Figures
Abstract
Reconstruction has progressed steadily since the 2011 TEPCO’s Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear Power Station accident. However, some people still hesitate to eat foods from Fukushima or to travel there, and there are concerns about the health risks of radiation. We investigated the relationships among reconstruction-related behavior, risk perception, types of information, and information sources, in order to consider appropriate measures for providing information and promoting reconstruction-related behavior a number of years after the accident. We conducted an online questionnaire survey (n = 1000) of Tokyo residents. First, a factor analysis was conducted on knowledge associated with radiation. Two factors were extracted; namely, “physical knowledge” and “health/social knowledge.” We conducted structural equation modeling to construct a model of “knowledge,” “radiation risk perception,” and “intention concerning reconstruction-related behavior.” “Intention concerning reconstruction-related behavior” decreased with “radiation risk perception” and increased with “health/social knowledge.” In addition, “health/social knowledge” negatively affected “radiation risk perception;” this effect was not large, but it was significant. Second, respondents were clarified by information sources using a cluster analysis. Clusters that included respondents who got information from public relations materials issued by municipalities and websites of administrative agencies had a higher factor score for “health/social knowledge” than other clusters. The cluster of respondents who did not get any particular knowledge had the lowest factor score, which was significant, and also had a low “perception of reconstruction.”
Citation: Shirai K, Yoshizawa N, Takebayashi Y, Murakami M (2019) Modeling reconstruction-related behavior and evaluation of influences of major information sources. PLoS ONE 14(8): e0221561. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221561
Editor: Berta Schnettler, Universidad de La Frontera, CHILE
Received: February 18, 2019; Accepted: August 9, 2019; Published: August 23, 2019
Copyright: © 2019 Shirai et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.
Funding: KS and NY work in Mitsubishi Research Institute, Inc. and the questionnaire survey was done by using funding from Mitsubishi Research Institute, Inc. The funder provided support in the form of research funding for authors KS and NY, but did not have any additional role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. YT and MM (Fukushima Medical University) received no funding from Mitsubishi Research Institute, Inc. for this research purpose.
Competing interests: KS and NY work in Mitsubishi Research Institute, Inc. and the questionnaire survey was done by using Mitsubishi Research Institute, Inc. funding. YT and MM (Fukushima Medical University) received no funding from Mitsubishi Research Institute, Inc. for this research purpose. There are not restrictions on sharing of data and materials. This does not alter our adherence to PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.
Introduction
The 2011 Great East Japan Earthquake and accompanying accidents at Tokyo Electric Power Company (TEPCO)’s Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear Power Station damaged surrounding areas, especially in Fukushima Prefecture. However, reconstruction efforts have advanced steadily to date. The total gross output of Fukushima Prefecture increased in FY2015 to 108.9% of that of the pre-disaster year of 2010. In addition, the number of tourists who visited the prefecture during FY2016 recovered to 92.3% of that in the same pre-disaster year, while prefectural radiation monitoring of foods from the prefecture confirmed that the number of samples exceeding standard values decreased to 0.03% of the total during the same period [1]. Some previous studies revealed that estimated doses from foods after the accident were limited [2,3].
On the other hand, a certain number of people still avoid buying foods from Fukushima Prefecture or visiting the prefecture on group tours. Murakami et al. found dread-risk (e.g., instinctively dreaded; will increase, is difficult to reduce, has a fatal effect, is associated with genetic risk, etc.) perception of dietary radionuclides among residents living outside Fukushima prefecture was higher than among Fukushima residents [4] and Igarashi reported residents living outside Fukushima Prefecture tended to avoid purchasing foods from Fukushima compared to residents living in Fukushima in 2017 [5]. According to a 2017 survey of Tokyo residents conducted by the authors, 26.3% and 35.0% of respondents stated that “they hesitate to eat because of concerns over radiation” when they or their family eat foods from the prefecture, respectively. Regarding trips to Fukushima Prefecture, 28.0% and 36.9% of respondents answered that “they hesitate to visit because of concerns over radiation” when they or their family visit the prefecture, respectively [6]. These results indicated that anxiety over radiation was not only negatively affecting industry in the prefecture, but was also disturbing reconstruction activities. Because buying more food from Fukushima prefecture and traveling more to the prefecture would lead to the faster reconstruction of Fukushima Prefecture, the perception associated with such behavior is called “Perception concerning Reconstruction-related Behavior" hereinafter in this report.
