Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Socioeconomic, racial and ethnic differences in patient experience of clinician empathy: Results of a systematic review and meta-analysis

  • Brian W. Roberts,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Cooper University Health Care, Camden, New Jersey, United States of America, Department of Emergency Medicine, Cooper Medical School of Rowan University, Camden, New Jersey, United States of America, Center for Humanism, Cooper Medical School of Rowan University, Camden, New Jersey, United States of America

  • Nitin K. Puri,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Cooper University Health Care, Camden, New Jersey, United States of America, Department of Medicine, Cooper Medical School of Rowan University, Camden, New Jersey, United States of America

  • Christian J. Trzeciak,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Cooper University Health Care, Camden, New Jersey, United States of America

  • Anthony J. Mazzarelli,

    Roles Conceptualization, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Cooper University Health Care, Camden, New Jersey, United States of America, Department of Emergency Medicine, Cooper Medical School of Rowan University, Camden, New Jersey, United States of America, Center for Humanism, Cooper Medical School of Rowan University, Camden, New Jersey, United States of America

  • Stephen Trzeciak

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    trzeciak-stephen@cooperhealth.edu

    Affiliations Cooper University Health Care, Camden, New Jersey, United States of America, Center for Humanism, Cooper Medical School of Rowan University, Camden, New Jersey, United States of America, Department of Medicine, Cooper Medical School of Rowan University, Camden, New Jersey, United States of America

Abstract

Introduction

Empathy is essential for high quality health care. Health care disparities may reflect a systemic lack of empathy for disadvantaged people; however, few data exist on disparities in patient experience of empathy during face-to-face health care encounters with individual clinicians. We systematically analyzed the literature to test if socioeconomic status (SES) and race/ethnicity disparities exist in patient-reported experience of clinician empathy.

Methods

Using a published protocol, we searched Ovid MEDLINE, PubMed, CINAHL, EMBASE, CENTRAL and PsychINFO for studies using the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) Measure, which to date is the most commonly used and well-validated methodology for measuring clinician empathy from the patient perspective. We included studies containing CARE Measure data stratified by SES and/or race/ethnicity. We contacted authors to request stratified data, when necessary. We performed quantitative meta-analyses using random effects models to test for empathy differences by SES and race/ethnicity.

Results

Eighteen studies (n = 9,708 patients) were included. We found that, compared to patients whose SES was not low, low SES patients experienced lower empathy from clinicians (mean difference = -0.87 [95% confidence interval -1.72 to -0.02]). Compared to white patients, empathy scores were numerically lower for patients of multiple race/ethnicity groups (Black/African American, Asian, Native American, and all non-whites combined) but none of these differences reached statistical significance.

Conclusion

These data suggest an empathy gap may exist for patients with low SES. More research is needed to further test for SES and race/ethnicity disparities in clinician empathy and help promote health care equity.

Trial registration

Registration (PROSPERO): CRD42019142809.

Introduction

Empathy is sensing and detecting another’s emotions, resonating with their thoughts and feelings, and sharing and understanding their perspective. In health care, empathy is a vital clinical competency–an emotional bridge that drives compassionate care for patients [1]. As such, empathy is essential for high quality health care. Numerous studies published in the literature show that clinician empathy is associated with better patient outcomes across many different medical conditions [214].

Health care disparities are meaningful differences in health care quality that exist between population groups (e.g. race, ethnicity, gender, sexual orientation) not explained by variation in patient preferences, health care needs, or treatment guidelines, and often linked with socioeconomic disadvantage [15]. Although clinicians ought to have empathy for all patients, it is possible that disparities exist in empathy from clinicians. On a health care systems level, disparities in access to health care may be rooted in a societal lack of empathy for disadvantaged persons (e.g. institutionalized racism). Health care disparities occurring at the point of care with individual patients may be due to clinician bias (e.g. implicit or unconscious bias), and this may involve a lack of empathy. Examples include inadequate analgesia for Black/African American and Hispanic/Latino patients with painful conditions [1619], inappropriately low use of cardiac catheterization for Black/African American patients with possible acute myocardial infarction [20], and clinicians’ false assumptions that Black/African American patients will have poor adherence to treatment recommendations [21], among many others. Although some studies have reported that Black/African American patients and Hispanic/Latino patients have hospital experiences that are not worse than those of white, non-Hispanic patients [22, 23], other studies have shown that race/ethnicity and SES differences exist in patient satisfaction with clinicians [24, 25], possibly due to lower quality interpersonal interactions and clinician-patient relationships [2628], However, few data exist on SES and race/ethnicity disparities in patient experience of clinician empathy (e.g. interpersonal racism), specifically.

In addition, there may be important system level effects causing low SES patients to experience lower empathy from clinicians. The quality of health care services tends to vary inversely with the need for it in the population served (i.e. the “inverse care law”) [29, 30], As a result, low SES patients–who commonly have multimorbidity and the highest health care needs–often receive lower quality care, and this may include lower quality of clinician-patient relationships [31].

The Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) Measure is to date the most commonly used and well-validated methodology (i.e. proven reliability, internal validity and consistency [32]) for measuring clinician empathy from the patient perspective [33, 34]. The CARE Measure is a simple, rigorously tested, person-centered questionnaire that measures empathy in the context of a one-on-one therapeutic relationship between a clinician and a patient. Originally developed for assessing the empathy of primary care physicians, it has since been successfully implemented for other medical staff, allied health professionals and nurses. On a 41-point scale (range 10 [lowest] to 50 [highest]), the instrument measures a patient’s assessment of the empathy of a clinician, for example listening and understanding, being interested in the patient as a whole person, and showing compassion. The ten questions that comprise the CARE Measure are available at: http://www.caremeasure.org/CAREEng.pdf

We hypothesized that low SES patients (compared to not low SES patients) and Black/African American and Hispanic/Latino patients (compared to white, non-Hispanic patients) report lower empathy from clinicians. We aimed to test this hypothesis by conducting a systematic review and meta-analysis of all published studies containing data for patient assessment of clinician empathy using the CARE Measure.

Methods

Protocol and registration

We developed and published a protocol for this systematic review and meta-analysis [35]. The protocol was developed in accordance with the Cochrane Handbook [36], and reported in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis Protocols (PRISMA-P) statement [37]. We report our results in this manuscript in accordance with PRISMA and the Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) guidelines [38, 39]. We prospectively registered this systematic review in the PROSPERO international prospective register of systematic reviews (CRD42019142809). This systematic review did not collect individual patient-level data and therefore did not require ethical approval.

Eligibility criteria

We considered any study in which patients rated their clinicians’ empathy using the CARE Measure to be eligible for potential inclusion. Our inclusion criteria were: (1) contained data for patient-reported assessment of clinician empathy using the CARE Measure; and (2) provided CARE Measure data stratified by SES and/or race/ethnicity (including attempts to contact corresponding authors to obtain stratified data, when necessary). We considered studies eligible for inclusion regardless of language if the CARE Measure was previously validated in that language. We included both observational and interventional studies. We also included abstracts if they were published in a journal. We excluded studies for which stratified data could not be obtained. We also excluded studies that did not use the original CARE Measure (e.g. used an adaptation instead), and studies in which the CARE Measure was not completed by patients (e.g. completed by surrogates). We excluded editorials, correspondence, and review papers, as well as studies that were secondary reports of previously published studies.

Search and identification of studies

We searched the electronic databases generally considered to be the most important sources [36]: Ovid MEDLINE, PubMed, CINAHL, EMBASE, CENTRAL and PsycINFO. We also performed a supplementary search of Google Scholar. Our previously published search strategy was as follows [35]:

Ovid MEDLINE (and adapted for searching the other databases)

  1. “Consultation and Relational Empathy”.mp.
  2. (CARE adj3 (measure* or question* or index*)).ti,ab. and empath*.mp.
  3. (CARE adj3 (measure* or question* or index*)).ti,ab. and mercer.af.
  4. 1 or 2 or 3

We adopted this search strategy and search terms from a previously conducted, comprehensive and rigorous systematic review of the CARE Measure [32]. We consulted with a health librarian with expertise in systematic reviews who confirmed that the search strategy is methodologically sound. We searched from December 1, 2004 (date of the original publication of the CARE Measure) to present. We performed the search on May 21, 2020.

Study selection and data abstraction

Two independent reviewers performed a relevance screen of the titles and abstracts of identified studies for potential eligibility. After the relevance screen, we compared the exclusion logs for the two reviewers and we calculated the Kappa statistic for assessment of interobserver agreement. In cases of disagreement, we reviewed the full manuscript for inclusion. All studies identified as potentially relevant in the relevance screen underwent full manuscript review. For each study that underwent full manuscript review, if the manuscript did not report stratified data (i.e. by SES and/or race/ethnicity) we sent an email query to the corresponding author to request stratified data. If there was no response to the initial request, we sent up to three follow-up author query emails approximately one week apart to request the data.

Using a standardized data collection form, two reviewers independently abstracted data for the following: (a) clinical context; (b) total number of patients; (c) definition of low SES (if applicable); (d) number of patients stratified by SES; (e) CARE Measure data stratified by SES (i.e. mean and standard deviation [SD]); (f) number of patients stratified by race/ethnicity; (g) CARE Measure data stratified by race/ethnicity (mean and SD). Any disagreements in the above processes were resolved by consensus with a third reviewer.

