Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Different aspects of impulsivity in chronic alcohol use disorder with and without comorbid problem gambling

  • Ildikó Kovács ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    kovacs.ildiko.1@med.u-szeged.hu

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Szeged, Szeged, Hungary

  • Ildikó Demeter,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Szeged, Szeged, Hungary

  • Zoltán Janka,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Szeged, Szeged, Hungary

  • Zsolt Demetrovics,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Psychology, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary

  • Aniko Maraz ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Aniko Maraz, Bálint Andó

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute für Psychologie, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany

  • Bálint Andó

    Contributed equally to this work with: Aniko Maraz, Bálint Andó

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Szeged, Szeged, Hungary

Abstract

Background and aims

Alcohol use disorder (AUD) and problem gambling are highly comorbid disorders. This study aims to explore the role of four aspects of impulsivity (trait concept of impulsivity, choice impulsivity, impulsive aggression and response inhibition/decision-making) in long-term chronic AUD patients with and without problem or pathological gambling symptoms.

Methods

Cognitively intact chronic AUD patients were enrolled with (n = 32) and without (n = 71) problem gambling symptoms in an inpatient clinic for chronic alcohol users. Multiple facets of impulsivity, cognitive ability, psychopathological symptoms, alcohol and gambling severity were measured.

Results

Chronic AUD patients with gambling disorder symptoms showed longer lifetime alcohol consumption, more severe alcohol use and higher psychopathological symptom severity than AUD patients without gambling symptoms. Gambling severity correlated with overall trait impulsivity, but not with choice impulsivity, impulsive aggression or cognitive impulsivity with controlling for lifetime alcohol consumption, lifetime alcohol use and psychopathological symptom severity. High trait impulsivity and non-planning was associated with comorbid gambling symptoms in AUD patients, which was independent of the level of intelligence, age and psychopathological symptoms.

Conclusion

Comorbid gambling disorder symptoms in chronic AUD was connected to more severe alcohol-related variables. Higher trait impulsivity was also linked with gambling disorder symptoms in patients with chronic AUD. This accents the need of special focus on comorbid GD symptoms in AUD, since prognosis and treatment for them may vary.

Introduction

Alcohol use disorder (AUD) is one of the most frequently occurring substance use disorder, and it exhibits exceptionally high lifetime prevalence rate (30.3%) [1]. Chronic AUD is highly comorbid with many systemic diseases and is often present simultaneously with other mental disorders [2], from which the high co-occurrence of gambling disorder (GD) and AUD is well established. Studies have shown that AUD and GD are highly comorbid both in community [3,4] and in treatment seeking samples [5,6]. In a nationally representative US study, almost three-quarters (73.2%) of lifetime GD patients had AUD [7], while a population-based meta-analysis reported that 28% of lifetime GD patients report AUD as well [8], and other studies reported a 17–33% comorbidity of current GD in AUD patients [9,10]. AUD and GD share common symptomatology and demonstrate common underlying genetic vulnerabilities [11], from which impulsivity is considered to be an endophenotypic indicator in both disorders [12,13]. Moreover, there is evidence that higher impulsivity may not only be present for the clinical presentation of GD, but may also be present in case of milder, subclinical problem gambling [3,14,15].

Impulsivity can be defined as a complex, multidimensional construct that is frequently described as the repetitive execution of maladaptive behaviours resulting in potential negative consequences. Hence, impulsive actions can be regarded as unplanned and rapid reactions to external or internal stimuli with the aim of obtaining immediate gratification and/or pleasure [12,16]. Contemporary models of impulsivity highlight the importance of not only behavioural aspects of impulsiveness, but also underlying neuropsychological components [1719].

Impulsivity show complex neurobiological underpinnings, where the neurobiological and behavioural relationships between AUD, GD and impulsivity have been well documented [20,21]. The neurotoxic effects of chronic administration of alcohol and prolonged gambling behaviour are known to cause neural loss in “top-down” control regions of the brain, in the prefrontal cortex, the orbitofrontal cortex, the superior frontal association cortex, the supraoptic and paraventricular nuclei of the hypothalamus and the cerebellum, which have critical role in response inhibition, affective processing and decision-making, thus in impulsivity [2224]. A diagnostic feature of both AUD and GD is the inability to abstain from the addictive behaviour even when facing severe negative consequences [2527]. Such a failure in controlling one’s actions indicates inadequate inhibitory control, in which patients are unable to suppress the undesired, maladaptive act of gambling or drinking behaviour. This impairment reflects on deteriorated inhibitory control and decision-making, which represent a form of impulsivity [27,28].

Early impulsivity is proven to be a predictor of heavy drinking and also gambling behaviour in later life [2931]. Moreover, there is evidence that the acute administration of alcohol and gambling activity induce impulsivity in humans [27,32]. This directly leads to impulsive behaviour evolving into compulsion that is linked to the development of the chronic forms of AUD and GD [33], where impulsivity has been frequently connected to poor clinical outcomes [34,35] and is associated with negative concomitant features like increased relapse risk [36] or increased alcohol consumption and more severe gambling behaviour [3739].

Extensive literature exists on either diagnosed AUD or GD and their relations to impulsivity compared with healthy control groups, but up to now, only few studies have examined and compared AUD and GD patients in the same study in terms of any facet of impulsivity. [40] compared the differences of cognitive impulsivity measured by 2 neuropsychological tasks of 21 non-treatment-seeking problem or pathological gamblers and 21 AUD outpatients with healthy controls. They found shared deficits in ventral prefrontal cortical functions, while in tasks loading on dorsolateral prefrontal cortical functions were only impaired in AUD patients, presumably as a consequence of long-term alcohol consumption. [41] examined the differences of 3 decision-making tasks in 48 GD and 46 AUD outpatients and found that AUD patients performed marginally worse than the GD group. [42] studied the differences of a self-reported and a cognitive impulsivity task of 75 AUD and 44 GD patients recruited from mixed inpatient and outpatient settings. They revealed similar patterns of impulsivity in AUD and GD patients. These studies examined single diagnosis of AUD or GD; however, we could not identify studies that examined the subclinical emergence of problem gambling in hospitalised chronic AUD patients and its relations to impulsivity utilizing a comprehensive assessment battery.

