Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Population structure drives cultural diversity in finite populations: A hypothesis for localized community patterns on Rapa Nui (Easter Island, Chile)

  • Carl P. Lipo,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Anthropology, Environmental Studies Program, Harpur College of Arts and Sciences, Binghamton University, Binghamton, NY, United States of America

  • Robert J. DiNapoli ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    dinapoli@binghamton.edu

    Affiliation Department of Anthropology, Environmental Studies Program, Harpur College of Arts and Sciences, Binghamton University, Binghamton, NY, United States of America

  • Mark E. Madsen,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Anthropology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, United States of America

  • Terry L. Hunt

    Roles Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation The Honors College and School of Anthropology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States of America

Abstract

Understanding how and why cultural diversity changes in human populations remains a central topic of debate in cultural evolutionary studies. Due to the effects of drift, small and isolated populations face evolutionary challenges in the retention of richness and diversity of cultural information. Such variation, however, can have significant fitness consequences, particularly when environmental conditions change unpredictably, such that knowledge about past environments may be key to long-term persistence. Factors that can shape the outcomes of drift within a population include the semantics of the traits as well as spatially structured social networks. Here, we use cultural transmission simulations to explore how social network structure and interaction affect the rate of trait retention and extinction. Using Rapa Nui (Easter Island, Chile) as an example, we develop a model-based hypothesis for how the structural constraints of communities living in small, isolated populations had dramatic effects and likely led to preventing the loss of cultural information in both community patterning and technology.

Introduction

Rapa Nui is a small (164 km2) and remote island in the easternmost South Pacific (Fig 1) that was colonized by Polynesian voyagers in the 12th-13th Century AD [14]. The island is perhaps best known for the hundreds of multi-ton stone statues (moai) that the islanders constructed and transported over volcanic terrain to every part of the island and placed atop massive stone platforms (ahu) [57]. The magnitude of Rapa Nui’s monumental architecture is often seen as paradoxical in contrast with the island’s size. At just 23 km in its longest dimension, the island can be easily traversed at a walking pace in under a day. Curiously, for an island so small archaeological and ethnohistoric evidence indicates that pre-contact communities did not interact in an island-wide, panmictic fashion, but rather were organized into a series of distinctive subgroups. Traditionally, at least eight clan groups are known to have spatial territories in the late 19th and early 20th century [8,9]. Detailed analyses of the archaeological record, however, show that many aspects of community interaction likely occurred on an even more localized basis with communities consisting of multiple small, dispersed, and relatively autonomous groups [e.g., 10,11]. Analyses of spatial variability in biological and cultural traits suggest an island population structured into many relatively independent communities organized around ahu locations and their immediate local resources [e.g., 12,13]. This evidence points to a significant degree of diversity over small geographic areas on the island in cultural and biological traits. While this diversity and degree of localized community structure is unexpected for such a small island, no model-based hypotheses have been presented to account for these patterns.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Rapa Nui.

The location of Rapa Nui in the easternmost corner of Polynesia.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g001

Models of random drift offer a productive source of hypotheses for the patterns and distribution of diversity. Drift, whether among inherited genetic or cultural variants, is the change produced by the underlying mechanisms through which variation is sampled and passed on through time. Understanding how random drift can shape cultural diversity has been a significant focus of investigations for cultural evolutionary studies [e.g., 1419]. Using this concept, researchers have explored how one can detect drift from other factors that shape changes in trait frequencies through time and space [e.g., 2027]. As has been demonstrated, the effects of drift depend on the size of the population. In large populations, drift tends to cause relatively small changes in trait frequency. In small populations, however, drift can lead to rapid changes in trait frequencies, potentially resulting in fixation or loss of variants. In these situations, drift can produce dramatic differences in historic outcomes: two populations drawn from a single population may begin with an identical frequency of traits, but can rapidly diverge in composition. As we explore in this paper, the effect of drift in small populations often leads to rapid loss of richness (i.e., the number of variants within a population) and diversity (i.e., the distribution of variants within a population) [28].

The factors that drive the effects of drift—population size in particular—have received a great deal of attention as a possible explanation for cultural change. On one hand, some have argued that demography and population sizes strongly shaped cultural history. For example, Henrich [29] argues that the loss of technology among Tasmanian communities throughout the Holocene was a consequence of drift acting on the reduced size of isolated populations. In an opposite scenario, Powell et al. [30] have argued that increases in population sizes were part of the process leading to the explosion of technology and cultural variants in the Late Pleistocene [see also 3133]. Yet, these kinds of explanations have been strongly criticized [e.g., 3441].

While debate continues about whether changes in population size explain particular cases of cultural variability, it is well-established that drift contributes to cultural change among small and isolated populations [e.g., 31,4247]. Population size, however, is not the only significant factor that can change the degree to which drift leads to changes in cultural variability. Of particular note is the work of Premo [24,25,48,49], who has demonstrated how population structure, in particular, plays a key role in shaping diversity in populations. Derex and colleagues [5052] have also highlighted how the degree of population fragmentation maximizes the rate of accumulation of cultural traits. In particular, partially connected populations can produce highly diverse solutions to adaptive problems not possible in fully connected groups [50].

Understanding the effect of drift on cultural variability is particularly important in the study of island settings where populations tend to be isolated to relative degrees and limited in size compared with continental contexts. Small and isolated island populations provide model systems to study the relationship of limited population size, drift, community patterning, and interaction on cultural diversity [e.g., 12,5355]. For islands, the key aspect of drift and its effects is not just on factors that favor cultural trait accumulation [50,56,57], but also those favoring cultural trait retention [51,52]. The retention of traits can be beneficial when they confer information about past states and cumulative cultural solutions to environmental and social problems [58] (p.278). In isolated locations such as islands and where populations regularly face uncertainty about future environmental conditions [e.g., 5963], cumulative cultural knowledge has great potential value. Thus, mechanisms and strategies that preserve diversity and richness likely have fitness consequences. To examine factors that favor trait retention, we present forward time simulations that permit us to configure populations and their interactions in various ways. We first establish that the simulations conform to the expectations of the drift model and the findings of previous researchers [e.g., 24,28,50,52]. We then use this model to develop hypotheses about the history and community structure of Rapa Nui (Fig 1). Using the cultural transmission models presented here, we hypothesize that the observed patterning of community interaction may be related to the maintenance of cumulative cultural information. We then review archaeological spatial data from Rapa Nui to evaluate community patterns within the context of this model. While some components of the model predictions are difficult to thoroughly test given the somewhat limited archaeological data, the overall patterns in biological and artifactual remains suggest broad agreement with the core model expectations. We conclude with a discussion of these limitations as areas for future research.

Networks, population structure, and drift

The effects of random drift on variability are mechanical and largely depend on population sizes. Yet a significant factor is variability in population structure, which can play a key role in shaping cumulative cultural diversity in populations [e.g., 24,25,4852]. From these studies, it is clear that the impact of drift is greatest when populations are well-mixed. Or conversely, the greater the degree of structure within a population, the more likely that variability will be retained, all other things held constant. The relation between population structure and drift can be demonstrated by modeling population interaction as a network. Structure within a population can be represented by a network where vertices represent individuals (N) and edges represent the potential interaction between those individuals (e.g., mating or social learning). The structure of the network then varies by the number of edges between individual vertices (k, the network degree), from immediate neighbors (k = 2) to all other vertices (k = N-1).

