Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Emotional stability is related to 2D:4D and social desirability in women: Possible implications on subjective well-being and psychopathology

  • Ángel Rodríguez-Ramos ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    angel.rguezramos@gmail.com

    Affiliations Department of Genetics, University of Córdoba, Córdoba, Spain, Department of Psychology, University of Córdoba, Córdoba, Spain, Maimónides Biomedical Research Institute of Córdoba (IMIBIC), Córdoba, Spain, University Hospital Reina Sofía of Córdoba, Córdoba, Spain

  • Juan Antonio Moriana,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, University of Córdoba, Córdoba, Spain, Maimónides Biomedical Research Institute of Córdoba (IMIBIC), Córdoba, Spain, University Hospital Reina Sofía of Córdoba, Córdoba, Spain

  • Francisco García-Torres,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, University of Córdoba, Córdoba, Spain, Maimónides Biomedical Research Institute of Córdoba (IMIBIC), Córdoba, Spain, University Hospital Reina Sofía of Córdoba, Córdoba, Spain

  • Manuel Ruiz-Rubio

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Genetics, University of Córdoba, Córdoba, Spain, Maimónides Biomedical Research Institute of Córdoba (IMIBIC), Córdoba, Spain, University Hospital Reina Sofía of Córdoba, Córdoba, Spain

Abstract

Emotional stability-Neuroticism is a complex construct influenced by genetics and environmental factors. Women tend to exhibit higher neuroticism scores than men, which may be associated with an increased risk of suffering from some common mental conditions. Some authors have pointed out the influence of sex hormones, since they induce sexual differentiation of the brain that can lead to sex-specific behaviors. 2D:4D digit ratio is commonly used as a marker of prenatal sex hormones. In this study we analyzed whether there was an association between 2D:4D and personality measured through the BFQ in a homogeneous sample of 101 young women college students. We found a positive association between 2D:4D and emotional stability, as well as with its subdimensions emotion control and impulse control. This association could be quadratic and nonlinear. However, no association was found with the other four dimensions. We also measured anxiety, depression and global life satisfaction, variables related to neuroticism. We observed that emotional stability is positively associated to social desirability and global life satisfaction, and negatively related to anxiety and depression. On the other hand, we did not find any association between 2D:4D and anxiety, depression, and global life satisfaction. These results can be linked to other aspects such as subjective well-being and psychopathological symptoms. This study may help to better understand how these constructs are related and could lead to future projects to elucidated how these variables influence personality.

Introduction

Personality and psychopathology have been related since the time of the ancient Greeks and today the existence of this relationship has been evidenced by different studies [13]. Over the years different models have been postulated to describe and explain personality, among which the most recognized is the Five-Factor Model. This model was developed empirically through lexical studies of the trait terms in different languages, converging in a five-factor structure consisting of openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness and neuroticism [4]. Using this model sex differences have been found in some traits, with agreeableness and neuroticism being the most highlighted dimensions [5, 6]. Neuroticism is a stable personality trait included in almost all major models of personality and has been shown to predict many mental and physical disorders [7], and is one of the most important predictors of subjective well-being [8]. Neuroticism is moderately heritable (~ 47%) [7]. People with high neuroticism levels experience more negative emotions, tend to worry and have less control over their impulses and desires [7]. Previous studies have shown that women tend to be more neurotic than men [5, 6] and these differences become smaller with age, although small differences are present even in old age [9].

The fact that women tend to exhibit higher neuroticism scores could moderate the association between being a woman and an increased risk of suffering from some common mental disorders, such as mood and anxiety disorders [10, 11]. The causes of these differences are not yet fully understood, but some authors point to the influence of sex hormones [12]. These act on the nervous system through activational and organizational effects. Activational effects are transient and facilitate behavior in specific contexts depending on current hormone levels, generally during adolescence and adulthood. In contrast, organizational effects are permanent and lead to the final organization and structure of the nervous system [13]. These occur in specific stages of neural development during sensitive periods where the hormone effects are maximal, inducing sexual differentiation of the brain. There are two main periods during which sex hormones modify the brain and influence human psychological sex differences, early prenatal development and puberty [14, 15]. The mechanisms involved in this differentiation cause a dimorphic organization of certain brain regions that results in sex-specific behaviors [16]. In women there are also other periods with high fluctuations in hormone levels that seem to modify the brain, affecting certain aspects of behavior. This is specially frequent during pregnancy and menopausal transitions; but even short-term fluctuations could as occur in the menstrual cycle, although these changes often revert to baseline [17, 18].

The second to fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) is commonly used as a marker of the balance between prenatal testosterone and estrogen [19]. 2D:4D describes the relative length of the index finger to the ring finger which it is constant throughout life and tends to be lower in men than in women [20]. Several studies have estimated that 2D:4D is highly heritability (50–80%) [21]. In order to explain the effect of prenatal sex hormones on the digit ratio, multiple studies have attempted to find an association between 2D:4D and the CAG-repeat length of the androgen receptor gene, in which the longest alleles are related to a decreased androgen receptor transcription and sensitivity to testosterone [22]. These studies have shown several discrepancies. However, what seems to be clear is that there is not a significant relationship [23] or if there is, it is a weak association with this polymorphism [24]. So, although several studies have found an association between 2D:4D and direct measurements of prenatal sex hormones in human [25], this mechanism is still unknown. Additionally, the relationship between 2D:4D and testosterone levels in adults is not significant [23, 24]. Due to these discrepancies, some authors are skeptical about whether 2D:4D is a good candidate as a marker of early hormonal environment and instead propose other measures [26, 27].

