Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Absence of reliable physiological signature of illusory body ownership revealed by fine-grained autonomic measurement during the rubber hand illusion

  • Hugo D. Critchley ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    h.critchley@bsms.ac.uk

    Affiliations School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom, Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex and University of Brighton, Brighton, United Kingdom

  • Vanessa Botan,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom, Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom

  • Jamie Ward

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom, Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom

Abstract

The neural representation of a ‘biological self’ is linked theoretically to the control of bodily physiology. In an influential model, selfhood relates to internal agency and higher-order interoceptive representation, inferred from the predicted impact of efferent autonomic nervous activity on afferent viscerosensory feedback. Here we tested if an altered representation of physical self (illusory embodiment of an artificial hand) is accompanied by sustained shifts in autonomic activity. Participants (N = 37) underwent procedures for induction of the rubber hand illusion (synchronous stroking of own unseen hand and observed stroking of artificial hand) and a control condition (asychronous stroking). We recorded electrocardiography, electrodermal activity, and a non-invasive measure of multiunit skin sympathetic nerve activity (SKNA) from the chest. We compared these autonomic indices between task conditions, and between individuals who did and did not experience the illusion. Bayes factors quantified the strength of evidence for and against null hypotheses. Observed proprioceptive drift and subjective reports confirmed the efficacy of the synchronous (vs asynchronous) condition in inducing illusory hand ownership. Stringent discriminant analysis classified 24/37 individuals as experiencing the rubber hand illusion. Surprisingly, heart rate, heart rate variability, electrodermal activity, and SKNA measures revealed no autonomic differences between synchronous vs asynchronous conditions, nor between individuals who did or did not experience the rubber hand illusion. Bayes factors indicated substantial evidence for no physiological differences. In contrast to earlier reports, our autonomic data show the absence of a reliable change in physiological state during the rubber hand illusion. More encompassing perturbations of self-experience, for example in full body illusions, may nevertheless be coupled to, or facilitated by, changes in efferent autonomic activity and afferent viscerosensory feedback. Our findings suggest that such changes in bodily physiology are not sustained as an obligatory component of the rubber hand illusion.

Introduction

The representation of the body’s physiological state and physical boundaries is argued to be fundamental to self-perception and awareness. This representation is built upon the integration of sensory information across modalities with expectations and predictions regarding what our body should be doing [14]. The dependence of the experience of body ownership on dynamic coherence across senses is illustrated by the rubber hand illusion, induced through correspondence between somatosensory stimulation and visual feedback: An artificial ‘rubber’ hand is placed in front of a participant and seen to be stroked. At the same time, the matching own hand of the participant, is hidden from sight, yet stroked at the same frequency (synchronously). Most participants experience the rubber hand as part of their own body [4]. This experience is reported by the participant as a subjective feeling, potentially linked to internal (interoceptive) bodily sensations [5]. The strength of the illusion can be scored on questionnaires and visual analogue scales. Less subjective measures of the illusion include ‘proprioceptive drift’, where the judged location of the participant’s own hand shifts to be nearer the rubber hand. Physiological reactions can also offer objective measurements of the rubber hand illusion. For example when the rubber hand is threatened, for instance with being hit by a hammer, the degree of illusory embodiment can be inferred from the magnitude of sympathetic skin conductance response [6] or from the strength of motoric withdrawal of the participant’s own hand, evoked by the apparent threat [7]. However, even despite the relative consistency across studies of exaggerated reactivity to threats to an embodied rubber hand [8], these effects may be more nuanced, in some cases relating more to physical contact than threat [9,10].

Influential theories of consciousness suggest that there is a primacy to the (predictive) control of internal bodily state, through which the sense of self as a continuous, unitary, bounded experience arises from the inseparability of mental processes from the dynamics of interoceptive (inner physiological; viscerosensory) sensation and internal agency [1114]. Empirical data to support this is more limited. However, abnormalities in normative autonomic reactivity or interoceptive processes are associated with clinical conditions involving disturbed self-representation [15,16]. Furthermore, individuals with reduced ability to perform a heartbeat tracking task (a heuristic measure of sensitivity to interoceptive signals) are more susceptible to the rubber hand illusion [5], suggesting that a weaker model of ‘internal self’ predisposes to a more malleable representation of the body’s boundaries. Relatedly, if physiological information is introduced into the visual display of a virtual rubber hand or body (by colour changes pulsing normatively in time with the participant’s own heartbeat), this can increase the likelihood (and objective correlates of) the illusory experience of body ownership, indicating a binding role of interoceptive predictive experience in the representation of the physical self [17,18].

Within active inference models of interoception, autonomic drive to peripheral organs encompasses predictions about the desired internal state, changing the afferent feedback to manage better interoceptive prediction error [13,14]. As noted, interoceptive predictions and internal agency are central to current theoretical models of embodied selfhood [13]. By extension, one can hypothesize that changes in peripheral physiological state, mediated autonomically, would reflect a shift in bodily self-representation associated with the experience of the rubber hand illusion. Such changes are distinct from measuring reactions to threat provocation, in that they are proximate to the putative interoceptive mechanism underpinning conscious selfhood. Relevantly, a fall in temperature of substituted (participant’s own stroked) hand is described as an objective indicator of the illusion of embodiment [19]. Conversely, cooling the tested hand may facilitate the experience of the illusion [20]. In a stable environment, cooling of the skin surface can occur through changes in skin blood flow (perfusion) and via increased sweat production (driven by sympathetic nerves innervating sweat glands, whose activity is reflected in electrodermal measures such as skin conductance responses). Immobility during the induction of the rubber hand illusion may underlie local autonomic changes. This passive consequence of proprioceptive quiescence is potentially amplified by the suggested/intentional state evoked by synchronous stroking. Here, the physiological (peripheral vascular) state of the participant’s own hand better matches the cold lifeless visual appearance of the embodied rubber hand. However, not all studies have been able to replicate the finding of skin cooling in the participant’s hand corresponding to the rubber hand [21,22].

Few studies have tested explicitly for cardiac changes associated with the illusory body ownership, despite some studies [5,17], but not all [23,24], demonstrating a relationship between ‘cardioceptive sensitivity‘ (implying better heartbeat perception) and susceptibility to the rubber hand illusion, and other studies showing that the illusory experience can be modulated by heartbeat information [17,18]. Changes in heart rate and heart rate variability provide measures of the relative balance of antagonistic sympathetic and parasympathetic autonomic nervous activity on the atrioventricular pacemaker. This balance contributes, via the baroreflex, to blood pressure regulation. Stronger heartbeats increase afferent feedback from arterial aortic and carotid baroreceptors, which inhibits efferent sympathetic drive and enhances vagal parasympathetic slowing of heart rate. Cognitive, affective and behavioural states can engender an increase in cardiovascular arousal through ‘top-down’ suppression of the baroreflex, shifting the sympathovagal balance to increase heart rate and decrease (parasympathetically dominant) heart rate variability. Where studies have tested for changes in heart rate or heart rate variability in association with illusions of body ownership, few differences are reported [18]. In contrast, variance in electrodermal activity (not evoked by threat to the rubber hand illusion) may indicate the adoption of an illusory rubber hand within a participant’s bodily representation, differentiating between the synchronous stroking condition (that best induces the illusion) versus asynchronous ‘control’ condition. However, these effects appear to be transient, suggesting that psychological novelty of the experience is a potential cause [25]. Changes in electrodermal activity, including tonic skin conductance level and phasic skin conductance responses, are an expression of sympathetic nerve activity regulating eccrine sweat gland function [26,27]. Electrodermal activity is usually measured from the palmar surface of the hand and has proved to be a sensitive and widely adopted index of central attention and arousal states. Of relevance to interoception, skin sweat glands do not possess dedicated visceral afferents, hence sensory feedback of electrodermal activity is absent or indirect (via correlated changes in physiological arousal in other organ systems; but see [28]). Moreover, reflecting organ specificity, the autonomic sympathetic innervation of the palmar skin is dissociable from sympathetic innervation of heart and vasculature [29].