As Schneider and colleagues [28] have shown, the effects of drift on diversity can be countered by a combination of mutation (or innovation) rate and/or highly structured (low k) networks. Following Schneider et al. [28], given a population of size N and mutation rate μ, drift dominates whenever 2μN⪡1. For 2μN⪢1, on the other hand, mutation dominates over the effect of random drift, maximizing diversity. The transition occurs at a threshold, μc = 1/2N, where the equilibrium distribution of trait frequencies becomes uniform. This threshold (kc) is a critical point, above which random drift is insensitive to population spatial structure. Below kc, small degrees of connectivity (k) offer high degrees of spatial structure that, when combined with mutation rate, lead to increases in overall diversity [e.g., 64,65]. In this way, the diversity of traits can be increased either by increasing the mutation rate at fixed k or by decreasing k at a fixed mutation rate. Schneider et al. [28] (p.15) remark that “in consonance with classical results, extreme restriction in [gene] flow is required for structuring to affect. In fact, the mutation rate above which drift is overcome changes significantly only when the degree of the network becomes very small.”

In terms of information retention within a population, our interest is not focused on the effect of mutation (or innovation), but on population structure (k). While innovation serves to increase overall diversity, its effect is to alter that information and thus the potential contribution it might have to future fitness consequences. Population structure, on the other hand, reflects the way individuals or groups of individuals within a population interact and could serve to retain information. To explore this effect, we need to model various consequences of different configurations of populations. Given the aggregate nature of the archaeological record, these models accommodate populations interacting at two different scales: individuals interacting within a small community of well-mixed structure, and those communities interacting amongst one another.

To illustrate how population structure affects diversity and the retention of traits, we used simuPOP [66,67] to simulate drift within populations of varying configurations. SimuPOP is a Python-based population simulator that allows one to evolve populations forward in time under varying configurations of mutation, recombination, migration, and population/subpopulation sizes. We based our simulations on a simple Wright-Fisher model [68,69] that explores changes in a haploid population of fixed size, N. Traits are modeled as values within a single locus, in a way that is equivalent to attributes along a single dimension [sensu 70]. In the model, traits for individuals are derived from each time-step by sampling with replacement from the pool of other individuals (i.e., the previous “generation”). This pattern of copying traits is effectively random, implying that an individual has an equal probability to interact with anyone else in the population. At each step when an individual copies traits, there is a fixed chance of innovation where a new trait is introduced.

To model variation in population structure, we divided the overall population into a varying number of subpopulations. Within each subpopulation, copying is assumed to be random but is not allowed between subpopulations. With subpopulations, drift can produce a unique combination of traits even if the initial conditions begin identically. Subpopulations can be configured to interact with other subpopulations by copying traits depending on number and pattern of links between points in the configuration (Fig 2). In simuPOP, we can also vary the between-group interaction rate (modeled in SimuPOP as “migraption probability”), the likelihood that individuals from a subpopulation copies traits from other subpopulations. All code for these simulations is available at https://github.com/clipo/network-drift.

thumbnail
Fig 2. An example of three network configurations of 25 subpopulations connected to varying degrees.

These networks are Watts-Strogatz small-world graphs [71]. The colored circles represent individual subpopulations and the lines between the circles represent the interaction between subpopulations. This figure shows three configurations of subpopulations connected just to neighbors (k = 2) to 10 other subpopulations (k = 10), and all other subpopulations (k = 24). In these models, there is an equal probability of copying between all connected subpopulations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g002

Our simulations explore the impact of 5000 individuals separated into a series of subpopulations that interacted under varying configurations (S1 Table). Our simulations are designed to approximate a single population on an island organized into a series of interacting groups (subpopulations); a pattern evident on pre-contact Rapa Nui [e.g., 10,11,72]. To isolate the effects of drift, we set the innovation rate to zero and began with identical subpopulations with an even distribution of traits along a single locus. In the simulation, each timestep is composed of an event where individuals in subpopulations copy traits randomly from within their subpopulation, and with a small probability of copying traits from another connected subpopulation. Note that these timesteps are not meant to represent “biological” generations, but simply repeated steps of maximum social learning among all individuals. To calculate a 95% confidence interval for diversity measures of the population over time, we aggregated values from multiple runs (e.g., 10 or more).

To evaluate the effects of network structure on diversity, at each time step we calculate diversity in trait frequencies in subpopulations using an FST statistic [73], a widely used estimator of Wright’s fixation index. The FST statistic is a measure of population differentiation based on trait differences between populations. FST is calculated as the correlation of randomly chosen trait values within the same subpopulation relative to that found in the entire population. FST is calculated as: (Eq 1) where p is the average frequency of the trait in the total population, σS2 is the variance of the frequency of the trait between different subpopulations. Values for FST range from 0 to 1. FST should be close to zero if populations are identical in their trait distributions. Values of 1 indicate significant differentiation among populations.

Schneider et al. [28] demonstrate that variability in network connectivity between individuals will affect overall diversity. For Rapa Nui, we were interested in examining whether community interaction patterns would promote the retention of information over time, thus providing a potential benefit to communities under changing and unpredictable conditions. To assess this possibility, we look at diversity, the distribution of traits in the population, and richness, the number of traits in the population. Thus, in our simulation, we measure FST as well as track richness values of traits in the overall population over time to see how network structure affects these values.

Network configuration

We first simulated 5000 individuals divided into 200 subpopulations and varied the number of connections between the subpopulations from k = 2, k = 50, and k = 120 (Figs 3 and 4). We ran each simulation for 2000 timesteps and tracked diversity and richness measures for each run. Multiple runs of the simulation provide a 95% confidence interval for the diversity and richness values and are represented as the grey lines surrounding each statistic in the following graphs. As seen in the figures, our results match the expectations of Schneider et al. [28]: the magnitude of drift’s effect on variability depends on the degree of network connectivity. The populations that are split into subpopulations with the lowest degree of connectivity (e.g., k = 2) retain the greatest diversity and richness compared to those configurations with higher degrees of connectivity.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Change in diversity (as measured by FST) over 2000 timesteps of a simulation with 200 subpopulations under varying network connectivity conditions (k = 2 [blue], k = 50 [green], k = 120 [red]).

Subpopulations are initialized with an even distribution of traits. Due to drift, diversity increases the greatest in the set of subpopulations that have the least degree of connectivity. In this and following figures, the grey bands around each data series represent the 95% confidence interval of the statistic across all simulation runs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Change in richness (number of different traits present in the population) over 2000 timesteps of a simulation with 200 subpopulations under varying network connectivity conditions (k = 2 [blue], k = 50 [green], k = 120 [red]).

In these simulations, populations begin with the same degree of richness. The loss of traits is dramatically quicker for networks with a greater degree of connectivity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g004

Network distance

The connectivity between vertices is just one dimension of how spatial configurations of populations can alter the effect of drift. The more general component of network structure is the overall distance between the vertices. The greater the distance between vertices (subpopulations), the lower the overall effects of drift. Distance between vertices is determined by two factors. First, the number of edges between vertices contribute to distances—the more edges that connect to each vertex, the shorter the distance and the greater the influence of drift on the population overall. Second, we can vary the number of vertices (i.e., subpopulations) that make up the population—the greater the number of vertices, the larger the distance between any two locations given the network connectivity. Thus, populations divided into an array of smaller subpopulations will experience the effects of drift less than those that are divided into fewer, larger subpopulations.