We abstracted and tabulated race/ethnicity data according to the race/ethnicity categories used in each of the included studies. To allow for pooling and comparing of data by race/ethnicity in a meta-analytic fashion we stratified abstracted data using the race/ethnicity categories for human subjects research recommended by the United States National Institutes of Health (NIH) [40]. For SES stratification, we adopted the definition of low SES used in each of the included manuscripts.

Assessing study quality (risk of bias)

We used the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale to assess risk of bias as recommended in the Cochrane Handbook for cohort studies [41, 42]. We used the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale rather than a scale specific to clinical trials because: (a) we needed to use a single scale that could be applied to all studies in the meta-analysis; (b) as expected, the vast majority (76%) of studies in the meta-analysis were observational-only studies, not clinical trials; and (c) the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale can be applied to both observational-only studies and clinical trials. The application of the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale to clinical trials is reasonable given that what we are analyzing is exposure (e.g. SES) and outcome (CARE Measure), and allocation/randomization are not relevant for what we are studying. We deemed studies to be low risk of bias if they had seven or more stars out of a possible nine stars on the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale.

Analysis

As recommended in the Cochrane Handbook [36], we began with a qualitative analysis. We collated studies and summarized individual study results in table format. Where possible and appropriate, we pooled data and performed a quantitative analysis with a meta-analytic approach. As described in the protocol [35], because heterogeneous populations are needed in order to assess differences between race/ethnicity or SES groups, we only performed quantitative analysis for studies that had sufficient diversity in race/ethnicity and SES in the population (defined as no single race/ethnicity or SES group comprising >90% of the study population). We used separate random effects models to calculate pooled effect sizes and report mean differences with corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for low SES versus not low SES patients, as well as all non-white versus white patients. We also used separate random effects models to make pairwise comparisons (versus white patients) for Black/African American, Hispanic/Latino, Asian, and Native American patients.

We used the I2 statistic to assess heterogeneity in study results for each random effects model, with the following thresholds for interpretation: low heterogeneity: 25–49%; moderate heterogeneity: 50–74%; high heterogeneity: 75% or higher [43]. We assessed for publication bias using funnel plots of the effect sizes against the precision of the studies.

Per our published protocol, we planned a sensitivity analysis restricted to studies with a low risk of bias as defined above. We also planned to analyze for possible interaction between SES and race/ethnicity, where possible, by comparing CARE Measure scores between SES categories stratified by race. We also performed post-hoc (i.e. not in our original protocol) analyses with meta-regression by year of publication, to test if there have been changes in empathy differences over time.

We used Stata 16 (StataCorp, College Station, TX) for all analyses.

Results

Our database searches yielded 1085 records. After removal of duplicates, there were 748 independent studies that underwent relevance screen. Our Kappa calculation for the relevance screen was 0.83, indicating good inter-observer agreement. Following the relevance screen, 137 studies underwent full manuscript review. Fig 1 displays the search, inclusion and exclusion of studies flow diagram.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Search, inclusion and exclusion of studies flow diagram.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247259.g001

Twenty-one studies were included in the qualitative analyses. Five studies were clinical trials and 16 studies were observational only. The countries where the studies were conducted were as follows: United States (12), United Kingdom (6), Malaysia (2), and China (1). There were no multi-national studies. The practice settings for the studies were: primary care (7), orthopedic surgery (4), multidisciplinary practice (3), sexual health (1), homeopathy (1), oral health (1), rehabilitation medicine (1), obesity medicine (1), emergency medicine (1), and oncology (1).

Three studies reported CARE Measure scores stratified by SES or race/ethnicity in the published manuscript [4446], and the remaining 18 studies required queries to corresponding authors to obtain stratified data. Table 1 displays the nine studies with CARE Measure data stratified by SES [4452]. Table 2 displays the 14 studies with CARE Measure data stratified by race/ethnicity [47, 51, 5364]. Two studies had CARE Measure data stratified by both SES and race/ethnicity and thus appear in both tables [47, 51].

thumbnail
Table 1. Studies containing CARE Measure data stratified by socioeconomic status.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247259.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Studies containing CARE Measure data stratified by race/ethnicity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247259.t002

Eighteen studies were included in the quantitative meta-analyses. Of the three studies excluded from the quantitative meta-analysis, two were excluded because there was not enough data to pool (i.e. only two studies in a Malaysian population) [53, 64] and one was excluded because there was insufficient patient diversity in the sample as defined in the methods [58]. The 18 studies in the meta-analysis included 9,708 patients in total, and 3,663 (38%) of the patients were either low SES or non-white.