Despite this, so many studies have found correlations between 2D:4D and multiple variables, including psychological aspects such as personality. Regarding to personality research, the Five-Factor Model have been the most widely investigated, using different tests. With the NEO Five-Factor Inventory, one study found a positive association with neuroticism, a negative association with agreeableness and a close tendency association with extraversion, all of them only with the right 2D:4D of women [28]. Concerning neuroticism, it has been observed a significant positive association of 2D:4D of both hand and only the left hand with neuroticism in women from two different populations, German and Chinese respectively [29]. A relationship with openness across the entire sample with both 2D:4D and separately between the right 2D:4D in men has also been described [30]. A research including four ethnic groups found a positive association between right 2D:4D and agreeableness regardless of the population origin [31]. Using the Five Factor Personality Inventory, a significant association was described in a whole sample and separately in both sexes between right 2D:4D and agreeableness, and also with conscientiousness but only in the whole sample [32]. With the Big Five Inventory, an investigation found a significant weak association of 2D:4D with extraversion and openness to experiences, and a tendency with agreeableness but not with other traits [33]. A recent study with data from a large population, using three items of the Big Five Inventory-SOEP, found a relationship between neuroticism and the mean of both 2D:4D and separately with the left hand. However, they have proposed that this association could be quadratic and nonlinear [34]. An internet study across 23 nations found a positive correlation between both 2D:4D and the cultural dimensions of uncertainty avoidance in both sexes and aggregate neuroticism only in men [35]. Two Italian investigations with the Big Five Questionnaire (BFQ) found a positive relationship with conscientiousness and agreeableness in a sample of skydivers [36], and a positive relationship with conscientiousness in a sample of cavers [37]. Recently, using the ten-item personality inventory a positive relationship have been found between the right 2D:4D and openness to new experiences and a negative one with disorganization and carelessness [38]. Additionally, in the same study they found a negative relationship between the left 2D:4D and dependable self-disciplined. Furthermore, in addition to using the Five-Factor Model, some studies have investigated the relationship with 2D:4D using other personality models such as the Eysenck Personality Model [39], the Cattell’s Sixteen Personality Factor Model [40, 41], and the HEXACO Personality Model [42].

Therefore, despite there are several studies that have shown an association between 2D:4D and personality, contradictory results have been found. Among these studies, only two have used the BFQ [36, 37], both with heterogeneous samples of people who choose high-risk activities, with less than 50 people and only measuring the 2D:4D ratio in the right hand. Consequently, these results could be biased. For this reason, we decided to study whether there was an association between 2D:4D and personality using the BFQ where we expect to find a small but significant association with some of the five personality traits, especially in neuroticism that had been one of the most reported. With this objective, we chose a homogeneous group of young women university students and we measured 2D:4D in both hands. Only women were included in this study because they generally tend to show higher scores on neuroticism, which could be interesting for predicting mental health. Although we studied the five dimensions of the Five-Factor Model, we have focused mainly on neuroticism and we have also considered some inter-related constructs such as anxiety, depression and global life satisfaction, since they are important for different mental disorders that have previously been related to 2D:4D [4346].

Materials and methods

Participants

Healthy young women university students from the Faculty of Sciences and the Faculty of Education of the University of Córdoba (Spain), were randomly selected to participate in the present research. In order to homogenize the sample, we established an age range between 18 and 28 years old and some participants were rejected due to older age or missing data. Finally, one hundred and one participants were selected. The characteristics of the sample are shown in Table 1. They provided written informed consent for an anonymous research. The study was approved by the ethical research committee of the Reina Sofía University Hospital (Córdoba, Spain).

Procedures and measures

The present study was carried out during practical classes where the students responded to different self-report tests, including BFQ, STAI, BDI and SWLS. In addition, a photograph of both hands was taken to measure the 2D:4D.

Big Five Questionnaire (BFQ).

The BFQ is a personality test based on the Five-Factor Model, that identifies five fundamental dimensions in the human personality. The Spanish version of the BFQ [47] consists in 132 multiple-choice items of a 5-points Likert scale. The scores are used to measure 5 different dimensions: Energy, Friendliness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability and Openness, which refer to the factors generally labeled Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism and Openness to experience, respectively [48]. Each dimension is organized into two facets which are: Energy (Dynamism and Dominance), Friendliness (Cooperativeness/Empathy and Politeness), Conscientiousness (Scrupulousness and Perseverance), Emotional Stability (Emotion Control and Impulse Control) and Openness (Openness to Culture and Openness to Experiences). The BFQ also includes a Lie scale that measures social desirability with the purpose of identifying people who tend to offer a distorted profile to project an erroneous image of themselves in a more or less intentional way. These dimensions and their subdimensions are measured on a continuum where an individual might be anywhere between the two extremes. Raw scores were transformed into standardized T-scores (mean 50, SD 10) to run the analysis. Reference values, internal consistency and scores of self-report questionnaires have been compiled in Table 2.

thumbnail
Table 2. Internal consistency, reference values and scores from self-report questionnaires.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248368.t002

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI).

