Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Cognitive bias analysis of young novice drivers’ observation abilities—A questionnaire-based study

  • Wang Xiang ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Wang Xiang, Xuemei Liu, Qingwan Xue

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Hunan Key Laboratory of Smart Roadway and Cooperative Vehicle-Infrastructure Systems, Changsha University of Science & Technology, Changsha, Hunan, China

  • Xuemei Liu ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Wang Xiang, Xuemei Liu, Qingwan Xue

    Roles Methodology, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Hunan Key Laboratory of Smart Roadway and Cooperative Vehicle-Infrastructure Systems, Changsha University of Science & Technology, Changsha, Hunan, China

  • Qunjie Peng ,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Shenzhen Transportation Design & Research Institute CO., LTD, Shenzhen, Guangdong, China

  • Qingwan Xue ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Wang Xiang, Xuemei Liu, Qingwan Xue

    Roles Writing – original draft

    qwxue@ncut.edu.cn

    Affiliation Beijing Key Laboratory of Urban Intelligent Traffic Control Technology, North China University of Technology, Beijing, China

  • Wei Hao ,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Hunan Key Laboratory of Smart Roadway and Cooperative Vehicle-Infrastructure Systems, Changsha University of Science & Technology, Changsha, Hunan, China

  • Ji Yu

    Roles Data curation, Methodology

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Shenzhen Transportation Design & Research Institute CO., LTD, Shenzhen, Guangdong, China

Abstract

Observation ability, which is the basis of following decision-making and vehicle manipulation behaviour, is of great importance while driving. However, the subject self-cognition and objective assessment of driving ability are usually different, especially for the young novice drivers. In this paper, drivers’ observation abilities for both static traffic signs and markings and dynamic surrounding vehicles were investigated based on questionnaire data. Effects of gender and driving characteristics (driving year, driving frequency, driving time) on drivers’ observation abilities were verified by ANOVA analysis and structural equation model (SEM) from two aspects: drivers’ self-assessment scores (self-assessment) and mutual assessment scores (evaluated by others). Significant difference could be found between all the factors and drivers’ self-assessment scores, while only driving year had a significant effect on drivers’ mutual assessment scores. Besides, cognitive bias was found between all the driving year groups. It seemed that drivers with driving experience less than one year were always overconfident with their driving abilities. And drivers with driving experience more than three years usually gave the most conservative assessment scores for themselves and others. With more exposures to various traffic conditions, experienced drivers are more aware of their limitations on observing surrounding information, while young novice drivers still not realized their limitations on observing traffic signs and other vehicles in a right way.

1. Introduction

During the past ten years, there has been a surge in number of young drivers in China. It was estimated that the number of young novice drivers (with a driving license less than 3 years) was 89 million, accounting for 24% among all the drivers in 2018. Young novice drivers have a higher crash risk than all the other driving age due to their lack of driving experience [1, 2]. Thus, novice drivers have become an important factor affecting road safety and efficiency due to the great amount and lacking of driving experience [3].

In fact, driving is a complex dynamic process-control activity that consists of observation, cognition, thought and decision, and operation functions. Among which, observation is the first step and all the following activities are conducted based on observation [4, 5]. For novice drivers, because of insecurity and mental stress, they normally look straight ahead, while some important information around, e.g. traffic signs, and may be ignored. Therefore, their effective vision fields are narrower than experienced drivers [6]. In addition, due to their fewer exposures to complex road scenarios, young novice drivers’ normally have a lower awareness of the potential risks, compared to experienced drivers [7, 8]. Studies have found that novice drivers’ observation abilities could be alleviated significantly by improving their driving experience [9]. Following driving practice, more and more useful information including both static road environment and dynamic traffic participants can be noticed by novice drivers [10]. Although novice drivers’ observation ability can be enhanced by practice, their self-cognition of driving abilities cannot be improved by this way [11].

