Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Evolutionary relationships of courtship songs in the parasitic wasp genus, Cotesia (Hymenoptera: Braconidae)

  • Justin P. Bredlau,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    Affiliations Department of Biology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, Virginia, United States of America, Integrative Life Sciences, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, Virginia, United States of America

  • Karen M. Kester

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    kmkester@vcu.edu

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, Virginia, United States of America

Abstract

Acoustic signals play an important role in premating isolation based on sexual selection within many taxa. Many male parasitic wasps produce characteristic courtship songs used by females in mate selection. In Cotesia (Hymenoptera: Braconidae: Microgastrinae), courtship songs are generated by wing fanning with repetitive pulses in stereotypical patterns. Our objectives were to sample the diversity of courtship songs within Cotesia and to identify e underlying patterns of differentiation. We compared songs among 12 of ca. 80 Cotesia species in North America, including ten species that have not been recorded previously. For Cotesia congregata, we compared songs of wasps originating from six different host-foodplant sources, two of which are considered incipient species. Songs of emergent males from wild caterpillar hosts in five different families were recorded, and pattern, frequency, and duration of song elements analyzed. Principal component analysis converted the seven elements characterized into four uncorrelated components used in a hierarchical cluster analysis and grouped species by similarity of song structure. Species songs varied significantly in duration of repeating pulse and buzz elements and/or in fundamental frequency. Cluster analysis resolved similar species groups in agreement with the most recent molecular phylogeny for Cotesia spp., indicating the potential for using courtship songs as a predictor of genetic relatedness. Courtship song analysis may aid in identifying closely related cryptic species that overlap spatially, and provide insight into the evolution of this highly diverse and agriculturally important taxon.

Introduction

Acoustic signals are used by diverse groups of insects for species recognition, fitness displays, and courtship elicitation. Songs used during insect courtship are generally stereotypical within a species and likely play a role in reproductive isolation. Moreover, courtship songs may be a useful identifying character, especially among cryptic or closely related species [1]. For example, songs of Drosophila species groups are species-specific and have been studied for evolutionary patterns [25]. Furthermore, courtship song analyses have been used in conjunction with genetic, behavioral, and morphological data to reconstruct ancestral songs (e.g., grasshoppers [6]), to revise phylogenies (e.g., a genus of katydids [7]), and to identify cryptic species (e.g., lacewings [8,9] and sand flies [10]). In highly diverse taxa of parasitic wasps, acoustic signals may play a significant role in species differentiation and reproductive isolation.

Male parasitic wasps across multiple families produce wing fanning (also termed “wing vibration”) in response to female olfactory signaling [1118]. Wing fanning draws air and pheromones over olfactory organs for orientation to the female [19] and likely acts as a display of male fitness. Wing fanning at different amplitudes and velocities generates sound patterns with sound frequency corresponding to wing beat frequency [20]. Wing fanning generates substrate vibrations that are detected by nearby wasps, and substrate type effects mating success [21,22]. Male wing fanning is a necessary precursor for successful mating in many species. For example, in the aphid parasitoid, Lysiphlebus testaceipes (Braconidae: Aphidiinae), females mate only after wing fanning and are more likely to mate with males producing higher frequency and higher amplitude wing movement [23].

Wing fanning by parasitic wasps produces patterns of repeating pulses or buzzes, which together comprise a courtship song. For example, five genera of dipteran parasitoids in the subfamily Opiinae (Braconidae) produce songs with short repeating pulses of 40–200 ms at a frequency of 128–190 Hz [2426]. The aphid parasitoid, Aphidius ervi (Braconidae: Aphidiinae), produces repeating pulses lasting ~200 ms at 180 Hz with a ~200 ms pause between pulses [27]. Parasitoids of lepidopteran larvae within the Microgastrinae (Braconidae) produce courtship songs that are formed from a combination of low-amplitude and high-amplitude elements corresponding to changes in frequency [24]. For example, Glyptapanteles flavicoxis produces songs that consist of low-amplitude “percussion clicks” from wing vibrations followed by higher amplitude wingbeats increasing in frequency before transitioning back [28]. Considering estimates of 17,000–48,000+ species in Microgastrinae [29], the diversity and evolution of song patterns is almost entirely unknown. Investigating multiple courtship songs within one diverse genus that includes closely related cryptic species, phylogenetic data, and well-characterized model species would provide insight into the general patterns, diversity, and evolution of wasp courtship songs.

The large Microgastrinae genus Cotesia contains several species that have served as globally-important biocontrol agents of agricultural pests and as model systems for understanding host-parasitoid and tri-trophic interactions. For example, Cotesia sesamiae is a major biocontrol agent of maize stemborers in Africa, and long-term studies have revealed patterns driving host-associated specialization and co-evolution of virulent bracovirus genes [30]. Cotesia rubecula and C. glomerata, both successfully introduced to parts of North America to control the imported cabbageworm, Pieris rapae (e.g. [31]), have served as models for parasitoid behavior [13,32]. Cotesia congregata is a model system for studying tri-trophic interactions [33,34], insect learning [3537], insect immunology [38,39], and the genomics of symbiotic bracoviruses [40,41]. Courtship behavior of some Cotesia species has been studied to improve mass rearing in biological control programs (e.g. [21,42]). However, most species remain taxonomically undescribed and limited information is available beyond descriptions, host usage, and ranges for the majority of described species.

Courtship songs have been characterized in detail for four species of Cotesia [20,24,43]; however, no comparisons have been made among distantly related species, and the songs of most species clusters remain unknown. The most recent phylogeny of Cotesia based on four genes contains nineteen Cotesia species, several of which are common and well-studied [44]. This phylogeny provides a basic evolutionary framework for comparing courtship songs in Cotesia. Moreover, C. congregata, which is reported to parasitize at least fourteen Sphingidae species that feed on different plant families [45,46], offers an opportunity to compare courtship songs among multiple host-foodplant complex sources. Wasps from two of these host-foodplant complex sources, Manduca sexta on tobacco (“MsT”) and Ceratomia catalpae on catalpa (“CcC”), have diverged genetically and are likely incipient species [47]. These wasps display a lower male response rate to the female pheromones of the reciprocal source, slight differences in duration and frequency of some song elements, and typically produce sterile hybrid females resulting from CcC♂xMsT♀ crosses [15]. In this study we describe the courtship songs of ten additional species of Cotesia, and use clustering to explore the relationships and patterns among songs. Further, we identify song differences among select host-associated populations and incipient species of C. congregata.

Materials and methods

Parasitic wasp collection

Cotesia spp. were primarily collected from wild caterpillar hosts at multiple sites in the United States; some C. nr. phobetri came from an ongoing laboratory colony (Table 1). Caterpillars known to be hosts of Cotesia were targeted for collection, particularly Cotesia that have published gene sequences. When possible, wasps from different sites were collected for wider population sampling. In most cases, each wasp species came from a single host species. In contrast, C. congregata were collected from six different sphingid host species feeding on different plant families (Table 1). All permissions were obtained as necessary for field collections on both public and private land from property owners and managers. None of the species involved are listed as endangered or protected.

thumbnail
Table 1. Cotesia species collected, host-foodplant complex (for C. congregata), lepidopteran host names, collection locations (county/city, state; lat, long, datum: WGS84), and number of wasp broods collected at each site.