Since impulsivity is not only proven to be a diagnostic criterion and a risk factor for AUD and GD, but the long-term alcohol exposure and gambling behaviour might further result in the impairment of impulse control, which prompts the emergence of AUD and GD. Moreover, a recently published meta-analysis indicated that the impairment of some executive functions, particularly impulsive decision-making were even higher in patients with a non-substance-related addictive disorder (GD) than in patients with the substance-related condition of AUD [43]. Based on these, it is paramount to explore the different aspects of impulsivity and their presentation in long-term AUD patients, and whether the existence of comorbid GD symptoms differentiate them in terms of impulsivity.

Due to the multidimensional nature of impulsivity, we aimed to explore it from a complex point of view, incorporating both objective and subjective measures of impulsiveness: i) traditional trait concept of impulsivity, which represents the execution of nonplanned actions and the engagement in maladaptive behaviours with disregarding potential future consequences [44,45]; ii) the inability of delaying gratifications or discarding rewards as a measure of choice impulsivity [46]; iii) impulsive aggression [47]; iv) deficits in response inhibition and decision-making [48,49].

To our knowledge, this is the first study that explores the associations of different aspects of impulsivity in chronic AUD patients compared to chronic AUD patients with comorbid GD symptoms. We hypothesised that those AUD patients who exhibit comorbid GD symptoms are distinct from AUD patients without GD symptoms by expressing higher symptom severity of substance use and demonstrating higher levels of trait impulsivity, choice impulsivity, impulsive aggression and impulsive decision-making.

Methods

Procedure

A comprehensive research project was conducted at the Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Szeged, Hungary. Patients receiving inpatient treatment for chronic alcohol use disorder (AUD) were assessed for executive functions, personality traits, addiction characteristics, comorbid psychiatric conditions, addictive disorders and psychopathological symptom severity. In this part of the study, which focuses on the evaluation of objective and subjective measures of impulsivity, a total of 104 patients were enrolled. Patients who met the inclusion criteria of having an established DSM-5 diagnosis of AUD, who finished at least primary education and whose level of intelligence surpassed the level of intellectual disability (Fourth Edition of the Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale above 70) were included. Patients who had a history of any psychosis spectrum disorder, progressive neurodegenerative disorders, neurological diseases, diseases affecting their sight or reported acute alcohol abuse were excluded from this study. One recruited patient was excluded due to voluntary termination of inpatient treatment; thus, the final sample size was 103. Patients were classified into two groups based on the presence of comorbid GD symptoms (see Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. PRISMA flowchart of selection procedure.

DSM: The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition; AUD: alcohol use disorder; WAIS-IV: The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227645.g001

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Human Investigation Review Board, University of Szeged (ethical approval number: 49/B-53/2016KK). Written informed consent was obtained from each participant.

Measures

Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS).

The Hungarian version of the BIS-11 is based on the original English version [45], which contains 30 items measuring three dimensions of impulsivity: motor impulsivity, attentional impulsivity and nonplanning. Items are measured on a scale ranging from 0 to 3; higher scores indicate higher impulsivity. The Hungarian version adapted by [50] showed good reliability in our sample: Cronbach’s α = 0.805.

Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ).

The Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire is a widely-used self-evaluation test for measuring aggressive impulses in 4 facets: physical and verbal aggression, hostility and anger [51]. The Hungarian version was adapted by [52], and showed good internal consistency and reliability (in our sample: Cronbach’s α = 0.879).

Delay Discounting Task (DDT).

In the Delay Discounting Task participants are presented with a series of monetary choices in which they have to decide between two different amounts of money, which they hypothetically receive in different time intervals. Once displayed, the rewards vary between 0HUF and 55,000HUF in 2,500HUF increments (1 EUR is about 320 HUF). The two choices differ in receival times, which vary in 0, 1, 14, 60, 180 or 365 days; the receival of one amount being closer in time (e.g. “now”) and the other being later (“in 2 weeks”). The task consists of 138 hypothetical questions that are presented in random order (i.e., Which of the two choices would you select? 10,000HUF now or 55,000HUF two weeks later?). Patients indicate their choices by a single click on their preferred selection, then they could continue the test by clicking on the ‘Next question’ button. The aim of the DDT is to determine the point at which the selection of the immediate reward was preferred over the delayed reward, which is known as the ‘indifference point’, and it can be computed for each period of delay. The participant selects the immediate available selection above this indifference point (which is given in HUF), while below the indifference point the participant chooses the option with delay.

Iowa Gambling Task (IGT).

The Iowa Gambling Task is one of the most frequently used and ecologically valid assessment tools for measuring decision-making, in which participants are asked to choose 100 times from four decks of cards with different win/loss ratio to win as much money as they can. For this, participants have to abandon short-term advantageous choices that would result in immediate high rewards accompanied with more/higher losses and instead, they need to select from decks, which result in lower immediate rewards, but lower long-term losses as well, turning out to be long-term advantageous choices [26,53]. For data analysis, choices were divided into five blocks (20 selections in each block). The amount of money won, and the total net score calculated by subtracting the number of advantageous selections from disadvantageous choices were also compared.

Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST).

The WCST measures cognitive flexibility, executive functions and decision-making [54]. In this neuropsychological task, participants need to sort cards according to various criteria with the only feedback of the classification being correct or incorrect. Four sample cards are presented differing in pattern, colour and number of patterns to which participants need to match newly appearing cards. Cards can be classified according to their colour, their patterns or the number of patterns on each card. The classification rule changes every ten cards, which implies that when the participant has figured out the classification rule, they would eventually start making mistakes when the rule changes, and consequently they need to adapt to the changing rules. In our study we used the PEBL’s computerised version of card sorting task [55,56]. For data analysis, the number of correct responses, the number of incorrect responses and the number of perseverative errors were calculated.

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV).

The WAIS-IV is the most widely used intelligence scale to measure cognitive ability. The test consists of 10 subtests and 5 supplementary tests, which measure 4 major components of intelligence: Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), Perceptual Reasoning Index (PRI), Working Memory Index (WMI) and Processing Speed Index (PSI) [57]. The Hungarian standardization was made by [58]. In this study, we calculated the WAIS-IV total score, which is the combined performance of the VCI, PRI, WMI and PSI.

Addiction Severity Index (ASI).

The ASI is a semi-structured interview, which covers 7 potential problematic areas (Medical, Employment/Support Status, Alcohol, Drug, Legal, Family/Social and Psychiatric) covering recent and lifetime substance-related problems [59]. The Hungarian adaptation was made by Rácz, Pogány & Máthé-Árvay (2002) [60]. From the interview, the following variables were selected: start of alcohol misuse in years, lifetime alcohol consumption in years, and abstinence during last 30 days.