We demonstrate this effect in a series of simulations varying the number of subpopulations (Figs 5 and 6). Starting with a fixed population size of 5000, we vary the number of subpopulations by 20, 50, and 200, but fix the connectivity between subpopulations to just neighbors (i.e., k = 2). We then track diversity and richness over 5000 timesteps. As in the case where we varied the degree of connectivity, the populations divided into more subpopulations led to greater diversity and richness, even when subject to the same degree of connectivity.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Changes in diversity over 5000 timesteps of a simulation in which the network connectivity is kept constant (k = 2) but the number of subpopulations is increased (subpopulations = 20 [blue], subpopulations = 50 [green], subpopulations = 200 [red]).

In these simulations, populations begin an even distribution of traits. Diversity is substantially higher for network configurations with a larger number of subpopulations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g005

thumbnail
Fig 6. Changes in richness over 5000 timesteps of a simulation with network connectivity kept constant (k = 2), but the number of subpopulations increased (subpopulations = 20 [blue], subpopulations = 50 [green], subpopulations = 200 [red]).

In these simulations, populations begin with the same degree of richness. The loss of traits is dramatically quicker for networks with a fewer number of subpopulations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g006

The key finding is that the most critical factor in preserving variability in a population is the graph degree: the number of vertices between any two edges. The higher the graph degree, the greater the potential retention of diversity and richness. In a population structure modeled as a graph, degree increases inversely with the number of interactions between subpopulations as well as with the number of subpopulations. Given this factor, we expect population structures that are modeled as large small-world graphs [71], where subpopulations are linked only to neighbors, as particularly effective in retaining diversity and richness. In contrast, populations structured as random graphs with a low degree as well as those with hierarchical structure will tend to be less effective in retaining diversity and richness as a result of drift.

Interaction rate between communities

In the previous examples, we set the rate of interaction between communities (i.e., the probability of copying between subpopulations, referred to as the “migration rate” in the SimuPOP simulation package) to constant at a low level (i.e., 0.25%/timestep). Increasing the probability of interaction has a similar effect of increasing the impact of drift on population diversity and richness. The greater the migration probability, the greater the effects of drift. Figs 7 and 8 show the results of a series of simulation runs in which we varied the probability of between-group interaction (from 0.0001 to 0.005) and the degree of connectivity (from k = 5 to k = 190) while keeping the number of subpopulations constant (N = 200). The values of the between-group interaction rates are arbitrarily chosen to be low relative to the number of individuals in each subpopulation. The choice of the number of subpopulations evaluated is also arbitrary and selected to evaluate a range of at least one order of magnitude. The combination of low between-group interaction probability and low connectivity between subpopulations is especially potent in retaining richness. This pattern is consistent with our findings on how the number of subpopulations and rate of between-group interaction influence the richness and diversity: significant network degree results in greater diversity and richness (Figs 9 and 10).

thumbnail
Fig 7. The effects of network structure and the rate of between group-interaction on overall trait diversity within a population.

In this set of simulations, we divided populations of 5000 into a series of 200 subpopulations and varied connectivity from k = 5 to k = 190 in steps of 5. For each value of k, we ran the simulation with between-group interaction rates that ranged from 0.0001 to 0.005. The resulting data are the mean values of FST calculated from 10 runs at the point of 2000 timesteps. The values of diversity are shown as a heatmap where the high levels of diversity are illustrated in dark blue and the low levels are in light blue. Diversity is best maintained under conditions of the low rate of between-group interaction and low levels of connectivity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g007

thumbnail
Fig 8. The effects of trait network structure and between-group interaction rates on trait richness within a population.

In this set of simulations, we divided populations of 5000 into a series of 200 subpopulations and varied connectivity from k = 5 to k = 190 in steps of 5. For each value of k, we ran the simulation with the rate of between-group interaction ranging from 0.0001 to 0.005. The resulting data are the mean values of richness calculated from 10 runs at the point of 2000 timesteps. The richness of traits is best maintained under conditions of relatively low rates of between-group interaction and low levels of k.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g008

thumbnail
Fig 9. Diversity measured by FST among a population at 2000 timesteps with network connectivity of k = 2 with a variable number of subpopulations (5–200) and between-group interaction rates (0.0001–0.005).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g009

thumbnail
Fig 10. The richness of traits in a population at 2000 timesteps with network connectivity of k = 2 with a variable number of subpopulations (5–200) and between-group interaction rates (0.0001–0.005).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g010

The retention of rare traits

Our simulation results demonstrate the expected effects of drift as presented by Schneider et al. [28]: the degree of network distance and reduced interaction among subsets of individuals in a population strongly influences the retention of traits that would otherwise be lost to drift. In remote and isolated island settings, loss of traits could have potential implications for survival. The retention of traits would have particular value in the context of uncertainty or facing problems that occur only rarely. If traits offer information about past conditions, then they might offer advantages when similar conditions return. At the scale of an island population, only one group on the island would need to retain traits and share and act on those traits at an appropriate time. This condition would likely contribute to group-level fitness differences through differential sorting of community behavior. Thus, finding the conditions that maximize the retention of rare traits potentially sheds light on ways island communities adapted to serve this purpose.

Our simulations illustrated these conditions by exploring the configurations of the between-group interaction rate and degree of connectivity that favor the retention of rare information by examining the number of subpopulations that hold a unique trait not shared with any other subpopulation. For each combination of k and rate of between-group interaction, we ran the simulation for 2000 timesteps and then counted the number of subpopulations that held a trait not found elsewhere. As expected, the combination of factors that favor retention of rare traits consists of low between-group interaction and low levels of connectivity (Figs 11 and 12). Population structure, therefore, plays a key role in preserving rare but potentially crucial information.

thumbnail
Fig 11. The impact of network structure is determined by connectivity and rates of between-group interaction on the number of traits found only in one subpopulation after 2000 timesteps.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g011

thumbnail
Fig 12. The impact of network structure is determined by the number of subpopulations (k = 2) and the rates of between-group interaction on the number of traits found only in one subpopulation after 2000 timesteps.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g012

A model for population structure on Rapa Nui

Located almost 2000 km from Pitcairn Island, its closest intermittently inhabited neighbor, the island is one of the most isolated places on Earth. The archaeological record suggests the population was isolated soon after initial settlement, with little, if any, interaction with other islands of Polynesia [3,72,74,75]. The island’s subtropical environment is also quite marginal, with poor soil nutrients, limited surface freshwater, and no large coral reefs or a lagoon. While the island never had abundant resources or rich soils, it was transformed by humans over ca. 500 years through the introduction of the commensal Pacific rat [7678], forest clearance [79,80], and the establishment of vast lithic mulch gardens for food production [e.g., 8187]. Recent studies show that freshwater sources available in groundwater discharge (springs) predict the locations of ahu and point to community activities centered on this shared critical resource [8890]. Given the diminutive size of Rapa Nui and its relatively marginal environment, the island never supported a particularly large population. While there have been claims of large population sizes on the order of 17,500–30,000 individuals [e.g., 9194], the archaeological and historical evidence indicates a substantially smaller population likely around 5,000 maximum [3,8,10,11,72,95].