There is also anxiety concerning the health effects of radiation caused by radioactive materials that were scattered during the accidents at TEPCO’s Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear Power Station. In the Fukushima Health Management Survey -Mental Health and Lifestyle Survey- conducted by Fukushima Medical University and Fukushima Prefecture, residents of 13 municipalities spanning evacuation order areas of the prefecture were asked about the anticipated likelihood of post-disaster radiation causing cancer and affecting the health of next-generation residents. The number of respondents who answered “likely” or “very likely” had decreased to 32.5% (causing cancer; delayed risk) and 36.1% (affecting the health of the next generation; genetic risk) in 2018 [7], and Takebayashi et al. pointed out that rates for residents living in Fukushima who feel anxiety over radiation decreased from 2012 to 2015 for risk perceptions of radiation in the Fukushima Health Management Survey [8]. Meanwhile, in our similar questionnaire survey of Tokyo residents, 53.5% (delayed risk) and 49.8% (genetic risk) of respondents answered that radiation effects would be likely or very likely [6]. There is a possibility that this recognition of the health effects of radiation may have a negative influence on “Intention concerning Reconstruction-related Behavior.” Kudo and Nakayachi examined a buying behavior model for agricultural products produced in Fukushima prefecture using structural equation modeling, which indicated that anxiety over radiation and nuclear had a negative influence on buying intention [9].
Previous studies reported that various factors affected radiation risk perception after the accident, and the governing factors of radiation risk perception included demographics, disaster-related stressors, trusted information, and radiation-related variables [8]. Meanwhile, relevance between knowledge and risk perception was inconsistent [10, 11]. It is suggested that the effects on risk perception may differ depending on types of knowledge. Miura et al., classified types of knowledge associated with radiation, namely knowledge regarding effects on the human body and scientific knowledge, and showed that the former knowledge had a closer relationship with attitude towards foods from areas around TEPCO Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear Power Station than scientific knowledge [12]. It is useful for reducing radiation risk perception and promoting reconstruction-related behavior to investigate the relationship between risk perception and types of knowledge or reconstruction-related behavior and types of knowledge. However, these relationships have not been clarified.
In addition, with respect to the risk perception of the health effects of radioactive materials, Niiyama demonstrated that image affects factors such as seriousness of health effects and perception of accumulation in the body [13], and Takenishi and Takahashi pointed out the influence of the memory of accidents on the perception of the safety of raw vegetables [14]. In this way, factors such as image and memory play important roles in risk perception. Meanwhile, in our questionnaire survey, which asked respondents how long they had caught information on the nuclear accident and revitalization in Fukushima Prefecture after the disaster, almost half answered “about two years.” [6] Based on these findings, it is highly likely that many people stopped updating relevant information after a few years and do not have the latest information. Evaluating the relation between perception of reconstruction status in Fukushima and some factors associated with reconstruction-related behavior among people living outside Fukushima may be useful for promoting behavior.
Furthermore, it is revealed that there was a relationship between risk perception and information sources [4]. It is possible that types of information source affect knowledge and influence risk perception. Clarifying the relationship between types of information source and knowledge would help in constructing a strategy for information provision measures. However, such investigations have not been reported.
This study had two objectives. First, we classified types of knowledge and found relationships between risk perception and reconstruction-related behavior. Second, we revealed a relationship between knowledge and information sources. This was the first study to find relationships among reconstruction-related behavior, risk perception, types of information, and information sources after the accident.
Methods
Hypothetic model of reconstruction-related behavior
First, in order to understand the basic perception structure of reconstruction-related behavior, this study designed a simple hypothetic model consisting of three elements: “Intention concerning Reconstruction-related Behavior,” “Radiation Risk Perception,” and “Knowledge.” Here, as shown in Fig 1, we assume a one-way structure where “radiation risk perception” is influenced by “knowledge” and “Intention concerning reconstruction-related behavior” is affected by both “radiation risk perception” and “knowledge.”
In addition to the above three elements, the “perception of reconstruction status” of Fukushima Prefecture is also an important element. The perception of reconstruction status is thought to be interrelated with the above three elements, as well as limited effects on specific elements. Therefore, in this study, we also consider differences in models due to differences in perception of reconstruction status.
Participants
Ethical approval for the study was granted by the Fukushima Medical University Ethics Committee (Ethics Committee approval number: General 29353).
The subjects of the survey were men and women in their 20s to 60s living in Tokyo and Fukushima. However, we focused on the results only from Tokyo in this study because the main purpose of this study was to learn more about risk communication in order to reduce reputational damage. Because Tokyo is located more than 200km from TEPCO’s Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear Power Station, and since most Tokyo residents were not evacuated, the perceptions of Tokyo residents may lead to reputational damage. In addition, the number of residents of Fukushima who feel anxiety concerning radiation has decreased over the last few years [8], while half of Tokyo residents felt anxiety concerning radiation [6].