South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS).

The South Oaks Gambling Screen is a 20-item questionnaire based on the DSM-III criteria of pathological gambling [61]. The Hungarian version was adapted by [62] and demonstrated good internal consistency. Scores between 1 and 4 show problematic gambling, while 5 or more points indicate probable pathological gambling.

Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT).

The AUDIT is a 10-item self-evaluation screening test for assessing the severity of alcohol use and its adverse consequences developed by the World Health Organization [63]. The Hungarian adaptation by Gerevich, Bácskai & Rózsa (2006) [64] exhibited good validity and reliability (in our sample: Cronbach’s α = 0.763).

Symptom Checklist-90-R.

The Symptom Checklist-90-R is a 90-item self-report questionnaire for assessing a broad range of currently existing psychopathological symptoms [65]. Items are rated on a 0–4 scale where more points indicate the presence of more severe symptoms during the past week. The test measures nine symptom dimensions, and one of its three global indices, the Global Severity Index (GSI) can be used as an indicator of the severity of psychopathological symptoms. The Hungarian version showed excellent internal consistency and reliability in our sample: Cronbach’s α = 0.953 [66,67].

Data analysis.

Data analysis was conducted with IBM SPSS Statistics 24.0 software [68]. Based on the results of the SOGS, we divided our sample into two groups: 1) AUD who scored 0 on the SOGS were categorised as AUD patients without gambling symptoms (AUD group) and 2) those patients who scored 1 or above were labelled as AUD patients with probable or problem gambling symptoms (AUD+Gambling group). The DDT value was computed in Microsoft Excel 2016 with a hyperbolic equation fitted for each participant’s indifference point [69,70] with the use of the Solver subroutine: where V stands for the value of the indifference point, Std stands for the amount of money available (55,000HUF), k is a fitted parameter which indexes the rate of discounting, and X represents the length of delay. The steepness of the curve (k) is fitted to the subjective value of each point of delay. When the curve is steeper (meaning that the k value is closer to zero), the individual prefers immediate rewards over delayed ones, which represents more impulsive choices.

Independent-samples t-tests were used for determining the group differences for continuous variables and Chi-square test was used for categorical variables to compare demographic parameters. Partial correlation analysis was used to reveal the relationship between gambling symptom severity and different facets of impulsivity. Comparison of the two groups’ performance on the IGT was determined by Repeated Measures ANOVA and Repeated Measures ANCOVA; due to the results of the Mauchly's Test of Sphericity, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied. To examine the effect of demographic variables, psychopathology symptoms and measures of impulsivity on the likelihood that patients have problem gambling symptoms, binary logistic regressions were used with forward stepwise regression method [71].

Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen’s d [72], which is defined as the standardized difference between two means. According to [73], an effect size of 0.2–0.3 is considered to be a “small” effect size, 0.5 is a “medium” effect and above 0.8 is a “large” effect size.

Results

Sample characteristics

More than two-third (76%, n = 79 out of 103) of the participants were male. The mean age of participants was 45.7 years (SD = 10.35; age: 21–69). In total, 78.7% of the sample completed secondary education and 21.4% completed graduate education. Close to one third (31.1%) of the sample (n = 32) scored 1 or more points on the SOGS with scores ranging between 1 and 14 points (mean = 4.69, SD = 3.5); 18 people categorised as problematic gamblers and 14 as probable pathological gamblers. The two groups did not differ in gender, age, education, IQ, start of alcohol consumption or abstinence during the last 30 days, but the AUD+Gambling group was characterised by more severe alcohol use and longer lifetime alcohol consumption (see Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic, alcohol and gambling related characteristics of the sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227645.t001

Exploratory correlation matrix and group differences between gambling symptom severity and demographic variables, psychopathology symptoms and measures of impulsivity

Partial correlation with age, lifetime alcohol consumption and SCL-90-R GSI as covariates were conducted to explore the associations between these variables, where the severity of gambling symptoms (SOGS scores) showed significant correlation with the BIS Total Score (r = 0.278, p = 0.006), while Impulsive aggression measured with the BPAQ Total score (r = 0.128, p = 0.209), and neuropsychological measures of impulsivity, as the number of correct responses in the WCST (r = -0.046, p = 0.658), the number of total errors in the WCST (r = 0.054, p = 0.602), the number of perseverative errors in the WCST (r = -0.068, p = 0.510), the DDT (r = -0.118, p = 0.254), the total win on the IGT (r = -0.005, p = 0.962), the number of advantageous choices on the IGT (r = 0.065, p = 0.529), the number of disadvantageous choices on the IGT (r = -0.065, p = 0.529) and the IGT net score (r = 0.065, p = 0.529) did not show significant connection with the severity of gambling symptoms (see S1 Annex).

Based on the associations explored in the correlation matrix, independent sample t-tests were conducted to explore group differences in the subscales of the BIS. Fig 2 illustrates that the AUD+Gambling group had higher scores in the BIS Nonplanning (t(100) = -3.024, p = 0.003, Cohen’s d = -0.634) and the BIS Total scores (t(100) = -2.635, p = 0.010, Cohen’s d = -0.555), and a tendency toward significance in the BIS Motor Impulsivity (t(100) = -1.767, p = 0.080, Cohen’s d = -0.371).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Group differences in Barratt Impulsivity Scale.

AUD: chronic alcohol use disorder patients; AUD+Gambling: alcohol use disorder patients with problem or pathological gambling symptoms; BIS Motor Imp.: Barratt Impulsivity Scale Motor Impulsivity subscale; BIS Cognitive Imp.: Barratt Impulsivity Scale Cognitive Impulsivity subscale; BIS Nonplanning: Barratt Impulsivity Scale Nonplanning subscale; BIS Total: Barratt Impulsivity Scale Total score; the columns represent mean values, the error bars standard deviation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227645.g002

The effect of demographic variables, psychopathology symptoms and measures of impulsivity on gambling symptoms