Unpredictability in rainfall brought critical uncertainty to agricultural productivity and the availability of drinking water [11,12,96]. Based on an analysis of soil, historic rainfall data, topography, and substrate age, Morrison [11] (p.184) shows that “between one time and two times a decade many of the areas of the island are only marginally suitable for agriculture or not suitable at all.” With such unpredictability in conditions necessary for survival (i.e., food and water), past knowledge about problems and solutions would have selective value. For example, if an individual knew how to survive an extended drought using particular cultivation or water management strategies, such as many unique strategies used by Rapanui people [e.g., 87,89], that individual and their community would be better off than those without such knowledge. The effects of drift on this small and isolated population would thus prove challenging for retaining cultural knowledge shared in oral traditions by individual-to-individual social learning mechanisms. Yet, given that the Rapanui people thrived for 500 years before the arrival of Europeans [1,3,97], islanders must have adapted strategies of community patterning and the use of technology to mitigate the effects of drift.

Based on the effects seen in these simulations, we can examine how the population structure of Rapa Nui could have led to trait retention despite the island’s limited size and isolated, small population. Here, we model various configurations of the population using archaeologically known spatial locations of communities. Following ethnohistoric accounts and archaeological research [e.g., 6,10,11,72], we know that image ahu (statue platforms) served as a central feature of numerous small, dispersed communities. We used the locations of image ahu to model varying degrees of interaction among these communities and evaluate the outcomes of the overall island diversity and richness of shared traits. The model is parameterized with 150 subpopulations corresponding to the number of ahu sites. It is important to note that the number of distinct subpopulations is unknown and we simply use this high number of subpopulations for the purposes of illustration. The key aspect of the model is not the absolute number of subpopulations, but the degree of interaction between them (k). Fig 13 shows three configurations of contact between ahu communities: from interactions that are limited only to intermediate neighbors (k = 5), to a scenario of communities interacting regionally with 50 other locations (k = 50), and to a configuration of communities interacting with nearly all other locations (k = 140). Using 5000 individuals as the overall population of the island and a low rate of between-group interaction (0.0001), we simulated interaction and tracked diversity and richness at the scale of the island.

thumbnail
Fig 13. Interaction configurations of Rapa Nui communities as modeled by ahu locations.

(A) Image ahu locations as determined by Martinsson-Wallin [6] and DiNapoli et al. [90]; (B) Connectivity of communities interacting primarily with local neighbors; (C) Population structure of local communities interacting with the 50 nearest other communities; and (D) Nearly panmictic population structure of local communities interacting with 140 other locations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g013

Using these models, we simulated population interaction for 2000 timesteps under conditions of low between-group interaction (i.e., 0.0001/copying event) and with no mutation (Figs 14 and 15). Note that the length of the simulation time steps is somewhat arbitrary and was selected simply to ensure convergence to a steady state. As expected, conditions of extremely low connectivity (k = 5) resulted in a high degree of diversity (measured as FST) and retained the greatest degree of richness over the span of the simulation. We also conducted a parameter sweep to look at the relations between between-group interaction rate and k network connectivity (Figs 16 and 17).

thumbnail
Fig 14. Impact of drift on trait diversity (measured as FST) on Rapa Nui communities located around image ahu under three network configurations (k = 5, 50, 140).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g014

thumbnail
Fig 15. Impact of drift on trait richness on Rapa Nui communities located around image ahu under three network configurations (k = 2, 50, 140).

Notably, the lower the degree of connectivity (k) the greater the retention of traits.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g015

thumbnail
Fig 16. Trait richness after 2000 timesteps in the set of subpopulations modeled on Rapa Nui as a function of network connectivity (k) and between-group interaction rate.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g016

thumbnail
Fig 17. Traits found only in one subpopulation as a consequence of network connectivity (k) and between-group interaction rate after 2000 timesteps.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g017

Overall, our findings suggest that if conditions on Rapa Nui favored the retention of information, we would expect the patterns of interaction among communities to be highly localized—strongly spatially biased to the point that individual communities would primarily share information internally and with limited interaction to just immediate neighboring communities.

Rapa Nui community patterning

Given the consequences of drift structured by the interaction between subpopulations, do the modeling results help us account for the observed archaeological record of the island? The archaeological evidence suggests the number of subpopulations was high relative to the overall small size of the island. Settlement pattern analyses of the surface archaeological record support the notion that pre-contact communities were remarkably small, numerous, and centered around ahu locations [10,11,72]. While we lack comprehensive chronological information for many ahu, a recent model-based synthesis of radiocarbon dates indicates that at least some of these ceremonial platforms were built and used contemporaneously over the course of the island’s history [1]. While Stevenson [98100] argues that there were just 11 communities on the island, this claim is founded on the assumption that the largest 11 ahu structures were the focal points for the larger territorial units. The available archaeological and historical data, however, indicate that there was a much larger number of relatively independent communities centered around the numerous smaller image ahu. Analyses by Morrison [11] for example, show that the island-wide settlement patterns is characterized by a series of repeated sets of functionally redundant feature classes (e.g., domestic features, earth ovens, walled gardens, etc.) extending over just 300–500 meters in area. This finding is consistent with early ethnohistoric observations. In 1786, for example, the French captain La Pérouse [101] (p.26) notes “the conjectures which may be formed respecting the government of these islanders are, that they compose a single nation, divided into as many districts as there are morais [ahu]; because it is to be remarked, that the villages are built near these burying places.” This historical observation reinforces Morrison’s [11] conclusion that Rapa Nui’s settlement pattern is characterized by a series of multiple small communities centered around ahu locations.

It is most likely that interaction across the island was nested and varied in spatial distances from extremely local to more island-wide interaction. While communities almost certainly interacted to some degree at larger scales to procure certain raw materials from their source locations, the characteristics of artifacts made from these materials show highly localized scales of cultural transmission. Pukao, the large red scoria “hats” associated with at least 50 moai, were mostly quarried from a single location at Puna Pau and transported to various locations around the island, and their formal variability is spatially patterned by proximity of their destinations at ahu sites [102]. Though more analyses are needed, the moai (statues) and ahu vary stylistically by spatial proximity over the island, and in the case of moai were mostly carved from Rano Raraku statue quarry [e.g., 6,7,100]. Furthermore, several researchers have concluded that the moai quarry shows evidence of multiple independent work areas [e.g., 9,103,104]. Similarly, obsidian was an important raw material available from four source locations on the southwestern part of the island, and obsidian artifacts are abundant in archaeological contexts across the island [105]. While islanders made similar classes of obsidian artifacts, such as stemmed obsidian tools (mata‘a), frequency seriations by Lipo et al. [106,107] demonstrate that formal variability in the hafted portion of mata’a is strongly spatially biased. Likewise, earth ovens (umu) also show patterns of similarity in shape reflecting localized interaction. In the case of umu on the southwest sector of Rapa Nui [10,108], and frequency seriation results show spatial patterns indicating traditions of making an umu reflect sharing among immediate neighbors (Fig 18). Together, the available data and analyses of formal variability in a range of artifact classes indicate that while islanders did produce similar kinds of artifacts from common raw material source locations, formal variability in material culture shows that information sharing about manufacture styles and techniques was often constricted to small local areas.

thumbnail
Fig 18. Spatial patterns of seriation solutions of umu seriation.