Self-cognition of driving ability is an important factor which may affect drivers’ driving decision and their vehicle control behaviour [12]. But drivers are usually not able to make exact judgments during driving, i.e., judgment errors or inconsistencies between the judgment itself and the real traffic situation may occur, which is called cognitive bias. Cognitive bias of driving ability has been widely approved in previous studies [13, 14]. It was shown that many factors can affect drivers’ self-cognition of driving ability, such as age, gender, education, and driving practice [15, 16]. Results generated due to cognitive bias can be categorized into two types: one is underestimation and the other one is overestimation. For underestimation, drivers’ vehicle control behaviour tends to be conservative, e.g., keeping at a low driving speed; for overestimation, drivers usually keep a positive attitude towards the current traffic situation, which make their driving behaviour radical, e.g., speeding, frequently changing lanes [17, 18]. Although conservative drivers may have a lower crash risk compared with aggressive drivers, the traffic efficiency cannot be ensured.

For drivers’ psychology, accurate perception of own driving skill is especially desirable for traffic safety [19], since by knowing one’s own strengths and weaknesses, one can take efficient compensatory action to reduce collision risk [20]. Therefore, eliminate cognitive bias or reduce cognitive bias in some extents is of great importance. The purpose of this paper is to investigate the cognitive bias (overconfidence, or normal, or lack of confidence) of novice drivers’ observation abilities on both static objects (such as traffic facilities) and dynamic objects (such as surrounding vehicles). Factors such as gender and driving information (driving year, driving frequency and driving time) were considered when drivers rating the observation abilities for themselves and others. A structural equation model was built to investigate the casual relationship between each factor and drivers’ self-assessment and mutual-assessment.

2. Methodology

2.1 Participants

In this study, a total of 1031 questionnaires were collected by an online questionnaire survey. Through checking the integrity of the questionnaire answers and their relevance for similar questions, there are 569 valid questionnaires and 462 invalid questionnaires. As the aim of this study is to investigate young novice drivers’ observation abilities while driving, 435 respondents with the age between 18 and 25 years were chosen as the young novice group, and 134 drivers who were older than 25 were included in this study as the experienced drivers group. Table 1 shows the demographics information of all the participants included in this study.

2.2 Methodology

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Changsha University of Science & Technology to conduct anonymous questionnaire survey, and no additional informed participant consent was required. The data derived from drivers’ questionnaire drafted by Dr. Wang Xiang was analyzed. Dr. Wang Xiang and Xuemei Liu in Transportation Research Center administered this questionnaire. Moreover, the details about the questionnaire can be seen in S1 Appendix.

For the questionnaire adopted in this paper, participants were asked to complete two sections: one is self-assessment and the other section is mutual-assessment. For both self-assessment and mutual-assessment sections, participants had to rate their own and other’s observation abilities on a scale of one to ten from two aspects: drivers’ observation abilities of static object (road marking and signs) and dynamic object (signal and surrounding vehicles). And one referred to the weakest observation ability, and ten referred to the best observation ability. Fig 1 shows the basic structure and information included in the self-assessment survey. Similarly, each participant needed to rate other participants’ observation ability of static and dynamic information, e.g., How do you think drivers with driving experience less than one year can observe traffic signs (such as directional sign and stop sign) while driving? There are two questions for both static and dynamic information. The assessment score is the average value of two assessment scores for static (or dynamic) information. In addition, previous results were extracted as the objective driving ability based on meta-analysis to compare the self-assessment and mutual-assessment information.

The questionnaire had a high level of internal consistency as measured by Cronbach’s alpha (0.841). The significant value of Bartlett’s test of sphericity (p <0.001) and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) value of 0.722 indicated that the assumptions for factor analysis were met. Based on survey data, ANOVA was adopted in this study to test and compare the difference of observation ability among demographics (gender) and driving information (e.g., driving frequency).