To explore the effect of demographic variables, psychopathology symptoms and measures of impulsivity on gambling symptoms, we performed two binary logistic regressions with forward stepwise regression method with AUD vs. AUD+Gambling as dependent variable. The first binary logistic regression was performed with age, gender, IQ measured by the WAIS-IV and SCL-90-R GSI as covariates. The BPAQ Total score, the BIS Total score, the number of correct responses in the WCST, the number of total errors in the WCST, the number of perseverative errors in the WCST, the DDT, the total win on the IGT, the number of advantageous choices on the IGT, the number of disadvantageous choices on the IGT and the IGT net score on the likelihood that patients have problem gambling symptoms were entered as predictors. Assumption of collinearity was tested and resulted in no indication of multicollinearity (Tolerance below 0.865 and VIF below 3.645 for every variable in the model). The baseline model (B = -0.895, S.E. = 0.220, Wald χ2(1) = 16.507, p ≤ 0.001, OR = 0.408) had an accuracy of 71.0% overall percentage. The binary logistic regression model was statistically significant (χ2(1) = 7.324, p = 0.007; R2 = 0.101; Hosmer-Lemeshow goodness-of-fit test: χ2(8) = 2.703, p = 0.958). Increasing BIS Total score was associated with the increased likelihood of having problem gambling symptoms (B = 0.057, S.E. = 0.022, Wald χ2(1) = 6.631, p = 0.010, OR = 1,059, 95% CI = 1.014–1.105) while all the other variables had a non-significant effect in the final model.

In the second binary logistic regression the BIS and BPAQ subscales were also included besides the total scores, namely: BIS Motor Impulsivity Score, BIS Cognitive Impulsivity Score, BIS Nonplanning Score, BPAQ Verbal Aggression Score, BPAQ Physical Aggression, BPAQ Hostility score and BPAQ Anger score. Additionally, the number of correct responses in the WCST, the number of total errors in the WCST, the number of perseverative errors in the WCST, the DDT, the total win on the IGT, the number of advantageous choices on the IGT, the number of disadvantageous choices on the IGT and the IGT net score were included as predictors with age, gender, IQ measured by the WAIS-IV and SCL-90-R GSI as covariates. Assumption of collinearity was tested and resulted in no indication of multicollinearity (Tolerance below 0.819 and VIF below 3.808 for every variable in the model). The baseline model (B = -0.895, S.E. = 0.220, Wald χ2(1) = 16.507, p ≤ 0.001, OR = 0.408) had an accuracy of 70.0% overall percentage. The binary logistic regression model was statistically significant (χ2(1) = 8.914, p = 0.003; R2 = 0.122; Hosmer-Lemeshow goodness-of-fit test: χ2(7) = 9.121, p = 0.244). Increasing BIS Nonplanning score was associated with the increased likelihood of having problem gambling symptoms (B = 0.143, S.E. = 0.051, Wald χ2(1) = 7.844, p = 0.005, OR = 1,154, 95% CI = 1.044–1.275), while all the other variables had a non-significant effect in the final model.

Discussion

Hospitalized patients with long-term chronic alcohol use disorder (AUD) with or without gambling disorder (GD) symptoms were compared on an extensive test battery that assessed four sub-dimensions of impulsivity. The traditional trait concept of impulsivity was measured by the Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS), which is the most commonly used internally consistent measure of trait impulsivity in clinical setting. The ability of postponing gratifications or delaying immediate rewards was assessed by the Delay Discounting Task (DDT), which is a widely accepted method for understanding impulsive choices. Impulsive aggression was examined with the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ), and deficits of decision-making and response inhibition, as measurements of cognitive impulsivity, were assessed with the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST).

In our sample, the concurrent AUD and problem or pathological gambling symptoms were associated with more severe alcohol use, longer lifetime alcohol consumption and higher levels of trait impulsivity measured by the BIS. When controlled for age, lifetime alcohol consumption and SCL-90-R GSI, GD symptom severity showed significant correlation with trait impulsivity, and only trait impulsivity was related to the increased likelihood of having GD symptoms. Overall, these findings were not due to the result of group differences in age, nor were they mediated by the measured differences in years of alcohol consumption or psychiatric symptom severity evaluated in this study.

Our results indicate that only higher trait impulsivity was associated with comorbid GD symptoms in chronic AUD patients, which was independent of intelligence, age, gender and psychopathological symptom severity. Moreover, the nonplanning aspect of trait impulsivity was associated with the occurrence of comorbid GD symptoms in chronic AUD. This link conforms to previous literature, since in functional imaging studies, the nonplanning dimension of impulsivity measured with the BIS correlated with volumes of the right middle cingulate gyrus, the left anterior cingulate gyrus, left middle frontal gyrus, left middle cingulate gyrus and right orbitofrontal gyrus [74]. Frontal lobe dysfunction is a leading symptom of the alcohol-related impairment in the brain that prolonged AUD causes [75], thus the risk of developing comorbid GD in chronic AUD is especially high.

In our sample of long-term chronic AUD patients, trait impulsivity proved to be a determinant factor, in which AUD patients with comorbid GD symptoms exhibited higher trait impulsivity than patients without them, and trait impulsivity was also associated with more severe substance use symptoms. There is mount evidence on the negative consequences of trait impulsivity in alcohol use and gambling as well, expressed both in clinical and subclinical forms. [76] examined the role of trait impulsivity measured with the BIS in excessive alcohol consumption and alcohol misuse and proved the role of trait impulsivity as a risk factor in alcohol misuse. Concerning diagnosed AUD, several epidemiological [77], cross-sectional [36,41] and longitudinal studies [78,79] thoroughly supported the maladaptive role of higher trait impulsivity in AUD patients. Similarly in GD, [80] compared non-problematic, at-risk and problem gamblers in terms of cognitive impulsivity and found that at-risk and problem gamblers also showed elevated BIS Motor and Attentional Impulsivity. Also in case of clinically diagnosed GD, several studies verified higher trait impulsivity compared to healthy controls [81,82].

Impulsive choice-making is a predominant feature both in AUD and GD. [83] showed a more rapid discounting of delayed rewards among AUD patients, while [25] indicated that GD symptom severity is associated with higher choice impulsivity in a delay discounting task. [84] examined substance abusers with and without problem gambling and indicated that substance abusers with GD symptoms discounted delayed rewards more rapidly than their patients without GD symptoms. Even though it has been previously verified that GD had an additive effect on delay discounting rates, in our study delay discounting was not associated with GD symptoms nor did it increase the likelihood of having GD symptoms in AUD patients. A reason for that may lie in the fact that patients suffering from addictive disorders with or without GD symptoms are documented to discount delayed rewards more rapidly than healthy controls, thus addictive disorders themselves are associated with higher discounting rates [85,86].