Frequency seriation of umu shape variability by location. There are two viable solutions to this frequency seriation. Data from Table 1 in McCoy [108].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250690.g018

Another aspect of Rapa Nui material culture is rongorongo, a system of glyphs carved into wooden tablets. The antiquity of rongorongo is unknown, although current evidence suggests some of the known tablets likely date to the post-contact period [8,109,110]. The single radiocarbon date from a rongorongo tablet collected in 1871 [110] (Beta-184112, 80 +/- 40 BP) calibrates to 1695–1725 cal AD (11.7%) and 1805–1950 cal AD (83.7%) using the SHcal20 calibration curve [111]. Based on ethnographic work by Metraux [8] (pp. 389–409), these glyphs likely represent abstract concepts consistent with mnemonic devices for recording memorized chants of stories or genealogies. As Metraux [8] (p.404) describes, “all the traditions and all the statements of the natives agree that the tablets were associated with memorized chants. … the symbols formed a sort of pictography in the sense that each glyph was associated with a particular sentence or group of words in a chant. The symbols did not correspond exactly to a specific chant, but each tablet could be used with many chants, and several sentences were linked with each image.” Why do we see this form of information transfer on Rapa Nui and not elsewhere in Polynesia [8,109]? While arrangements of knots were used for similar mnemonic purposes elsewhere in Polynesia [109] (p.167-169), these systems are fairly simple compared to the potential informational complexity encoded by the rongorongo scripts and tablets. Our results suggest an intriguing hypothesis that the glyphs of rongorongo served as mnemonic devices that were particularly key in encoding, transmitting, and retaining cultural information in Rapa Nui’s precarious environment.

In addition to the evidence for material culture, highly localized interaction is reflected in human remains. Genetic evidence from pre-contact skeletal material points to strong localized interaction. Dudgeon’s [112,113] research using genetic data from human teeth demonstrates a high proportion of similarity at the site and subpopulation level. Dudgeon [112] also shows that patterns of minor and trace elements found in the dental enamel are regionally distinctive, as do other stable isotope analyses [e.g., 114,115]. In his 2008 analysis using Mahalanobis’ posterior classification of human remains, Dudgeon successfully groups instances of human remains by spatial proximity more than eighty percent of the time. This result points to individuals who were largely constrained in their consumption of food and water to localized areas of the island. Analyses of pre-contact skeletal traits show similar localized patterns. For example, variation in non-metric cranial traits reveals strong spatial patterning [116], and the appearance and frequency of rare discrete traits point to limited intra-island gene flow [113]. Gill and colleagues [13,117] note the high frequency of discrete traits in some co-interred individuals on the north and west coasts and for cave internments near ahu on the south coast. These discrete features include the bipartite patella and fused Sacroiliac joints. Notably, females show less mobility than males. In a study using minimum genetic distance, Stefan [118] found evidence of greater between-group homogeneity within the male skeletal sample, indicating higher island-wide mobility of males compared to females. Taken together, the biological and archaeological data suggest strongly localized patterns of cultural and genetic transmission. Given the model results, we hypothesize that such locally structured interaction on Rapa Nui would have promoted retention of cultural diversity and richness; an adaptation that mitigated the potentially deleterious effects of drift on this small island with a small overall population.

Limitations and future directions

Despite its small size, Rapa Nui communities appear to have been relatively small, numerous, and interacted primarily with close neighbors. The adaptive aspects of this highly-localized community patterning may be explained by the cultural transmission model presented: increases in overall network distance were beneficial in the retention of cultural information that might otherwise be at risk from the effects of drift. Cumulative cultural information would have value facing challenges arising beyond the lifespan of single individuals. The adaptive aspects of localized interaction on Rapa Nui may have ensured that cultural diversity and information were not lost among communities that were small and isolated. Such solutions work to optimize group interactions in the creation and retention of innovations. Retaining localized knowledge can be particularly important on small islands where conditions for success may require highly specific knowledge systems [e.g., 119]. In some cases, these knowledge systems may need to be highly localized; an important consideration in planning for uncertainty of climate change for these island locations.

While the archaeological, historical, and biological evidence broadly conforms to the model expectations, there remain limitations in the data that preclude more rigorous model fitting and comparison. While different lines of evidence offer a coherent picture of highly localized genetic and cultural transmission, these broad scale patterns are somewhat at odds with finer scale parameterizations and predictions of the cultural transmission model. The available archaeological data, particularly from abundant lithic artifacts (mata‘a) and cooking features (umu), are relevant to examine the models’ predictions, but detailed information remains unrecorded or unanalyzed for much of the island. Once larger sample sizes from broader spatial scales become available, promising future research would be to examine the fit between temporal and spatial patterns of artifactual frequency data and the model expectations using Approximate Bayesian Computation or other means of model assessment [e.g., 27,120].

The case we present from Rapa Nui furthers our understanding of the mechanisms that drive changes in cultural diversity more broadly, which remains an important and highly debated issue in cultural evolutionary research. Several pioneering studies proposed models whereby changes in effective population sizes may account for large-scale changes in cultural complexity in human history [e.g., 29,30,121]. These studies, however, have often not explicitly accounted for the fact that humans live in variable social networks, and recent models have shown that population structure is equally important to demography in influencing changes in the diversity and complexity of cultural traits [e.g., 28,5052]. Here, we provide a model for exploring how changes in the configuration and patterns of interaction among and between subpopulations can dramatically increase diversity as well as retain richness in even relatively small and isolated populations.

Acknowledgments

We thank Thegn Ladefoged and two anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions that improved the manuscript. We also thank Hiroki Sayama and the Center for Collective Dynamics of Complex Systems (CoCo) at Binghamton University for helpful feedback on the modeling. A version of this paper was presented at the 84th annual meeting of the Society for American Archaeology in Albuquerque, NM in the symposium Defining and Measuring Diversity in Archaeology, and we thank the organizers, Metin Eren and Briggs Buchanan.