Fig 2 displays the results of the random effects model for SES. Overall, compared to patients with not low SES, low SES was associated with lower ratings of clinician empathy (mean CARE difference = -0.87 [95% CI -1.72 to -0.02]). Heterogeneity in the model was moderate (I2 = 72%).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Forest plot of the results of the random effects model for patient-reported experience of clinician empathy comparing patients with low socioeconomic status (SES) to patients with not low SES.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247259.g002

Fig 3 displays the results of the random effects model for all non-white patients compared to white patients. While we found that overall non-white patients reported lower clinician empathy compared to white patients, this difference was not statistically significant (mean CARE difference = -0.57 [95% CI -1.45 to 0.31]). Heterogeneity in the model was low (I2 = 25%).The results of the separate (pairwise) random effects models for Black/African American, Hispanic/Latino, Asian, and Native American patients (compared to white patients) appear in the (S1S4 Figs). In summary, compared to white patients, empathy scores were numerically lower for patients of multiple race/ethnicity groups (Black/African American, Asian, Native American, as well as all non-whites combined) but none of these differences reached statistical significance.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Forest plot of the results of the random effects model for patient-reported experience of clinician empathy comparing all non-white patients to white patients.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247259.g003

In the post-hoc meta-regression by year of publication, we found that empathy differences over time were increasing for both SES and race/ethnicity (for SES: -0.18 [95% CI -0.35 to -0.02] per year comparing low SES to not low SES; for race/ethnicity: -0.22 [95% CI -0.43 to -0.01] per year comparing non-white to white patients). These data suggest that empathy differences may be widening over time.

Only one of the included studies met our definition of low risk of bias by the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (scores for each quality domain appear in the S1 Table); therefore, we were unable to perform the sensitivity analysis restricted to low risk of bias studies. Because only two of the studies had stratified data by both SES and race/ethnicity, we also were unable to analyze for possible interaction between SES and race/ethnicity. Visual inspection of the funnel plots for the SES and race/ethnicity analyses did not suggest publication bias (S5 and S6 Figs).

Discussion

The purpose of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to generate preliminary data for testing the hypothesis that health care disparities exist in patient experience of clinician empathy (i.e. an empathy “gap”). After quantitatively analyzing 18 independent studies of the CARE Measure including more than nine thousand patients (and nearly 40% being low SES or non-white), we found that low SES patients had significantly lower patient-reported assessments of clinician empathy compared to patients with SES that was not low. Although we did not find statistically significant differences in empathy by race/ethnicity, we point to a trend that merits further research. The CARE Measure scores were consistently numerically lower for multiple race/ethnicity patient groups (compared to white patients), including Black/African American, Asian, Native American, and all non-whites combined. In addition, none of the included studies reported significantly higher empathy for any group of non-white patients compared to whites.

We believe there is uniqueness in this report for two reasons. First, rather than testing for SES and race/ethnicity differences in patient satisfaction in the broad sense, we focused specifically on the element of clinician empathy. Second, although all health care disparities are likely rooted in a systemic lack of empathy for disadvantaged people (i.e. systems level), few data exist on disparities in individual clinician empathy during face-to-face health care encounters due to implicit bias (i.e. interpersonal level) [28].

In addition to implicit bias, there also may be important system-level factors influencing our results, especially as it relates to lower empathy for patients with low SES. According to the inverse care law, patients with the highest health care needs (such as patients with multimorbidity–who are often patients with low SES [65]) often experience lower quality care [29, 30]. Clinicians who care for low SES patients with complex needs and multimorbidity are often under-resourced, under extreme stress, and at higher risk for burnout. In addition, low SES patients may have shorter consultations with clinicians. All of these factors may contribute to patients with low SES experiencing less empathy [31].

We consider this work preliminary in nature given that there are important limitations to consider. First, the clinical significance of small differences in the CARE Measure are unclear, despite statistical significance (i.e. for SES). We are not aware of any research into the clinical significance of specific CARE Measure increments. However, our post-hoc analyses identified that such an empathy gap may be widening over time. While exploratory in nature, these post-hoc analyses provide additional scientific rationale for future studies investigating the existence of an empathy gap, as well as studies aimed at increasing clinician empathy for disadvantaged populations. Further, given recent evidence that people from low SES communities are less likely to respond to patient experience surveys resulting in non-response bias (i.e. responders are not representative of the target patient population) [66], future research needs to also focus on alternative methods to assess patient experience in disadvantaged populations so as to not underestimate possible differences in clinician empathy.

Another potential limitation is that our meta-analysis was limited to studies of the CARE Measure, and did not incorporate other previously published measures of clinician empathy. Our rationale was that we wanted to perform a quantitative meta-analysis, and this requires a single measure approach to limit heterogeneity and permit pooling of data. We selected the CARE Measure because to date it is the most commonly used assessment of clinician empathy from the patient perspective, and it has very well-validated methodology (i.e. proven reliability, internal validity and consistency) [32]. Nonetheless, it is possible that studies using a different measure would find different results [67, 68]. We also acknowledge heterogeneity (I2) in the random effects model for SES.