The STAI is a psychological questionnaire designed to assess the level of state and trait anxiety [52]. State anxiety reflects a transient emotional state at a specific moment and that changes over time; while trait anxiety is a relatively stable personality characteristic over time and different situations which refers to a tendency to experience anxiety. The Spanish version of the STAI has a good internal consistency (.80 for state anxiety and .88 for trait anxiety) [49]. It consists in 40 items, 20 to assess state anxiety and 20 for trait anxiety. In each item, participants responded with a 4‐point Likert scale from 0 to 3 points as “very much”, “likely”, “not so much,” or “not at all”, indicating in which extent they agree or disagree with the statements. Higher total scores indicate greater anxiety.

Beck’s Depression Inventory (BDI).

The BDI is an inventory created to measure characteristic attitudes and symptoms of depression [53]. The Spanish version of the BDI-II (α = .88) [50] consists of 21 multiple-choice questions with four different self-report statements rated on a scale from 0 to 3. The total score was calculated by adding the score of all the responses, with higher scores indicating greater symptom severity. The total score ranges from 0 to 63 points and different cutoff ranges have been established: 0–13 minimal depression, 14–19 mild depression, 20–28 moderate depression, and 29–63 severe depression.

Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS).

The SWLS is a psychological scale used to measure global life satisfaction, a cognitive component of the subjective well-being [54]. Like the original, the Spanish version of the SWLS (α = .88) [51] consists of five statements in which the participants refer to their lives using a 7-point Likert scale from 1 “completely disagree” to 7 “completely agree”. Scores are added together to obtain an overall score where high scores indicate higher levels of life satisfaction and vice versa. Different categories have been stablished according to the scores obtained: 31–35 extremely satisfied; 26–30 satisfied; 21–25 slightly satisfied; 20 neutral; 15–19 slightly dissatisfied; 10–14 dissatisfied; and 5–9 extremely dissatisfied.

2D:4D digit ratio measurement.

A photograph of both hands was taken with a digital camera [55]. The hands were held in supination with the full extension of the fingers. 2D:4D was computed as the quotient of the index and the ring fingers. The lengths of these fingers were measured from the bottom crease to the tip of the finger using ImageJ software. In case there were more than one crease, the most proximal one was used [28]. Measurements were carried out by two independent raters and the mean of both was used. Inter-rater reliability was calculated using an intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) in which were assumed that people effects are random and measurements effects are fixed. The ICC obtained for the average of the two raters were 0.89 for the left hand and 0.90 for the right, so they presented an excellent reliability [56].

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 25. Bivariate correlations between the different quantitative variables analyzed were performed using the Pearson’s correlation coefficient. A multiple linear regression was carried out through the stepwise method to stablish the explanatory power of the different variables related to emotional stability on it as the dependent variable. The results were accepted as significant at p≤.05.

Results

We found a positive correlation between the left 2D:4D and the dimension emotional stability (Table 3), as well as with its two subdimensions, emotion control and impulse control. On the contrary, no association was found with the other four dimensions: energy, friendliness, conscientiousness and openness, nor any of its subdimensions; and neither with anxiety, depression, global life satisfaction or social desirability. On the other hand, no significant relationships were found with the right 2D:4D. Similar results have been observed with the square of the 2D:4D of both hands (Table 3). Although a significant correlation could be obtained randomly given the large number of correlations performed, we believe that it is very unlikely that significant correlations, if they were random, would occur exactly in the expected cases (emotional stability and its subdimensions).

thumbnail
Table 3. Statistical results of correlation analysis between 2D:4D and self-report variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248368.t003

Referring to emotional stability, some associations have been observed with social desirability, anxiety, depression and global life satisfaction, all of them showing a high significant association (Table 4). All these relationships were also found for its two subdimensions. Likewise, social desirability, in addition to the association with emotional stability, showed a significant positive association with global life satisfaction and negative with anxiety and depression (Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Main associations found related to emotional stability as personality trait.

Arrows indicate that there is a significant correlation between the two linked constructs and the value indicates the Pearson’s correlation coefficient between them. ** p≤.01; ***p≤.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248368.g001

thumbnail
Table 4. Statistical results of correlation analysis between emotional stability, and its subdimensions, with the other self-report variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248368.t004

Furthermore, we evaluated the explanatory power of these variables on emotional stability. The results showed that the significant variables explained 54.7% of the variance in emotional stability (R2 = .547, F(3,101) = 41.240, p < .001). The significant predictor variables were trait anxiety, social desirability, and the square of left 2D:4D. The contribution of each one to the model has been collected in Table 5.

Discussion

2D:4D is positively related to emotional stability but not with the other four dimensions

In the present study we described for the first time a positive correlation between the left 2D:4D and emotional stability, and with its subdimensions emotion control and impulse control. These correlations were not found with the right 2D:4D. Therefore, our results indicate that lower 2D:4D is associated with lower emotional stability (higher neuroticism) in women, and consequently we presume that is also associated with higher prenatal testosterone exposure. These results are partially in disagreement with previous studies which found a relationship between 2D:4D and the neuroticism-emotional stability dimension but seem to be in the opposite direction [28, 29, 35, 3942]; or, conversely, have found no relationship between these variables [3033, 3638]. In most of the previous studies neuroticism was measured through the Neo Five-Factor Inventory, and here we measured emotional stability through the BFQ. Although the dimensions neuroticism in the Neo Five-Factor Inventory and emotional stability in the BFQ have shown a high negative correlation between them and, therefore, both seem to measure the same dimension [48]; their division into several subdimensions are different, probably grouping different traits in different ways in each test. In the regression model, the square of the left 2D:4D has been shown to explain a part of the variance in emotional stability, as recently has been proposed [34]. So, it would be interesting to continue investigating this possible nonlinear relationship in subsequent studies. Therefore, taking into account all the data, it is the first time that a positive relationship has been shown between 2D:4D and emotional stability, which may contribute to a better understanding of the relationship between 2D:4D and emotional stability, and some psychopathological conditions as mentioned below.