Previous studies have explored that poor response inhibition—that may be related to impulsive aggression—is affected by acute alcohol consumption [8789] and chronic alcohol abuse and dependence as well [90,91]. The control of response inhibition is mediated by prefrontal/orbito-frontal and limbic/thalamic cortical circuits, and the impairment of these interconnected circuitries may induce excessive aggressive responding, thus impulsive aggression [92,93]. In our study, we measured impulsive aggression with the BPAQ, which is a widely used self-evaluation test in clinical setting for assessing trait aggression. In our sample, BPAQ total scores correlated with gambling symptom severity, which conform to current scientific results. Similarly, growing number of studies have reported a link between gambling and aggressive behaviour [94]. While in a longitudinal study of males tested at age 12, 15 and 18, [95] found that early aggressive behaviour measured with the BPAQ leads to the increase of alcohol consumption, but they did not find that alcohol use have led to later aggressive behaviour. Similarly, the presence of aggressive impulses has been well-documented in clinical populations of AUD and GD as well [96,97].

Risky decision-making and response inhibition as forms of impulsivity have been hypothesised to play a central role in addictive processes. Gambling tasks with risks and rewards like the IGT and tasks measuring executive (frontal lobe) functions like the WCST have been widely used to assess decision-making capacities and deficits in response inhibition in individuals with AUD and GD. Compared to healthy controls, AUD and GD patients both exhibit deficits on the IGT, meaning that they more frequently choose the larger immediate reward despite the presence of a larger concomitant punishment [98101]. Similarly, it has also been documented that in case of chronic long-term alcohol consumption the performance on executive function tests like the WCST deteriorated [102,103]; also a previous study indicated that GD patients had more perseverative errors on the WCST, which is another measure of cognitive flexibility [104]. Our results do not conform to these previous findings, since in our sample we could not identify differences on the IGT or the WCST between chronic AUD patients with or without GD symptoms, nor did these test results contribute to the likelihood of having GD symptoms. The reason behind the lack of difference between long-term AUD and AUD with comorbid GD symptoms might be reasoned with the effects of chronic alcohol consumption in those cortical regions that play essential role in response inhibition and decision-making [41,105,106], which in this case did not result in the even higher deficit of the neurocognitive performance of AUD patients with symptoms of GD compared to patients with only AUD.

Regarding the lack of differences in other aspects of impulsivity measured in this study, meta-analyses [18,19] indicate that impulsivity is not a unitary construct and it has different manifestations in AUD and GD compared to healthy populations. Ioannidis et al. (2019) argued that clinical and even subclinical GD is characterized by general disfunction in inhibitory control, thus impulsive cognitive disfunction, while another meta-analysis [71] established that both AUD and GD show impairment in cognitive impulsivity, GD patients exhibiting even higher impairment on impulsive decision-making.

However, a few limitations need to be taken into account when interpreting the results of this study. Since impulsivity is regarded as a complex and multifactorial phenomenon, there is no clear-cut consensus for defining and operationalizing each component of it, even the number of components and their separability are questioned. We operationalized impulsivity by neuropsychological and self-measurement tests assessing four domains: trait impulsivity, choice impulsivity, impulsive aggression and impulsive decision-making; thus, the present study is limited by this notion.

Additionally, in the present study, impulsive aggression was measured by the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ), which is related to impulsiveness; however, it does not explicitly measure impulsivity, since it includes items of premeditated antisocial behaviour and hostility. Moreover, considering the cross-sectional nature of the current study, conclusions concerning causality between comorbid AUD and GD symptoms and distinct facets of impulsivity cannot be drawn. In this study, we could only establish a link between longer lifetime alcohol consumption, more severe alcohol consumption and heightened trait impulsivity in AUD patients with comorbid GD symptoms compared to sole AUD. Since the prolonged misuse of alcohol leads to impulsive actions across one's life course, and this influence both objective and subjective measures of impulsivity. Future research would benefit from the longitudinal evaluation of the different aspects of impulsivity in chronic AUD populations. Taking into consideration its different presentations in AUD comorbid with GD symptoms may contribute to clearing directions for providing target-specific and effective treatment approaches.

Supplementary information for this manuscript can be found online: https://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.11473296

Supporting information

S1 Annex. Correlation matrix for study variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227645.s001

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

Zsolt Demetrovics was supported by the Hungarian National Research, Development and Innovation Office (NKFIH-1157-8/2019-DT, KKP126835).

Bálint Andó was supported by the Geza Hetenyi Grant (Grant Number: SZTE-ÁOK-KKA-2019-HG).

Special thanks to Sándor Rózsa for providing the WAIS-IV assessment tool and the certification for its application.