Importantly, as it pertains to the analyses by race/ethnicity, none of the studies contained information on race/ethnicity of the clinicians. Therefore, in our study it is not possible to account for race concordance/discordance (or in-group/out-group bias). We also acknowledge that the CARE Measure is a patient’s assessment of the empathy of clinicians (e.g. physicians) only. Others in the health care environment (e.g. staff, registrars, etc.) may have interactions with patients that shape patients’ experience of empathy during health care encounters in a meaningful way, and this would not necessarily be captured by the CARE Measure. We also need to acknowledge that because individual patient-level data were not collected, we could not establish our own uniform definition of low SES, and instead we relied on the definition of low SES that the authors used in each individual study. Further, only two of the studies contained data stratified by both SES and race/ethnicity so interaction could not be assessed. It is also unclear if our results reflect a true difference in clinician empathy for patients with low SES or are simply a reflection of patients who are experiencing socioeconomic deprivation. However, results from previous work refute the latter, by comparing CARE scores with objectively measured aspects of patient-centered care [45].

Lastly, we are not aware of any studies that have tested if disadvantaged persons (by either SES and/or race/ethnicity) have different expectations for clinician empathy in health care encounters due to history of discrimination (e.g. institutionalized and interpersonal racism), and this could potentially affect the results.

Finding meaningful disparities in clinician empathy would have important implications for public health because clinician empathy is vital for high quality health care. We believe the results of this systematic review and meta-analysis are important preliminary data supporting that an empathy gap may exist for disadvantaged people in face-to-face health care encounters with clinicians. More research to further test this hypothesis and help promote health care equity is warranted.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Forest plot of the results of the random effects model for patient-reported experience of clinician empathy comparing Black/African American patients to white patients.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247259.s002

(JPG)

S2 Fig. Forest plot of the results of the random effects model for patient-reported experience of clinician empathy comparing Hispanic/Latino patients to white patients.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247259.s003

(JPG)

S3 Fig. Forest plot of the results of the random effects model for patient-reported experience of clinician empathy comparing Asian patients to white patients.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247259.s004

(JPG)

S4 Fig. Forest plot of the results of the random effects model for patient-reported experience of clinician empathy comparing Native American patients to white patients.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247259.s005

(JPG)

S5 Fig. Funnel plot for the random effects model of empathy and socioeconomic status (SES).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247259.s006

(JPG)

S6 Fig. Funnel plot for the random effects model of empathy and race/ethnicity (all non-whites compared to whites).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247259.s007

(JPG)

S1 Table. Assessment of the quality of each study using the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (range 0–9 stars).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247259.s008

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank all of the corresponding authors of included studies who, in response to our author queries, provided stratified data (i.e. by SES and race/ethnicity) for analysis. We especially thank Prof. Stewart Mercer, as well as Shari Barlow, Dr. Bruce Barrett, Mary Checovich, Dr. Adriana Foster, Dr. Syed Shahzad Hasan, Dr. John Licciardone, Yi En Low, Wan Juen Ng, Dr. Raymond Parrish II, and Dr. Wai Yew Yang.