On the other hand, although we did not find any significant association between 2D:4D and the other four dimensions and these results agree with several previous investigations, other studies have shown controversial results. For example, referring to agreeableness, some studies, like ours, have not found a significant association with 2D:4D [29, 30, 37, 42], others have found a positive correlation [31, 32, 36] and others a negative correlation [28, 33]. This inconsistency is another example that more research is needed in this field. These discrepancies could be due to the different tests used to measure personality and also the high heterogeneity between the samples used. So, as our results support, in the future would be important to use homogeneous samples and evaluate possible nonlinear relationships, as well as optimize the best type of study to understand how exactly 2D:4D is related to personality. In any case, what seems clear is that if there is a relationship between personality and 2D:4D, it possibly explains only a small percentage of the individual differences observed in the different traits.

The association between 2D:4D and emotion control and impulse control could explain its relationship with mental disorders

Neuroticism has previously been associated with anxiety, depression, and lower subjective well-being [7, 8]. Subjective well-being is established through three related components: frequent positive affect, infrequent negative affect and cognitive evaluations such as global life satisfaction [57]. It has been proposed that the influence of personality on subjective well-being accounts between 39% and 63% [58], with emotional stability-neuroticism being one of the most important predictors of both cognitive and affective components of subjective well-being [8]. In line with these previous observations, we found that emotional stability is positively related to global life satisfaction, and negatively with depression and anxiety. In turn, global life satisfaction correlates negatively with anxiety and depression. Thus, when people exhibit higher levels of emotional stability, they will experience greater global life satisfaction and less anxiety and depression, which would result in greater subjective well-being [8].

Anxiety, depression and subjective well-being are important variables for suicide risk and several mental disorders, including anxiety-, mood- and eating disorders, all of which have been reported to be related to 2D:4D [4346]. Despite these observations, we did not find any association between these variables and 2D:4D, which agrees with the limited literature on this topic. With regard to anxiety, most associations with 2D:4D in the general population have been found in men but not in women [44, 59]. In relation to depression, some discrepancies have been observed, with some studies showing associations between depression and 2D:4D only in men [60, 61], only in women [45] or no association [34, 39]. Regarding subjective well-being, most of the studies have not found an association with 2D:4D; however, a recent article have pointed out to a possible inverse U-shaped relationship [62]. Although more research is needed in this field, we suggest that 2D:4D, instead of acting directly on anxiety, depression, and subjective well-being, would act on emotional stability, specifically on impulse control and emotion control, that indirectly would influence these variables. Thus, prenatal testosterone would affect emotion control and impulse control which would moderate the association between 2D:4D and some types of mental disorders.

On the other hand, we have also found that emotional stability is positively correlated with social desirability. At the same time social desirability is negatively related to anxiety and depression and positively with global life satisfaction. However, we did not find a significant association between social desirability and 2D:4D, which coincide with previous reports that studied this association [36, 37, 63, 64]. Social desirability is an additional predictor of the components of subjective well-being, along with personality traits [8]. It is described as the tendency of an individual to offer a biased answer to personality items in order to present himself in a more socially desirable way than his real answer would be [65]. Otherwise, other authors have indicated that more than a scale to measure response bias, social desirability measures a positive and prosocial personality trait [66], where self-control might be implicated in socially desirable behaviors and it is linked to the universal need to belong [67]. Our results are in agreement with previous studies that have investigated personality using the BFQ and had found a positive association between emotional stability and social desirability, measured through the lie-scale [36, 37]. Moreover, they found a negative correlation between social desirability and anxiety, and with the tendency to suffer from mood disorders. Likewise, other studies have shown a negative association between social desirability and neuroticism, and also with alexithymia, which seems to be moderated by higher neuroticism scores [68]. Therefore, greater social desirability will lead to better control of impulses and emotions in order to develop socially desirable behaviors to do the right thing, which is often a socially desirable act. In this sense, low scores on social desirability will be related to some psychopathological symptoms such as hostility, depression or social withdrawal [68]. On the other hand, people with high scores that do not show high levels of self-control, and also high self-monitors characterized by chameleon-like and in somehow manipulative behaviors, will only seek the approval of others, without thinking about the correct behavior [67].

2D:4D and impulse control: Possible implications in psychopathology

Impulse control is a subdimension of emotional stability that refers to the ability to control discontent, irritation and anger [48]. This construct is closely linked to others, such as impulsivity, which is also a main symptom of ADHD, aggression or any disruptive behavior. Although we did not directly measure these constructs, other studies have investigated them and have found some results that might be related to ours.

Some studies have investigated the association between 2D:4D and ADHD, which is characterized by inattention, impulsivity, and in some cases, hyperactivity. These symptoms are the same for children and adults. However, some studies suggest that in adults, rather than inattention, it is more specific to consider executive dysfunction [69], and support that emotional dysregulation is a hallmark symptom of ADHD in adults [70]. Similarly, ADHD has presented a certain personality profile generally characterized by high scores in neuroticism and low scores in conscientiousness, and also has shown lower extraversion and agreeableness than healthy controls [71, 72]. So, the various symptoms are associated with personality traits in a yet unknown interaction. Previous work has associated prenatal exposure to testosterone with increased hyperactivity-impulsivity in girls and not in preschool-age boys. They proposed that hyperactivity-impulsivity might be sensitive to the organizational effects of sex hormones and, thus, high prenatal exposure to testosterone may increase risk of ADHD in girls [73].