Cotesia congregata is divided into six host-foodplant complexes (H-FPC) abbreviated by host species and plant name (tobacco, catalpa, holly/pawpaw, Virginia creeper, and privet, respectively).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.t001

Caterpillars, usually collected before parasitization status was known, were reared on their host plant in plastic containers under ambient laboratory conditions until parasitoid egression or pupation. Individual unattached Cotesia cocoons were placed in clear gel capsules (size 00) 2–4 days after egression. Cotesia species forming a connected cocoon mass were chilled upon adult emergence and placed in individual capsules or vials. Adults were sexed under a dissecting microscope. Wasp songs were recorded within 24 hours of emergence. Voucher samples of each species were both point pinned and stored in 95% EtOH at -20°C. The song of one species included in analysis (Cotesia marginiventris) was obtained from a USDA-ARS sound library (https://www.ars.usda.gov/ARSUserFiles/3559/soundlibrary.html) and originally described by Sivinski & Webb [24].

Audio recordings

Males in capsules were randomly selected from each brood for recording. Individuals were placed in an open paper arena with a drop of honey as a food source to encourage them to stay in the arena. Courtship songs were induced by exposing individual males to an immobilized female of the same species. Songs were recorded using a miniature omnidirectional microphone (model 4060, DPA, Longmont, CO; 20–20,000 Hz) positioned 5–7 mm above the male and a high resolution digital audio recorder (model 702, Sound Devices, Reedsburg, WI; 48 kHz sampling rate, 24 bit resolution) in a sound isolation booth (Industrial Acoustics, Bronx, NY) at 23 ± 1.5°C and 40–55% RH. Generally, one recording per brood was analyzed; however, recordings of different individuals were analyzed for species with fewer than four collected broods. Additional individuals of C. congregata were recorded to test for relatively small differences among host-foodplant sources. Additional C. nr. phobetri were recorded from each brood because they could not initially be identified to a known species. Duration of song elements and fundamental frequency were quantified using Raven Pro v1.3 [48]. Waveforms were high-pass filtered at 100 Hz to reduce background noise.

Songs were divided into multiple elements based on acoustic characteristics shared across species (Fig 1). “Pauses” occur between other song elements when wings are held motionless above the body and do not generate sound. “Pulses” are high amplitude elements comprising the greatest range of wing movement, referred to as “boings” in C. congregata [20]. “Terminal buzzes” immediately follow pulses and consist of continuous lower-amplitude sounds. “Pre-pulse buzzes” are steady lower amplitude sounds that precede a pulse with no pause in between. For “pulse-buzz units” we measured the duration of the pulse and buzz together. “Interpulse interval” is the time from the start of one high-amplitude pulse to the start of the next. Pulses, buzzes, and pauses make up the pulse-buzz units and interpulse interval, which were included because they make discrete units that may be important in species recognition. Song amplitude was not compared because distance from the microphone varied slightly among some species. Quantification of song elements started with the second complete pulse-buzz cycle and continued for six complete pulse-buzz cycles. Spectrograms of the entire song were produced using a short-time Fourier transform (Hann window, size = 2,000 samples, 50% overlap). Frequency spectra for song sections were calculated using fast Fourier transforms (Hann window, size = 1,000 samples, 50% overlap). Frequency of the first harmonic (fundamental frequency) was used in all comparisons.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Courtship songs of Cotesia species divided into acoustic elements based on relative amplitude and position for use in analysis and comparisons.

Represented are typical waveform segments of (A) C. congregata and (B) C. flavichonchae.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.g001

Comparisons and analysis

Several statistical procedures were used to determine differences in courtship songs, consolidate elements, and group songs by structure. The considerable differences among some songs present a challenge for direct comparisons using standard statistical tests. For example, some elements were drastically reduced or absent in some species. For statistical tests, the repeating song elements were averaged so that each individual wasp was treated as an N of 1. Where appropriate in similar species, duration and fundamental frequency were compared using analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Tukey’s post-hoc test. Song element durations were log transformed to meet the assumptions of ANOVA. Pulse and terminal buzz frequency were compared between adjacent elements of each song for each species and all species together using linear regression.

Principal components analysis (PCA) for each individual wasp was used to condense dimensionality of the data into principal components (PCs) based on communitive explained variance. Hierarchical cluster analysis (Ward method) of mean principal components of each species was used to group wasps. The resulting dendrogram was compared to the most recent molecular phylogeny of Cotesia [44] to determine if the same groups were resolved (see Table 2 for genes used and GenBank ascension numbers of species included in this study).

thumbnail
Table 2. Available genes and GenBank ascension numbers of Cotesia species used in this study.

Not all species have been sequenced using all four genes, and therefore are not included in the phylogeny.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.t002

Subsequent PCAs were performed on six different host-foodplant complex sources of C. congregata, on the MsT and CcC incipient species, and the two geographically isolated sources of C. nr. phobetri. Sources that displayed separation in the PCAs were followed by a Welch’s unequal variance t-test to compare duration and frequency of song elements. The feasibility of matching songs back to their species or population based on song elements and PCs was tested using linear discriminant analysis. All statistical and multivariate analyses were performed with JMP v11 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).

Results

Description of wasp songs

Eleven species of Cotesia were collected at different sites in the United States (Table 1); one additional species was from a published recording (C. marginiventris). Some target host species collected did not yield any Cotesia. Multiple broods were collected of each species except for the uncommon C. teleae, which came from a single Antheraea polyphemus larva that produced only one living male concurrently with females. Cotesia congregata were collected from six different sphingid host species. Cotesia nr. phobetri, (currently undescribed) were supplied from a laboratory colony originating from host Grammia incorrupta (formerly G. geneura) that feed on forbs in grassland habitat and found in Redington Pass, Pima County, AZ [49]. Initially unknown specimens found as cocoon clusters independent of hosts in a recently mowed horse pasture in Gloucester County, VA, were identified to be either C. nr. phobetri or a closely related sister species based on morphology, cocoon structure, similarity of habitat, and song structure (Discussion, 8th paragraph). Unparasitized caterpillars of Grammia virgo were found in an adjacent field, which suggest this species is the probable host. The other Cotesia species in our study were each collected from a single host species.

All twelve species of Cotesia generated songs by wing fanning that consisted of repeating high-amplitude pulses and, in most species, lower amplitude buzzes (Fig 2). Pulses were accompanied by abdominal movements. All males continued to produce songs until copulation or the female moved away; therefore, number of pulses was not used as a factor. Songs varied in duration of pulse, buzz, and pause elements (Table 3). Some songs were different enough from the others as to not require statistical comparisons of individual elements, e.g., C. flavicochae produces a song with unique elements. Moreover, songs with considerably longer element durations had greater variance than songs with relatively short element durations (e.g., compare pause duration between C. teleae and C. congregata in Table 3), resulting in violation of the assumptions for ANOVA. Although an ANOVA could not be performed with all 12 species, an ANOVA was performed with four species that had song elements of similar durations and frequencies. Among all species, fundamental frequency ranged from 176 Hz in C. phobetri to 328 Hz in C. euchaetis (Table 4). All songs produced detectable harmonics up to 4–5 kHz for pulses and 1–2 kHz for buzzes (Fig 2). Analysis of individual elements was useful for comparing similar or sister species but of limited use for comparisons across all species. Courtship songs were divided into groups using a combination of element duration and frequency, and general patterns rather than focusing on individual song elements.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Waveforms and spectrograms of typical courtship songs of twelve species of Cotesia.

Species are labeled by the first two letters of the species name.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.g002

thumbnail
Table 3. Duration (ms; mean ± SE) of male courtship song elements among species of Cotesia.

Cotesia congregata is divided by host-foodplant complex. N = number of individual wasps analyzed.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Fundamental frequency (Hz; mean ± SE) of male courtship song elements among species of Cotesia.