References

  1. 1. Riess H. The Science of Empathy. Journal of Patient Experience. 2017;4(2):74–7. Epub 2017/07/21. pmid:28725865
  2. 2. Attar HS, Chandramani S. Impact of physician empathy on migraine disability and migraineur compliance. Annals of Indian Academy of Neurology. 2012;15(Suppl 1):S89–94. pmid:23024571
  3. 3. Burns DD, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Therapeutic empathy and recovery from depression in cognitive-behavioral therapy: a structural equation model. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1992;60(3):441–9. pmid:1619098
  4. 4. Del Canale S, Louis DZ, Maio V, Wang X, Rossi G, Hojat M, et al. The relationship between physician empathy and disease complications: an empirical study of primary care physicians and their diabetic patients in Parma, Italy. Academic Medicine. 2012;87(9):1243–9. pmid:22836852
  5. 5. Flickinger TE, Saha S, Roter D, Korthuis PT, Sharp V, Cohn J, et al. Clinician empathy is associated with differences in patient-clinician communication behaviors and higher medication self-efficacy in HIV care. Patient Education and Counseling. 2016;99(2):220–6. pmid:26395313
  6. 6. Hojat M, Louis DZ, Markham FW, Wender R, Rabinowitz C, Gonnella JS. Physicians’ empathy and clinical outcomes for diabetic patients. Academic Medicine. 2011;86(3):359–64. pmid:21248604
  7. 7. Hollinger-Samson N, Pearson JL. The relationship between staff empathy and depressive symptoms in nursing home residents. Aging & Mental Health. 2000;4(1):56–65.
  8. 8. Kim SS, Kaplowitz S, Johnston MV. The effects of physician empathy on patient satisfaction and compliance. Evaluation & the Health Professions. 2004;27(3):237–51. pmid:15312283
  9. 9. Lelorain S, Bredart A, Dolbeault S, Sultan S. A systematic review of the associations between empathy measures and patient outcomes in cancer care. Psycho-oncology. 2012;21(12):1255–64. pmid:22238060
  10. 10. Mercer SW, Jani BD, Maxwell M, Wong SY, Watt GC. Patient enablement requires physician empathy: a cross-sectional study of general practice consultations in areas of high and low socioeconomic deprivation in Scotland. BMC Family Practice. 2012;13:6. pmid:22316293
  11. 11. Pereira L, Figueiredo-Braga M, Carvalho IP. Preoperative anxiety in ambulatory surgery: The impact of an empathic patient-centered approach on psychological and clinical outcomes. Patient Education and Counseling. 2016;99(5):733–8. pmid:26654958
  12. 12. Rakel DP, Hoeft TJ, Barrett BP, Chewning BA, Craig BM, Niu M. Practitioner empathy and the duration of the common cold. Family Medicine. 2009;41(7):494–501. pmid:19582635
  13. 13. Steinhausen S, Ommen O, Antoine SL, Koehler T, Pfaff H, Neugebauer E. Short- and long-term subjective medical treatment outcome of trauma surgery patients: the importance of physician empathy. Patient Preference and Adherence. 2014;8:1239–53. pmid:25258518
  14. 14. West CP, Huschka MM, Novotny PJ, Sloan JA, Kolars JC, Habermann TM, et al. Association of perceived medical errors with resident distress and empathy: a prospective longitudinal study. JAMA. 2006;296(9):1071–8. pmid:16954486
  15. 15. Kaiser Family Foundation. Disparities in health and health care. Available from: https://www.kff.org/disparities-policy/issue-brief/disparities-in-health-and-health-care-five-key-questions-and-answers/.
  16. 16. Bernabei R, Gambassi G, Lapane K, Landi F, Gatsonis C, Dunlop R, et al. JAMA. 1998;279(23):1877–82. pmid:9634258
  17. 17. Drwecki BB, Moore CF, Ward SE, Prkachin KM. Reducing racial disparities in pain treatment: the role of empathy and perspective-taking. Pain. 2011;152(5):1001–6. pmid:21277087
  18. 18. Hoffman KM, Trawalter S, Axt JR, Oliver MN. Racial bias in pain assessment and treatment recommendations, and false beliefs about biological differences between blacks and whites. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U S A. 2016;113(16):4296–301. pmid:27044069
  19. 19. Todd KH, Samaroo N, Hoffman JR. Ethnicity as a risk factor for inadequate emergency department analgesia. JAMA. 1993;269(12):1537–9. pmid:8445817
  20. 20. Schulman KA, Berlin JA, Harless W, Kerner JF, Sistrunk S, Gersh BJ, et al. The effect of race and sex on physicians’ recommendations for cardiac catheterization. The New England Journal of Medicine. 1999;340(8):618–26. pmid:10029647
  21. 21. van Ryn M, Burke J. The effect of patient race and socio-economic status on physicians’ perceptions of patients. Social Science & Medicine. 2000;50(6):813–28. pmid:10695979
  22. 22. Figueroa JF, Reimold KE, Zheng J, Orav EJ. Differences in Patient Experience Between Hispanic and Non-Hispanic White Patients Across U.S. Hospitals. Journal for Healthcare Quality. 2018;40(5):292–300. pmid:29252871
  23. 23. Figueroa JF, Zheng J, Orav EJ, Jha AK. Across US Hospitals, Black Patients Report Comparable Or Better Experiences Than White Patients. Health Affairs. 2016;35(8):1391–8. pmid:27503962
  24. 24. Doescher MP, Saver BG, Franks P, Fiscella K. Racial and ethnic disparities in perceptions of physician style and trust. Archives of Family Medicine. 2000;9(10):1156–63. pmid:11115223