Impulsivity appears to be higher during childhood and adolescence than adulthood [74], and an early manifestation of hyperactivity-impulsivity could predispose to disruptive behavior problems [75]. General disruptive behaviors such as aggression, hyperactivity and conduct problems have been linked to prenatal exposure to higher levels of testosterone, and to hyperactivity and conduct problems in preschool girls [73]. In the same direction, other research has shown that a low digit ratio is associated with anger, hostility, physical aggression (and verbal aggression approaching significance), total sensation seeking, thrill and adventure seeking [76], less precautionary behavior [36, 37], and with risk taking in women [77]. This last observation is supported with data showing that in women, optimism might moderate the association between right 2D:4D and financial risk taking [78]. Therefore, although more studies should be carried out, we can speculate that higher levels of prenatal testosterone in women could lead to a lower emotional stability, and also a lower impulse control, which could increase the risk of ADHD, aggression and other disruptive behaviors. These personality traits are likely to appear during childhood and remain even into adulthood.

A recent study has shown that AR rs6152 and OPRM1 rs2075572, SNPs of the androgen receptor and the β-endorphin receptor genes respectively, might affect impulsivity mediated by 2D:4D digit ratio [79]. Moreover, they found a relationship between AR rs6152 and left hand 2D:4D only in women. In fact, AR rs6152 has been related to polycystic ovary syndrome [80], which in turn have been related to higher levels of neuroticism [81]. Furthermore, some research have found an association between this polymorphism and the level of expression of the androgen receptor in both men and women [82]. Therefore, although previous studies with the AR CAG-repeat length and 2D:4D have shown that there is no significant relationship [23], and that there does not appear to be a relationship between levels of sex hormone in adults with 2D:4D [24], testosterone seems to regulate impulsivity in some way, at least in women. One hypothesis could be that testosterone may have a greater effect in women, since they exhibit more variable levels of prenatal testosterone than men, who generally exhibit higher levels of prenatal testosterone [73]. Hence, it would be interesting to further study all these variables together in order to better understand these relationships.

In summary, emotional stability-neuroticism is a personality dimension that have been linked to suffering from anxiety and various mood disorders. Previous studies have shown that women tend to be more neurotic than men, which could moderate the increased risk of them from suffering from some common mental disorders. Higher levels of neuroticism have also been shown to negatively affect subjective well-being. Emotional stability-neuroticism is influenced by several aspects, including genetics, 2D:4D digit ratio and social aspects such as social desirability. 2D:4D is considered an indirect measure of prenatal testosterone, which could modify the brain when it is still developing. Prenatal testosterone is likely to have a greater effect in women as they exhibit more variable levels of prenatal testosterone than men, especially in aspects such as impulse control and emotion control. This influence on the subdimensions of emotional stability might be the basis for other relationships previously found between 2D:4D and some mental disorders. On the other hand, with respect to social aspects, people with higher levels of social desirability may have better self-control to act in a socially desirable way.

Limitations

Some limitations of this study should be mentioned. First, we have used 2D:4D as somatic marker of prenatal testosterone, while its molecular mechanism and the exact relationship between them are not yet fully understood. On the other hand, given the activational effects of sex hormones, these may influence personality, therefore future studies should monitor the day of the menstrual cycle, the use of oral contraceptives and whether they are or have been pregnant, especially recently. Another limitation of our study is that the sample only includes university students and may not reflect the general population. Finally, although a power analysis was performed and most of the results agree with previous studies, we cannot discard completely the type I errors from multiple testing. Nevertheless, despite these limitations, this study could help to better understand how emotional stability and its subdimensions are influenced by prenatal testosterone and social aspects such as social desirability and could lead to future projects to elucidate the mechanisms involved in this relationship.

Conclusions

Emotional stability is a complex construct that is influenced by genetics and environmental factors. We have found results that suggest that in women 2D:4D could be positively related to emotional stability and to its subdimensions, impulse control and emotion control. In fact, these associations could be quadratic and nonlinear. Given that 2D:4D has been proposed as an indirect measure of prenatal testosterone, these results can be interpreted as that this hormone may affect emotional stability and its subdimensions, at least in women. The effect of prenatal testosterone on personality may, in turn, influence other aspects, including psychopathological symptoms such as impulsivity in ADHD and aggression. Furthermore, we have found that social desirability might also affect emotional stability and its subdimensions. Thus, people with higher levels of social desirability would better control their impulses to act in a socially desirable way, linked to the universal need to belong.