The survey was conducted from August 9 to August 17, 2017, using an online questionnaire of monitors by Cross Marketing, Inc. This company is one of the largest survey companies in Japan with 4.2 million panelists. The company set up a target number of participants, grouped according to sex, age, and residential area. It asked panelists to respond to the questionnaires until this target number of respondents was collected. The target number was 100 in Tokyo from each demographic of age (20s, 30s, 40s, 50s, 60s) and sex. Elderly individuals aged 70 years or older, who may have been unfamiliar with online surveys, were excluded. Respondents provided written consent to participate in the survey before the survey was conducted.
Inappropriate responses were excluded, such as in the case of a short response time. In addition, for the purpose of understanding the general consciousness of Tokyo residents, we excluded from this survey those who were from Fukushima Prefecture or had close relatives living in the prefecture. Furthermore, those who worked for research companies or nuclear power plants and those who were in reconstruction-related posts were excluded. There were no missing data in the surveys. Respondents were encouraged to answer the survey by being awarded points that could be exchanged for Internet points. The advantages of online surveys have been described in a previous study [15]. A total of 1,000 responses in Tokyo were obtained. Table 1 shows basic information on respondents.
It is to be noted that the part of the data used in this study has been described in our reports [6]. In this study, we conducted another analysis using these data.
Questionnaire
The questionnaire was made up largely of three components: questions intended to build a model for verifying the hypothesis model shown above; questions intended to evaluate differences in knowledge among information sources; and, socio-demographics.
First, the questions for model building included a set of questions concerning willingness to buy foods from Fukushima Prefecture and willingness to travel to the prefecture, in order to clarify “intention concerning reconstruction-related behavior.” Assuming that the quality and prices of foods from Fukushima Prefecture are almost the same as those from other prefectures, three options for answers were set up in connection with cases where respondents (respondents/foods) or their family members (family members/foods) would eat such foods—“eat and recommend positively (3),” “do not mind if they were from Fukushima or not (2),” and “hesitate to eat because of concerns over radiation (1).” Regarding travel to Fukushima Prefecture, similar options were set up in connection with the cases where respondents (respondents/travel) or their family members (family members/travel) would travel to Fukushima Prefecture—"travel and recommend positively (3),” “do not mind radiation (2),” and “hesitate to travel because of concerns over radiation (1).” The answers were obtained using a three-point Likert scale and scores of 3 to 1 were assigned to each affirmative answer so that those answers obtained a higher score.
Two questions were set up regarding “Radiation Risk Perception” following the questionnaire in the Fukushima Health Management Survey -Mental Health and Lifestyle Survey- conducted by Fukushima Medical University and Fukushima Prefecture [7]. Originally developed by Lindel and Barnes in the aftermath of the Three Mile Island Nuclear Pawer Station accident [16], the answers to these same questions in the Fukushima Health Management Survey were used in some studies following the accident in Fukushima [17–20]. The similarity between these indicators and dread risk perception based on Slovic’s theory [21] was discussed previously [8]. Concerning the onset of cancer and its impacts on the next and later generations, respectively, the questions asked were: “What do you think is the likelihood of damage to health (e.g. cancer onset) in later life as a result of current level of radiation exposure in Fukushima?” and “What do you think is the likelihood that the health of future (i.e. as-yet unborn) children and grandchildren will be affected as a result of current level of radiation exposure in Fukushima?” For these questions, a four-point Likert scale was used as follows: very unlikely (1), unlikely (2), likely (3), or very likely (4). It is to be noted that these questions did not represent risks to respondents, but to residents of Fukushima Prefecture. The value of Cronbach’s alpha was 0.929.
There are two types of knowledge, namely subjective knowledge [22] and objective knowledge [23]. Subjective knowledge was adopted to reduce the loads on respondents when they gave answers in this study, namely respondents chose options they knew. Two sets of questions were set up following the actual situation survey on consumer awareness of damage from rumors regularly conducted by the government [24]. First, we set up seven answer options for multiple choices, as shown in Table 2, in addition to the option “Others” and “I did not know that an inspection is being conducted.” Next, seven options were set up also for multiple choices with respect to radiation, radioactive materials, and radioactivity, as shown in the same Table 2, in addition to the options “Others” and “No particular knowledge.”
Furthermore, for the purpose of examining the detailed model, as a “perception of the reconstruction status of Fukushima Prefecture,” the question set was: “I feel that restoration and reconstruction of Fukushima prefecture are progressing.” We used a five-point Likert scale ranging from 5 (I think so) to 1 (I do not think so).
Next, questions were designed to evaluate differences in knowledge among information sources. As sources of information concerning the reconstruction status of Fukushima Prefecture, eleven sources were suggested for multiple choices, in addition to “Other sources” and “Did not particularly obtain information:” “Websites of administrative agencies,” “Websites of universities, research institutions, and medical institution,” “Websites other than the above two sources,” “Twitter,” “Facebook and other social networking services (SNSs), excluding Twitter,” “TV and radio,” “Newspapers and magazines,” “Advertisements and leaflets,” “Public relations materials issued by municipalities,” “Circulations of regional community associations,” and “Friends and acquaintances.”