Cotesia congregata is divided by host-foodplant complex. N = number of individual wasps analyzed.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.t004

The most common courtship song structure included pause-pulse-buzz elements repeating ca 2–3 times a second. A subset of four species with similar pause-pulse-buzz patterns was used to discern more nuanced differences. The songs differed in either duration of interpulse intervals and component elements (ANOVA: F3,37 = 8.03, p = 0.0003; Fig 3A) or fundamental frequency (ANOVA: F3,37 = 46.08, p < 0.0001; Fig 3B). In C. congregata, the courtship songs consisted of an initial buzz followed by repeating pulse-buzz elements with a short (20–26 ms) pause, followed by a pulse (“boing”) that decays into a buzz [20]. Courtship songs of C. phobetri and C. euchaetis had similar patterns, although terminal buzzes after pulses were longer than in C. congregata. Cotesia nr. phobetri songs were similar to those of C. euchaetis but with shorter terminal buzzes. Cotesia marginiventris songs had pulse-buzzes that varied more in duration, producing a warble sound [21,24]. The song of C. marginiventris did not contain the discrete high-amplitude pulses present in C. congregata, C. phobetri, and C. euchaetis, but otherwise followed a similar pattern. Cotesia glomerata was similar but lacked discrete pauses between pulse-buzz elements and had a sudden, <50 ms, power spike at the start of each pulse that was not observed in other species (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 3. (A) Mean (± SE) duration of interpulse interval divided into pause (bottom), pulse (middle), and terminal buzz (top) elements and (B) fundamental frequency for four Cotesia species with similar patterns of song structure in the “rubecula” phylogenetic group.

Letters indicate significant differences (ANOVA followed by Tukey’s post-hoc test, p < 0.05). Species with similar time elements differ in frequency.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.g003

Three species produced courtship songs that consisted of rapid repeating pulses without long terminal buzzes. Cotesia empretiae produced a pulse train of ~2 seconds that consisted of short repeating pulses with different durations. Cotesia diacrisiae produced rapid short pulses at a rate of four per second. Cotesia orobenae also produced rapid repeating pulses that were shorter in duration and had longer pauses compared to C. diacrisiae. These three species could be readily distinguished by waveform patterns (Fig 2).

Three species produced songs with long pauses between pulses. Cotesia rubecula produced songs with pulses similar in duration and pacing to those of C. phobetri and C. congregata but lacked a terminal buzz. Cotesia teleae produced a pulse-buzz element with pauses that were about five times longer than those of the C. congregata group. The C. flaviconchae courtship song was substantially different from other Cotesia songs. It was the only species that produced songs with a buzz before the high-amplitude pulse (“pre-pulse buzz”) lasting 486 ± 32 ms at 286 ± 2 Hz. The buzz-pulse repeated every 4–9 seconds, while all other songs repeated in less than a second.

Analysis of all species

Songs from all 12 Cotesia species were grouped using PCA and cluster analysis. Frequency of adjacent pulse and terminal buzz elements could not be accurately calculated in all sections of songs produced by some species due to short durations of one of these elements (e.g., C. empretiae); however, adjacent pulses and buzzes were correlated in song sections containing sufficiently long durations of both elements (r2 = 0.65, d.f. = 770, p < 0.0001; Pulse = -31.0 + 1.1*TBuzz; Fig 4). Therefore, frequency was consolidated into one term by using the “pulse-buzz unit” frequency in the PCA. The PCA using the seven song elements resulted in four PCs explaining 91.7% of total variance. PC1 was best represented by duration of the pulse-buzz unit, interpulse interval, and pre-pulse buzz, PC2 by frequency, PC3 almost entirely by pause duration, and PC4 by frequency, pause duration, and pulse duration (Table 5). The factor scores of the first four PCs differed significantly among some, but not all, species (ANOVA: F10,61 = 83.7, 32.7, 17.1, 27.1 respectively; p < 0.0001; all pairwise comparisons not shown). For example, C. flaviconchae differs significantly from all other species by PC1 and PC2, whereas species with highly similar song patterns such as C. congregata and C. phobetri may only differ by one PC (in this case PC4) but not the others.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Scatterplot of fundamental frequency (Hz) of adjacent pulse and terminal buzz elements in courtship songs across ten Cotesia species and six host-foodplant sources of C. congregata.

Pulses and buzzes were generally correlated in song sections containing both elements (all species, linear regression: r2 = 0.65, p < 0.0001; Pulse = -31.0 + 1.1*TBuzz).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.g004

thumbnail
Table 5. Component matrix (eigenvectors), eigenvalues, and explained variance resulting from principal components analysis of male courtship songs from twelve species of Cotesia.

Parameters strongly associated with song elements are bold.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.t005

Differences in one or more PCs among species indicated groups with most species forming close clusters with some overlap (Fig 5). Species separated from the main cluster and containing relatively long duration elements had greater variance in PCs (e.g., C. flaviconchae and C. rubecula). Hierarchical cluster analysis of species using the first four PC mean factor scores resolved four main groups (Fig 6). Group 1 consists of wasps with short rapid pulses, group 2 with pulses and terminal buzzes, group 3 with long pauses between pulses, and group 4 of only C. flavichonchae with a thus far unique song pattern. Groupings from the cluster analysis generally reflect genetic groups [44], with the exception of C. rubecula which lacks the terminal buzz found in all other related species in the “rubecula” group (Fig 7).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Scatterplots of factors of a principal components analysis of seven Cotesia courtship song elements.

(A) PC1 vs. PC2, (B) PC1 vs PC3, (C) PC3 vs PC2. Each data point represents a single individual coded by species. (D) 3-dimensional plot of the mean PC of each species color-coded by dominant courtship song pattern (red—rapid pulses; green—pulse-buzz; blue—long pauses; orange—long buzz-pulse). Letter codes indicate species name. Cotesia congregata is represented by the MsT host-foodplant complex. Some species are separated from the others by one or more PCs; most species form a close cluster with some overlap.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.g005

thumbnail
Fig 6. Hierarchical clustering using the first four principal components of seven courtship song elements to group Cotesia species.

Each species is represented by a 2-second waveform section.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.g006

thumbnail
Fig 7. Cotesia courtship songs of representative species overlaid on a maximum likelihood tree from the analysis of four genes [44].

Recorded species are in ovals and colors match those in the dendrogram produced by hierarchical cluster analysis using principal components of song elements. Cotesia diacrisiae has been added as a sister species of C. empretiae based on analysis of the NADH1 gene. Nodes have bootstrap values (above, 100 replicates) and Baysian posterior probabilities (below). Non-Cotesia outgroups have been removed for clarity (rooted with Chelonus inanitus). Courtship song grouping generally follows the genetic phylogeny with the exception of C. rubecula. The molecular phylogeny was used and modified with permission of J. Whitfield, University of Illinois, USA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.g007

Differentiation of C. congregata host-foodplant complex sources

Songs from the different C. congregata host-foodplant complex sources could not be distinguished by courtship songs alone. The PCA using the six song elements present in C. congregata resulted in three PCs explaining 88.8% of total variance and four PCs explaining 99.9% of total variance. PC1 was most represented by pulse-buzz unit duration, interpulse interval, and terminal buzz duration, PC2 by pause duration and frequency, PC3 by pulse duration, and PC4 by frequency (Table 6). A PCA of the two geographic sources of MsT and the one source of CcC host-foodplant complexes produced a similar component matrix (Table 7). High overlap of every PC prevents discrimination of host-plant complex sources (Fig 8) and geographically separated populations (Fig 9), even if means of some elements and the first threes PCs differ significantly between some groups (ANOVA: p < 0.001).

thumbnail
Fig 8. Scatterplots of principal components resulting from principal components analysis of courtship song elements produced by six host-foodplant complex sources of Cotesia congregata.