  25. 25. Okunrintemi V, Khera R, Spatz ES, Salami JA, Valero-Elizondo J, Warraich HJ, et al. Association of Income Disparities with Patient-Reported Healthcare Experience. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2019;34(6):884–92. pmid:30783877
  26. 26. Elliott AM, Alexander SC, Mescher CA, Mohan D, Barnato AE. Differences in Physicians’ Verbal and Nonverbal Communication With Black and White Patients at the End of Life. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management. 2016;51(1):1–8. pmid:26297851
  27. 27. Saha S, Arbelaez JJ, Cooper LA. Patient-physician relationships and racial disparities in the quality of health care. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93(10):1713–9. pmid:14534227
  28. 28. Cooper LA, Roter DL, Carson KA, Beach MC, Sabin JA, Greenwald AG, et al. The associations of clinicians’ implicit attitudes about race with medical visit communication and patient ratings of interpersonal care. American Journal of Public Health. 2012;102(5):979–87. pmid:22420787
  29. 29. Hart JT. The inverse care law. Lancet. 1971;1(7696):405–12. pmid:4100731
  30. 30. McLean G, Guthrie B, Mercer SW, Watt GC. General practice funding underpins the persistence of the inverse care law: cross-sectional study in Scotland. British Journal of General Practice. 2015;65(641):e799–805. pmid:26622032
  31. 31. Mercer SW, Zhou Y, Humphris GM, McConnachie A, Bakhshi A, Bikker A, et al. Multimorbidity and Socioeconomic Deprivation in Primary Care Consultations. Annals of Family Medicine. 2018;16(2):127–31. pmid:29531103
  32. 32. Howick J, Steinkopf L, Ulyte A, Roberts N, Meissner K. How empathic is your healthcare practitioner? A systematic review and meta-analysis of patient surveys. BMC Medical Education. 2017;17(1):136. pmid:28823250
  33. 33. Mercer SW. The CARE Measure: Scottish Government Chief Scientist Office. Available at: http://www.caremeasure.org/CAREEng.pdf.
  34. 34. Mercer SW, Maxwell M, Heaney D, Watt GC. The consultation and relational empathy (CARE) measure: development and preliminary validation and reliability of an empathy-based consultation process measure. Family Practice. 2004;21(6):699–705. pmid:15528286
  35. 35. Roberts BW, Trzeciak CJ, Puri NK, Mazzarelli AJ, Trzeciak S. Racial and socioeconomic disparities in patient experience of clinician empathy: a protocol for systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2020;10(6):e034247. pmid:32595149
  36. 36. Higgins JPT, T J., Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, et al. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions, version 6.0: The Cochrane Collaboration; 2019.
  37. 37. Shamseer L, Moher D, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015: elaboration and explanation. BMJ. 2015;350:g7647. pmid:25555855
  38. 38. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Group P. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine. 2009;6(7):e1000097. pmid:19621072
  39. 39. Stroup DF, Berlin JA, Morton SC, Olkin I, Williamson GD, Rennie D, et al. Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: a proposal for reporting. Meta-analysis Of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) group. JAMA. 2000;283(15):2008–12. pmid:10789670
  40. 40. National Institutes of Health. Inclusion enrollment report. Bethesda, MD, USA. Available from: https://grants.nih.gov/grants/funding/2590/enrollmentreport.pdf.
  41. 41. Higgins JPT, Green S. Tools for assessing methodological quality or risk of bias in non-randomized studies (section 13.5.2.3). In: Higgins JPT, Green S, editors. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 510: The Cochrane Collaboration; 2011.
  42. 42. Wells GA, Shea B, O’Connell D, Peterson J, Welch V, Losos M, et al. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses. Available from: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp.
  43. 43. Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine. 2002;21(11):1539–58. pmid:12111919
  44. 44. Jani B, Bikker AP, Higgins M, Fitzpatrick B, Little P, Watt GC, et al. Patient centredness and the outcome of primary care consultations with patients with depression in areas of high and low socioeconomic deprivation. British Journal of General Practice. 2012;62(601):e576–81. pmid:22867682
  45. 45. Mercer SW, Higgins M, Bikker AM, Fitzpatrick B, McConnachie A, Lloyd SM, et al. General Practitioners’ Empathy and Health Outcomes: A Prospective Observational Study of Consultations in Areas of High and Low Deprivation. Ann Fam Med. 2016;14(2):117–24. pmid:26951586
  46. 46. Mercer SW, McConnachie A, Maxwell M, Heaney D, Watt GC. Relevance and practical use of the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) Measure in general practice. Family Practice. 2005;22(3):328–34. pmid:15772120
  47. 47. Barrett B, Brown R, Rakel D, Rabago D, Marchand L, Scheder J, et al. Placebo effects and the common cold: a randomized controlled trial. Annals of Family Medicine. 2011;9(4):312–22. pmid:21747102