References

  1. 1. DiNapoli RJ, Rieth TM, Lipo CP, Hunt TL. A model-based approach to the tempo of “collapse”: The case of Rapa Nui (Easter Island). J Archaeol Sci. 2020;116: 105094.
  2. 2. Hunt TL, Lipo CP. Late colonization of Easter island. Science. 2006;311: 1603–1606. pmid:16527931
  3. 3. Hunt TL, Lipo C. The Archaeology of Rapa Nui (Easter Island). In: Cochrane EE, Hunt TL, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Prehistoric Oceania. New York: Oxford University Press; 2018. pp. 416–449. Available: http://www.oxfordhandbooks.com/view/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199925070.001.0001/oxfordhb-9780199925070-e-026.
  4. 4. Wilmshurst JM, Hunt TL, Lipo CP, Anderson AJ. High-precision radiocarbon dating shows recent and rapid initial human colonization of East Polynesia. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011;108: 1815–1820. pmid:21187404
  5. 5. Lipo CP, Hunt TL, Haoa SR. The ‘walking’ megalithic statues (moai) of Easter Island. J Archaeol Sci. 2013;40: 2859–2866.
  6. 6. Martinsson-Wallin H. Ahu—The Ceremonial Stone Structures of Easter Island. Uppsala: Societas Archaeologica Upsaliensis; 1994.
  7. 7. Van Tilburg JA. Easter Island: Archaeology, Ecology, and Culture. Washington, D.C: Smithsonian Institution Press; 1994.
  8. 8. Métraux A. Ethnology of Easter Island. Honolulu: Bishop Museum Press; 1940.
  9. 9. Routledge K. The Mystery of Easter Island. New York: Cosimo; 1919.
  10. 10. McCoy PC. Easter Island Settlement Patterns in the Late Prehistoric and Protohistoric Periods. New York: Easter Island Committee; 1976.
  11. 11. Morrison A. An archaeological analysis of Rapa Nui settlement structure: a multi-scalar approach. Unpublished PhD Dissertation, University of Hawaii, Manoa. 2012.
  12. 12. DiNapoli RJ, Morrison AE, Lipo CP, Hunt TL, Lane BG. East Polynesian Islands as Models of Cultural Divergence: The Case of Rapa Nui and Rapa Iti. J Isl Coast Archaeol. 2018;13: 206–223.
  13. 13. Gill GW, Stefan VH. Rapanui origins, relationships, and warfare: A summary in theoretical context. In: Stefan VH, Gill GW, editors. Skeletal Biology of the Ancient Rapanui (Easter Islanders). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2016. pp. 286–302.
  14. 14. Bentley RA, Hahn MW, Shennan SJ. Random drift and culture change. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;271: 1443–1450. pmid:15306315
  15. 15. Bentley RA, Lipo CP, Herzog HA, Hahn MW. Regular rates of popular culture change reflect random copying. Evol Hum Behav. 2007;28: 151–158.
  16. 16. Hahn MW, Bentley RA. Drift as a mechanism for cultural change: an example from baby names. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003;270: S120–S123. pmid:12952655
  17. 17. Herzog HA, Bentley RA, Hahn MW. Random drift and large shifts in popularity of dog breeds. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2004;271: S353–S356. pmid:15504016
  18. 18. Lipo C, Madsen M, Dunnell R, Hunt T. Population Structure, Cultural Transmission, and Frequency Seriation. J Anthropol Archaeol. 1997;16: 33.
  19. 19. Shennan SJ, Wilkinson JR. Ceramic Style Change and Neutral Evolution: A Case Study from Neolithic Europe. Am Antiq. 2001;66: 577–593.
  20. 20. Kandler A, Shennan S. A non-equilibrium neutral model for analysing cultural change. J Theor Biol. 2013;330: 18–25. pmid:23538207
  21. 21. Kandler A, Wilder B, Fortunato L. Inferring individual-level processes from population-level patterns in cultural evolution. R Soc Open Sci. 2017;4. pmid:28989786
  22. 22. Kandler A, Steele J. Innovation diffusion in time and space: effects of social information and of income inequality. Diffus Fundam. 2009;11: 1–17.
  23. 23. Mesoudi A, Lycett SJ. Random copying, frequency-dependent copying and culture change. Evol Hum Behav. 2009;30: 41–48.
  24. 24. Premo LS. Effective Population Size and the Effects of Demography on Cultural Diversity and Technological Complexity. Am Antiq. 2016;81: 605–622.
  25. 25. Premo L, Scholnick J. The spatial scale of social learning affects cultural diversity. Am Antiq. 2011;76: 163–176.
  26. 26. Premo LS, Tostevin GB. Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape: Exploring the Effects of Taskscape Visibility on Cultural Diversity. PLOS ONE. 2016;11: e0161766. pmid:27583682
  27. 27. Crema ER, Edinborough K, Kerig T, Shennan SJ. An Approximate Bayesian Computation approach for inferring patterns of cultural evolutionary change. J Archaeol Sci. 2014;50: 160–170.
  28. 28. Schneider DM, Martins AB, de Aguiar MAM. The mutation–drift balance in spatially structured populations. J Theor Biol. 2016;402: 9–17. pmid:27132184
  29. 29. Henrich J. Demography and Cultural Evolution: How Adaptive Cultural Processes can Produce Maladaptive Losses: The Tasmanian Case. Am Antiq. 2004;69: 197–214.
  30. 30. Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG. Late Pleistocene Demography and the Appearance of Modern Human Behavior. Science. 2009;324: 1298–1301. pmid:19498164
  31. 31. Kline MA, Boyd R. Population size predicts technological complexity in Oceania. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2010;277: 2559–2564. pmid:20392733
  32. 32. Marquet PA, Santoro CM, Latorre C, Standen VG, Abades SR, Rivadeneira MM, et al. Emergence of social complexity among coastal hunter-gatherers in the Atacama Desert of northern Chile. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109: 14754–14760. pmid:22891345
  33. 33. Martens JP. Scenarios where increased population size can enhance cumulative cultural evolution are likely common. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2019;116: 17160–17160. pmid:31420520
  34. 34. Collard M, Buchanan B, O’Brien MJ, Scholnick J. Risk, mobility or population size? Drivers of technological richness among contact-period western North American hunter–gatherers. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2013;368: 20120412. pmid:24101622
  35. 35. Collard M, Ruttle A, Buchanan B, O’Brien MJ. Population Size and Cultural Evolution in Nonindustrial Food-Producing Societies. PLoS ONE. 2013;8: e72628. pmid:24069153
  36. 36. Collard M, Buchanan Briggs, O’Brien MJ. Population Size as an Explanation for Patterns in the Paleolithic Archaeological Record: More Caution Is Needed. Curr Anthropol. 2013;54: S388–S396.
  37. 37. Fay N, Kleine ND, Walker B, Caldwell CA. Increasing population size can inhibit cumulative cultural evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2019;116: 6726–6731. pmid:30872484
  38. 38. Fay N, Kleine ND, Walker B, Caldwell CA. Reply to Martens: Various factors may enable large populations to enhance cumulative cultural evolution, but more evidence is needed. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2019;116: 17161–17162. pmid:31420519
  39. 39. Vaesen K. Cumulative Cultural Evolution and Demography. PLoS ONE. 2012;7: e40989. pmid:22848419
  40. 40. Vaesen K, Collard M, Cosgrove R, Roebroeks W. Population size does not explain past changes in cultural complexity. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016; 201520288. pmid:27298472
  41. 41. Vaesen K, Collard M, Cosgrove R, Roebroeks W. Reply to Henrich et al.: The Tasmanian effect and other red herrings. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113: E6726–E6727. pmid:27791150