Finally, socio-demographics asked were composed of sex, age, prefecture of residence, prefecture of birth, the presence/absence of close relatives who were born or are living in Fukushima Prefecture, occupation, marital status, and presence/absence of children.
Statistical analyses
To classify types of knowledge, we conducted a factor analysis using the maximum likelihood method and Promax rotation. Promax rotation was used because correlation between factors concerning knowledge was assumed. We extracted two factors based on a parallel analysis, scree test, and Kaiser-Guttman method. From the results of the parallel analysis, the maximum number of factors can be judged to be 2. Also, from the least squares partial correlation examined, we confirmed that the minimum factor number was 1. Based on the above results and the possibility of interpretation, the number of factors was judged to be 2.
Structural equation modeling was conducted to evaluate the model of relationships among knowledge, risk perception, and reconstruction-related behavior. For the analysis, SPSS Amos 22.0 (IBM co., USA) was used. Factor score by estimating confirmatory factor analysis was used for observed variables about knowledge and the score for each answer was used as other observed variables in the model. Four error correlations were set between observed variables constituting latent variables because it seemed that there were correlations particularly between the same behavior with different subjects (“respondents/foods” and “family members/foods,” or “respondents/travel” and “family members/travel”) and correlations between the same subjects with different behavior (“respondents/foods” and “respondents/travel,” or “family members/foods” and “family members/travel”) (S2 Fig).
Furthermore, to compare the difference in values of the path coefficient within the model due to “perception of reconstruction status” in Fukushima Prefecture, the respondents were classified into two groups—the Positive group (P group, n = 223), who responded that Fukushima is recovering, and the Not Positive group (NP group, n = 777), who responded that Fukushima is not recovering or who were unable to answer. Then, structural equation modeling was conducted again in the same model.
A cluster analysis was conducted using Ward’s method to clarify the respondents using differences among sources of information on reconstruction status in Fukushima Prefecture. When the respondents chose some options, they were assigned a score of 1 and the options not chosen were assigned a score of 0. The number of clusters was decided by the researchers based on their means of contexts.
To determine the difference in factor scores for knowledge that affected radiation risk perception and reconstruction-related behavior among clusters, an analysis of variances was conducted. F-test for equality of variance among clusters was rejected at p <. 01 and Games-Howell was used as a post-hoc test. A multiple regression analysis was also conducted using factor scores of “health/social knowledge” as an objective variable, and “physical knowledge” and the cluster divided based on information sources as an explanatory variable to adjust the factor score of “physical knowledge.” The cluster that had the largest number of respondents was set as a reference. In addition, to compare clusters concerning differences in “perception of reconstruction status,” a Chi-square test was conducted.
Results
Descriptive statistics for “intention concerning reconstruction-related behavior,” “radiation risk perception,” and “perception of reconstruction status”
The average score ± standard deviation (SD) was 1.89 ± 0.63 concerning food eaten by respondents (respondent/foods), compared to 1.77 ± 0.65 concerning food eaten by their family members (family members/foods). As for travel to Fukushima Prefecture, the score was 1.88 ± 0.65 for visits by respondents (respondents/travel), compared to 1.76 ± 0.66 for visits by family members (family members/travel), showing similar results to those concerning foods.
For “Radiation Risk Perception,” the average score ± SD for the onset of cancer was 2.55 ± 0.91, and the score for the influence on the next and later generations was 2.48 ± 0.90. About half of the respondents chose a higher likelihood for both items (535 respondents for the onset of illness and 498 for influence on the next and later generations).
Regarding “Perception of Reconstruction Status” in Fukushima Prefecture, the average score ± SD was 2.71 ± 1.02, indicating that not a few respondents felt that restoration and reconstruction were not progressing as fast as they had expected.
Perception model of reconstruction-related behavior
As a result of the factor analysis, two factors were extracted as shown in Table 3. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy was 0.917. Bartlett’s test was p < .01. Cronbach’s α in Factor 1 and Factor 2 was 0.827 and 0.784, respectively. The correlation coefficient between two factors was 0.715. The first factor was named “physical knowledge” because it contained many items indicating the physical properties of radiation. The second factor was named “health/social knowledge” because it contained many items related to food inspections currently conducted in Japan and items related to the health effects of radiation.