Here, we tested if autonomically-mediated changes in bodily state would reflect the alteration in conscious bodily self-representation associated with adoption of the rubber hand illusion. Participants underwent a rubber hand illusion induction protocol [4,5,810]. This involved synchronous stroking of a visible artificial right hand and the participant’s own hidden right hand. A control comparison condition was also conducted in which the stroking of the two (real and rubber) hands was asynchronous. We predicted that changes in body ownership (experience of the rubber hand illusion) would be apparent as differences in autonomic reactivity over synchronous compared to asynchronous stroking conditions and between those who did and did not experience the illusion. We recorded for analysis; 1) electrocardiography (to quantify heart rate and vagally-mediated heart rate variability, HRV); 2) electrodermal activity from the participants right hand (to quantify the magnitude and frequency of sympathetic skin responses), and; 3) a relatively novel non-invasive proposed measure of skin sympathetic nerve activity (SKNA), recorded over the chest, in a distribution reflecting sympathetic activity of the stellate ganglion [3032]. This SKNA measure was included as a fine-grained direct measure of efferent autonomic nerve traffic. We tested the hypothesis that cardiac, electrodermal, and SKNA autonomic indices would differentiate the synchronous induction vs asynchronous control conditions in the rubber hand illusion protocol, and between individuals who experienced the illusion to a greater extent [33]. Motivated by the theoretical and empirical link to perceptual aspects of interoception [5,13,14,17,18], we undertook exploratory analyses to test for associations between (objective and subjective) measures of the rubber hand illusion with individual differences in heartbeat tracking performance and interoceptive metacognitive insight (awareness) [34,35].

Materials and methods

Participants

A total of 37 participants (mean age = 21.59, SD = 3.69; 31 females) completed the study. Participants were recruited from students and staff at the University of Sussex via electronic advertisement. Ethical approval was obtained from the Brighton and Sussex Medical School Research Governance and Ethics Committee (BSMSREGC) and the Science and Technology Research Ethics Committee of the University of Sussex. All participants give written informed consent.

Sample size was computed using G-power calculator for paired t-tests. The computation parameters included a medium effect size (d = 0.5), α set at 0.05, and power set at 0.8. The resulting sample size was of 34 participants. Considering that the effect sizes between synchronous and asynchronous conditions are usually medium/large [19,36,37] a sample of 34 participants was considered adequate.

Rubber hand illusion paradigm

Testing was conducted in a climate-controlled room. In the rubber hand illusion task, the participant’s right arm was placed in a box (86 cm×60 cm×20 cm), screened from view. A life-size artificial ‘rubber’ model of a right hand was placed midline within the visible section of the box, directly in front of participant body. The distance between the participant’s right index finger and the index finger of the rubber hand was 20 cm.

Stroking with a paintbrush was applied by the experimenter (VB) to the index finger of the participant’s hidden hand and the artificial hand. Two conditions were performed in a counterbalanced order across participants: 1) Synchronous stroking when the timing of the brush strokes on the participant’s own hand and on the rubber hand was coincided. 2) Asynchronous stroking when the timing of the brush strokes on the participant’s hand and rubber hand was out of phase by approximately 625ms). Each condition lasted two minutes. At the beginning of each condition, the participant estimated the location of her/his right index fingertip three times by reading the corresponding number along a one-meter ruler, whose visible position varied each time to prevent the participant repeating responses in subsequent readings. Post-induction finger location judgements were obtained in the same manner as prior to the induction. Proprioceptive drift was calculated by subtracting the average of the pre-induction finger location judgements from the average of post-induction finger location judgement:

After each condition, the participant completed the RHI questionnaire comprising 10 items divided into three subscales: ownership, location, and agency, see Table 1 for further details. The items were measured on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree) and the average score for each subscale was calculated [33].

thumbnail
Table 1. Rubber hand illusion questionnaire items and subscales.

(See [33]).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237282.t001

Physiological measures

Heart rate and heart rate variability.

Electrocardiographic signals were recorded using CED 1902 and 1401 hardware and fed into Spike 2 software (Cambridge Electronic Design Ltd; Cambridge UK) applying a 10Hz high bandpass filter and 100 Hz low bandpass filter [38]. For the analysis, a threshold was applied to isolate R-wave peaks and to extract the number of heartbeats in a given time interval. This gave measures of heart rate (HR) (beats per time interval) and heart rate variability (HRV) expressed as the root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) between normal heartbeats, the primary time-domain measure for short-term variation, strongly correlated with high-frequency variations and an indicator of the vagally-mediated (parasympathetic) changes reflected in HRV [39]. Both HR and HRV were calculated for synchronous and asynchronous conditions, averaged over the two minute for each condition.

Electrodermal activity (skin conductance responses).

Electrodermal activity was recorded using two finger electrodes (CED2502) and electrolyte gel placed on the palmar surface of the index and middle finger of the participant’s right hand. Signals were recorded into Spike 2 software using CED 1401 hardware. Analyses of skin conductance responses (SCRs) were performed on data exported to Matlab using Ledalab (V3.4.9) software. Adaptive data smoothing was applied, and continuous decomposition analysis was performed with extraction of continuous phasic and tonic activity. All SCR-onsets and amplitudes of above-threshold SCRs (a minimum of 0.01 microS) were then used to compute the average SCR amplitude per condition and a total number of SCR peaks per condition over the two minutes [26].

Skin sympathetic nerve activity (SKNA).

Following methods described by Doytchinova and colleagues [3032], six electrocardiographic electrodes were placed over the chest of the participant, two placed on the wrists, and two placed on the ankles (Fig 1). This signal was captured with PowerLab 16/35 (ADInstruments, Dunedin NZ) and recorded and displayed using LabChart v7.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Electrode positioning for recording of skin sympathetic nerve activity (SKNA).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237282.g001

To record SKNA, we followed methods described by Doytchinova and colleagues [3032]: Electocardiographic (ECG) patch electrodes were used to record high frequency electrical signals bandpass filtered between 500Hz and 1000Hz and derive the average signal (aSKNA) for each of the two experimental conditions. Correspondence between aSKNA activity and stellate ganglion function has been established in animal and human studies [32].