References

  1. 1. Trull TJ, Widiger TA. Dimensional models of personality: The five-factor model and the DSM-5. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2013;15: 135–146. pmid:24174888
  2. 2. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D. Linking “Big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2010;136: 768–821. pmid:20804236
  3. 3. Sperandeo R, Messina G, Iennaco D, Sessa F, Russo V, Polito R, et al. What Does Personality Mean in the Context of Mental Health? A Topic Modeling Approach Based on Abstracts Published in Pubmed Over the Last 5 Years. Front Psychiatry. 2020;10: 938. pmid:31998157
  4. 4. Widiger TA, Crego C. The Five Factor Model of personality structure: an update. World Psychiatry. 2019;18: 271–272. pmid:31496109
  5. 5. Costa PT, Terracciano A, McCrae RR. Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising findings. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;81: 322–331. pmid:11519935
  6. 6. Weisberg YJ, Deyoung CG, Hirsh JB. Gender Differences in Personality across the Ten Aspects of the Big Five. Front Psychol. 2011;2: 178. pmid:21866227
  7. 7. Rodríguez‐Ramos Á, Moriana JA, García‐Torres F, Ruiz‐Rubio M. Emotional stability is associated with the MAOA promoter uVNTR polymorphism in women. Brain Behav. 2019;9: e01376. pmid:31448578
  8. 8. Brajša-Žganec A, Ivanović D, Kaliterna Lipovčan L. Personality Traits and Social Desirability as Predictors of Subjective Well-being. Psychol Top. 2011;20: 261–276. Available: https://pt.ffri.hr/index.php/pt/article/view/2/6
  9. 9. Soto CJ, John OP, Gosling SD, Potter J. Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big Five domains and facets in a large cross-sectional sample. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011;100: 330–48. pmid:21171787
  10. 10. Goodwin RD, Gotlib IH. Gender differences in depression: The role of personality factors. Psychiatry Res. 2004;126: 135–142. pmid:15123392
  11. 11. Eaton NR, Keyes KM, Krueger RF, Balsis S, Skodol AE, Markon KE, et al. An invariant dimensional liability model of gender differences in mental disorder prevalence: evidence from a national sample. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121: 282–8. pmid:21842958
  12. 12. Riecher-Rössler A. Sex and gender differences in mental disorders. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2017;4: 8–9. pmid:27856397
  13. 13. Auyeung B, Lombardo M V., Baron-Cohen S. Prenatal and postnatal hormone effects on the human brain and cognition. Pflugers Archiv European Journal of Physiology. 2013. pp. 557–571. pmid:23588379
  14. 14. Vigil P, del Río JP, Carrera B, Aránguiz FC, Rioseco H, Cortés ME. Influence of sex steroid hormones on the adolescent brain and behavior: An update. Linacre Q. 2016;83: 308–329. pmid:27833209
  15. 15. Berenbaum SA, Beltz AM. Sexual differentiation of human behavior: Effects of prenatal and pubertal organizational hormones. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2011. pp. 183–200. pmid:21397624
  16. 16. Turano A, Osborne BF, Schwarz JM. Sexual Differentiation and Sex Differences in Neural Development. Current topics in behavioral neurosciences. 2018. pmid:29967999
  17. 17. Rehbein E, Hornung J, Sundström Poromaa I, Derntl B. Shaping of the female human brain by sex hormones–a review. Neuroendocrinology. 2020 [cited 28 Mar 2020]. pmid:32155633
  18. 18. Catenaccio E, Mu W, Lipton ML. Estrogen- and progesterone-mediated structural neuroplasticity in women: evidence from neuroimaging. Brain Structure and Function. Springer Verlag; 2016. pp. 3845–3867. pmid:26897178
  19. 19. Manning JT, Kilduff L, Cook C, Crewther B, Fink B. Digit Ratio (2D:4D): A Biomarker for Prenatal Sex Steroids and Adult Sex Steroids in Challenge Situations. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne). 2014;5: 9. pmid:24523714
  20. 20. Kim TB, Kim KH. Why is digit ratio correlated to sports performance? J Exerc Rehabil. 2016;12: 515–519. pmid:28119871
  21. 21. Kalichman L, Batsevich V, Kobyliansky E. Heritability estimation of 2D:4D finger ratio in a Chuvashian population-based sample. Am J Hum Biol. 2019;31. pmid:30635958
  22. 22. Manning JT, Bundred PE, Newton DJ, Flanagan BF. The second to fourth digit ratio and variation in the androgen receptor gene. Evol Hum Behav. 2003;24: 399–405. (03)00052-7
  23. 23. Zhang K, Yang X, Zhang M, Wang C, Fang P, Xue M, et al. Revisiting the relationships of 2D:4D with androgen receptor (AR) gene and current testosterone levels: Replication study and meta-analyses. J Neurosci Res. 2020;98: 353–370. pmid:31359506
  24. 24. Warrington NM, Shevroja E, Hemani G, Hysi PG, Jiang Y, Auton A, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies nine novel loci for 2D:4D finger ratio, a putative retrospective biomarker of testosterone exposure in utero. Hum Mol Genet. 2018;27: 2025–2038. pmid:29659830
  25. 25. Manning JT, Fink B. Are there any “direct” human studies of digit ratio (2D:4D) and measures of prenatal sex hormones? Early Hum Dev. 2017;113: 73–74. pmid:28927753
  26. 26. Wong WI, Hines M. Interpreting digit ratio (2D:4D)-behavior correlations: 2D:4D sex difference, stability, and behavioral correlates and their replicability in young children. Horm Behav. 2016;78: 86–94. pmid:26542674