  42. 42. Aoki K, Lehmann L, Feldman MW. Rates of cultural change and patterns of cultural accumulation in stochastic models of social transmission. Theor Popul Biol. 2011;79: 192–202. pmid:21315753
  43. 43. Bell AV. Linking Observed Learning Patterns to the Evolution of Cultural Complexity. Curr Anthropol. 2015;56: 277–281.
  44. 44. Henrich J, Boyd R, Derex M, Kline MA, Mesoudi A, Muthukrishna M, et al. Understanding cumulative cultural evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113: E6724–E6725. pmid:27791123
  45. 45. Kempe M, Mesoudi A. An experimental demonstration of the effect of group size on cultural accumulation. Evol Hum Behav. 2014;35: 285–290.
  46. 46. Kobayashi Y, Aoki K. Innovativeness, population size and cumulative cultural evolution. Theor Popul Biol. 2012;82: 38–47. pmid:22555004
  47. 47. Riede F. The Loss and Re-Introduction of Bow-and-Arrow Technology: A Case Study from the Northern European Late Paleolithic. Lithic Technol. 2009;34: 27–45.
  48. 48. Premo LS, Hublin J-J. Culture, population structure, and low genetic diversity in Pleistocene hominins. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2009;106: 33–37. pmid:19104042
  49. 49. Premo LS, Kuhn SL. Modeling Effects of Local Extinctions on Culture Change and Diversity in the Paleolithic. PLoS ONE. 2010;5: e15582. pmid:21179418
  50. 50. Derex M, Boyd R. Partial connectivity increases cultural accumulation within groups. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113: 2982–2987. pmid:26929364
  51. 51. Derex M, Mesoudi A. Cumulative Cultural Evolution within Evolving Population Structures. Trends Cogn Sci. 2020;24: 654–667. pmid:32466991
  52. 52. Derex M, Perreault C, Boyd R. Divide and conquer: intermediate levels of population fragmentation maximize cultural accumulation. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2018;373: 20170062. pmid:29440527
  53. 53. Barton NH. Natural selection and random genetic drift as causes of evolution on islands. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;351: 785–795. pmid:8693020
  54. 54. DiNapoli RJ, Leppard TP. Islands as Model Environments. J Isl Coast Archaeol. 2018;13: 157–160.
  55. 55. Frankham R. Do island populations have less genetic variation than mainland populations? Heredity. 1997;78: 311–327. pmid:9119706
  56. 56. Lehmann L, Aoki K, Feldman MW. On the number of independent cultural traits carried by individuals and populations. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2011;366: 424–435. pmid:21199846
  57. 57. Strimling P, Sjöstrand J, Enquist M, Eriksson K. Accumulation of independent cultural traits. Theor Popul Biol. 2009;76: 77–83. pmid:19427878
  58. 58. Henrich J. The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter. Illustrated Edition. Princeton Oxford: Princeton University Press; 2017.
  59. 59. Allen MS. Bet-hedging strategies, agricultural change, and unpredictable environments: historical development of dryland agriculture in Kona, Hawaii. J Anthropol Archaeol. 2004;23: 196–224.
  60. 60. Allen MS. Oscillating climate and socio-political process: the case of the Marquesan Chiefdom, Polynesia. Antiquity. 2010;84: 86–102.
  61. 61. Dickinson WR. Holocene Sea-Level Record on Funafuti and Potential Impact of Global Warming on Central Pacific Atolls. Quat Res. 1999;51: 124–132.
  62. 62. DiNapoli RJ, Morrison AE. A spatiotemporal model of risk and uncertainty for Hawaiian dryland agriculture and its implications for ahupua’a community formation. J Archaeol Sci Rep. 2017;15: 109–119.
  63. 63. Lee CT, Tuljapurkar S, Vitousek PM. Risky Business: Temporal and Spatial Variation in Preindustrial Dryland Agriculture. Hum Ecol. 2006;34: 739–763.
  64. 64. de Aguiar MAM, Baranger M, Baptestini EM, Kaufman L, Bar-Yam Y. Global patterns of speciation and diversity. Nature. 2009;460: 384. pmid:19606148
  65. 65. Martins AB, de Aguiar MAM, Bar-Yam Y. Evolution and stability of ring species. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2013;110: 5080–5084. pmid:23479635
  66. 66. Peng B, Kimmel M. simuPOP: a forward-time population genetics simulation environment. Bioinformatics. 2005;21: 3686–3687. pmid:16020469
  67. 67. Peng B, Kimmel M, Amos CI. Forward-Time Population Genetics Simulations: Methods, Implementation, and Applications. New York: Wiley-Blackwell; 2015. Available: https://www.wiley.com/en-us/Forward+Time+Population+Genetics+Simulations%3A+Methods%2C+Implementation%2C+and+Applications-p-9780470503485.
  68. 68. Fisher RA. XXI.—On the Dominance Ratio. Proc R Soc Edinb. 1923;42: 321–341.
  69. 69. Wright S. Evolution in Mendelian Populations. Genetics. 1931;16: 97–159. pmid:17246615
  70. 70. Dunnell RC. Systematics in Prehistory. New York: The Free Press; 1971.
  71. 71. Watts DJ, Strogatz SH. Collective dynamics of ‘small-world’ networks. Nature. 1998;393: 440–442. pmid:9623998
  72. 72. Hunt TL, Lipo CP. The Statues That Walked: Unraveling the Mystery of Easter Island. New York: Free Press; 2011.
  73. 73. Weir BS, Cockerham CC. Estimating F-Statistics for the Analysis of Population Structure. Evolution. 1984;38: 1358–1370. pmid:28563791
  74. 74. Barnes SS, Matisoo-Smith E, Hunt TL. Ancient DNA of the Pacific rat (Rattus exulans) from Rapa Nui (Easter Island). J Archaeol Sci. 2006;33: 1536–1540.
  75. 75. Hunt TL, Lipo CP. Evidence for a Shorter Chronology on Rapa Nui (Easter Island). J Isl Coast Archaeol. 2008;3: 140–148.
  76. 76. Hunt TL. Rethinking Easter Island’s ecological catastrophe. J Archaeol Sci. 2007;34: 485–502.
  77. 77. Hunt TL, Lipo CP. Chronology, deforestation, and “collapse:” Evidence vs. faith in Rapa Nui prehistory. Rapa Nui J. 2007;21: 85–97.
  78. 78. Hunt TL, Lipo CP. Revisiting Rapa Nui (Easter Island) ‘“Ecocide.”‘ Pac Sci. 2009;63: 601–616.
  79. 79. Mann D, Edwards J, Chase J, Beck W, Reanier R, Mass M, et al. Drought, vegetation change, and human history on Rapa Nui (Isla de Pascua, Easter Island). Quat Res. 2008;69: 16–28.
  80. 80. Rull V. The deforestation of Easter Island. Biol Rev. 2020;95: 124–141. pmid:31599482
  81. 81. Bork H-R, Mieth A, Tschochner B. Nothing But Stones? A review fo the extent and technical efforts of prehistoric stone mulching on Rapa Nui. Rapa Nui J. 2004;18: 10–14.
  82. 82. Stevenson CM, Wozniak J, Haoa S. Prehistoric agricultural production on Easter Island (Rapa Nui), Chile. Antiquity. 1999;73: 801–812.
  83. 83. Stevenson CM, Jackson TL, Mieth A, Bork H-R, Ladefoged TN. Prehistoric and early historic agriculture at Maunga Orito, Easter Island (Rapa Nui), Chile. Antiquity. 2006;80: 919–936.
  84. 84. Ladefoged TN, Flaws A, Stevenson CM. The distribution of rock gardens on Rapa Nui (Easter Island) as determined from satellite imagery. J Archaeol Sci. 2013;40: 1203–1212.
  85. 85. Vitousek PM, Chadwick OA, Hotchkiss SC, Ladefoged TN, Stevenson CM. Farming the Rock: A biogeochemical perspective on intensive agriculture in Polynesia. J Pac Archaeol. 2014;5: 51–61.
  86. 86. Wozniak JA. Prehistoric Horticultural Practices on Easter Island: Lithic Mulched Gardens and Field Systems. Rapa Nui J. 1999;13: 95–99.