The sampling rate was set at 10000 samples/s and bandpass filtering between 500Hz and 1000Hz was applied. This filtering is shown to provide the best signal-to-noise ratio for skin sympathetic nerve activity (SKNA) recordings [30,32]. Quantitative signal data analysis was conducted, following the published protocol [30]. Here, the average SKNA (aSKNA) was calculated for specific time windows by dividing the total voltage within a time window to the number of samples within that specific time window at a sampling rate of 10,000/s. To test if SKNA would provide better temporal resolution than other autonomic measures, we initially used 30s-time windows; the total number of samples within the time window was 300,000. The 30s aSKNA was used in the initial statistical analyses presented below and further computed for 120s for comparison with physiological measures.

Interoceptive accuracy and awareness.

Interoceptive accuracy was measured using the heartbeat tracking task [34,35] containing six trials with varying interval durations of 25, 30, 35, 40, 45 and 50 seconds played in a randomised order. The participant was instructed to silently count the number of heartbeats perceived in the given interval and to report them at the end of each trial. The actual number of heartbeats was measured using medical grade pulse oximeter with a soft sensor (Nonin Medical Inc, Plymouth MN USA; Xpod 8000S). For each trial, the accuracy score was derived using the following formula:

The resulting scores of each trial were averaged, yielding the overall value for each participant [35]. Confidence judgements were taken at the end of each trial, participants being asked to rate the confidence they had in their reported number of heartbeats. Their response was recorded on a 10 points continuous visual analogue scale (VAS) from ‘total guess/no heartbeat awareness’ to ‘complete confidence/full perception of heartbeat’. Interoceptive awareness was then calculated using the Pearson correlation between interoceptive accuracy and confidence rating [35].

A median split was performed in exploratory analysis of the relationship between interoceptive performance on the heartbeat tracking task, The two groups (low and high interoception) had 17 participants each and the median point was 0.71. Due to technical issues, interoceptive data was not available for three participants (16 in the higher interoception group, 14 in the lower interoception group). Therefore, this reduced the degrees of freedom to 32.

Statistical analyses.

Final statistical analyses were run in SPSS25 (IBM Statistics). Independent samples t-tests and their Bayesian equivalent (i.e. independent samples normal) tests were run on proprioceptive drift and subjective ratings for two groups (low vs high interoceptive accuracy) of seventeen participants each. Paired sample t-tests and their Bayesian equivalent (i.e. related samples normal) tests were performed to compare physiological measures between conditions. Pearson correlations were conducted across physiological measures. Pearson correlations and their Bayesian equivalents were also run to establish the relation between drift and interoceptive accuracy and awareness. Equal variances were assumed and Rouder’s method was applied for the Bayesian analyses. When normality was not assumed (i.e. questionnaire data and aSKNA data), non-parametric tests were run to re-confirm the results (see Supplementary material).

A discriminant analysis was run to distinguish between participants who did and did not show a strong experience of the illusion, using the difference between synchronous and asynchronous conditions in subjective ratings and in proprioceptive drift as predictors. The correlations between these predictors was relatively low (r = 0.367) and Wilks’ Lambda was statistically significant for both predicting variables (p<0.001), confirming their adequacy for the analysis. Following this discriminant analysis, independent samples t-tests and their Bayesian equivalent (i.e. independent samples normal) tests were used to compare aSKNA results between the two groups of participants classified according to strength of their experience of the illusion.

Results

Behavioural results

Proprioceptive drift.

Proprioceptive drift data was normally distributed for both synchronous and asynchronous drift measures (Shapiro Wilk normality tests; p = 0.091 and p = 0.945 respectively) and normality Q-Q plots. For the entire sample, proprioceptive drift was greater in the synchronous condition (M = 26.0 mm, SD = 5.46) than in the asynchronous condition (M = 4.6mm, SD = 3.67), t(36) = 3.097, p = 0.004, 95% CI [7.38, 35.41], d = 7.56, BH(0,1) = 0.123, thus there is moderate evidence for H1. Therefore, according to a typical control procedure, the RHI was successfully induced (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Proprioceptive drift in synchronous and asynchronous conditions expressed in millimetres (mm: Median ± 95% CI).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237282.g002

Each participant judged the location of their unseen index finger on prior to and after each synchronous and asynchronous stroking condition (synchrony relative to the observed stroking of a visible artificial ‘rubber’ hand placed midline, directly in front of the participant). Proprioceptive drift is the objective finding that the experience of the illusion is accompanied by the participant judging his/her own hand to be located nearer to the artificial hand. The amount of measure ‘drift’ in a participant’s rating of his/her unseen hand is thus used as an objective measure of the illusion. Here, this measure of embodiment shows the synchronous condition induced the illusory experience.

Subjective reports

The three subscales of the rubber hand illusion questionnaire [33] were independently analysed providing the following results: ownership, t(36) = 7.825, p<0.001, 95% CI [1.23, 2.10], d = 1.28, BH(0,1) = 1.0 × 10−3; location, t(36) = 6.187, p<0.001, 95% CI [0.87, 1.72], d = 0.73, BH(0,1) = 1.0 × 10−3; agency, t(36) = 5.036 p<0.001, 95% CI [0.87, 2.00], d = 0.85, BH(0,1) = 1.0 × 10−3. As such, there was decisive evidence for H1, indicating that the illusion was successfully induced (Table 2; Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Distribution of subjective ratings for each condition and each subscale of the rubber hand illusion questionnaire.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237282.g003

thumbnail
Table 2. Showing means and standard deviations for subjective ratings in each condition and for each subscale of the rubber hand illusion questionnaire [33].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237282.t002

Discriminant analysis.

Participants with greater experience of the illusion could be distinguished by discriminant analysis from those with weaker or no experience of the illusion. There was a medium correlation between predictors (proprioceptive drift and subjective ratings) (r = 0.367) and Wilks’ Lambda was statistically significant for both predicting variables (p<0.001), confirming their adequacy for the analysis. An Eigenvalue (= 2.220) explained 100% of the variance with a high canonical correlation of 0.830. The model indicated two groups (illusion vs non-illusion) and had a high sensitivity (91.7%) and specificity (100%). The discriminant analysis indicated that twenty-four participants experienced the illusion (21 Females, M = 22.15 yrs, SD = 4.81) and thirteen participants had minimal experience of the illusion (10 Females, M = 21.29 yrs, SD = 3.00). The groups did not differ by age (t(35) = 0.763, p = 0.505), nor gender (χ2 = 0.694, p = 0.405).

Physiological results

Heart rate and heart rate variability differences between synchronous and asynchronous conditions.

Across the entire group, over the two minute period of each condition, heart rate during synchronous rubber hand condition was shown (substantial evidence) to be equivalent to that observed in the asynchronous condition (t(36) = 0.088, p = 0.930, 95% CI [-1.84, 2.001], d = 0.299, BH(0,1) = 7.79). Heart rate variability (RMSSD) during synchronous and asynchronous conditions was also similar (anecdotal evidence; t(36) = 1.425, p = 0.163, 95% CI [-2.41, 0.95], d = 0.221, BH(0,1) = 2.97). Together, these results indicate that cardiac physiology ‘sympathetic’ heart rate and parasympathetic RMSSD did not differentiate the synchronous stroking condition, associated with the induction and experience of the rubber hand illusion from asynchronous ‘illusion free’ control condition.