References

  1. 1. Hasin DS, Grant BF. The National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC) Waves 1 and 2: review and summary of findings. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2015;50(11):1609–40. pmid:26210739
  2. 2. World Health Organization, Management of Substance Abuse Team, World Health Organization. Global status report on alcohol and health 2018. [Internet]. 2018 [cited 2018 Nov 15]. Available from: http://www.who.int/substance_abuse/publications/global_alcohol_report/en/
  3. 3. Cunningham-Williams RM, Cottler LB, Compton WM, Spitznagel EL. Taking chances: problem gamblers and mental health disorders—results from the St. Louis Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study. Am J Public Health. 1998;88(7):1093–6. pmid:9663161
  4. 4. Manning V, Dowling NA, Lee S, Rodda S, Garfield JBB, Volberg R, et al. Problem gambling and substance use in patients attending community mental health services. J Behav Addict. 2017;6(4):678–88. pmid:29254361
  5. 5. Feigelman W, Wallisch LS, Lesieur HR. Problem gamblers, problem substance users, and dual-problem individuals: An epidemiological study. Am J Public Health. 1998;88(3):467–70. pmid:9518986
  6. 6. Stinchfield R, Kushner MG, Winters KC. Alcohol use and prior substance abuse treatment in relation to gambling problem severity and gambling treatment outcome. J Gambl Stud. 2005;21(3):273–97. pmid:16134009
  7. 7. Petry NM, Stinson FS, Grant BF. Comorbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other psychiatric disorders: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. J Clin Psychiatry. 2005;66(5):564–74. pmid:15889941
  8. 8. Lorains FK, Cowlishaw S, Thomas SA. Prevalence of comorbid disorders in problem and pathological gambling: systematic review and meta-analysis of population surveys. Addict Abingdon Engl. 2011;106(3):490–8.
  9. 9. Daghestani AN, Elenz E, Crayton JW. Pathological gambling in hospitalized substance abusing veterans. J Clin Psychiatry. 1996;57(8):360–3. pmid:8752019
  10. 10. Lesieur HR, Rosenthal RJ. Pathological gambling: A review of the literature (prepared for the American Psychiatric Association task force on DSM-IV committee on disorders of impulse control not elsewhere classified). J Gambl Stud. 1991;7(1):5–39. pmid:24242968
  11. 11. Slutske WS, Eisen S, True WR, Lyons MJ, Goldberg J, Tsuang M. Common genetic vulnerability for pathological gambling and alcohol dependence in men. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(7):666–73. pmid:10891037
  12. 12. Bottesi G, Ghisi M. Pathological gambling: Compulsive -impulsive spectrum disorder, behavioural addiction or both? Phenotypic and endophenotypic evidence. Psychopathol Rev. 2014;1(1):2–25.
  13. 13. Salvatore JE, Gottesman II, Dick DM. Endophenotypes for alcohol use disorder: An update on the field. Curr Addict Rep. 2015;2(1):76–90. pmid:26236574
  14. 14. Buja A, Lion C, Scioni M, Vian P, Genetti B, Vittadello F, et al. SOGS-RA gambling scores and substance use in adolescents. J Behav Addict. 2017;6(3):425–33. pmid:28783970
  15. 15. Vitaro F, Arseneault L, Tremblay RE. Impulsivity predicts problem gambling in low SES adolescent males. Addict Abingdon Engl. 1999;94(4):565–75.
  16. 16. Verdejo-García A, Lawrence AJ, Clark L. Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32(4):777–810. pmid:18295884
  17. 17. Rochat L, Billieux J, Gagnon J, Van der Linden M. A multifactorial and integrative approach to impulsivity in neuropsychology: insights from the UPPS model of impulsivity. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2018;40(1):45–61. pmid:28398126
  18. 18. Stephan RA, Alhassoon OM, Allen KE, Wollman SC, Hall M, Thomas WJ, et al. Meta-analyses of clinical neuropsychological tests of executive dysfunction and impulsivity in alcohol use disorder. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2017;43(1):24–43. pmid:27712350
  19. 19. Ioannidis K, Hook R, Wickham K, Grant JE, Chamberlain SR. Impulsivity in gambling disorder and problem gambling: A meta-analysis. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2019;43(1):24–43.
  20. 20. Mitchell MR, Potenza MN. Addictions and personality traits: Impulsivity and related constructs. Curr Behav Neurosci Rep. 2014;1(1):1–12. pmid:24772382
  21. 21. Perry JL, Carroll ME. The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2008;200(1):1–26.
  22. 22. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(11):1481–9. pmid:16251991
  23. 23. Fein G, Bachman L, Fisher S, Davenport L. Cognitive impairments in abstinent alcoholics. West J Med. 1990;152(5):531–7. pmid:2190421
  24. 24. Harding AJ, Wong A, Svoboda M, Kril JJ, Halliday GM. Chronic alcohol consumption does not cause hippocampal neuron loss in humans. Hippocampus. 1997;7(1):78–87. pmid:9138671
  25. 25. Alessi SM, Petry NM. Pathological gambling severity is associated with impulsivity in a delay discounting procedure. Behav Processes. 2003;64(3):345–54. pmid:14580703
  26. 26. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994;50(1–3):7–15. pmid:8039375
  27. 27. Hodgins DC, Holub A. Components of impulsivity in gambling disorder. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2015;13(6):699–711. pmid:26568706
  28. 28. Mitchell JM, Fields HL, D’Esposito M, Boettiger CA. Impulsive responding in alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29(12):2158–69. pmid:16385186
  29. 29. Dussault F, Brendgen M, Vitaro F, Wanner B, Tremblay RE. Longitudinal links between impulsivity, gambling problems and depressive symptoms: a transactional model from adolescence to early adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(2):130–8. pmid:20854365
  30. 30. Hentges RF, Shaw DS, Wang M-T. Early childhood parenting and child impulsivity as precursors to aggression, substance use, and risky sexual behavior in adolescence and early adulthood. Dev Psychopathol. 2018;30(4):1305–19. pmid:29151377
  31. 31. Jacob L, Haro JM, Koyanagi A. Relationship between attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and problem gambling: A mediation analysis of influential factors among 7,403 individuals from the UK. J Behav Addict. 2018;7(3):781–91. pmid:30238788
  32. 32. McCarthy DM, Niculete ME, Treloar HR, Morris DH, Bartholow BD. Acute alcohol effects on impulsivity: Associations with drinking and driving behavior. Addict Abingdon Engl. 2012;107(12):2109–14.
  33. 33. Berlin HA, Hollander E. Understanding the differences between impulsivity and compulsivity. Psychiatr Times. 2008;25(8):58.
  34. 34. Charney DA, Zikos E, Gill KJ. Early recovery from alcohol dependence: Factors that promote or impede abstinence. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2010;38(1):42–50. pmid:19632079