Next, structural equation modeling was conducted to verify the model for “knowledge,” “radiation risk perception,” and “intention concerning reconstruction-related behavior.” The results are shown in Fig 2. Both paths in the figure were significant (p < .10). The values of GFI (Goodness of Fit Index), AGFI (Adjusted GFI), and CFI (Comparative Fit Index) shown in the figure are 0.95 or more, which is statistically significant. In addition, RMSEA (Root Mean Square Error of Approximation) was 0.056. From these values, it can be judged that the model is statistically acceptable [25]. The path coefficients of observed variables constituting “radiation risk perception” were 0.92 (delayed risk perception) and 0.95 (genetic risk perception). Those constituting “intention concerning reconstruction-related behavior” were 0.77 ~ 0.81. (respondents would eat foods from Fukushima; 0.78, their family members would eat them; 0.77, respondents would travel to Fukushima; 0.77, and their family members would travel there; 0.81).
In the model, first, a negative influence was seen for “radiation risk perception” on “Intention concerning reconstruction-related behavior.” “Health/social knowledge” had a positive influence on it, meaning that more “health/social knowledge” causes a stronger “Intention concerning reconstruction-related behavior.” When the values of the paths are compared, it was found that the absolute value of the path from “radiation risk perception” is higher and the influence is greater. Also, concerning the influence of knowledge on "radiation risk perception,” while the influence of “health/social knowledge” was negative and the influence of "physical knowledge" was positive, showing that whether the direction of influence is positive or negative depended on type of knowledge.
Respondents were divided into P group and NP group, and structural equation modeling was conducted again. The results for each group are shown in Figs 3 and 4. The value of RMSEA improved a little.
**p < .05, *p < .10. The respondents in P group (n = 223) answered that Fukushima is recovering.
**p < .05. The respondents in NP group (n = 777) answered that Fukushima is not recovering or who were unable to decide.
The coefficient of the path from “health/social knowledge” to “perception concerning reconstruction-related behavior” was moderately higher in the NP group (.20) than in the P group (.11), and a significant influence was seen only in the NP group.
The two groups were compared for the paths from the two types of knowledge to “radiation risk perception.” For the path from “health/social knowledge” to “radiation risk perception,” a negative effect was suggested in the P group, although no significant influence was seen in either group. On the other hand, for the path from “physical knowledge” to “radiation risk perception,” the path was not significant in the P group, while “radiation risk perception” significantly increased in the NP group.
In addition, a difference in path coefficient was tested statistically in the two models. No significant difference was found in either path.
Differences among main information sources
As a result of the cluster analysis, respondents were divided into seven clusters (Table 4). This number was judged because there were too many information sources in several clusters when they were divided into fewer than seven clusters, whereas some clusters contained similar information sources when divided into more than seven clusters. Respondents were permitted to choose multiple answer options. For this reason, the number of answers does not accurately match the number of respondents in a cluster.
Inter-cluster differences were compared with respect to “health/social knowledge,” which influenced “perception concerning reconstruction-related behavior” and “radiation risk perception” as discussed in the results of the SEM. As a result, the factor scores for “health social knowledge” differed significantly among the clusters (Fig 5).
Cluster 3 and Cluster 7 had a higher factor score for “health/social knowledge” than other clusters. Cluster 3 and Cluster 7 were groups that relied on public relations materials issued by municipalities and websites of administrative agencies as information sources. Cluster 2 had a significantly low factor score for “health/social knowledge,” compared to other clusters, and this cluster was a group which did not obtain any particular information. A multiple regression analysis was also conducted and the results indicate that by belonging to Cluster 3 or Cluster 7, people can increase “health/social knowledge.” These are consistent with the results above. (S9 Table).
The result of the Chi-square test showed that there was a significant difference in “perception of reconstruction” among clusters at p < .01 (Table 5). In Cluster 1, the proportion of the P group was significantly higher. This was group obtained information from various sources such as mass media, SNS, and friends. In Cluster 2, the proportion of the P group was significantly lower. This group did not obtain specific information.
Discussion
Effects of radiation risk perception and knowledge on behavior
The analysis of the perception model of behavior related to reconstruction confirmed that “perception concerning reconstruction-related behavior” was increased by lowering “radiation risk perception.” Furthermore, “health/social knowledge” has a direct and an indirect positive influence as the effect of “radiation risk perception” on “perception concerning reconstruction-related behavior.” The value of direct influence (0.17) was higher than that of indirect influence (0.04 (= -0.09×-0.40)). These results were consistent with the previous report as Kudo and Nakayachi indicated that a judgement with knowledge had positively direct and indirect (through a lowering of radiation-related anxiety) effects on intention to buy food from Fukushima [9]. The knowledge used in the judgement with knowledge was not similar to “Physical knowledge” but to “health/social knowledge.” Based on this, in order to promote reconstruction-related behavior, such as encouraging travel to Fukushima Prefecture and purchasing foods from the prefecture, it is essential to distribute relevant information on such matters as the health impacts of radiation and food inspections. It was a natural reaction to be anxious about radiation after the accident, and a high risk perception should not therefore be blamed. However, it should be also noted that a low risk perception might act to enhance reconstruction-related behavior.