Heart rate and heart rate variability differences between participants who experienced the illusion and those who did not.

For the synchronous condition, we observed no differences in heart rate between participants who did and did not experience the rubber hand illusion (anecdotal evidence; t(34) = -1.423, p = 0.164, 95% CI [-12.86, 1.79], d = 5.18, BH(0,1) = 1.70]. The same was true for heart rate variability (substantial evidence; t(34) = -0.195, p = 0.846, 95% CI [-21.2, 17.7], d = 0.067, BH(0,1) = 3.94). For the asynchronous condition, there were also no group differences in heart rate (anecdotal evidence; t(34) = -1.344, p = 0.188, 95% CI [-13.6, 0.255], d = 0.48, BH(0,1) = 1.80) nor heart rate variability (substantial evidence; t(34) = 0.786, p = 0.437, 95% CI [-9.88, 21.7], d = 0.28, BH(0,1) = 3.07). Together, these results indicate with moderate evidence that cardiac physiology, encompassing ‘sympathetic’ heart rate and parasympathetic RMSSD, did not differentiate between individuals who did and did not experience the rubber hand illusion.

Electrodermal activity differences between synchronous and asynchronous conditions.

We observed no significant difference between the synchronous and asynchronous conditions in the mean amplitude of skin conductance responses (substantial evidence; t(36) = -0.988, p = 0.330, 95% CI [-2.45, 12.92], d = 0.206, BH(0,1) = 4.88); nor frequency (substantial evidence; t(36) = -0.911, p = 0.369, 95% CI [-2.45, 12.92], d = 0.089, BH(0,1) = 5.235) over the two minute of both conditions. As with the cardiac measures, the data were collected over the two minutes during which the participant received regular somatosensory stimulation. f SCR events were infrequent (mean ± std synchronous condition 5.92 ± 7.3; asynchronous 6.65 ± 9.0).

Electrodermal activity differences between participants who experienced the illusion and those who did not.

For the synchronous condition, we observed no differences in mean amplitude of skin conductance responses (SCRs) between participants who did and did not experience the rubber hand illusion (very strong evidence; t(34) = -0.561, p = 0.578, 95% CI [-0.76, 0.051], d = 3.36, BH(0,1) = 13.50). The same was true for frequency of SCRs (substantial evidence; t(34) = -0.373, p = 0.711, 95% CI [-6.51 14.60], d = 0.13, BH(0,1) = 3.94). For the asynchronous condition, there were also no observed group differences in mean amplitude of SCRs (substantial evidence; t(34) = 0.60, p = 0.550, 95% CI [-0.047, 0.094], d = 0.22, BH(0,1) = 3.42), nor number of SCRs (substantial evidence t(34) = -0.21, p = 0.983, 95% CI [-6.17, 6.03], d = 0.01, BH(0,1) = 4.00). Together, these results indicate that sympathetic electrodermal activity (SCRs) did not differentiate the synchronous stroking condition, associated with the induction and experience of the rubber hand illusion from asynchronous ‘illusion free’ control condition. Moreover, SCRs did not differ between those individuals who experienced the illusion from those that did not.

SKNA differences between synchronous and asynchronous conditions.

We tested for aSKNA effects at a slightly higher temporal resolution anticipating greater sensitivity [30]. Time-windows of 30s seconds were used to investigate differences between synchronous and asynchronous conditions. Four time-windows for each condition (30s, 60s, 90s, and 120s) were compared. The results obtained are as follows: 30s, t(36) = -1.311, p = 0.198, 95% CI [-0.02, 0.004], d = 0.011, BH(0,1) = 3.441; 60s, t(36) = -0.741, p = 0.464, 95% CI [-0.02, 0.009], d = -0.07, BH(0,1) = 5.991; 90s, t(36) = -0.674, p = 0.505, 95% CI [-0.01, 0.007], d = 0.05, BH(0,1) = 6.272; 120s, t(36) = 0.053, p = 0.958, 95% CI [-0.01, 0.01], d = 0.01, BH(0,1) = 7.809. Therefore, these data provide strong evidence for H0, indicating that there are no differences in aSKNA between the two conditions (Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Average skin nerve activity (aSKNA) for synchronous and asynchronous conditions over each 30 s time-window expressed as means ± 1SE.

Following established methods [30], continuous recording of SNKA was processed to give average scores over the 30s for each synchronous (active–associated with rubber hand illusion) and asynchronous (control–not associated with the rubber hand illusion) stroking conditions of the task. No consistent differences between conditions were observed across participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237282.g004

SKNA differences between participants who experienced the illusion and those who did not.

The aSKNA differences between participants who got the illusion and those who did not as indicated by the discriminant factor analysis were analysed for each condition and each time window. For the synchronous condition, the results are as follows: 30s, t(35) = -0.366, p = 0.716, 95% CI [-0.59, 0.40], d = 0.13, BH(0,1) = 3.777; 60s, t(35) = -0.368, p = 0.715, 95% CI [-0.59, 0.41], d = -0.13, BH(0,1) = 3.775; 90s, t(35) = -0.394, p = 0.696, 95% CI [-0.60, 0.40], d = 0.14, BH(0,1) = 3.743; 120s, t(35) = -0.358, p = 0.722, 95% CI [-0.59, 0.41], d = 0.13, BH(0,1) = 3.787. Therefore, there was substantial evidence for H0, with Bayes factors higher than 3, indicating that there were no differences in aSKNA between the two groups in the synchronous condition.

For the asynchronous condition, the results are as follows: 30s, t(35) = -0.365, p = 0.717, 95% CI [-0.60, 0.42], d = 0.12, BH(0,1) = 3.779; 60s, t(35) = -0.381, p = 0.705, 95% CI [-0.61, 0.41], d = 0.13, BH(0,1) = 3.759; 90s, t(35) = -0.393, p = 0.697, 95% CI [-0.61, 0.42], d = 0.14, BH(0,1) = 3.745; 120s, t(35) = -0.381, p = 0.705, 95% CI [-0.61, 0.42], d = 0.13, BH(0,1) = 3.759. Therefore, there was substantial evidence for H0, with Bayes factors higher than 3, indicating that there were no differences in aSKNA between the two groups in the asynchronous condition (Fig 5).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Average skin nerve activity (aSKNA) in participants who were classified has having a strong versus minimal experience of the rubber hand illusion means ± 1SE.

Following the methods of Doytchinova and colleagues [3032], continuous recording of SNKA was processed to give average scores over the 30s for each; A: Synchronous (active–associated with rubber hand illusion) and; B: Asynchronous (control–not associated with the rubber hand illusion) stroking conditions of the task. No differences between individual who did and did not experience the illusion were observed for either the active or control condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237282.g005

Correlations across physiological measures.