(A) PC1 vs. PC2, (B) PC1 vs PC3, (C) PC3 vs PC2. Songs from different host-plant complex sources cannot be reliable distinguished from each other.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.g008

thumbnail
Fig 9. Scatterplots of principal components resulting from principal components analysis of courtship song elements produced by MsT and CcC host-foodplant complex sources of Cotesia congregata.

(A) PC1 vs. PC2, (B) PC1 vs PC3, (C) PC3 vs PC2. MsT wasps originated from two sources in Virginia and Indiana. Songs from different host-plant complex sources or different locations cannot be reliably distinguished from each other.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.g009

thumbnail
Table 6. Component matrix (eigenvectors), eigenvalues, and explained variance resulting from principal components analysis of male courtship songs from host-foodplant complex sources of Cotesia congregata.

Parameters strongly associated with song elements are bold.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.t006

thumbnail
Table 7. Component matrix (eigenvectors), eigenvalues, and explained variance resulting from principal components analysis of male courtship songs from MsT and CcC host-foodplant complex sources of Cotesia congregata.

MsT are divided into wasps originating from Virginia and Indiana. Parameters strongly associated with song elements are bold.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.t007

Differentiation of C. nr. phobetri by location

Songs from the two sources of C. nr. phobetri could be clearly distinguished by courtship songs. The PCA using the six song elements present in C. nr. phobetri resulted in three PCs explaining 94.5% of total variance and the fourth PC explaining the remaining variance. PC1 was most represented by duration of pulse and buzz components, PC2 by pause duration and frequency, PC3 by pause duration, and PC4 by pulse duration (Table 8). The C. nr. phobetri populations from Virginia and Arizona can be reliably distinguished by PC1 (pulse and buzz durations) but not the other PCs (pause duration and frequency) (Fig 10). Linear discriminant analysis sorts wasps by population with 100% accuracy. Mean duration of the pulse and buzz components were longer in songs of wasps originating in Arizona than Virginia (unequal variance t-test, p < 0.001), with mean pulse-buzz unit duration 0.13 s longer (t13 = -8.6, p < 0.0001). Overall song pattern and structure remained the same in wasps from both populations and were more similar to each other than to the other wasp species analyzed (Figs 4 and 5).

thumbnail
Fig 10. Scatterplots of principal components resulting from principal components analysis of courtship song elements produced by Cotesia nr. phobetri originating in Arizona and Virginia.

(A) PC1 vs. PC2, (B) PC1 vs PC3, (C) PC3 vs PC2. Songs from these two locations can be reliably distinguished from each other based on PC1 (active song element durations), but not PC2 or PC3 (frequency and pause duration). Mean pulse-buzz duration is 0.13 s longer in songs of wasps from Arizona vs. Virginia (t13 = -8.6, p < 0.0001).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.g010

thumbnail
Table 8. Component matrix (eigenvectors), eigenvalues, and explained variance resulting from principal components analysis of male courtship songs from two sources of Cotesia nr. phobetri: Virginia and Arizona.

Parameters strongly associated with song elements are bold.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.t008

Discussion

Courtship songs of the twelve species of Cotesia presented in this study were variable yet distinguishable. Songs were characterized quantitatively by dividing songs into elements that were shared across most species. Principal components analysis was used to reduce dimensionality of correlated song elements. Species grouped using hierarchical cluster analysis corresponded to groups in the genetic phylogeny [44], with one exception. Some species that have not been sequenced can be provisionally placed into these pre-identified groups based on courtship song characteristics. Likewise, the general pattern of courtship songs may be predicted for species that have been placed within a phylogeny but were not found and recorded in this study. However, the relationships among the groups do not correspond strictly to the genetic phylogeny and the large differences among groups make their placement difficult. Differentiation within C. congregata by host-foodplant complex was not reliable; in contrast, C. nr. phobetri could be identified by source. Courtship song analysis has potential use for the systematics of this genus.

Songs were generally unique to each species and could be distinguished by waveform and frequency characteristics. All songs consisted of repeating pulse and/or buzz elements generated from wing fanning, which is common among parasitic wasps. The song pattern was stereotypical for each species (Fig 2 and supplemental audio). Notably, songs of all species had overlapping ranges in the duration and frequency of some elements (Fig 5). Songs of some species with relatively longer elements (e.g., the pause element produced by C. rubecula) were more variable. The species representing the “rubecula” group produced the most similar song patterns (short pause, pulse, terminal buzz) as indicated by tight clustering in the PCA analysis (Fig 5). Songs of some species in this group differed only in the duration or frequency of one or a few elements (Fig 3). Although we attempted to capture the primary acoustic variation among species, some unique characteristics were not integrated into the PCA, e.g., the pre-pulse power spike produced by C. glomerata and the warble sound produced by C. marginiventris, which can be used to easily distinguish these species from others with otherwise similar song patterns. We do not rule out that some as yet unrecorded species may produce songs so similar to a sister species as to not be distinguishable, as was the case among the host-foodplant complexes of C. congregata. Moreover, playback experiments are necessary to determine whether wasps can distinguish between songs of closely related species. Importantly, some clustering of species was expected given the morphological and genetic similarity of many members of Cotesia.

Phylogenetics and taxonomy of the Microgastrinae are active areas of study [50]. Most work has been at the subfamily or genus level [5154], on closely related species clusters or cryptic species [47,5557], or by determining the evolutionary relationship with symbiotic viruses [5861]. Considering the diversity of Cotesia and the difficulty of producing high-resolution phylogenies, not all of the same genes or all common species have yet been sequenced. Therefore, some species in this study cannot be placed reliably in current phylogenies (e.g., C. phobetri, C. teleae). Eight of the recorded species are included in the most recent genetic phylogeny for Cotesia reported by Michel-Salzat and Whitfield [44]. This phylogeny contains four identified groups, three of which are represented in our current study. Courtship songs of two species in the fourth group (“melanoscela”) have been described in other studies [22,43]. In our study, species phylogenetically grouped together had similar courtship songs with the exception of C. rubecula. Several species not placed on the genetic phylogeny can be putatively placed within a group based on song characteristics (e.g., C. phobetri). The most basal genetic group containing C. empretiae and C. diacrisiae had songs that consisted of rapid repeated pulses although the placement of these species has low nodal support. Most songs of derived groups consisted of pulses and longer terminal buzzes.

The apparent phylogenetic signal of courtship songs allows for predictions of song structure before recording. For example, most species in the “rubecula” group have a pause-pulse-buzz pattern of similar duration (Fig 7). Species such as C. schizurae and C. electrae likely have songs similar in pattern to C. congregata and not more different than the more distantly related C. euchaetis or C. marginiventris. In both the genetic phylogeny and acoustic dendrogram, C. glomerata was placed as a sister group to most of the “rubecula” group, with its main distinction being the pre-pulse power spike. In the “kariyai” group, C. cyaniridis is predicted to have a song with a buzz that leads directly into a pulse with a long interpulse duration. The song may differ in details of timing and frequency, but otherwise should sound similar to the closely related C. flaviconchae (Fig 7). These predictions are supported by the recent recording of two male C. schizurae (host Schizura unicornis; 37.7549, -77.3458) from the same brood. This species, closely related to C. congregata, was targeted for collection but not initially found. The courtship song of C. schizurae (S13 Audio) is similar enough in structure and duration of pulses, buzzes, and pauses to that of C. congregata to be firmly placed as a closely related species. Although courtship songs can be used to construct species groups, predicting evolutionary relationships among these groups is more challenging due to the lack of data for intermediate species. Among other insect groups, phylogenetic signals have been reported for the diverse songs of psyllids, but divergence may occur more rapidly among sympatric species [62]. Predicting the courtship songs of other acoustically active insect groups is challenging due to rapid diversification of song characteristics unrelated to genetic distance (e.g., the Drosophila willistoni species complex [3] and Chrysoperla lacewings [63]).