  27. 27. Lolli L, Batterham AM, Kratochvíl L, Flegr J, Weston KL, Atkinson G. A comprehensive allometric analysis of 2nd digit length to 4th digit length in humans. Proceedings Biol Sci. 2017;284. pmid:28659446
  28. 28. Fink B, Manning JT, Neave N. Second to fourth digit ratio and the ‘big five’ personality factors. Pers Individ Dif. 2004;37: 495–503.
  29. 29. Sindermann C, Li M, Sariyska R, Lachmann B, Duke É, Cooper A, et al. The 2D:4D-Ratio and Neuroticism Revisited: Empirical Evidence from Germany and China. Front Psychol. 2016;7: 811. pmid:27375513
  30. 30. Burton LA, Guterman E, Baum G. Effect of Prenatal Androgen on Adult Personality: Greater Openness with More Female-Typical 2D:4D Digit Ratios. Curr Psychol. 2013;32: 197–202.
  31. 31. Rostovtseva V, Butovskaya M, Mkrtchjan R. 2d: 4d, Big Fives and Aggression in Young Men of Caucasian, Ural and Asian Origin. Soc Evol Hist. 2019;18: 110–126.
  32. 32. Luxen MF, Buunk BP. Second-to-fourth digit ratio related to Verbal and Numerical Intelligence and the Big Five. Pers Individ Dif. 2005;39: 959–966.
  33. 33. Lippa RA. Finger lengths, 2D:4D ratios, and their relation to gender-related personality traits and the Big Five. Biol Psychol. 2006;71: 116–121. pmid:16360883
  34. 34. Lautenbacher LM, Neyse L. Depression, neuroticism and 2D:4D ratio: evidence from a large, representative sample. Sci Rep. 2020;10. pmid:32636433
  35. 35. Manning JT, Fink B. Is low digit ratio linked with late menarche? Evidence from the BBC internet study. Am J Hum Biol. 2011;23: 527–33. pmid:21547980
  36. 36. Massimino S, Rinella S, Buscemi A, Similia E, Perciavalle V, Perciavalle V, et al. Digit ratio, personality and emotions in skydivers. Biomed Reports. 2019;10: 39–46. pmid:30588302
  37. 37. Rinella S, Buscemi A, Massimino S, Perciavalle V, Tortorici MM, Tomaselli DG, et al. Risk-taking behavior, the second-to-fourth digit ratio and psychological features in a sample of cavers. PeerJ. 2019;2019. pmid:31720126
  38. 38. Bagepally BS, Majumder J, Kotadiya SM. Association between second to fourth digit ratio and personality among Indian men. Am J Hum Biol. 2020;32. pmid:31584225
  39. 39. Austin EJ, Manning JT, McInroy K, Mathews E. A preliminary investigation of the associations between personality, cognitive ability and digit ratio. Pers Individ Dif. 2002;33: 1115–1124.
  40. 40. Lindová J, Hrušková M, Pivoňková V, Kuběna A, Flegr J. Digit ratio (2D:4D) and Cattell’s personality traits. Eur J Pers. 2008;22: 347–356.
  41. 41. Barman U, Sil P. A study on correlation between 2D:4D with personality traits among young adult women. Int J Physiol Nutr Phys Educ. 2019;4: 420–423. Available: http://www.journalofsports.com/
  42. 42. Shaw AZ. The effect of prenatal androgen exposure on the development of neural reactivity systems: A study of the HEXACO Personality Inventory. Pers Individ Dif. 2013;55: 19–23.
  43. 43. Quinton SJ, Smith AR, Joiner T. The 2nd to 4th digit ratio (2D:4D) and eating disorder diagnosis in women. Pers Individ Dif. 2011;51: 402–405. pmid:21765573
  44. 44. Evardone M, Alexander GM. Anxiety, Sex-Linked Behaviors, and Digit Ratios (2D:4D). Arch Sex Behav. 2009;38: 442–455. pmid:17943431
  45. 45. Smedley KD, McKain KJ, McKain DN. 2D:4D digit ratio predicts depression severity for females but not for males. Pers Individ Dif. 2014;70: 136–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.039
  46. 46. Lenz B, Kornhuber J. Cross-national gender variations of digit ratio (2D:4D) correlate with life expectancy, suicide rate, and other causes of death. J Neural Transm. 2018;125: 239–246. pmid:29164314
  47. 47. Caprara GV, Barbaranelli C, Borgogni L. BFQ. Cuestionario Big Five. Madrid: TEA; 1995.
  48. 48. Caprara GV, Barbaranelli C, Borgogni L, Perugini M. The “big five questionnaire”: A new questionnaire to assess the five factor model. Pers Individ Dif. 1993. pmid:11654089
  49. 49. Fonseca-Pedrero E, Paino M, Sierra-Baigrie S, Lemos-Giráldez S, Muñiz J. Propiedades Psicométricas del “Cuestionario de Ansiedad Estado-Rasgo” (STAI) en Universitarios. Behav Psychol Psicol Conduct. 2012.
  50. 50. Sanz J, Perdigón A, Vázquez C. Adaptación española del inventario para la depresión de Beck-II (BDI-II): propiedades psicométricas en población general. Clín salud. 2003.
  51. 51. Vázquez C, Duque A, Hervás G. Satisfaction with life scale in a representative sample of Spanish adults: Validation and normative data. Span J Psychol. 2013;16. pmid:24230945
  52. 52. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA Consult Psychol Press. 1970.
  53. 53. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory–II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1996.
  54. 54. Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsem RJ, Griffin S. The Satisfaction With Life Scale. J Pers Assess. 1985;49: 71–75. pmid:16367493
  55. 55. Krishnakumar M, Atheeshwar S, Chandrasekar MD. Myopia and digit ratio in medical college students. PLoS One. 2014;9. pmid:24587043
  56. 56. Koo TK, Li MY. A Guideline of Selecting and Reporting Intraclass Correlation Coefficients for Reliability Research. J Chiropr Med. 2016;15: 155–163. pmid:27330520
  57. 57. Tov W, Diener E. Subjective Wellbeing. The Encyclopedia of Cross-Cultural Psychology. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2013. pp. 1239–1245. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118339893.wbeccp518