The same analysis also showed that “radiation risk perception” was reduced by “health/social knowledge” and enhanced by “physical knowledge.” The relationship between risk perception and knowledge was not consistent [10,11]; however, we indicated that the effects of knowledge on risk perception depended on types of knowledge. These effects were weak, but significant.
Dose exposure at the time of the accident was limited and strict management such as restrictions on the distribution of foods has been conducted. Getting “health/social knowledge” may reduce anxiety over radiation in the circumstances, whereas “physical knowledge” seemingly increased anxiety over radiation because a possibility of effects of radiation on their health was perceived through physical knowledge about radiation. On the other hand, Wim et al. indicated that risk perception increased information needs and information seeking behavior [26]. There was a possibility of a bidirectional relationship between knowledge and risk perception; however, it is highlighted that “health/social knowledge” affected “radiation risk perception” negatively even if the possibility of bidirectional relationship was taken into account.
Factors other than knowledge were presumed to have a strong relationship with risk perception because the effects of “health/social knowledge” on “radiation risk perception” were significant, but weak. In the previous study, gender, age, and evacuation experience were reported as factors [8]. In general, risk perception reflected a cultural world view [27] and was not easily changed [28]. Nevertheless, our results offered important suggestions for the possibility of intervening in risk perception with knowledge. Most of the selection rate concerning knowledge-related questions answered by those living in Tokyo was approximately 10% lower than in Fukushima. In addition, Tokyo residents had a higher risk perception than Fukushima residents [6]. Following the accident in Fukushima, various anxiety-reducing activities were initiated. For example, Fukushima Medical University offered health counseling to city residents, including those in the evacuation area [29]. It is possible that Fukushima residents acquired knowledge about radiation, thus reducing their radiation risk perception, as a result of these activities.
In addition, the results of model verification on groups having different levels of perception of reconstruction status suggested that when people believe that reconstruction is in progress, “health/social knowledge” is highly likely to reduce “radiation risk perception.” Furthermore, the path from “physical knowledge” to “radiation risk perception” was significantly positive only in the NP group. From these findings, in order to reduce radiation risk perception, it is considered important first to know the reconstruction status.
Relationship between knowledge and information sources
The results of knowledge differences due to information sources indicated that factor scores of “health/social knowledge” were higher in Cluster 3 and Cluster 7, which relied for information on administrative websites and public relations materials issued by municipalities. This suggests that administrative agencies and local governments provide more information, or that other sources do not provide sufficient information on health effects. Murakami et al. showed trust in central government as information sources contributed negatively to radiation risk perception [4]. People who trusted central government might have a low radiation risk perception by getting information about “health/social knowledge” from the central government. Regarding the reconstruction status of Fukushima Prefecture, many respondents in the group not obtaining information on reconstruction status did not think that reconstruction was proceeding. This indicated that a certain number of people only have information that was initially provided immediately after the accidents at TEPCO’s Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear Power Station.
In conclusion, to raise “perception concerning reconstruction-related behavior,” it is necessary to increase “health/social knowledge” and to reduce “radiation risk perception.” Also, because “radiation risk perception” is reduced by “health/social knowledge,” it is important to obtain “health/social knowledge” both directly and indirectly. In addition, it was found that groups who use information from administrative agencies were likely to have “health/social knowledge.” From this, two approaches are suggested to enhance “health/social knowledge.” First, more opportunities should be provided for many people to access administrative information. It may be useful for administrative agencies to have several channels with various media and people can reach administrative information indirectly. Second, more information related to “health/social knowledge” should be disseminated by other media. Little information on health effects attributed to radiation was contained in TV programs a month after the accident [30]. It seemed to have a big influence that TV, newspapers, and magazines used by many people as information sources gave “health/social knowledge.”
In order to reduce “radiation risk perception,” that is, to eliminate misunderstandings concerning the health impacts of radiation, post-accident information should be constantly updated as well as knowledge on the progress of reconstruction activities, rather than conveying basic physical information on radiation. In addition, it would be effective to convey social knowledge, such as information on the health effects of radiation and food inspections. Communicating only physical basic knowledge of radiation without health/social knowledge to people who do not think that reconstruction is progressing may increase radiation risk perception. In particular, Cluster 2 (did not particularly obtain information) showed the lowest health/social knowledge and perception of reconstruction. This cluster has the largest number of people and a third of the people in all clusters. In order to eliminate misunderstandings of the health effects of radiation, it would be particularly important to update information for people who have no particular information on the reconstruction status of Fukushima Prefecture.