Cotesia rubecula is the only species that deviates from expectation. It is genetically placed with species with pulse-terminal buzz patterns (e.g., C. congregata) but lacks a discrete terminal buzz (Fig 2). The most parsimonious explanation is that C. rubecula secondarily lost the long terminal buzz and replaced it with a long pause. The time between pulses is similar to those within its genetic group. Alternatively, C. rubecula may not belong in this group, although high nodal support for its inclusion makes this possibility less likely (Fig 7). Notably, C. rubecula has other characteristics that differentiate it from most species recorded in the “rubecula” and “glomerata” groups—it is solitary and the largest Cotesia species recorded. Determining whether a solitary or gregarious life history or relative size influences courtship song characteristics would require recording a broader range of species, particularly more solitary species (the only other being C. marginiventris in this study). Moreover, C. rubecula and C. glomerata are the only species collected that utilize the same host, Pieris rapae on Brassicaceae. Their songs may display greater divergence in part due to character displacement, which has been demonstrated for the songs of few insects [64]; however, extensive surveying suggests competitive exclusion between these two species over most of the range in the United States [65].

Cotesia teleae has a song that challenges direct placement into a group. The pattern of the pulse-buzz unit is similar in many ways to those of the “rubecula” group; however, it has a short pulse with a high energy terminal buzz that loses amplitude at the end (Fig 2). Most distinctly, there are long pauses between pulses. Possibly, it belongs with the “rubecula” group—the cluster analysis supports a relation with C. rubecula—but its placement remains less certain than for other species without either genetic information or another species with a similar song pattern. The song of C. teleae is also the only one analyzed using a single male. A single parasitized polyphemus caterpillar (Antheraea. polyphemus) yielded few adult wasps. The brood began egression in October with most wasp larvae going into diapause, which was not broken in the lab and yielded only a single male concurrently with females. Considering that courtship songs are conserved within species and this male was healthy, the recorded song is presumably a reliable representation of this species. A second male emerged without a living conspecific female present and would not initiate courtship when presented with other species. Attempts to find a second brood over multiple years failed. Since additional samples of C. teleae are improbable, it was included in this study.

The “melanoscela” group, containing C. sesamiae and C. flavipes, is the only major group not included in this analysis. These two species, widely used as biocontrol agents of stemborer pests, are not native to North America and could not be acquired for this study. Similar to other Cotesia spp., their songs consist of repeating pulse, buzz, and pause elements with a frequency of 222–290 Hz [22,43] (note: our terminology differs from that used in these references). The overall pattern of higher-amplitude pulse decaying into a longer terminal buzz has some structural similarities to songs of C. marginiventris and others in the “rubecula” group; however, the time between high amplitude elements (termed buzz 1 in [22,43]) is nearly a full second in C. flavipes and C. sesamiae, which is considerably longer than most of the other Cotesia species recorded. The species of the “melanoscela” group would form a distinct cluster based on reported song characteristics, and may be placed close to the “rubecula” group based on overall pattern.

Courtship song analysis can be used to match unidentified wasps to species, particularly those with similar morphology. Two cases occurred during this study in which parasitoid cocoons were found separated from their host. Unknown Cotesia cocoons found on a garden tomato plant were identified as C. orobenae upon recording. Presumably, the cross-stripped cabbageworm (Evergestis rimosalis) hosts had decimated nearby cabbages and had then migrated to the tomato before parasitoid egression. The hosts were absent, leaving only the wasp cocoons remaining on the leaves (C. orobenae cocoons typically do not remain attached to the host). In the second case, three loose bundles of parasitoid cocoons were found with no host in a mowed horse pasture in Virginia, USA. After recording the adults, the species acoustically matched those grouped with C. congregata but not any currently recorded species. Subsequently, cocoons of C. nr. phobetri that originated from Arizona were received. These wasps produced courtship songs that very closely resembled those of the unknown Virginia wasps, and also were similar in morphology, cocoon structure, and habitat. This is the first known record of C. nr. phobetri outside of Arizona. Considering that this species utilizes a common caterpillar genus, Grammia, as hosts in a common habitat type, they may be widespread in the United States.

Courtship song elements may differ even among closely related species or host-associated populations. For example, allopatric populations of C. sesamiae and C. flavipes utilizing different hosts had courtship songs that differed in element duration and frequency [43]. Likewise, C. congregata originating from hosts M. sexta on tobacco (MsT) and Ce. catalpae on catalpa (CcC) differed significantly in pulse and pause durations though the differences were not enough to reliably distinguish all individuals [15]. We expanded this earlier finding by using four additional host-foodplant sources of C. congregata in Virginia and an additional population of MsT wasps from Indiana (Table 1). These six total sphingid host species represent two subfamilies so thus were phylogenetically diverse [66]. Mean song element duration (pulse and pause) and PCs differ among MsT and CcC wasps; however, the degree of range overlap with the additional sources prevents reliable discrimination by source (Figs 8 and 9). The slight differences may indicate recent reproductive isolation that over time may become discrete differences under sexual selection or genetic drift. Breeding crosses using these additional sources of C. congregata indicate a pattern of asymmetric hybrid female sterility with either MsT or CcC wasps, suggesting only two primary lineages (Bredlau et al., in submission).

In contrast, geographically separated populations of C. nr. phobetri differ in song element durations (Fig 10), even though they are similar enough to be recognized as the same species (S7 and S8 Audio). We cannot determine whether the Virginia and Arizona populations represent sister species, host-associated races, or isolated populations without additional information on reproductive compatibility and range. These populations are separated by 3,150 km and thus may be expected to have some differences in song elements regardless of species status. Another possibility is that laboratory rearing of the Arizona population for three years could have resulted in slight changes in courtship song elements, as reported for other braconid wasps [25]. Collecting wild C. nr. phobetri at multiple sites would be required to make that assessment. The other geographically separated samples came from C. glomerata and C. rubecula; however, not enough individuals were recorded to discern acoustic differences within these species.

Relatively small differences in songs among closely related species indicate a phylogenetic signal that may have useful applications for systematics. Sexual selection likely plays a role in the differentiation of some songs. However, within the majority of the “rubecula” group, songs consist of similar pause-pulse-buzz patterns more indicative of a slow build-up of differences rather than active sexual selection. Furthermore, song differences in reproductively isolated incipient species of C. congregata are slight and cannot be used to reliably identify host-foodplant complex sources. Song differentiation of reproductively isolated species may change over time via genetic drift in the absence of strong sexual selection, producing minor changes in elements among closely related species. In this scenario, songs may seem arbitrarily different among species with relatively small changes, yet still be conserved within species. Courtship shortly after emergence on the natal host-plant may play a role in limiting contact among sympatric, closely related species, thereby reducing selective pressure on courtship song differentiation. Likewise, other factors such as host-plant learning and adaptations to host immune systems may play a greater role in parasitic wasp speciation, leading to differing rates of song differentiation. In contrast, species clusters of Drosophila are reported to have large differences in courtship songs, suggesting strong sexual selection leading to differentiation before other traits [24,67]. Playback experiments using the D. buzzatii species cluster demonstrate that females are more likely to accept males with a conspecific song, supporting the role of sexual selection [68]. Likewise, cryptic species complexes of lacewings [8] and sand flies [10] can be reliably distinguished by courtship song patterns.