We computed correlations across autonomic measures, heart rate, heart rate variability, and skin conductance responses, and aSKNA for both synchronous and asynchronous conditions (Table 3). Overall, these revealed little evidence between synchronous and asynchronous conditions (hence rubber hand illusion) for a systematic shift in the relationship between physiological variables that could represent a difference in patterning of bodily control. In particularly, we tested explicitly for a changing relationship between heart rate and heart rate variability. Typically, heart rate increases in heart rate are balanced by heart rate variability, reflecting baroreflex activity. In stress/arousal states suppression of the baroreflex may change this relationship by inhibiting cardiovagal tone, decreasing heart rate variability and allowing heart rate and blood pressure to rise unchecked. We observed that during the asynchronous (control) condition heart rate was significantly negatively correlated with heart rate variability (R(35) = -0.593; p = 0.000, 95CI [-0.769, -0.333] B = 0.005). In the sychronous condition, associated with experience of the rubber hand illusion, this relationship reduced in strength (R(35) -0.317, p = 0.056, 95CI [-0.581, 0.008] B = 1.278). However, the test of the interaction, i.e. differences between the HR-HRV correlation for synchronous versus asynchronous conditions, did to reach significance Z = -1.460, p = 0.144, indicating a lack of compelling evidence across participants of a change in cardiac as a consequence of the rubber hand illusion induction (or effect).

thumbnail
Table 3. Correlations between Heart rate (HR), heart rate variability (HRV*), skin conductance responses (SCR) and aSKNA for synchronous (Syn) and asynchronous (Asyn) experimental conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237282.t003

Interoceptive accuracy

To test whether individual differences in interoceptive sensitivity influenced the experience of the rubber hand illusions in this sample [5], first a median split was used to divide participants into high interoceptive accuracy and low interoceptive accuracy groups [35]. We then tested for group differences between in strength of the rubber hand illusion. During synchronous proprioceptive drift, there was substantial evidence for no observable difference between high and low interceptive accuracy groups (t(32) = -0.734, p = 0.468, 95% CI [-15.48, 32.94], d = 0.25, BH(0,1) = 3.183). Similarly, there was substantial evidence for no difference for asynchronous proprioceptive drift, arguably a control for ‘suggestibility’ [33] (t(32) = 0.466, p = 0.644, 95% CI [-12.56, 20.02], d = 0.16, BH(0,1) = 3.656).

Regarding subjective ratings in the synchronous condition, there were no differences on any of the subscales; ownership (t(32) = 1.132, p = 0.266, BH(0,1) = 2.323); location (t(32) = -0.344, p = 0.733, BH(0,1) = 3.816); agency (t(32) = -0.225, p = 0.824, BH(0,1) = 3.929). Regarding subjective ratings in the asynchronous condition, there were no differences on any of the subscales: ownership (t(32) = 0.983, p = 0.334, BH(0,1) = 2.252); location (t(32) = -0.249, p = 0.805, BH(0,1) = 3.910); agency (t(32) = -0.239, p = 0.813, BH(0,1) = 3.918). Again, for both synchronous and asynchronous conditions, Bayes factors showed the evidence for no interoceptive group effects to be substantial for location and agency while the measures for ownership was anecdotal [40].

Correlation analyses were run between measures of proprioceptive drift and interoceptive accuracy and awareness scores. There was substantial evidence for no correlation between interoceptive accuracy and synchronous drift (r = -0.277, p = 0.197, BH(0, 1) = 3.284), asynchronous drift (r = -0.138, p = 0.436, BH(0, 1) = 5.561), nor the difference between synchronous and asynchronous drift (r = -0.107, p = 0.547, BH(0, 1) = 6.273). The same trend was observed for interoceptive awareness and synchronous drift (r = -0.064, p = 0.721, BH(0, 1) = 7.050), asynchronous drift (r = -0.097, p = 0.585, BH(0, 1) = 6.479), and the difference between synchronous and asynchronous drift (r = -0.001, p = 0.996, BH(0, 1) = 7.512). Together, these results indicate no significant difference between low- and high-interoceptive perceivers in the strength of the illusion as indicated by proprioceptive drift and/or subjective ratings.

Discussion

We set out to test whether the induction and experience of the rubber hand illusion is associated with reliable embodied changes in peripheral systemic (rather than localised) autonomic function, linked theoretically to the notion of interoceptive predictive coding as a basis to the integrity of conscious self-representation. The initial ambition was to extract, from measures of peripheral physiology, a change in efferent autonomic drive [610,19,25,31,41]. This, we hypothesised, might encode a change in interoceptive prediction as a signature of the shift in the conscious experience of bodily self when an artificial limb is adopted as part of one’s own body [1117]. Our study incorporated two important features: first, the use of SKNA, a novel approach to record non-invasively from sympathetic nerves [3032] and; second, the use of Bayes factors to determine strength of evidence for and against the null hypothesis [40]. In our findings, we produced robust evidence for successful induction of the rubber hand illusion, yet we observed no systematic changes across cardiac and electrodermal autonomic measures during the induction procedure (synchronous stroking) relative to a control procedure (asynchronous stroking). Moreover, when participants were partitioned by a discriminant analysis according to whether or not the illusory experience was strong, autonomic measures in either condition did not distinguish between these two groups. We also did not replicate a previously reported association between sensitivity to internal bodily signals (as indexed heuristically by performance accuracy on a heartbeat tracking task) and lower susceptibility to the rubber hand illusion [5].

Our results, in keeping with previously work, showed a partial correlation between proprioceptive drift and subjective ratings, re-confirming previous evidence to suggest that proprioceptive drift and subjective ratings are two independent measures of the rubber hand illusion [37,42,43], but see also [36]. Furthermore, these measures can reflect different dimensions of bodily ownership: body-location (linked to proprioceptive drift) and body-ownership (linked to the subjective experience of ownership and/or agency) [44]. Our overarching hypothesis predicted that the subjective representation of bodily self and agency might have the closest link to the regulation in internal interoceptive state.

Retrospectively, seeking a bodily signature of the rubber hand illusion was ambitious. We pursued more systemic autonomic measures, expressed thoughout the body (cardiovascular, electrodermal and SKNA responses), rather than more limb-specific change. Previous reports of hand cooling associated with rubber hand illusion found this to be localised to the specific side of the illusion [19].

We measured heart rate and heart rate variability, both indices of central autonomic cardiac control with systemic impact, capturing the interaction of sympathetic and parasympathetic (vagus nerve) cardiac innervation [38,39]. Heart rate itself is often used as a proxy for sympathetic cardiac influences; heart rate typically increases with decreasing heart rate variability as changes in sympathovagal balance produce cardiovascular arousal. Heart rate variability in particularly is a widely used measure linked to health, well-being and cognitive flexibility, where decreases in heart rate variability accompany stress, anxiety and negative affective experience [45,46]. These decreases typically relate to a suppression of the baroreflex by top-down brain signals, enabling heart rate and blood pressure to rise together to meet behavioural allostatic demands. However, within our data, we did not find any clear association with the experience of illusory limb ownership, suggesting cardiovascular homeostasis need not adapt to this change in body representation.