  87. 87. Wozniak JA. Subsistence strategies on Rapa Nui (Easter Island): prehistoric gardening practices on Rapa Nui and how they relate to current farming practices. In: Haoa-Cardinali S, Ingersoll KB, Ingersoll DW Jr, Stevenson CM, editors. Cultural and Environmental Change on Rapa Nui. New York: Routledge; 2018. pp. 87–112.
  88. 88. Brosnan T, Becker MW, Lipo CP. Coastal groundwater discharge and the ancient inhabitants of Rapa Nui (Easter Island), Chile. Hydrogeol J. 2019;27: 519–534.
  89. 89. Hixon SW, DiNapoli RJ, Lipo CP, Hunt TL. The Ethnohistory of Freshwater Use on Rapa Nui (Easter Island, Chile). J Polyn Soc. 2019;128: 163–190.
  90. 90. DiNapoli RJ, Lipo CP, Brosnan T, Hunt TL, Hixon S, Morrison AE, et al. Rapa Nui (Easter Island) monument (ahu) locations explained by freshwater sources. PLOS ONE. 2019;14: e0210409. pmid:30629645
  91. 91. Bahn P, Flenley J. Easter Island, Earth Island: The Enigmas of Rapa Nui. Fourth Edition. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield; 2017.
  92. 92. Diamond J. Easter’s end. Discover. 1995;9: 62–69.
  93. 93. Diamond J. Collapse: how societies choose to fail or succeed. New York: Viking; 2005.
  94. 94. Puleston CO, Ladefoged TN, Haoa S, Chadwick OA, Vitousek PM, Stevenson CM. Rain, Sun, Soil, and Sweat: A Consideration of Population Limits on Rapa Nui (Easter Island) before European Contact. Front Ecol Evol. 2017;5: 1–14.
  95. 95. Lipo CP, DiNapoli RJ, Hunt TL. Commentary: Rain, Sun, Soil, and Sweat: A Consideration of Population Limits on Rapa Nui (Easter Island) before European Contact. Front Ecol Evol. 2018;6.
  96. 96. Louwagie G, Stevenson CM, Langohr R. The impact of moderate to marginal land suitability on prehistoric agricultural production and models of adaptive strategies for Easter Island (Rapa Nui, Chile). J Anthropol Archaeol. 2006;25: 290–317.
  97. 97. DiNapoli RJ, Lipo CP, Hunt TL. Revisiting warfare, monument destruction, and the ‘Huri Moai’ phase in Rapa Nui (Easter Island) culture history. J Pac Archaeol. 2020 [cited 11 Oct 2020]. Available: https://pacificarchaeology.org/index.php/journal/article/view/313.
  98. 98. Stevenson CM. Corporate Descent Group Structure in Easter Island Prehistory. Ph.D. Dissertation, Pennsylvania State University. 1984.
  99. 99. Stevenson CM. The socio-political structure of the southern coastal area of Easter Island: AD 1300–1864. In: Kirch PV, editor. Island Societies: Archaeological Approaches to Evolution and Transformation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1986. pp. 69–77.
  100. 100. Stevenson CM. Territorial divisions on Easter Island in the 16th century: evidence from the distribution of ceremonial architecture. In: Ladefoged TN, Graves MW, editors. Pacific Landscapes: Archaeological Approaches. Los Osos: Bearsville Press; 2002. pp. 211–229.
  101. 101. La Pérouse J-F de G comte de. A voyage round the world, performed in the years 1785, 1786, 1787, and 1788 by the Boussole and Astrolabe, under the command of J.F.G. de la Pérouse. Published by order of the National Assembly, under the superintendence of L.A. Milet-Mureau. The Third Edition. Vol II. London: Lackington, Allen, and Co.; 1807.
  102. 102. Hixon SW, Lipo CP, McMorran B, Hunt TL. The colossal hats (pukao) of monumental statues on Rapa Nui (Easter Island, Chile): Analyses of pukao variability, transport, and emplacement. J Archaeol Sci. 2018;100: 148–157.
  103. 103. Hamilton S, Nahoe Arellano S, Richards C, Torres H. F. Quarried away: thinking about landscapes of megalithic construction on Rapa Nui (Easter Island). In: David B, Thomas J, editors. Handbook of Landscape Archaeology. World Archaeological Congress; 2008. pp. 176–186.
  104. 104. Richards C, Croucher K, Paoa T, Parish T, Tucki E, Welham K. Road my body goes: re-creating ancestors from stone at the great moai quarry of Rano Raraku, Rapa Nui (Easter Island). World Archaeol. 2011;43: 191–210.
  105. 105. Stevenson CM, Ladefoged TN, Haoa S, Chadwick O, Puleston C. Prehistoric Obsidian Exchange on Rapa Nui. J Isl Coast Archaeol. 2013;8: 108–121.
  106. 106. Lipo CP, Hunt TL, Hundtoft B. Stylistic variability of stemmed obsidian tools (mata’a), frequency seriation, and the scale of social interaction on Rapa Nui (Easter Island). J Archaeol Sci. 2010;37: 2551–2561.
  107. 107. Lipo CP, Hunt TL, Hundtoft B. An Analysis of Stylistic Variability of Stemmed Obsidian Tools (Mata’a) on Rapa Nui (Easter Island). In: Goodale N, Andrefsky W, editors. Lithic Technological Systems and Evolutionary Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2015. pp. 225–238. Available: http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=fvpDBgAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PA225&dq=hunt+lipo+mataa+antiquity&ots=v2ERAgMdGg&sig=vaI0JD_J3IBlfAztxI6QKFKKjHU.
  108. 108. McCoy PC. Stone-lined earth ovens in Easter Island. Antiquity. 1978;52: 204–216.
  109. 109. Fischer SR. Rongorongo: The Easter Island Script: History, Traditions, Text. New York: Oxford University Press; 1997.
  110. 110. Orliac C. The Rongorongo tablets from Easter Island: botanical identification and 14C dating. Archaeol Ocean. 2005;40: 115–119. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1834-4453.2005.tb00597.x.
  111. 111. Hogg AG, Heaton TJ, Hua Q, Palmer JG, Turney CS, Southon J, et al. SHCal20 SOUTHERN HEMISPHERE CALIBRATION, 0–55,000 YEARS CAL BP. Radiocarbon. undefined/ed; 1–20.
  112. 112. Dudgeon JV. The genetic architecture of the late prehistoric and protohistoric Rapanui. PhD Dissertation, University of Hawaii, Manoa. 2008.
  113. 113. Dudgeon JV, Commendador AS, Tromp M. Archaeogenetics and paleodemographic estimation of founding populations: Features of residential geography on Rapa Nui. In: Gill GW, Stefan VH, editors. Skeletal Biology of the Ancient Rapanui (Easter Islanders). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2016. pp. 202–221. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139151856.012
  114. 114. Commendador AS, Dudgeon JV, Finney BP, Fuller BT, Esh KS. A stable isotope (δ13C and δ15N) perspective on human diet on rapa nui (Easter Island) ca. AD 1400–1900. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2013;152: 173–185. pmid:23996514
  115. 115. Polet C, Bocherens H. New insights into the marine contribution to ancient Easter Islanders’ diet. J Archaeol Sci Rep. 2016;6: 709–719.
  116. 116. Chapman PM. The Role of Biological Anthropology in Easter Island Research. Rapa Nui J. 1996;10: 53–56.
  117. 117. Gill GW, Haoa S, Owsley DW. Easter Island: Implications of osteological findings. Rapa Nui J. 1997;11: 64–71.
  118. 118. Stefan VH. Craniometric Variation and Homogeneity in Prehistoric/Protohistoric Rapa Nui (Easter Island) Regional Populations. Am J Phys Anthropol. 1999;110: 407–419. pmid:10564572
  119. 119. McMillen HL, Ticktin T, Friedlander A, Jupiter SD, Thaman R, Campbell J, et al. Small islands, valuable insights: systems of customary resource use and resilience to climate change in the Pacific. Ecol Soc. 2014;19. Available: https://www.jstor.org/stable/26269694.
  120. 120. Crema ER, Kandler A, Shennan S. Revealing patterns of cultural transmission from frequency data: equilibrium and non-equilibrium assumptions. Sci Rep. 2016;6: 39122. pmid:27974814
  121. 121. Shennan S. Demography and Cultural Innovation: a Model and its Implications for the Emergence of Modern Human Culture. Camb Archaeol J. 2001;11: 5–16.