Courtship songs of Cotesia spp. are structurally complex and highly variable compared to those produced by other braconids. For example, courtship songs of other microgastrines vary from the consistent wing fanning sounds that increase in amplitude ca. every 2 seconds produced by Glyptapantales flavicoxis [28] (in a genus closely related to Cotesia [44]) to the short repeating pulse trains of Microplitis croceipes that may merge into a warble [24]. More distantly related braconids, such as those in the Opiinae [2426], typically produce short repeating pulses similar in structure to the song of C. diacrisiae. Wasps in the Aphidiinae [27] and Euphorinae (Bredlau, unpublished) produce longer pulses (200–220 ms) with longer pauses between pulses (200–370 ms) that do not trail into terminal buzzes, similar in structure to the song of C. rubecula. The song of at least one ichneumonid, Compoletis sonorensis, also produces a song consisting of pulses (215 ms) and long pauses (215 ms) (Bredlau, unpublished). Among parasitic wasps, constant wing fanning or repeating pulses are the most common patterns. Considering that all Cotesia songs consist of pulses in one form or another, the ancestral song most likely also consisted of pulses or consistent wing fanning varying in amplitude that later developed pauses before the high amplitude components. Similarity of songs among species such as C. rubecula and those in other subfamilies likely evolved independently. For example, convergence of song structure has been reported for allopatric cryptic species of Chrysoperla lacewings [69].

The analysis presented in this study has several limitations. Even the relatively simple songs of parasitic wasps contain multiple acoustic elements and frequencies, often with different degrees of variance depending on song structure. Principal component analysis is useful for reducing the dimensionality of complex datasets to uncorrelated variables, and in identifying elements that contain the greatest variance. Moreover, PCA is a common method of data exploration widely understood by biologists and has been used in the comparison of songs in diverse taxa including birds (e.g. [70,71]) and insects [4,8,10,63,72]. We used PCA as a means to reduce the acoustic data for comparison and included the number of PCs that adequately explained aspects of the courtship songs in the cluster analysis. However, limitations such as uneven scaling of long vs short elements and time vs frequency were not accounted for because their biological significance is unknown. No one element could adequately capture the differences among songs; furthermore, some elements calculated were intentionally redundant. Alternative methods to a cluster analysis that consider the probability of a given acoustic tree among all possible trees should be considered with additional data. Additionally, the sequencing of all sampled wasps with additional genes or recordings of those already sequenced will permit a more thorough examination of song trait evolution.

This comprehensive study of courtship song diversity within a genus of parasitic wasps, Cotesia, implicates a wide diversity of song patterns that can be divided into groups based on duration and frequency of song elements. The basal song most likely consisted of regular pulses generated by high-amplitude wing strokes, as seen in other members of subfamily Microgastrinae [24]. This song diverged into the several distinct patterns among the major groups of Cotesia. Many wasps not yet recorded can likely be placed into these groups based on a combination of song structure and morphology. The unique structure of songs for each species can potentially be used for species recognition and as a reproductive barrier between cryptic species; however, the influence of sexual selection is uncertain. Despite measurable differences among species, the songs among C. congregata host-foodplant complexes cannot be reliable distinguished, suggesting that song differentiation does not proceed without other reproductive barriers. In total, fifteen Cotesia species have been recorded out of the estimated 1,000 species globally [50]. Considering the size of this genus, other entirely new song patterns may yet be discovered. When combined with additional genetic data, courtship song analysis should prove useful in determining the systematics and evolutionary history of groups of parasitic wasps, particularly in this highly diverse and agriculturally important taxon.

Supporting information

S1 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (10 s) of a male Cotesia congregata (host Manduca sexta on tobacco from Indiana, USA population).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s001

(WAV)

S2 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (10 s) of a male Cotesia diacrisiae.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s002

(WAV)

S3 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (10 s) of a male Cotesia empretiae.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s003

(WAV)

S4 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (10 s) of a male Cotesia euchaetis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s004

(WAV)

S5 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (15 s) of a male Cotesia flaviconchae.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s005

(WAV)

S6 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (10 s) of a male Cotesia glomerata.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s006

(WAV)

S7 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (10 s) of a male Cotesia nr. phobetri (Arizona, USA population).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s007

(WAV)

S8 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (10 s) of a male Cotesia nr. phobetri (Virginia, USA population).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s008

(WAV)

S9 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (10 s) of a male Cotesia orobenae.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s009

(WAV)

S10 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (10 s) of a male Cotesia phobetri.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s010

(WAV)

S11 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (10 s) of a male Cotesia rubecula.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s011

(WAV)

S12 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (10 s) of a male Cotesia teleae.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s012

(WAV)

S13 Audio. Audio recording of a courtship song segment (10 s) of a male Cotesia schizurae.

This species was not included in the analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s013

(WAV)

S1 Appendix. Mean song element duration and frequency by wasp for different Cotesia species.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210249.s014