A structural equation model (SEM) was built to explore the direct and indirect causal effects between each factors and drivers’ observation abilities. SEM can weigh the influences of the exogenous and endogenous variables [21], which combined with factor analysis and simultaneous equation models has been widely used as a linear-in-parameter multivariate statistical modeling technique. In this study, the SEM approach was used to investigate driving cognition as revealed by the causal relationships between driving years, self-assessment and mutual-assessment. The software package IBM SPSS Analysis of Moment Structures (AMOS) 21 was used to perform SEM analyses.

Drivers’ mean self-assessment score (SAS) and mutual-assessment score (MAS) in terms of gender, driving frequency, driving year and driving time are shown in Tables 2 and 3, respectively.

thumbnail
Table 2. Descriptive statistical of mean self-assessment score.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Descriptive statistical of mean mutual-assessment score.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.t003

3. Results

3.1 Self-assessment analysis

A one-way ANOVA was conducted between driving year and drivers’ observation ability self-assessment scores under both static information and dynamic information, respectively. Significant effects of driving year on SAS were found for both static information condition (F = 3.968, P<0.05) and dynamic information condition (F = 3.372, P<0.05). Generally, drivers’ SAS of observation ability increased with their driving years, as shown in Fig 2. Furthermore, effects of drivers’ gender, driving frequency and driving time on SAS were considered. For both static and dynamic conditions, male drivers’ SAS are higher than female drivers (see Fig 3). And with drivers’ driving year increased, both male and female drivers’ SAS increased, except for female drivers with driving experience of 1–3 years. Similarly, significant effects of driving frequency and driving time on drivers’ SAS were found for both static and dynamic conditions (show in Table 4). It was shown that with more practical driving experience, i.e., a higher driving frequency and more driving time, drivers’ SAS of observation ability increased as well (as shown in Figs 4 and 5, respectively).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Effect of driving year on self-assessment scores (SAS) under different driving year conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Effect of gender on self-assessment scores (SAS) under different driving year conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Effect of driving frequency on self-assessment score (SAS) under different driving year conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Effect of driving time on self-assessment scores (SAS) under different driving year conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.g005

thumbnail
Table 4. One-way ANOVA between drivers’ SAS and their characteristics.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.t004

3.2 Mutual-assessment analysis

Analysis between driver characteristics and drivers’ MAS were conducted as well. As shown in Table 5, only driving year has a significant effect on MAS. Similar with the SAS results in Section 3.1, drivers’ MAS increased significantly with the growth of their driving years for both static and dynamic objects, as shown in Fig 6. However, for drivers in in “>3” group, no significant effect of driving year was found (F = 3.372, P>0.05).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Effect of driving year on mutual-assessment scores (MAS) under different driving year conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.g006

thumbnail
Table 5. One-way ANOVA between MAS and driver characteristics.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.t005

3.3 Structural equation model

The basic structural correlation among all the variables is shown in Fig 7. The relationship between the hypothesized latent variables that affect the observation ability is quantified. Table 6 shows the definitions and input codes for the model. It was shown that driving year has a positive correlation effect on the self-rating score of drivers (the effect value is 0.36). The self-rating score of drivers with long driving year is higher than that of drivers with low driving year. However, driving year has a negative correlation effect on drivers’ MAS (the effect value is -0.38), and the scores given by drivers with long driving year is lower than that of drivers with low driving year. And drivers’ SAS has a positive correlation effect on MAS (the effect value is 0.45). The high self-assessment score of drivers also has a higher mutual assessment score. The SEM results are consistent with the results in section 3.1 and section 3.2.

thumbnail
Fig 7. Structural equation model (statistically significant at p<0.05).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.g007

To examine data fitting performance, a wide range of fit criteria were considered and listed in Table 7. It was shown that all the fitted variables met model requirements, which indicated that the model adopted in this study reach an acceptable fitness level.