Our findings of no sympathetic electrodermal or SKNA changes associated with the rubber hand illusion contrast with recent observations that differences in skin conductance occur during the rubber hand illusion [25]. Yet even in this study, transient effects were observed, suggesting the novelty of the experience was a contributing factor. Increases in phasic and tonic electrodermal activity were also reported in an earlier study, associated with the period leading up to the onset of the illusory experience of body ownership, and amplified in people who report high levels of anomalous bodily experiences [41]. Both these studies suggest that autonomic changes measurable electrodermally, if they occur, may be brief, and mostly related to the cognitive and emotional and somatosensory experience of the induction process.

Those rubber hand illusion studies that report a fall in skin temperature in the ‘replaced hand’ have been influential and widely cited as evidence for a deep physical embodiment [19]. Temperature after a 7–8 min stroking period is reported as lower during synchronous compared to asynchronous stroking on the test hand. However, no temperature difference is reported on the non-stimulated hand. This observation suggests that hand cooling might relate to the illusionary disowning of the real hand in favour of the rubber substitute. Alternative explanations include greater immobility of the target hand (during synchronous stroking) and/or the illusion) features of the synchronous stoking that might better elicit peripheral e.g. axo-axonal reflex hypo-perfusion. Social factors may also play a role [22]. Mechanisms for observed hand cooling are relevant to other observations: While passive disowning of the replaced hand most likely is accompanied by reduced autonomic nervous drive to the limb, the increase in electrodermal activity occasionally reported with the rubber hand illusion is also consistent with activation of sympathetic vasoconstrictor neurons within skin vasculature, which can actively reduce skin temperature [47]. However, the effect appear unreliable, with studies not consistently observing cooling associated with the rubber hand illusion [21]. In our study, there was strong evidence for no effect of the procedure or the experience of the illusion on either electrodermal activity in the hand or skin sympathetic activity measured from the chest surface. Nevertheless, our two-minute induction process was relatively short and it remains possible that transient changes at the onset of the illusion were not sustained long enough to affect the average skin conductance data. For SKNA measures, although we analysed these reportedly sensitive response data over shorter (30 s) time-periods over the course of each experimental condition, we nevertheless identified no effect to suggest a transient response.

Sympathetic nerve traffic can be quantified using microneurography [29,4850], in which microelectrodes are inserted into peripheral nerves (median or common peroneal) to record from sympathetic axon bundles [29,4852]. The relatively recent introduction of non-invasive skin sympathetic nerve activity (SKNA) recordings provides a means of overcoming some of the logistical limitations of invasive microneurography [3032,51]. Subcutaneous nerve activity recorded from the chest wall of dogs correlates with nerve activity within stellate ganglion and corresponding sympathetic nervous effects on the heart [52]. This SKNA is reportedly a more sensitive index of cardiac sympathetic tone than heart rate variability derived measures [53]. Validation studies in humans have established the sensitivity of SKNA recordings to autonomic challenges in healthy individuals, to lidocaine deactivation of stellate ganglion, to vagus nerve stimulation, and to pathological changes in cardiac rhythm [30,32,51]. As a measure of multiunit sympathetic activity relevant to cardiovascular control, the research potential of this approach is exciting. Innovatively, our experimental study applied published methods for recording SKNA [30,32] to test for efferent neural signatures of central changes in bodily self-representation. Contrary to our predictions however, we report no differences in this purportedly sensitive measure between the active (synchronous stroking) and control (asynchronous stroking) conditions of the rubber hand illusion, whether or not a strong experience of change in embodied representation occurred.

Here we examined the rubber hand illusion, rather than the arguably more encompassing experience of a full body illusion, typically achieved using virtual reality head mounted displays [18,54,55]. The full body illusion affects the whole individual and the strength of body ownership can be measured through whole body spatial drift (i.e. shift in perceived location) as well as through subjective ratings. In comparison, the local modulation of body ownership with induction of the rubber hand illusion represents a limitation of the present study. Where tonic changes in physiological state are reported in association with the rubber hand illusion (e.g. temperature drop), such changes seem only to occur in the disowned arm [19,20]. In contrast, some studies of the full body illusion report widespread reductions in body temperature [54]. Moreover, visual feedback of ‘systemic’ heartbeat rhythms feedback will enhance the experience of full body illusions [18,55,56] (and the rubber hand illusion [17]). However, and perhaps contrary to what we may have predicted, studies of the full body illusion have failed to demonstrate significant tonic changes in autonomic state including heart rate and heart rate variability [18], and skin conductance level SCL [57]. This was even the case for studies in which cardiovisual synchrony enhanced the experience of the full body illusion [18,56], even though cardiac autonomic entrainment might facilitate the illusory experience. However, as with the rubber hand illusion [6,8], phasic electrodermal response (SCR) evoked by contact and reaction to pain are affected by illusory body ownership [57,58]. Such altered autonomic reactivity (even in the absence of a tonic autonomic change) nevertheless suggests that the full body illusion is accompanied by a resetting of central physiological regulatory sensitivity. One study of the full body illusion [59] observed changes in electrocortical rhythms during illusory self-location, notably differences in alpha power over sensorimotor and precentral cortical regions: While both brain alpha oscillations and autonomic (electrodermal or cardiac) measures are both modulated by attention and cognitive arousal, there is no simple interdependence between these central and peripheral measures. However, the identified cortical brain regions are known sources of autonomic drive (e.g. in motor central command) [60], suggesting that the electrocortical change may affect the regulation and reactivity of bodily physiology (even interoceptive prediction) in context of the whole body illusion. It remains unclear if the experience of the full body illusion would be better reflected in direct measures of sympathetic nerve traffic (SKNA).

One interesting challenge within the field of body-brain Interaction (including illusions of body ownership, interoception and autonomic psychophysiology) is the pursuit of objective measures, unbiased by situational and individual confounds such implicit task demands, response bias, and suggestibility. Tests of interoceptive sensitivity, especially the heartbeat tracking task, have been criticised for not providing an unconfounded measure of sensory perception; individual differences in performance are subject to a number of non-sensory factors including knowledge of one’s heart rate practice effects and other top down beliefs [61,62]. These widely acknowledged associations are of lesser or greater relevance depending on what question the research study aims to address and, correspondingly, how the data are interpreted [62]. Relevant to this study, individual differences in social compliance, suggestibility and/or neuroticism are recognized determinants of behaviours relating to ‘strength of self-representation’. While clinic drug trials are ‘blinded’ to manage the unwanted influence of expectancies and biases, suggestibility (including hypnotizability/phenomenological control) influence many tasks designed to access conscious processes, including the rubber hand illusion [63]. Individual differences in suggestibility can account for around 10 percent of the variance in subjective measures of the rubber hand illusion [63]. Such observations indicate that superordinate domain-general beliefs and expectancies influence perceptual behavioural and experiential measures. Moreover, they highlight the importance of participant-level susceptibility and experiment-level context in shaping subjective experience. In this study, we did not assess if suggestibility influenced autonomic responses to the induction or experience of the rubber hand illusion, beyond using the established asynchronous stroking as a standard control condition. Our autonomic data showed a lack of difference between synchronous and asynchronous conditions and between participants who did and did not experience the illusion. Our findings do not test whether suggestion affects autonomic correlates of the rubber hand illusion (as is observed with visual imagery [64]). Moreover, we also did not observe effects associated with individual differences in performance of a heartbeat counting task, a heuristic measure of interoceptive perception that is recognised to be sensitive to top-down expectancies and bias [61,62]. Increased performance on this task is linked to enhanced autonomic reactivity and to resistance to rubber hand illusion [5]. Although this latter observation has not always been replicated [23,24], it remains somewhat paradoxical that suggestibility facilitates both heartbeat tracking performance measures and experience of the rubber hand illusion.