Limitations of this study and future perspectives
This study had some limitations. The first limitation is a potential participant bias. We used an online questionnaire survey, which may introduce biases. However, participants with no interest in the topic may have been motivated to respond to the questionnaire by receiving reward points. This may be more effective for reducing biases than mail surveys or central location testing. In addition, there were insignificant differences in risk perception of nuclear power plants between interview and online survey [31].
Second, we conducted the survey more than six years after the accident, and this was not a crisis, but a post-crisis situation, namely recovery situation. There is room for consideration as to whether our findings can be used for risk communication in a crisis situation.
Third, this was a cross-sectional study and causality was not necessarily clarified. In order to determine it, a cohort study or an intervention study needs to be conducted. After the accident, content about radiation was taught at some high schools and junior high schools proactively, and attention should be paid to its effects.
Despite these limitations, we found that the relationship between knowledge and risk perception varied among types of knowledge and that “health/social knowledge” might be effective for promoting “perception concerning reconstruction-related behavior” and for lowering “radiation risk perception.” In addition, people who used administrative agencies as information sources were likely to have much information about “health/social knowledge.” These findings would be particularly useful for risk communication in a recovery situation.
Supporting information
S1 Fig. Parallel analysis and eigenvalues of factor analysis.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221561.s001
(PDF)
S2 Table. Pearson’s correlation coefficient between options of knowledge.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221561.s004
(PDF)
S3 Table. Arithmetic mean and standard deviation of observed variables (Total).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221561.s005
(PDF)
S4 Table. Pearson’s correlation coefficient between observed variables (Total).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221561.s006
(PDF)
S5 Table. Arithmetic mean and standard deviation of observed variables (P group).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221561.s007
(PDF)
S6 Table. Pearson’s correlation coefficient between observed variables (P group).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221561.s008
(PDF)
S7 Table. Arithmetic mean and standard deviation of observed variables (NP group).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221561.s009
(PDF)
S8 Table. Pearson’s correlation coefficient between observed variables (NP group).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221561.s010
(PDF)
S9 Table. Multiple regression analysis regarding “health/social knowledge”.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221561.s011
(PDF)
References
- 1.
Fukushima Prefecture; c2014 [cited 2018 Dec 20], https://www.pref.fukushima.lg.jp/sec/11045b/10sihyo.html. [in Japanese]
- 2. Murakami M, Oki T. Estimated dietary intake of radionuclides and health risks for the citizens of Fukushima City, Tokyo, and Osaka after the 2011 nuclear accident. Plos One. 2014; 9: e112791. pmid:25390339
- 3. Tsubokura M, Kato S, Nomura S, Gilmour S, Nihei M, Sakuma Y, et al. Reduction of high levels of internal radio-contamination by dietary intervention in residents of areas affected by the Fukushima Daiichi nuclear plant disaster: A case series. Plos One. 2014; 9: e100302. pmid:24932486
- 4. Murakami M, Nakatani J, Oki T. Evaluation of risk perception and risk-comparison information regarding dietary radionuclides after the 2011 Fukushima nuclear power plant accident. Plos One. 2016; 11: e0165594. pmid:27802304
- 5.
Igarashi Y. Genpatsu jiko to syoku -shijo, communication, sabetsu-, Chuko shinsyo. 2018;
- 6.
Mitsubishi Research Institute, Inc. Fukushima Reconstruction: Current status and Radiation Health Risks; [cited 2018 Dec 20], https://www.mri.co.jp/opinion/column/trend/trend_20171226.html.
- 7.
Radiation Medical Science Center for the Fukushima Health Management Survey, The 32nd Prefectural Oversight Committee Meeting for Fukushima Health Management Survey; [cited 2019 Jan 10]. http://fmu-global.jp/survey/the-32st-prefectural-oversight-committee-meeting-for-fukushima-health-management-survey/
- 8. Takebayashi Y, Lyamzina Y, Suzuki Y, Murakami M. Risk perception and anxiety regarding radiation after the 2011 Fukushima nuclear power plant accident: A systematic qualitative review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017; 14: 1306.
- 9. Kudo D. and Nakayachi K. Reputational risk caused by The Great East Japan Earthquake: A study on consumer factors leading to restrained buying, Japanese Journal of Social Psychology. 2014; 30(1), 35–44.
- 10. Yoshida K; Orita M, Goto A, Kumagai A, Yasui K, Ohtsuru A, et al. Radiation-related anxiety among public health nurses in the Fukushima Prefecture after the accident at the Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear Power Station: A cross-sectional study. BMJ Open. 2016; 6, e013564. pmid:27798037
- 11. Hino Y, Murakami M, Midorikawa S, Ohtsuru A, Suzuki S, Tsuboi K, et al. Explanatory Meetings on Thyroid Examination for the “Fukushima Health Management Survey” after the Great East Japan Earthquake: Reduction of Anxiety and Improvement of Comprehension. Tohoku J. Exp. Med. 2016; 239, 333–343. pmid:27535010
- 12. Asako M, Takashi K, Kanako O. Four-wave panel survey on attitudes towards foodstuffs from areas contaminated by the Fukushima Daiichi nuclear disaster, Japanese Journal of Social Psychology. 2016; 32(1), 10–21.