3.4 Cognitive bias

3.4.1 Cognitive bias of drivers under different driving year groups.

A T-test was adopted in this paper between drivers’ SAS and MAS to explore drivers’ cognitive bias. Significant difference was found for both static object (t = 11.97, P<0.01) and dynamic object (t = 13.34, P<0.01). Fig 8 shows the comparison under different driving year conditions. Drivers’ SAS is much higher than MAS when drivers are in “<1” and “1~3” driving year group. However, for the “>3” driving year group, drivers’ SAS is slightly lower than MAS.

thumbnail
Fig 8. Comparisons of mean self-assessment and mutual-assessment score.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.g008

Detailed difference between drivers’ SAS and MAS in each driving year group was listed in Table 8 and Fig 9. For drivers with driving experience less than 1 year, their SAS are significant different from their MAS given by all driving year groups (e.g. “<1”→“<1”, “1~3”→“<1”, “>3”→“<1”). Drivers’ mean SAS of observation ability for static object in this group is 7.21, which is much higher than the MAS given by others, i.e., 6.11, 6.33 and 5.44, respectively. The same trend can be found for the dynamic objects’ observation ability. And the difference between SAS and MAS is greater when observe dynamic object than static object. For drivers those in driving year group “1~3”, significant difference from MAS was only found in driving year group “>3”, e.g., for static object, the T-test result was 12.662 (7.59 vs. 6.81), and for dynamic object, the T-test result was 9.741 (7.52 vs. 6.84). And for driving year group “>3”, no significant difference was found between drivers’ SAS and MAS given by the “>3” group. Different from driving year “<1” and “1–3” group, drivers’ mean SAS in this group is slightly lower than the MAS given by the “<1” and “1–3” group. For static object, drivers’ mean SAS is 7.9, while the MAS given by “<1” and “1–3” group are 8.13 and 8.31, respectively. Same results can be found for the dynamic object’s observation ability (7.89 vs. 8.32 and 8.46).

thumbnail
Fig 9. Comparison between self-assessment and mutual-assessment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.g009

thumbnail
Table 8. Differences between drivers’ self-assessment and mutual-assessment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.t008

The mutual-assessment difference was investigated in this study as well. Both assessors and assessed objects were divided by three driving years group (e.g. “<1”, “1~3”, “>3”). Mutual-assessment scores could be analyzed from two aspects (See Table 9 and Fig 9). For same assessors, drivers MAS given to others in different driving year groups are significantly different. Drivers’ MAS increased significantly with the growth of their driving years for both static and dynamic objects, as shown in Fig 9. For same assessed objects, drivers’ MAS given by others are significantly different when the assessed objects are drivers in “<1” group and “1~3” group. Drivers in “>3” group gave the lowest scores, drivers in “<1” group gave the intermediate scores, while drivers in “1~3” group gave the highest scores. Drivers’ MAS given by others are not significantly different when the assessed objects are drivers in “>3” group.

thumbnail
Table 9. Mutual-assessment difference among different driving year groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.t009

3.4.2 Meta-analyses of cognitive biases.

In this section, results obtained in this paper were compared with previous studies, from the aspects of novice drivers’ observation ability. As shown in Table 10, drivers’ driving ability and behavior are known to be affected by individual factors, such as gender, driving year, driving frequency, driving times etc. [15, 16, 22]. In this paper, gender, driving year, driving time and driving frequency all have significant effects on drivers’ observation abilities, for both static and dynamic objects. Consistent with previous studies, male drivers’ observation ability scores were higher than female drivers. Generally, drivers with more practical driving experience (e.g., longer driving year or more driving times) has better observation abilities than drivers with less driving experience.

thumbnail
Table 10. Meta-analyses of cognitive biases of influencing factors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.t010

Table 11 shows the cognitive bias between self-assessment and mutual assessment. The degree of cognitive bias between self-assessment and mutual assessment depends on the comparison method used to evaluate novice drivers (mean, ordinary observer, expert). In this paper, the proportion of novice drivers who overestimate their observation ability is lower than that of previous studies, which may be due to inconsistent comparison methods. The mutual assessment data are novice drivers’ assessment of other novice drivers, which is an average comparison.

thumbnail
Table 11. Meta-analyses of cognitive bias between self-assessment and mutual assessment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.t011

4. Discussion

For drivers’ self-assessment, gender, driving year, driving frequency and driving time all had significant effects on drivers’ SAS of observation abilities. Generally, male drivers are more confident than female drivers with higher SAS. And drivers with more practical driving experience (longer driving year, higher driving frequency and more driving time) are more confident than drivers driving less. All the findings obtained in this study were consistent with previous studies [23, 2527].