In conclusion, we undertook detailed physiological monitoring of participants during the rubber hand illusion. We found no systematic effect on state measures of autonomic activity that might discriminate between the induction procedure, or the experience of owning an illusory body part when compared to a control condition. We obtained, for the first time in this context, SKNA recordings, indexing skin sympathetic nerve activity proximally related to stellate ganglion activity. We used Bayes factors to confirm the absence of autonomic differences. Our study illustrates that tonic changes in bodily physiology are not obligatory accompaniments of an alteration in this type of conscious self-representation.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for the advice and help of Dr David Watson in setting up the data acquisition platform, and to Prof Peter Taggart for highlighting and advising on the SKNA methodology.

References

  1. 1. Blanke O. Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2012; 13: 556–571. pmid:22805909
  2. 2. Gallagher S. Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2000; 4: 14–21. pmid:10637618
  3. 3. van den Bos E & Jeannerod M. Sense of body and sense of action both contribute to self-recognition. Cognition 2002; 85: 177–187. pmid:12127698
  4. 4. Botvinick M & Cohen J. Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature 1998; 391: 756. pmid:9486643
  5. 5. Tsakiris M, Tajadura-Jiménez A, & Costantini M. Just a heartbeat away from one’s body: Interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representations. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 2011; 278: 2470–2476. pmid:21208964
  6. 6. Armel KC & Ramachandran VS. Projecting sensations to external objects: Evidence from skin conductance response. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 2003; 270; 1499–1506. pmid:12965016
  7. 7. Kilteni K, Normand J-M, Sanchez-Vives MV, Slater M. Extending body space in immersive virtual reality: A very long arm illusion. PLoS One 2012; 7: e40867. pmid:22829891
  8. 8. Ehrsson HH, Wiech K, Weiskopf N, Dolan RJ, Passingham RE. Threatening a rubber hand that you feel is yours elicits a cortical anxiety response. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104: 9828–33. pmid:17517605
  9. 9. Ma K, Hommel B. The virtual-hand illusion: effects of impact and threat on perceived ownership and affective resonance Front Psychol. 2013; 4:, 604. pmid:24046762
  10. 10. Johnson MI, Smith E, Yellow S, Mulvey MR. A preliminary investigation into psychophysiological effects of threatening a perceptually embodied rubber hand in healthy human participants. Scand J Pain. 2016; 11: 1–8. pmid:28850446
  11. 11. Damasio AR. The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. 1999.
  12. 12. Craig AD. How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2009; 10: 59–70. pmid:19096369
  13. 13. Seth A, Tsakiris M. Being a Beast Machine: The Somatic Basis of Selfhood Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2018; 22: 969–981. pmid:30224233
  14. 14. Barrett LF, Simmons WK. Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2015; 16: 419–29. pmid:26016744
  15. 15. Sierra M, Senior C, Dalton J, McDonough M, Bond A, Phillips ML, et al. Autonomic response in depersonalization disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002; 59: 833–8. pmid:12215083
  16. 16. Koreki A, Garfkinel SN, Mula M, Agrawal N, Cope S, Eilon T, et al. Trait and state interoceptive abnormalities are associated with dissociation and seizure frequency in patients with functional seizures. Epilepsia 2020; 61: 1156–1165. pmid:32501547
  17. 17. Suzuki K, Garfinkel SN, Critchley HD & Seth AK. Multisensory integration across exteroceptive and interoceptive domains modulates self-experience in the rubber-hand illusion. Neuropsychologia 2013; 51: 2909–2917. pmid:23993906
  18. 18. Aspell JE, Heydrich L, Marillier G, Lavanchy T, Herbelin B, Blanke O. Turning body and self inside out: visualized heartbeats alter bodily self-consciousness and tactile perception. Psychol Sci 2013; 24: 2445–53. pmid:24104506
  19. 19. Moseley GL, Olthof N, Venema A, Don S, Wijers M, Gallace A, et al. Psychologically induced cooling of a specific body part caused by the illusory ownership of an artificial counterpart. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A 2008; 105: 13169–13173. pmid:18725630
  20. 20. Kammers MPM, Rose K, Haggard P. Feeling numb: temperature, but not thermal pain, modulates feeling of body ownership. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49: 1316–1321. pmid:21354190
  21. 21. de Haan AM, Van Stralen HE, Smit M, Keizer A, Van der Stigchel S, & Dijkerman HC. No consistent cooling of the real hand in the rubber hand illusion. Acta Psychologica 2017; 179: 68–77. pmid:28735225
  22. 22. Rohde M, Wold A, Karnath HO, Ernst MO. The human touch: skin temperature during the rubber hand illusion in manual and automated stroking procedures. PLoS One 2013; 8: e80688. pmid:24260454
  23. 23. Crucianelli L, Krahé C, Jenkinson PM, Fotopoulou AK. Interoceptive ingredients of body ownership: Affective touch. Cortex 2018; 104: 180–192. pmid:28532579
  24. 24. Horváth A, Ferentzi E, Bogdány T, Szolcsányi T, Witthöft M, Köteles F. Proprioception but not cardiac interoception is related to the rubber hand illusion Cortex 2020; 132: 361–373 pmid:33031978
  25. 25. D’Alonzo M, Mioli A, Formica D, Di Pino G. Modulation of Body Representation Impacts on Efferent Autonomic Activity. J Cogn Neurosci. 2020; 32; 1104–1116. pmid:31951156
  26. 26. Braithwaite J, Watson DG, Jones R, Rowe M. A guide for analysing electrodermal activity (EDA) & skin conductance responses (SCRs) for psychological experiments. Psychophysiology 2013; 49:1017–1034.
  27. 27. Boucsein W. Electrodermal activity (2nd ed.). Springer Science + Business Media. 2012 https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-1126-0.
  28. 28. Critchley HD, Elliott R, Mathias CJ, Dolan RJ. Neural activity relating to generation and representation of galvanic skin conductance responses: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 2000; 20: 3033–40. pmid:10751455
  29. 29. Wallin BG, Donadio V, Karlsson T, Kallio M, Nordin M, Elam M. Arousal increases baroreflex inhibition of muscle sympathetic activity. Acta Physiol Scand 2003; 177: 291–8. pmid:12608999
  30. 30. Doytchinova A, Hassel JL, Yuan Y, Lin H, Yin D, Adams D, et al. Simultaneous noninvasive recording of skin sympathetic nerve activity and electrocardiogram. Heart Rhythm 2017; 14: 25–33. pmid:27670627