  48. 48. Bikker AP, Fitzpatrick B, Murphy D, Forster L, Mercer SW. Assessing the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) Measure in sexual health nurses’ consultations. BMC Nursing. 2017;16:71. pmid:29204104
  49. 49. Bikker AP, Fitzpatrick B, Murphy D, Mercer SW. Measuring empathic, person-centred communication in primary care nurses: validity and reliability of the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) Measure. BMC Family Practice. 2015;16:149. pmid:26493072
  50. 50. Bikker AP, Mercer SW, Reilly D. A pilot prospective study on the consultation and relational empathy, patient enablement, and health changes over 12 months in patients going to the Glasgow Homoeopathic Hospital. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine. 2005;11(4):591–600. pmid:16131282
  51. 51. Hannan J, Sanchez G, Musser ED, Ward-Peterson M, Azutillo E, Goldin D, et al. Role of empathy in the perception of medical errors in patient encounters: a preliminary study. BMC Research Notes. 2019;12(1):327. pmid:31182161
  52. 52. Yu FS, Yip BH, Kung K, Fung CS, Wong CK, Lam AT, et al. The Association of Types of Training and Practice Settings with Doctors’ Empathy and Patient Enablement among Patients with Chronic Illness in Hong Kong. PLoS One. 2015;10(12):e0144492. pmid:26658427
  53. 53. Babar MG, Hasan SS, Yong WM, Mitha S, Al-Waeli HA. Patients’ Perceptions of Dental Students’ Empathic, Person-Centered Care in a Dental School Clinic in Malaysia. Journal of Dental Education. 2017;81(4):404–12. pmid:28365604
  54. 54. Kootstra TJM, Wilkens SC, Menendez ME, Ring D. Is Physician Empathy Associated With Differences in Pain and Functional Limitations After a Hand Surgeon Visit? Clinical Orthopaedics and Related Research. 2018;476(4):801–7. pmid:29481341
  55. 55. LaVela SL, Heinemann AW, Etingen B, Miskovic A, Locatelli SM, Chen D. Relational empathy and holistic care in persons with spinal cord injuries. Journal of Spinal Cord Medicine. 2017;40(1):30–42. pmid:26833180
  56. 56. Licciardone JC, Schmitt ME, Aryal S. Empathy in Medicine Osteopathic and Allopathic Physician Interpersonal Manner, Empathy, and Communication Style and Clinical Status of Their Patients: A Pain Registry-Based Study. Journal of the American Osteopathic Association. 2019;119(8):499–510. pmid:31305871.
  57. 57. McVay M, Steinberg D, Askew S, Bennett GG. Provider Counseling and Weight Loss Outcomes in a Primary Care-Based Digital Obesity Treatment. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2019;34(6):992–8. pmid:30891688
  58. 58. Menendez ME, Chen NC, Mudgal CS, Jupiter JB, Ring D. Physician Empathy as a Driver of Hand Surgery Patient Satisfaction. Journal of Hand Surgery. 2015;40(9):1860–5 e2. pmid:26231482
  59. 59. Moss J, Roberts MB, Shea L, Jones CW, Kilgannon H, Edmondson DE, et al. Healthcare provider compassion is associated with lower PTSD symptoms among patients with life-threatening medical emergencies: a prospective cohort study. Intensive Care Medicine. 2019;45(6):815–22. pmid:30911803
  60. 60. Parker PA, Banerjee SC, Matasar MJ, Bylund CL, Schofield E, Li Y, et al. Cancer worry and empathy moderate the effect of a survivorship-focused intervention on quality of life. Psycho-oncology. 2020;29(6):1012–1018. pmid:32128909
  61. 61. Parrish RC 2nd, Menendez ME, Mudgal CS, Jupiter JB, Chen NC, Ring D. Patient Satisfaction and its Relation to Perceived Visit Duration With a Hand Surgeon. Journal of Hand Surgery. 2016;41(2):257–62 e1-4. pmid:26718069
  62. 62. Weaver FM, Binns-Calvey A, Gonzalez B, Kostovich C, LaVela S, Stroupe KT, et al. Alerting Doctors About Patient Life Challenges: A Randomized Control Trial of a Previsit Inventory of Contextual Factors. MDM Policy and Practice. 2019;4(1):2381468319852334. pmid:31192310
  63. 63. Wilkens SC, Ring D, Teunis T, Lee SP, Chen NC. Decision Aid for Trapeziometacarpal Arthritis: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Journal of Hand Surgery. 2019;44(3):247 e1- e9. pmid:30031600
  64. 64. Yang WY, Low YE, Ng WJ, Ong SH, Jamil JA. Investigation of empathy amongst dietetic interns at selected primary and tertiary health-care facilities. Nutrition and Dietetics. 2020;77(2):231–9. pmid:31199060
  65. 65. Barnett K, Mercer SW, Norbury M, Watt G, Wyke S, Guthrie B. Epidemiology of multimorbidity and implications for health care, research, and medical education: a cross-sectional study. Lancet. 2012;380(9836):37–43. pmid:22579043
  66. 66. Roberts BW, Yao J, Trzeciak CJ, Bezich LS, Mazzarelli A, Trzeciak S. Income Disparities and Nonresponse Bias in Surveys of Patient Experience. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2020;35(7):2217–2218. pmid:32006345
  67. 67. Roberts BW, Roberts MB, Yao J, Bosire J, Mazzarelli A, Trzeciak S. Development and Validation of a Tool to Measure Patient Assessment of Clinical Compassion. JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(5):e193976. pmid:31099870
  68. 68. Sabapathi P, Roberts MB, Fuller BM, Puskarich MA, Jones CW, Kilgannon JH, et al. Validation of a 5-item tool to measure patient assessment of clinician compassion in the emergency department. BMC Emergency Medicine. 2019;19(1):63. pmid:31684885