As for drivers’ mutual-assessment, no significant effects of drivers’ gender, driving frequency and driving time on MAS were found. But drivers’ MAS for both static and dynamic objects increased with the rising of driving year, which has been verified by previous studies [24, 26]. For both self-assessment and mutual-assessment, significant difference can be found for different driving year conditions. Thus, a SEM model was adopted in section 3.3. And the results of the SEM model also proved it.

Cognitive biases are tendencies commonly used to acquire and process information by filtering it through one’s own beliefs and experiences [35]. The difference between SAS and MAS, and also difference between MAS and MAS, both could be regarded as cognitive bias. According to the results in section 3.4, two kinds of cognitive bias can be obtained.

The first cognitive bias is the overconfidence induced by novice drivers. For the driving year “<1” group, their SAS were much higher than the MAS. Studies have shown that drivers with low driving ability are less accurate in self-assessment than those with high driving ability [36]. If the mutual-assessment scores of experienced drivers (i.e., driving year “>3” group) are objective, 76.45% of novice drivers are overconfident, especially for the driving year “< 1”group In other words, drivers with driving experience less than one year in “<1” group consider that although their observation ability is lower than drivers with more driving years, but themself is the outstanding person among drivers with equivalent driving experience.

The second cognitive bias is the irregular mutual-assessment of drivers in “1~3” group. Generally, drivers’ mutual-assessment scores given ought to show a certain trend, for example liking the sustained growth trend of self-assessment scores following the increase of driving year. However according to the comparison of drivers’ mutual-assessment scores given, drivers in “1~3” group gave the most optimistic assessment. And drivers in “>3” group gave the most conservative assessment (even less than drivers in “<1” group), which is consistent with their conservative driving behavior and safer vehicle maneuvers of experience drivers [37].

One cause of cognitive bias is that respondents wanted to present their positive views to the researchers even though considering themselves no higher than average [38]). The form of online survey in this paper could weaken this effect because lacking of the face-to-face mental stress. More likely, the inaccurate self-assessment of observation ability was caused by the lack of accurate cognition of driving ability [36]. This can well explain the first cognitive bias of novice drivers’ overconfident in this paper.

For the irregular mutual-assessment of drivers in “1~3” group, it showed their most optimistic attitude for driving. However, although drivers in “1~3” group involved in fewer traffic accidents (17.2% vs. 18%), involved more casualty accidents (16.7% vs. 15%) than drivers in “<1” group [39]. This may be caused by their incomplete driving experience. When novice drivers started driving, they used to keep extremely conservative driving behavior because of the fear of unknown driving environment, such as low speed and narrow visual field [40, 41]. This makes novice drivers always faced relatively simple driving tasks. High frequent visual stimuli of relative low risk driving tasks could make them skillfully observe general driving scenarios, which may let them believe an illusion of completely controllable. In other words, general confident rising may lead to drivers in “1~3” group feel that driving is an easy job. In fact, there are some complex driving scenarios they did not meet, such as the sudden intrusion of surrounding vehicle into their trajectory.