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

Jessica Bray, Megan Ayers, Kanchan Joshi, Dominique Grim, Chase Stratton, Jessica Johnston, Char’Rese Finney, Claudia Schulze, Eva Houk, Katia Hoskins, Marina Williams, and Wan-ling Chiu found caterpillars in the Richmond, VA area. Kathryn Bredlau and Erin Wall found wasps in Gloucester, VA. Michael Garvey supplied C. flavichonchae. Paul Ode supplied some C. glomerata. Jerome Weis supplied C. rubecula. Michael Singer and Melissa Bernardo supplied C. nr. phobetri from their colony. Buster Tyson and Paul Semtner provided access to C. congregata collection sites. We thank all of the other property owners who granted permission to collect on their land. Many Kester Lab undergraduate interns assisted with rearing caterpillars. Michael Fine provided recording space and valuable suggestions on the manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Sueur J. Insect species and thier songs. In: Drosopoulos S, Claridge M, editors. Insect Sounds and Communication: Physiology, Behaviour, Ecology and Evolution. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis; 2006. pp. 207–217.
  2. 2. Ritchie MG, Gleason JM. Rapid evolution of courtship song pattern in Drosophila willistoni sibling species. J Evol Biol. 1995;8: 463–479.
  3. 3. Gleason JM, Ritchie MG. Evolution of courtship song and reproductive isolation in the Drosophila willistoni species complex: Do sexual signals diverge the most quickly? Evolution. 1998;52: 1493–1500. pmid:28565374
  4. 4. Oliveira CC, Manfrin MH, Sene FDM, Etges WJ. Evolution of male courtship songs in the Drosophila buzzatii species cluster. In: Michalak P, editor. Speciation: Natural Processes, Genetics and Biodiversity. New York: Nova Science Publishers; 2013. pp. 137–164.
  5. 5. Klappert K, Mazzi D, Hoikkala A, Ritchie MG. Male courtship song and female preference variation between phylogeographically distinct populations of Drosophila montana. Evolution. 2007;61: 1481–1488. pmid:17542854
  6. 6. Nattier R, Robillard T, Amedegnato C, Couloux A, Cruaud C, Desutter-Grandcolas L. Evolution of acoustic communication in the Gomphocerinae (Orthoptera: Caelifera: Acrididae). Zool Scr. 2011;40: 479–497.
  7. 7. Montealegre-Z. F, Morris GK. The spiny devil katydids, Panacanthus Walker (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae): An evolutionary study of acoustic behaviour and morphological traits. Syst Entomol. 2004;29: 21–57.
  8. 8. Henry CS, Brooks SJ, Johnson JB, Duelli P. Revised concept of Chrysoperla mediterranea (Hölzel), a green lacewing associated with conifers: Courtship songs across 2800 kilometres of Europe (Neuroptera: Chrysopidae). Syst Entomol. 1999;24: 335–350.
  9. 9. Henry CS, Brooks SJ, Duelli P, Johnson JB. Discovering the true Chrysoperla carnea (Insecta: Neuroptera: Chrysopidae) using song analysis, morphology, and ecology. Ann Entomol Soc Am. 2002;95: 172–191.
  10. 10. Vigoder FM, Souza NA, Brazil RP, Bruno R V., Costa PL, Ritchie MG, et al. Phenotypic differentiation in love song traits among sibling species of the Lutzomyia longipalpis complex in Brazil. Parasites and Vectors. 2015;8: 290. pmid:26017472
  11. 11. McNeil JN, Brodeur J. Pheromone-mediated mating in the aphid parasitoid, Aphidius nigripes (Hymenoptera: Aphididae). J Chem Ecol. 1995;21: 959–972. pmid:24234412
  12. 12. Yoshida S. Behaviour of males in relation to the female sex pheromone in the parasitoid wasp, Anisopteromalus calandrae (Hymenoptera: Pteromalidae). Entomol Exp Appl. 1978;23: 152–162.
  13. 13. Field S, Keller M. Courtship and intersexual signaling in the parasitic wasp Cotesia rubecula (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). J Insect Behav. 1993;6: 737–750.
  14. 14. Gordh G, DeBach P. Courtship behavior in the Aphytis lingnanensis group, its potential usefulness in taxonomy, and a review of sexual behavior in the parasitic Hymenoptera (Chalcidoidea: Aphelinidae). Hilgardia. 1978;46: 37–75.
  15. 15. Bredlau JP, Kester KM. Pre- and postzygotic barriers to reproduction between two host-foodplant complex sources of the parasitic wasp, Cotesia congregata (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). Ann Entomol Soc Am. 2015;108: 1026–1036.
  16. 16. Quicke DLJ. Parasitic Wasps. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1997.
  17. 17. Van Den Assem J, Putters FA. Patterns of sound produced by courting chalcidoid males and its biological significance. Entomol Exp Appl. 1980;27: 293–302.
  18. 18. Danci A, Schaefer PW, Schopf A, Gries G. Species-specific close-range sexual communication systems prevent cross-attraction in three species of Glyptapanteles parasitic wasps (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). Biol Control. 2006;39: 225–231.
  19. 19. Vinson SB. Courtship behavior and evidence of a sex pheromone in the parasitoid Campoletis sonorensis (Hymenoptera: Ichneumonidae). Environ Entomol. 1972;1: 409–414.
  20. 20. Bredlau JP, Mohajer YJ, Cameron TM, Kester KM, Fine ML. Characterization and generation of male courtship song in Cotesia congregata (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). PLoS One. 2013;8: e62051. pmid:23630622
  21. 21. Joyce AL, Hunt RE, Bernal JS, Bradleigh Vinson S. Substrate influences mating success and transmission of courtship vibrations for the parasitoid Cotesia marginiventris. Entomol Exp Appl. 2008;127: 39–47.
  22. 22. Joyce AL, White WH, Medina RF. Host plants impact courtship vibration transmission and mating success of a parasitoid wasp, Cotesia flavipes (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). Evol Ecol. 2014;28: 361–372.
  23. 23. Benelli G, Kavallieratos NG, Donati E, Giunti G, Stefanini C, Canale A. Singing on the wings! Male wing fanning performances affect female willingness to copulate in the aphid parasitoid Lysiphlebus testaceipes (Hymenoptera: Braconidae: Aphidiinae). Insect Sci. 2016;23: 603–611. pmid:25641835
  24. 24. Sivinski J, Webb JC. Acoustic signals produced during courtship in Diachasmimorpha (= Biosteres) longicaudata (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) and other Braconidae. Ann Entomol Soc Am. 1989;82: 116–120.
  25. 25. Joyce AL, Aluja M, Sivinski J, Vinson SB, Ramirez-Romero R, Bernal JS, et al. Effect of continuous rearing on courtship acoustics of five braconid parasitoids, candidates for augmentative biological control of Anastrepha species. BioControl. 2010;55: 573–582.
  26. 26. Canale A, Benelli G, Lanzo F, Giannotti P, Mazzoni V, Lucchi A. The courtship song of fanning males in the fruit fly parasitoid Psyttalia concolor (Szépligeti) (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). Bull Entomol Res. 2013;103: 303–309. pmid:23302745
  27. 27. Villagra CA, Pinto CF, Penna M, Niemeyer HM. Male wing fanning by the aphid parasitoid Aphidius ervi (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) produces a courtship song. Bull Entomol Res. 2011;101: 573–9. pmid:21554798
  28. 28. Danci A, Takács S, Schaefer PW, Gries G. Evidence for acoustic communication in the parasitoid wasp Glyptapanteles flavicoxis. Entomol Exp Appl. 2010;136: 142–150.
  29. 29. Rodriguez JJ, Fernández-Triana JL, Smith MA, Janzen DH, Hallwachs W, Erwin TL, et al. Extrapolations from field studies and known faunas converge on dramatically increased estimates of global microgastrine parasitoid wasp species richness (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). Insect Conserv Divers. 2013;6: 530–536.
  30. 30. Kaiser L, Dupas S, Branca A, Herniou EA, Clarke CW, Capdevielle Dulac C, et al. The Cotesia sesamiae story: Insight into host-range evolution in a Hymenoptera parasitoid and implication for its use in biological control programs. Genetica. 2017;145: 455–468. pmid:28939989
  31. 31. Van Driesche RG. Biological control of Pieris rapae in New England: Host suppression and displacement of Cotesia glomerata by Cotesia rubecula (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). Florida Entomol. 2008;91: 22–25.
  32. 32. Bezemer TM, Harvey JA, Kamp AFD, Wagenaar R, Gols R, Kostenko O, et al. Behaviour of male and female parasitoids in the field: Influence of patch size, host density, and habitat complexity. Ecol Entomol. 2010;35: 341–351.
  33. 33. Kester KM, Barbosa P. Behavioral responses to host foodplants of two populations of the insect parasitoid Cotesia congregata (Say). Oecologia. 1994;99: 151–157. pmid:28313961
  34. 34. Barbosa P, Gross P, Kemper J. Influence of plant allelochemicals on the tobacco hornworm and its parasitoid, Cotesia congregata. Ecology. 1991;72: 1567–1575.