  35. 35. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric Aspects of Impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry [Internet]. 2001 Nov 1 [cited 2018 Dec 18]; Available from: https://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/doi/abs/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783
  36. 36. Evren C, Durkaya M, Evren B, Dalbudak E, Cetin R. Relationship of relapse with impulsivity, novelty seeking and craving in male alcohol-dependent inpatients. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2012;31(1):81–90. pmid:21450046
  37. 37. Aragay N, Barrios M, Ramirez-Gendrau I, Garcia-Caballero A, Garrido G, Ramos-Grille I, et al. Impulsivity profiles in pathological slot machine gamblers. Compr Psychiatry. 2018;83:79–83. pmid:29625378
  38. 38. Stevens AK, Littlefield AK, Talley AE, Brown JL. Do individuals higher in impulsivity drink more impulsively? A pilot study within a high risk sample of young adults. Addict Behav. 2017;65:147–53. pmid:27816040
  39. 39. Yakovenko I, Fortgang R, Prentice J, Hoff RA, Potenza MN. Correlates of frequent gambling and gambling-related chasing behaviors in individuals with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. J Behav Addict. 2018;7(2):375–83. pmid:29788756
  40. 40. Lawrence AJ, Luty J, Bogdan NA, Sahakian BJ, Clark L. Problem gamblers share deficits in impulsive decision-making with alcohol-dependent individuals. Addict Abingdon Engl. 2009;104(6):1006–15.
  41. 41. Goudriaan AE, Oosterlaan J, de Beurs E, van den Brink W. Decision making in pathological gambling: A comparison between pathological gamblers, alcohol dependents, persons with Tourette syndrome, and normal controls. Cogn Brain Res. 2005;23(1):137–51.
  42. 42. Bottesi G, Ghisi M, Ouimet AJ, Tira MD, Sanavio E. Compulsivity and impulsivity in pathological gambling: Does a dimensional-transdiagnostic approach add clinical utility to DSM-5 classification? J Gambl Stud. 2015;31(3):825–47. pmid:24863627
  43. 43. Kovács I, Richman MJ, Janka Z, Maraz A, Andó B. Decision making measured by the Iowa Gambling Task in alcohol use disorder and gambling disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2017;181:152–61. pmid:29055269
  44. 44. Eysenck SBG, Pearson PR, Easting G, Allsopp JF. Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults. Personal Individ Differ. 1985;6(5):613–9.
  45. 45. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995;51(6):768–74. pmid:8778124
  46. 46. Ainslie G. Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol Bull. 1975;82(4):463–96. pmid:1099599
  47. 47. Blair RJR. The neurobiology of impulsive aggression. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2016;26(1):4–9. pmid:26465707
  48. 48. Franken IHA, van Strien JW, Nijs I, Muris P. Impulsivity is associated with behavioral decision-making deficits. Psychiatry Res. 2008;158(2):155–63. pmid:18215765
  49. 49. Logan GD. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language. Abil Inhib Thought Action Users Guide Stop Signal Paradigm. 1994;189–239.
  50. 50. Paksi B, Demetrovics Z, Magi A, Felvinczi K. [The methodology and sample description of the National Survey on Addiction Problems in Hungary 2015 (NSAPH 2015)]. Neuropsychopharmacol Hung. 2017;19(2):55–85. pmid:28918416
  51. 51. Buss AH, Perry M. The aggression questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1992;63(3):452–9. pmid:1403624
  52. 52. Gerevich J, Bácskai E, Czobor P. The generalizability of the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2007;16(3):124–36. pmid:17849418
  53. 53. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997;275(5304):1293–5. pmid:9036851
  54. 54. Grant DA, Berg EA. A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. J Exp Psychol. 1948;38(4):404–11. pmid:18874598
  55. 55. Berg EA. A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. J Gen Psychol. 1948;39:15–22. pmid:18889466
  56. 56. Piper BJ, Mueller ST, Geerken AR, Dixon KL, Kroliczak G, Olsen RHJ, et al. Reliability and validity of neurobehavioral function on the Psychology Experimental Building Language test battery in young adults. PeerJ. 2015;3:e1460. pmid:26713233
  57. 57. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2008.
  58. 58. Rózsa S, Kő N. A Wechsler intelligenciateszttel szerzett nemzetközi eredmények áttekintése. In: A WISC-IV gyermek intelligenciateszt magyar kézikönyve Hazai tapasztalatok, vizsgálati eredmények és normák. Budapest: OS Hungary Tesztfejlesztő Kft.; 2008. p. 9–16.
  59. 59. Butler SF, Budman SH, Goldman RJ, Newman FL, Beckley KE, Trottier D, et al. Initial validation of a computer-administered Addiction Severity Index: the ASI-MV. Psychol Addict Behav J Soc Psychol Addict Behav. 2001;15(1):4–12.
  60. 60. Rácz J, Pogány C, Máthé-Árvay N. Az EuropASI (Addikció Súlyossági Index) magyar nyelvű változatának reliabilitás- és validitásvizsgálata. Magy Pszichol Szle. 2002;57(4):587–603.
  61. 61. Lesieur HR, Blume SB. The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): a new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. Am J Psychiatry. 1987;144(9):1184–8. pmid:3631315
  62. 62. Gyollai A, Urbán R, Kun B, Paksi B, Arnold P, Balázs H, et al. Problémás és patológiás szerencsejáték Magyarországon: a South Oaks szerencsejáték kérdoiv magyar verziójának (SOGS-HU) hazai alkalmazása. Psychiatr Hung. 2011;26(4):230–40. pmid:22058255
  63. 63. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption—II. Addict Abingdon Engl. 1993;88(6):791–804.
  64. 64. Gerevich J, Bácskai E, Rózsa S. Prevalence of hazardous alcohol use. Psychiatr Hung. 2006;21(1):45–56. pmid:16783031
  65. 65. Derogatis LR. SCL-90-R Administration, Scoring and Procedures Manual II for the Revised Version and Other Instruments of the Psychopathology Rating Scale Series. Towson, MD: Clinical Psychometric Research; 1986. 61 p.
  66. 66. Urbán R, Arrindell WA, Demetrovics Z, Unoka Z, Timman R. Cross-cultural confirmation of bi-factor models of a symptom distress measure: Symptom Checklist-90-Revised in clinical samples. Psychiatry Res. 2016;239:265–74. pmid:27039011
  67. 67. Urbán R, Kun B, Farkas J, Paksi B, Kökönyei G, Unoka Z, et al. Bifactor structural model of symptom checklists: SCL-90-R and Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) in a non-clinical community sample. Psychiatry Res. 2014;216(1):146–54. pmid:24524946
  68. 68. Corp IBM. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.; 2016.
  69. 69. Mazur JE. An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In: The effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value. Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1987. p. 55–73. (Quantitative analyses of behavior, Vol. 5.).
  70. 70. Mitchell SH. Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1999;146(4):455–64.
  71. 71. Kovács I, Demeter I, Janka Z, Demetrovics Z, Maraz A, Andó B. Dataset for manuscript entitled ‘Different aspects of impulsivity in chronic alcohol use disorder with and without comorbid problem gambling’ [Internet]. 2019 [cited 2019 Aug 24]. Available from: https://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.11473296