Based on above analysis, we tried to describe the changing mechanism of novice drivers’ observation ability following the rising of driving years (See Fig 10). Novice drivers are general overconfident, especially for drivers in “1~3” group. Regular driving practice will rapidly improve novice drivers’ observation ability for simple scenario. As the rising of confident, novice drivers gradually try a more difficult driving task, such as higher speed and more lanes change [42]. Then they may meet part complex scenario which corresponds to a higher need for observation. The observation difficult improvement will slow the growth trend of self-confidence, until it gets closer and closer to actual observation. Even part experienced drivers may less self-confident after coping with more and more complex scenarios, especially seeing or involving in a traffic accident.

thumbnail
Fig 10. Observation ability change schematic following the rising of driving years.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251195.g010

What can we do to improve novice drivers’ driving safety? We can provide solutions from three aspects. One is to reduce their cognitive observation ability. Provide driver feedback is a feasible method can help them overcome the pilot inaccurate self-assessment, now the European countries have the self-assessment feedback in driver training, as part of the driver’s license system [43]. For example, after some unsuccessful driving process, driving scene reproduction to novice driver could make them know their weaknesses clearly. The other is to improve their actual observation ability. Training is the feasible method, especially for complex scenarios novice drivers not often to meet [44]. For example, few novice drivers could deal with emergency collision avoidance successfully because the frequency of incident is very low in the real world, but repeating training can improve emergency response performance. Finally for traffic management department, extensive publicity and education to novice drivers is essential and effective. In addition, new traffic management technology (e.g. active luminous signs) and mobile on-board equipment (e.g. intelligent warning) are also worthy of consideration to assist novice drivers.

5. Conclusion

The first three years after novice drivers getting driving license is the critical improving period to gain driving experience and ability, and is the unsafe period easily involved in an accident. It is crucial whether novice drivers have accurate cognition for their driving abilities, especially observation abilities of static and dynamic traffic information while driving. This paper analyzed drivers’ self-assessment and mutual-assessment scores among different driving years based on online survey data, and results shown that two cognitive biases for novice drivers’ observation abilities occurred. The first cognitive bias was the general overconfidence induced by novice drivers. The second cognitive bias was the highlighted risk for drivers in “1~3” group because of their incomplete driving experience. And then observation ability change schematic following the rising of driving years was tried to present to guide the safety improving solutions for novice drivers. However, subjective survey results could describe the change trend, but not enough to dissect the specific possible causes and influence degree. Therefore, the next study will use driving simulator and eye-track to test novice drivers’ observation ability under detail driving scenarios, and then find their definite deficiency based on hierarchical driving observation difficulty (simple, complex, emergency).