  35. 35. Lentz-Ronning AJ, Kester KM. Effect of sequential learning experiences on searching responses and sex ratio allocations of the gregarious insect parasitoid, Cotesia congregata (Say) (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). J Insect Behav. 2013;26: 165–175.
  36. 36. Lentz AJ, Kester KM. Postemergence experience affects sex ratio allocation in a gregarious insect parasitoid. J Insect Behav. 2008;21: 34–45.
  37. 37. Kester KM, Barbosa P. Postemergence learning in the insect parasitoid, Cotesia congregata (Say) (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). J Insect Behav. 1991;4: 727–742.
  38. 38. Beckage N, Tan F, Schleifer K, Lane R, Cherubin L. Characterization and biological effects of Cotesia congregata polydnavirus on host larvae of the tobacco hornworm, Manduca sexta. Arch Insect Biochem Physiol. 1994;195: 165–195.
  39. 39. Amaya KE, Asgari S, Jung R, Hongskula M, Beckage NE. Parasitization of Manduca sexta larvae by the parasitoid wasp Cotesia congregata induces an impaired host immune response. J Insect Physiol. 2005;51: 505–512. pmid:15893997
  40. 40. Bézier A, Herbinière J, Lanzrein B, Drezen J-M. Polydnavirus hidden face: The genes producing virus particles of parasitic wasps. J Invertebr Pathol. 2009;101: 194–203. pmid:19460382
  41. 41. Chevignon G, Thézé J, Cambier S, Poulain J, Da Silva C, Bézier A, et al. Functional annotation of Cotesia congregata bracovirus: Identification of viral genes expressed in parasitized host immune tissues. J Virol. 2014;88: 8795–8812. pmid:24872581
  42. 42. Avila GA, Withers TM, Holwell GI. Courtship and mating behaviour in the parasitoid wasp Cotesia urabae (Hymenoptera: Braconidae): Mate location and the influence of competition and body size on male mating success. Bull Entomol Res. 2017;107: 439–447. pmid:27974053
  43. 43. Joyce AL, Bernal JS, Vinson SB, Hunt RE, Schulthess F, Medina RF. Geographic variation in male courtship acoustics and genetic divergence of populations of the Cotesia flavipes species complex. Entomol Exp Appl. 2010;137: 153–164.
  44. 44. Michel-Salzat A, Whitfield JB. Preliminary evolutionary relationships within the parasitoid wasp genus Cotesia (Hymenoptera: Braconidae: Microgastrinae): Combined analysis of four genes. Syst Entomol. 2004;29: 371–382.
  45. 45. Krombein KV, Hurd PD Jr., Smith DR, Burks BD. Catalog of Hymenoptera in America North of Mexico. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institute Press; 1979.
  46. 46. Tietz HM. An Index to the Described Life Histories, Early Stages, and Hosts of the Macrolepidoptera of the Continental United States and Canada. Sarasota, FL: The Allyn Museum of Entomology; 1972.
  47. 47. Kester KM, Eldeib GM, Brown BL. Genetic differentiation of two host–foodplant complex sources of Cotesia congregata (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). Ann Entomol Soc Am. 2015;108: 1014–1025.
  48. 48. Charif RA, Waack AM, Strickman LM. Raven Pro 1.3 User’s Manual. Ithaca, NY: Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology; 2008.
  49. 49. Stireman JO, Singer MS. Spatial and temporal variation in the parasitoid assemblage of an exophytic polyphagous caterpillar. Ecol Entomol. 2002;27: 588–600.
  50. 50. Whitfield JB, Austin AD, Fernandez-Triana JL. Systematics, biology, and evolution of microgastrine parasitoid wasps. Annu Rev Entomol. 2018;63: 389–406. pmid:29058979
  51. 51. Murphy N, Banks JC, Whitfield JB, Austin AD. Phylogeny of the parasitic microgastroid subfamilies (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) based on sequence data from seven genes, with an improved time estimate of the origin of the lineage. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2008;47: 378–95. pmid:18325792
  52. 52. Banks JC, Whitfield JB. Dissecting the ancient rapid radiation of microgastrine wasp genera using additional nuclear genes. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2006;41: 690–703. pmid:16854601
  53. 53. Smith MA, Fernández-Triana JL, Eveleigh E, Gómez J, Guclu C, Hallwachs W, et al. DNA barcoding and the taxonomy of Microgastrinae wasps (Hymenoptera, Braconidae): Impacts after 8 years and nearly 20 000 sequences. Mol Ecol Resour. 2013;13: 168–76. pmid:23228011
  54. 54. Whitfield JB, Mardulyn P, Austin AD, Dowton M. Phylogenetic relationships among microgastrine braconid wasp genera based on data from the 16S, COI and 28S genes and morphology. Syst Entomol. 2002;27: 337–359.
  55. 55. Kankare M, Van Nouhuys S, Hanski I. Genetic divergence among host-specific cryptic species in Cotesia melitaearum aggregate (Hymenoptera: Braconidae), parasitoids of checkerspot butterflies. Ann Entomol Soc Am. 2005;98: 382–394.
  56. 56. Muirhead KA, Murphy NP, Sallam N, Donnellan SC, Austin AD. Phylogenetics and genetic diversity of the Cotesia flavipes complex of parasitoid wasps (Hymenoptera: Braconidae), biological control agents of lepidopteran stemborers. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2012;63: 904–14. pmid:22450357
  57. 57. Kankare M, Shaw MR. Molecular phylogeny of Cotesia Cameron, 1891 (Insecta: Hymenoptera: Braconidae: Microgastrinae) parasitoids associated with Melitaeini butterflies (Insecta: Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae: Melitaeini). Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2004;32: 207–220. pmid:15186808
  58. 58. Whitfield JB. Phylogeny of microgastroid braconid wasps, and what it tells us about polydnavirus evolution. In: Austin AD, Dowton M, editors. Hymenoptera: Evolution, Biodiversity and Biological Control. Clayton, Australia: CSIRO Press; 2000. pp. 97–105.
  59. 59. Dupas S, Gitau CW, Branca A, Le Rü BP, Silvain J-F. Evolution of a polydnavirus gene in relation to parasitoid-host species immune resistance. J Hered. 2008;99: 491–9. pmid:18552349
  60. 60. Whitfield JB. Estimating the age of the polydnavirus/braconid wasp symbiosis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99: 7508–7513. pmid:12032313
  61. 61. Herniou EA, Huguet E, Theze J, Bezier A, Periquet G, Drezen J-M. When parasitic wasps hijacked viruses: Genomic and functional evolution of polydnaviruses. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2013;368: 20130051. pmid:23938758
  62. 62. Percy DM, Taylor GS, Kennedy M. Psyllid communication: Acoustic diversity, mate recognition and phylogenetic signal. Invertebr Syst. 2006;20: 431.
  63. 63. Henry CS, Wells MLM. Adaptation or random change? The evolutionary response of songs to substrate properties in lacewings (Neuroptera: Chrysopidae: Chrysoperla). Anim Behav. 2004;68: 879–895.
  64. 64. Marshall DC, Cooley JR. Reproductive character displacement and speciation in periodical cicadas, with description of a new species, 13-year Magicicada neotredecim. Evolution. 2000;54: 1313–1325. pmid:11005298
  65. 65. Herlihy M V., Van Driesche RG, Abney MR, Brodeur J, Bryant AB, Casagrande RA, et al. Distribution of Cotesia rubecula (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) and its displacement of Cotesia glomerata in Eastern North America. Florida Entomol. 2012;95: 461–467.
  66. 66. Kawahara AY, Mignault AA, Regier JC, Kitching IJ, Mitter C. Phylogeny and biogeography of hawkmoths (Lepidoptera: Sphingidae): Evidence from five nuclear genes. PLoS One. 2009;4: e5719. pmid:19492095
  67. 67. Veltsos P, Wicker-Thomas C, Butlin RK, Hoikkala A, Ritchie MG. Sexual selection on song and cuticular hydrocarbons in two distinct populations of Drosophila montana. Ecol Evol. 2011;2: 80–94. pmid:22408728
  68. 68. Iglesias PP, Hasson E. The role of courtship song in female mate choice in South American Cactophilic Drosophila. PLoS One. 2017;12: e0176119. pmid:28467464
  69. 69. Henry CS, Wells MLM, Simon CM. Convergent evolution of courtship songs among cryptic species of the carnea group of green lacewings (Neuroptera: Chrysopidae: Chrysoperla). Evolution. 1999;53: 1165–1179. pmid:28565530
  70. 70. Päckert M, Sun YH, Fischer BS, Tietze DT, Martens J. A phylogeographic break and bioacoustic intraspecific differentiation in the Buff-barred Warbler (Phylloscopus pulcher) (Aves: Passeriformes, Phylloscopidae). Avian Res. 2014;5: 1–12.
  71. 71. Zhao M, Alström P, Hu R, Zhao C, Hao Y, Lei F, et al. Phylogenetic relationships, song and distribution of the endangered Rufous-headed Robin Larvivora ruficeps. Ibis. 2017;159: 204–216.
  72. 72. Eberhard MJB, Eberhard SH. Evolution and diversity of vibrational signals in Mantophasmatodea (Insecta). J Insect Behav. 2013;26: 352–370.