  72. 72. Rosnow RL, Rosenthal R, Rubin DB. Contrasts and correlations in effect-size estimation. Psychol Sci. 2000;11(6):446–53. pmid:11202488
  73. 73. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
  74. 74. Cho SS, Pellecchia G, Aminian K, Ray N, Segura B, Obeso I, et al. Morphometric correlation of impulsivity in medial prefrontal cortex. Brain Topogr. 2013;26(3):479–87. pmid:23274773
  75. 75. Sullivan EV, Harris RA, Pfefferbaum A. Alcohol’s effects on brain and behavior. Alcohol Res Health. 2010;33(1–2):127–43. pmid:23579943
  76. 76. Keough MT O’Connor RM. Interactive Effects of the BIS and the BAS on trajectories of alcohol misuse after university graduation. Subst Abuse Res Treat. 2016;9(Suppl 1):33–40.
  77. 77. Johnson SL, Turner RJ, Iwata N. BIS/BAS Levels and psychiatric disorder: An epidemiological study. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2003;25(1):25–36.
  78. 78. Evren C, Yilmaz A, Can Y, Bozkurt M, Evren B. EPA-0489—Status of impulsivity in 12 month follow-up among male alcohol dependent inpatients. Eur Psychiatry. 2014;29:1.
  79. 79. Rubio G, Jiménez M, Rodríguez-Jiménez R, Martínez I, Ávila C, Ferre F, et al. The role of behavioral impulsivity in the development of alcohol dependence: A 4-Year Follow-Up Study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32(9):1681–7. pmid:18631324
  80. 80. Yan W-S, Zhang R-R, Lan Y, Li Y-H, Sui N. Comparison of impulsivity in non-problem, at-risk and problem gamblers. Sci Rep [Internet]. 2016 Dec 15 [cited 2018 Dec 20];6. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5156948/
  81. 81. Choi J-S, Kim HS, Kim G-Y, Jeon Y, Park SM, Lee J-Y, et al. Similarities and differences among Internet gaming disorder, gambling disorder and alcohol use disorder: A focus on impulsivity and compulsivity. J Behav Addict. 2014;3(4):246–53. pmid:25592310
  82. 82. Lee JY, Park SM, Kim YJ, Kim DJ, Choi S-W, Kwon JS, et al. Resting-state EEG activity related to impulsivity in gambling disorder. J Behav Addict. 2017;6(3):387–95. pmid:28856896
  83. 83. Petry NM. Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2001;154(3):243–50.
  84. 84. Petry NM, Casarella T. Excessive discounting of delayed rewards in substance abusers with gambling problems. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1999;56(1):25–32. pmid:10462089
  85. 85. Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1999;128(1):78–87. pmid:10100392
  86. 86. Madden GJ, Petry NM, Badger GJ, Bickel WK. Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: drug and monetary rewards. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1997;5(3):256–62. pmid:9260073
  87. 87. Fillmore MT, Blackburn J. Compensating for alcohol-induced impairment: alcohol expectancies and behavioral disinhibition. J Stud Alcohol. 2002;63(2):237–46. pmid:12033701
  88. 88. Fillmore MT, Marczinski CA, Bowman AM. Acute tolerance to alcohol effects on inhibitory and activational mechanisms of behavioral control. J Stud Alcohol. 2005;66(5):663–72. pmid:16331852
  89. 89. Marczinski CA, Fillmore MT. Compensating for alcohol-induced impairment of control: effects on inhibition and activation of behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2005;181(2):337–46.
  90. 90. Hildebrandt H, Brokate B, Eling P, Lanz M. Response shifting and inhibition, but not working memory, are impaired after long-term heavy alcohol consumption. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(2):203–11. pmid:15099142
  91. 91. Kamarajan C, Porjesz B, Jones KA, Choi K, Chorlian DB, Padmanabhapillai A, et al. Alcoholism is a disinhibitory disorder: neurophysiological evidence from a Go/No-Go task. Biol Psychol. 2005;69(3):353–73. pmid:15925035
  92. 92. Kringelbach ML. The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(9):691–702. pmid:16136173
  93. 93. Metzger CD, Eckert U, Steiner J, Sartorius A, Buchmann JE, Stadler J, et al. High field FMRI reveals thalamocortical integration of segregated cognitive and emotional processing in mediodorsal and intralaminar thalamic nuclei. Front Neuroanat. 2010;4:138. pmid:21088699
  94. 94. Parke A, Griffiths MD. Aggressive behaviour in adult slot machine gamblers: A qualitative observational study. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2005;2(50–58).
  95. 95. White HR, Brick J, Hansell S. A longitudinal investigation of alcohol use and aggression in adolescence. J Stud Alcohol Suppl. 1993;11:62–77. pmid:8410965
  96. 96. Korman L, Collins J, Littman-Sharp N, Skinner W, McMain S, Mercado V. Randomized control trial of an integrated therapy for comorbid anger and gambling. Psychother Res J Soc Psychother Res. 2008;18(4):454–65.
  97. 97. McPherson A, Martin C. A contemporary review of the alcohol/aggression relationship and the Buss‐Perry Aggression Questionnaire for use in an alcohol dependent population. J Aggress Confl Peace Res. 2010;2(1):45–56.
  98. 98. Cavedini P, Riboldi G, Keller R, D’Annucci A, Bellodi L. Frontal lobe dysfunction in pathological gambling patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51(4):334–41. pmid:11958785
  99. 99. Cordovil De Sousa Uva M, Luminet O, Cortesi M, Constant E, Derely M, De Timary P. Distinct effects of protracted withdrawal on affect, craving, selective attention and executive functions among alcohol-dependent patients. Alcohol Alcohol Oxf Oxfs. 2010;45(3):241–6.
  100. 100. Forbush KT, Shaw M, Graeber MA, Hovick L, Meyer VJ, Moser DJ, et al. Neuropsychological characteristics and personality traits in pathological gambling. CNS Spectr. 2008;13(4):306–15. pmid:18408650
  101. 101. Murray DE, Durazzo TC, Mon A, Schmidt TP, Meyerhoff DJ. Brain perfusion in polysubstance users: relationship to substance and tobacco use, cognition, and self-regulation. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;150:120–8. pmid:25772434
  102. 102. Guillot CR, Fanning JR, Bullock JS, McCloskey MS, Berman ME. Effects of alcohol on tests of executive functioning in men and women: A dose response examination. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2010;18(5):409–17. pmid:20939644
  103. 103. Houston RJ, Derrick J, Leonard K, Testa M, Quigley B, Kubiak A. Effects of heavy drinking on executive cognitive functioning in a community sample. Addict Behav. 2014;39(1):345–9. pmid:24459697
  104. 104. Potenza MN, Kosten TR, Rounsaville BJ. Pathological gambling. JAMA. 2001;286(2):141–4. pmid:11448261
  105. 105. Lawrence AJ, Luty J, Bogdan NA, Sahakian BJ, Clark L. Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2009;207(1):163–72.
  106. 106. Mackey S, Allgaier N, Chaarani B, Spechler P, Orr C, Bunn J, et al. Mega-analysis of gray matter volume in substance dependence: General and substance-specific regional effects. Am J Psychiatry. 2018;appiajp201817040415.