Supporting information

References

  1. 1. Lee Y. C., & Winston F. K. (2016). Stress induction techniques in a driving simulator and reactions from newly licensed drivers. Transportation Research Part F Traffic Psychology & Behaviour, 42(pt.1), 44–55.
  2. 2. Yang Z., Yu Q., Zhang W., & Shen H. (2021). A comparison of experienced and novice drivers’ rear-end collision avoidance maneuvers under urgent decelerating events. Transportation Research Part F Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 76, 353–368.
  3. 3. Hassan H. M. (2016). Investigation of the self-reported aberrant driving behavior of young male saudi drivers: a survey-based study. Journal of Transportation Safety & Security, 8:2, 113–128. pmid:27648455
  4. 4. Chan C. Y. (2006). Characterization of driving behaviors based on field observation of intersection left-turn across-path scenarios. IEEE Transactions on intelligent transportation systems, 7(3), 322–331.
  5. 5. Walshe E. A., Winston F. K., & Romer D. (2017). 127 Attention, distraction, and crashes: working memory and texting while driving predict crashes in young novice drivers. SAVIR 2017.
  6. 6. Crundall D., Andrews B., van Loon E., & Chapman P. (2010). Commentary training improves responsiveness to hazards in a driving simulator. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42(6), 2117–2124.
  7. 7. Osborn A. F. and Owens D. A. (2010). Change Blindness: A Comparison of Selective Attention of Novice and Experienced Drivers. Journal of Vision. August 2, 10(7): 201;
  8. 8. Hickford A. J. (2010). Driver Assessment Route Trial: Literature Review. Transportation Research Group, School of Civil Engineering and Environment, University of Southampton, UK.
  9. 9. Mc Cartt A.T., Mayhew D.R., Braitman K.A., Ferguson S.A., Simpson H.M. (2009). Effects of age and 557 experience on young driver crashes: review of recent literature. Traffic Injury Prevention, 10, 209–219. pmid:19452361
  10. 10. Underwood G. (2007). Visual attention and the transition from novice to advanced driver. Ergonomics, 50 (8), 1235–1249. pmid:17558667
  11. 11. Martinussen Laila M., Møller Mette, Prato Carlo G. (2017). Accuracy of young male drivers’self-assessments of driving skill, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, Volume 46, Part A, 228–235.
  12. 12. Salvucci D. D. (2006). Modeling driver behavior in a cognitive architecture. Human factors, 48(2), 362–380. pmid:16884055
  13. 13. Falk B. (2010). Do drivers become less risk-prone after answering a questionnaire on risky driving behaviour? Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42(1), 235–244. pmid:19887164
  14. 14. Young K. L., & Salmon P. M. (2012). Examining the relationship between driver distraction and driving errors: A discussion of theory, studies and methods. Safety science, 50(2), 165–174.
  15. 15. Lucidi Fabio; Giannini Anna Maria; Sgalla Roberto. (2010). Young novice driver subtypes: Relationship to driving violations, errors and lapses. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42(6), 1689–1696. pmid:20728618
  16. 16. Zuckerman M. (2007). Sensation Seeking and Risky Driving, Sports, and Vocations. Sensation seeking and risky behavior (p.73–106). American Psychological Association.
  17. 17. Shbeeb L., Awad W. E., Suliman M. R., & Mujahed J. (2005). The relation between speed-lane choice and road accidents in Jordan. Proceedings of the Third International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, June, 2005: 416–425.
  18. 18. Tang J., Liu F., Zhang W., Ke R., & Zou Y. (2018). Lane-changes prediction based on adaptive fuzzy neural network. Expert Systems with Application, 91, 452–463.
  19. 19. Sundström A. (2008). Construct validation and psychometric evaluation of the self-efficacy scale for driver competence. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24(3), 198–206.
  20. 20. Horswill M. S., Sullivan K., Lurie-Beck J. K., & Smith S. (2013). How realistic are older drivers’ ratings of their driving ability? Accident Analysis & Prevention, 50, 130–137. pmid:23200449
  21. 21. Oud J., & Folmer H. (2008). A structural equation approach to models with spatial dependence. Geographical analysis, 40(2), 152–166.
  22. 22. Sundström A. (2005). Self-assessment of knowledge and abilities: a literature study. Umeå University. Sweden.
  23. 23. Wang Yuan, Bao Shan, Du Wenjun, Ye Zhirui, Sayer James R. (2017). Examining drivers’ eye glance patterns during distracted driving: insights from scanning randomness and glance transition matrix. Journal of Safety Research, 63, 149–155. pmid:29203013
  24. 24. Chapman P., Underwood G., & Roberts K. (2002). Visual search patterns in trained and untrained novice drivers. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 5(2), 157–167.