- 13. Niiyama Y. The public psychology and an interactive risk communication on effects of radiation contamination of food on health: scrutinizing information necessary for acquisition of knowledge, Journal of Rural Problems. 2012; 188, 345–354.
- 14. Takenishi A, Takahashi K. The psychological process in the safety evaluation of fresh vegetables: memories that threatened the safety function as the availability heuristic, Journal of agricultural policy research. 2009; 16, 1–19.
- 15. Oki S, Nakayachi K. Paradoxical effects of the record-high Tsunamis caused by the 2011 Tohoku Earthquake on public judgments of danger. Int J Disaster Risk Reduction. 2012; 2: 37–45.
- 16. Lindell MK, Barnes VE. Protective response to technological emergency: Risk perception and behavioral intention. Nuclear Safety. 1986; 27: 457–467.
- 17. Suzuki Y, Yabe H, Yasumura S, Ohira T, Niwa S, Ohtsuru A, et al. Psychological distress and the perception of radiation risks: the Fukushima health management survey. Bull World Health Organ. 2015; 93, 598–605. pmid:26478623
- 18. Yoshida K, Shinkawa T, Urata H, Nakashima K, Orita M, Yasui K, et al. Psychological distress of residents in Kawauchi village, Fukushima Prefecture after the accident at Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear Power Station: the Fukushima Health Management Survey. PeerJ. 2016; 4, e2353. pmid:27635326
- 19. Murakami M, Hirosaki M, Suzuki Y, Maeda M, Yabe , Yasumura S, et al. Reduction of radiation-related anxiety promoted wellbeing after the 2011 disaster: "Fukushima Health Management Survey". Journal of Radiological Protection. 2018; 38(4), 1428–1440. pmid:30375359
- 20. Suzuki Y, Takebayashi Y, Yasumura S, Murakami M, Harigane M, Yabe H, et al. Changes in risk perception of the health effects of radiation and mental health status: The Fukushima Health Management Survey. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018; 15(6), 1219. pmid:29890768
- 21. Slovic P. Perception of risk. Science. 1987; 236: 280–285. pmid:3563507
- 22. Imai H, Okumiya K, Fukutomi E, Wada T, Ishimoto Y, Kimura Y, et al. Association between risk perception, subjective knowledge, and depression in community- dwelling elderly people in Japan. Psychiatry Res. 2015; 227, 27–31. pmid:25813776
- 23. Perko T, Zeleznik N, Turcanu C, Thijssen P. Is knowledge important? Empirical research on nuclear risk communication in two countries. Health Phys. 2012; 102, 614–625. pmid:22570920
- 24.
Consumer Affairs Agency, Government of Japan. Fuhyo higai ni kansuru shohisya isiki no jittaityousa (dai 11 kai) ni tuite; 2018 [cited 2018 Dec 20]. http://www.caa.go.jp/disaster/earthquake/understanding_food_and_radiation/pdf/understanding_food_and_radiation_180307_0003.pdf.
- 25. Hu Li-tze, Bentler Peter M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives, Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 1999; 6:1, 1–55.
- 26. Kahan D.M, Braman D, Gastil J, Slovic P, Mertz C.K. Culture and identity-protective cognition: explaining the white-male effect in risk perception. J. Empir. Leg. Stud. 2007; 4, 465–505.
- 27. Kellens W, Zaalberg R, De Maeyer P. The informed society: An analysis of the public’s information seeking behavior regarding coastal flood risks. Risk Analysis. 2012; 32 (8), 1369–1381. pmid:22150511
- 28. Suzuki Y, Takebayashi Y, Yasumura S, Murakami M, Harigane M, Yabe H, et al. Changes in risk perception of the health effects of radiation and mental health status: The Fukushima Health Management Survey, International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018; 15(6), 1219.
- 29.
Education Center for Disaster Medicine, Fukushima Medical University, Yorozu Soudan Jigyou [cited 2019 July 22]. https://www.fmu.ac.jp/home/cmecd/ecdm/yorozu1.html
- 30. Numada M, Meguro K. Analysis of a prime time of TV program after the 2011 Tohoku earthquake. Seisan kenkyu. 2013; 65(4), 373–377.
- 31. Kihikawa H, Murayama R, Gujinaga A, Uchiyama I. The Validity of Web Survey on Risk Perception and Environmental Problem. Japanese Journal of Risk Analysis. 2018; 28(1), 3–11.