5):571–6. pmid:24041213
  53. 53. DiVincenti L, Meirelles LAD, Westcott RA. Safety and clinical effectiveness of a compounded sustained-release formulation of buprenorphine for postoperative analgesia in New Zealand White rabbits. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association. 2016;248(7):795–801. pmid:27003021
  54. 54. Young T, Creighton E, Smith T, Hosie C. A novel scale of behavioural indicators of stress for use with domestic horses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 2012;140(1–2):33–43.
  55. 55. McCobb EC, Patronek GJ, Marder A, Dinnage JD, Stone MS. Assessment of stress levels among cats in four animal shelters. Javma-Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association. 2005;226(4):548–55. pmid:15742695