  25. 25. Underwood G., Crundall D., & Chapman P. (2011). Driving simulator validation with hazard perception. Transportation research part F: traffic psychology and behaviour, 14(6), 435–446.
  26. 26. Dukic T., Ahlstrom C., Patten C., Kettwich C., Kircher K. (2013). Effects of electronic billboards on driver distraction. Traffic Injury Prevention, 14, 469–476. pmid:23682577
  27. 27. Pradhan, A. K., Hammel, K. R., DeRamus, R., Pollatsek, A., Noyce, D. A., & Fisher, D. L. (2003). The use of eye movements to evaluate the effects of driver age on risk perception in an advanced driver simulator. In Proceedings of the 82 nd Annual Meeting of the Transportation Research Board, January, Washington, DC.
  28. 28. Amado S., Arıkan E., Kaça G., Koyuncu M., & Turkan B. N. (2014). How accurately do drivers evaluate their own driving behavior? An on-road observational study. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 63, 65–73. pmid:24269581
  29. 29. Rolim C., & Baptista P. (2018). Comparing drivers’ self-perception on driving behaviour changes with real world driving performance data: Lisbon case-study. Travel behaviour and society, 11: 86–92.
  30. 30. Park, George D; Allen, Huei-Yen Winnie; and Rosenthal, Theodore K. (2015). Novice Driver Simulation Training Potential for Improving Hazard Perception and Self-Confidence While Lowering Speeding Risk Attitudes for Young Males. In: Proceedings of the Eighth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, June 22–25, 2015, Salt Lake City, Utah. Iowa City.
  31. 31. Eby D. W., Molnar L. J., Shope J. T., Vivoda J. M., & Fordyce T. A. (2003). Improving older driver knowledge and self-awareness through self-assessment: The driving decisions workbook. Journal of safety research, 34(4), 371–381. pmid:14636659
  32. 32. Tronsmoen T. (2010). Associations between driver training, determinants of risky driving behaviour and crash involvement. Safety Science, 48 (1), 35–45.
  33. 33. Freund B., Colgrove L. A., Burke B. L., & McLeod R. (2005). Self-rated driving performance among elderly drivers referred for driving assessment. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 37(4), 613–618.
  34. 34. Horrey W. J., Lesch M. F., Mitsopoulos-Rubens E., & Lee J. D. (2015). Calibration of skill and judgment in driving: Development of a conceptual framework and the implications for road safety. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 76, 25–33. pmid:25560901
  35. 35. Hicks E. P., & Kluemper G. T. (2011). Heuristic reasoning and cognitive biases: are they hindrances to judgments and decision making in orthodontics?. American Journal of Orthodontics & Dentofacial Orthopedics, 139(3), 297–304. pmid:21392683
  36. 36. Craen S. D., Twisk D. A. M., Hagenzieker M. P., Elffers H., Brookhuis K. A. (2011). Do young novice drivers overestimate their driving skills more than experienced drivers? Different methods lead to different conclusions. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 43(5), 1660–1665. pmid:21658492
  37. 37. Yan W, Xiang W, Wong S.C., Yan X.D., Li Y.C., Hao W. Effects of hands-free cellular phone conversational cognitive tasks on driving stability based on driving simulation experiment. Transportation research part F: traffic psychology and behaviour, 58 (2018), 264–281.
  38. 38. Craen S. D., Twisk D. A. M., Hagenzieker M. P., Elffers H., & Brookhuis K. A. (2007). Do young novice drivers overestimate their driving skills. In Young researchers seminar in SWOW, Brno, Czech Republic.
  39. 39. Yixuan Sun. (2014). Traffic accident analysis based on data mining. PhD Thesis, Beijing Jiaotong University, Beijing.
  40. 40. Malik Husnain & Rakotonirainy A. & Larue Gregoire & Maire Frederic. (2011). How consistent are drivers in their driving? A driver training perspective. WIT Transactions on the Built Environment. 116.
  41. 41. Rantanen E, Goldberg J. H. (1999). The effect of mental workload on The visual field size and shape. Ergonomics, 42(6): 816–34. pmid:10340026
  42. 42. Tang J., Yu S., Liu F., Chen X., & Huang H. (2019). A hierarchical prediction model for lane-changes based on combination of fuzzy c-means and adaptive neural network. Expert Systems with Applications, 130, 265–275.
  43. 43. Dogan E., Steg L., Delhomme P., & Rothengatter T. (2012). The effects of non-evaluative feedback on drivers’ self-evaluation and performance. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 45, 522–528. pmid:22269538
  44. 44. Martín-delosReyes Luis Miguel, Jiménez-Mejías Eladio, Martínez-Ruiz Virginia, et al. (2019). Efficacy of training with driving simulators in improving safety in young novice or learner drivers: a systematic review. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology & Behaviour, 62, 58–65.