  58. 58. Steel P, Schmidt J, Shultz J. Refining the Relationship Between Personality and Subjective Well-Being. Psychol Bull. 2008;134: 138–161. pmid:18193998
  59. 59. De Bruin EI, Verheij F, Wiegman T, Ferdinand RF. Differences in finger length ratio between males with autism, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, ADHD, and anxiety disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48: 962–965. pmid:17109783
  60. 60. Martin SM, Manning JT, Dowrick CF. Fluctuating Asymmetry, Relative Digit Length, and Depression in Men. Evol Hum Behav. 1999;20: 203–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00006-9
  61. 61. Bailey AA, Hurd PL. Depression in men is associated with more feminine finger length ratios. Pers Individ Dif. 2005;39: 829–836. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.12.017
  62. 62. Nye JVC, Bryukhanov M, Polyachenko S. 2D:4D and individual satisfaction: Evidence from the Russian social survey. Pers Individ Dif. 2019;142: 85–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.01.031
  63. 63. Schwarz S, Mustafić M, Hassebrauck M, Jörg J. Short- and long-term relationship orientation and 2D:4D finger-length ratio. Arch Sex Behav. 2011;40: 565–574. pmid:21063903
  64. 64. Schwerdtfeger A, Heims R, Heer J. Digit ratio (2D:4D) is associated with traffic violations for male frequent car drivers. Accid Anal Prev. 2010;42: 269–274. pmid:19887167
  65. 65. Lavrakas P. Encyclopedia of Survey Research Methods. Thousand Oaks, California; 2008. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412963947
  66. 66. Andrejević M, Meshi D, van den Bos W, Heekeren HR. Individual differences in social desirability are associated with white-matter microstructure of the external capsule. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2017;17: 1255–1264. pmid:29110184
  67. 67. Uziel L. Rethinking social desirability scales: From impression management to interpersonally oriented self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science. Perspect Psychol Sci; 2010. pp. 243–262. pmid:26162157
  68. 68. Messina A, Fogliani AM, Paradiso S. Association between alexithymia, neuroticism, and social desirability scores among Italian graduate students. Psychol Rep. 2010;107: 185–192. pmid:20923063
  69. 69. Kessler RC, Green JG, Adler LA, Barkley RA, Chatterji S, Faraone S V., et al. Structure and diagnosis of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Analysis of expanded symptom criteria from the adult ADHD clinical diagnostic scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67: 1168–1178. pmid:21041618
  70. 70. Hirsch O, Chavanon ML, Riechmann E, Christiansen H. Emotional dysregulation is a primary symptom in adult Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). J Affect Disord. 2018;232: 41–47. pmid:29477097
  71. 71. Knouse LE, Traeger L, O’Cleirigh C, Safren SA. Adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and five-factor model traits in a clinical sample: A structural equation modeling approach. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2013;201: 848–854. pmid:24080671
  72. 72. Van Dijk FE, Mostert J, Glennon J, Onnink M, Dammers J, Vasquez AA, et al. Five factor model personality traits relate to adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder but not to their distinct neurocognitive profiles. Psychiatry Res. 2017;258: 255–261. pmid:28844557
  73. 73. Roberts BA, Martel MM. Prenatal testosterone and preschool Disruptive Behavior Disorders. Pers Individ Dif. 2013;55: 962–966. pmid:25598567
  74. 74. Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The Adolescent Brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124: 111. pmid:18400927
  75. 75. Martel MM, Pierce L, Nigg JT, Jester JM, Adams K, Puttler LI, et al. Temperament pathways to childhood disruptive behavior and adolescent substance abuse: testing a cascade model. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37: 363–73. pmid:18787942
  76. 76. Hampson E, Ellis CL, Tenk CM. On the Relation Between 2D:4D and Sex-Dimorphic Personality Traits. Arch Sex Behav. 2008;37: 133–144. pmid:18075733
  77. 77. Hönekopp J. Relationships between digit ratio 2D:4D and self-reported aggression and risk taking in an online study. Pers Individ Dif. 2011;51: 77–80.
  78. 78. Barel E. 2D:4D, Optimism, and Risk Taking. Curr Psychol. 2019;38: 204–212.
  79. 79. Pearce E, Wlodarski R, Machin A, Dunbar RIM. Associations between neurochemical receptor genes, 2D:4D, impulsivity and relationship quality. Biol Lett. 2018;14: 20180642. pmid:30518641
  80. 80. Peng CY, Long XY, Lu GX. Association of AR rs6152G/A gene polymorphism with susceptibility to polycystic ovary syndrome in Chinese women. Reprod Fertil Dev. 2010;22: 881–885. pmid:20450840
  81. 81. Cesta CE, Kuja-Halkola R, Lehto K, Iliadou AN, Landén M. Polycystic ovary syndrome, personality, and depression: A twin study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017;85: 63–68. pmid:28825976
  82. 82. Kassem HS, Abou RM, Seoud E, Aly AS, Ibrahim HA, Ramly MA El. A Study of the androgen receptor gene polymorphism and the level of expression of the androgen receptor in androgenetic alopecia among Egyptians. J Med Hum Genet. 2009. Available: https://www.ajol.info/index.php/ejhg/article/view/44158