  31. 31. Everett TH 4th, Doytchinova A, Cha YM, Chen PS. Recording sympathetic nerve activity from the skin. Trends Cardiovasc Med 2017; 27:463–472. pmid:28619579
  32. 32. Kusayama T, Wong J, Liu X, He W, Doytchinova A, Robinson EA, et al. Simultaneous noninvasive recording of electrocardiogram and skin sympathetic nerve activity (neuECG). Nat Protoc 2020; 15: 1853–1877. pmid:32313253
  33. 33. Longo MR, Schüür F, Kammers MP, Tsakiris M & Haggard P. What is embodiment? A psychometric approach. Cognition 2008; 107: 978–99833. pmid:18262508
  34. 34. Schandry R. Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology 1981; 18, 483–488. pmid:7267933
  35. 35. Garfinkel SN, Seth AK, Barrett AB, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biological Psychology 2015; 104: 65–74. pmid:25451381
  36. 36. Tsakiris M, Haggard P. The rubber hand illusion revisited: Visuotactile integration and self-attribution Journal of Experimental Psychology 2005; 31: 80–91. pmid:15709864
  37. 37. Rohde M, Di Luca M, Ernst MO, The rubber hand illusion: Feeling of ownership and proprioceptive drift do not go hand in hand. PLoS ONE, 2011; 6: e21659. pmid:21738756
  38. 38. Fedotov AA. Selection of parameters of bandpass filtering of the ECG signal for heart rhythm monitoring systems. Biomedical Engineering 2016; 50: 114–118.
  39. 39. Shaffer F & Ginsberg JP. An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Frontiers in Public Health 2017; 5: 258. pmid:29034226
  40. 40. Keysers C, Gazzola V, Wagenmakers EJ. Using Bayes factor hypothesis testing in neuroscience to establish evidence of absence. Nat Neurosci. 2020; 23: 788–799. pmid:32601411
  41. 41. Braithwaite JJ, Broglia E, Watson DG. Autonomic emotional responses to the induction of the rubber-hand illusion in those that report anomalous bodily experiences: Evidence for specific psychophysiological components associated with illusory body representations. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2014; 40: 1131–45. pmid:24635201
  42. 42. Carruthers G. Toward a cognitive model of the sense of embodiment in a (rubber) hand. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2013; 20: 33–60.
  43. 43. Riemer M, Bublatzky F, Trojan J, & Alpers GW. Defensive activation during the rubber hand illusion: Ownership versus proprioceptive drift. Biological Psychology, 2015; 109: 86–92. pmid:25960069
  44. 44. Serino A, Alsmith A, Costantini M, Mandrigin A, Tajadura-Jimenez A, & Lopez C. Bodily ownership and self-location: components of bodily self-consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 2013; 22: 1239–1252. pmid:24025475
  45. 45. Drury RL, Porges S, Thayer J, Ginsberg JP. Heart Rate Variability, Health and Well-Being: A Systems Perspective. Front Public Health 2019. 7:323. pmid:31750285
  46. 46. Mulcahy JS, Larsson DEO, Garfinkel SN, Critchley HD. Heart rate variability as a biomarker in health and affective disorders: A perspective on neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage 2019. 202:116072. pmid:31386920
  47. 47. Bini G, Hagbarth KE, Hynninen P, Wallin BG Thermoregulatory and rhythm-generating mechanisms governing the sudomotor and vasoconstrictor outflow in human cutaneous nerves J Physiol (Lond) 1980; 306: 537–552. pmid:7463376
  48. 48. Sverrisdóttir YB, Schultz T, Omerovic E, Elam M. Sympathetic nerve activity in stress-induced cardiomyopathy. Clin Auton Res 2012; 22: 259–64. pmid:22492095
  49. 49. Vallbo ÅB. Microneurography: how it started and how it works. J Neurophysiol. 2018; 120: 1415–1427. pmid:29924706
  50. 50. Macefield VG. Recording and quantifying sympathetic outflow to muscle and skin in humans: methods, caveats and challenges. Clin Auton Res. 2020; pmid:32588247
  51. 51. Kusayama T, Wan J, Doytchinova A, Wong J, Kabir RA, Mitscher G, et al. Skin sympathetic nerve activity and the temporal clustering of cardiac arrhythmias. JCI Insight 2019; 4: e125853. pmid:30811928
  52. 52. Jiang Z, Zhao Y, Doytchinova A, Kamp NJ, Tsai WC, Yuan Y, et al. (Using skin sympathetic nerve activity to estimate stellate ganglion nerve activity in dogs. Heart Rhythm 2015; 12:1324–32. pmid:25681792
  53. 53. Chan YH, Tsai WC, Shen C, Han S, Chen LS, Lin SF, et al. Subcutaneous nerve activity is more accurate than heart rate variability in estimating cardiac sympathetic tone in ambulatory dogs with myocardial infarction. Heart Rhythm 2015; 12: 1619–27. pmid:25778433
  54. 54. Salomon R, Lim M, Pfeiffer C, Gassert R, & Blanke O. Full body illusion is associated with widespread skin temperature reduction. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 2013; 7: 65. pmid:23898244
  55. 55. Heydrich L, Aspell JE, Marillier G, Lavanchy T, Herbelin B, Blanke O. Cardio-visual full body illusion alters bodily self-consciousness and tactile processing in somatosensory cortex. Sci Rep. 2018; 8: 9230. pmid:29915337
  56. 56. Iodice P, Porciello G, Bufalari I, Barca L, & Pezzulo G. An interoceptive illusion of effort induced by false heart-rate feedback. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2019; 116: 13897–13902. pmid:31235576
  57. 57. Romano D, Pfeiffer C, Maravita A, Blanke O. Illusory self-identification with an avatar reduces arousal responses to painful stimuli. Behav Brain Res. 2014; 261: 275–81. pmid:24412686
  58. 58. Petkova VI, Ehrsson HH. If I were you: perceptual illusion of body swapping. PLoS One. 2008; 3: e383.
  59. 59. Lenggenhager B, Halje P, Blanke O. Alpha band oscillations correlate with illusory self-location induced by virtual reality. Eur J Neurosci. 2011; 33: 1935–43. pmid:21395867
  60. 60. Thornton JM, Guz A, Murphy K, Griffith AR, Pedersen DL, Kardos A, et al. Identification of higher brain centres that may encode the cardiorespiratory response to exercise in humans. J Physiol. 2001; 533: 823–36. pmid:11410638
  61. 61. Ring C, Brener J. Heartbeat counting is unrelated to heartbeat detection: A comparison of methods to quantify interoception. Psychophysiology 2018; e13084. pmid:29633292
  62. 62. Corneille O, Desmedt O, Zamariola G, Luminet O, Maurage P. A heartfelt response to Zimprich et al. (2020), and Ainley et al. (2020)’s commentaries: Acknowledging issues with the HCT would benefit interoception research. Biological psychology 2020; 152: 107869. pmid:32061686
  63. 63. Lush P, Botan V, Scott RB, Ward J, Dienes Z. Phenomenological control: response to imaginative suggestion predicts measures of mirror touch synaesthesia, vicarious pain and the rubber hand illusion Nature Comms 2020 in press [2019. psyarxiv.com].
  64. 64. Wang Y, Morgan WP. The effect of imagery perspectives on the psychophysiological responses to imagined exercise. Behav Brain Res. 1992; 52: 167–74. pmid:1294196