Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Phantom Phone Signals in youths: Prevalence, correlates and relation to psychopathology

  • Simone Pisano ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Writing – original draft

    pisano.simone@gmail.com

    Affiliation Department of Neuroscience, AORN Santobono-Pausilipon, Naples, Italy

  • Pietro Muratori,

    Roles Data curation, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation IRCCS Stella Maris, Scientific Institute of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, Calambrone, Italy

  • Vincenzo Paolo Senese,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of Campania "Luigi Vanvitelli", Naples, Italy

  • Chiara Gorga,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Mental and Physical Health and Preventive Medicine, University of Campania, Naples, Italy

  • Margherita Siciliano,

    Roles Investigation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Mental and Physical Health and Preventive Medicine, University of Campania, Naples, Italy

  • Marco Carotenuto,

    Roles Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Mental and Physical Health and Preventive Medicine, University of Campania, Naples, Italy

  • Raffaella Iuliano,

    Roles Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Neonatology and Neonatal Pathology, Hospital ‘‘Loreto Mare”, Naples, Italy

  • Carmela Bravaccio,

    Roles Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Translational Medicine, Federico II University of Naples, Naples, Italy

  • Simona Signoriello,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Mental and Physical Health and Preventive Medicine, University of Campania, Naples, Italy

  • Antonella Gritti,

    Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Educational Sciences, Suor Orsola Benicasa University, Naples, Italy

  • Antonio Pascotto,

    Roles Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Mental and Physical Health and Preventive Medicine, University of Campania, Naples, Italy

  • Gennaro Catone

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Educational Sciences, Suor Orsola Benicasa University, Naples, Italy

Abstract

The term Phantom Phone Signals (PPS) refers to the perception of a mobile phone ringing, vibrating and blinking when in fact it did not. Data in youth are lacking, and controversies exist on whether PPS is related to psychopathology. In the present study, we showed data on the prevalence of PPS in a population (N = 2959) of students aged 10 to 14 years. We also explored the possible association between PPS and emotional or behavioural problems. Our results showed that PPS is a relatively common phenomenon with a prevalence rate of 58.9%, being more frequent in females. In univariate and multivariate analyses, we also found an association between the presence of PPS and emotional problems and temper tantrums, after accounting for relevant covariates. PPS is a relevant phenomenon to be considered in youth. It is common and may be a signal for emotional problems.

Introduction

“Phantom Vibration” (PV) or “Phantom Ringing” (PR) refers to the mistaken perception of a phone vibrating or ringing, respectively, when in fact it did not; in previous literature, other terms were used, such as ‘‘ringxiety,” ‘‘vibranxiety”, ‘‘FauxCellArm” and the all-inclusive term Phantom Phone Signals (PPS) proposed by Tanis et al. [1], which includes all kinds of perceptions of phone signals. Although some differences were noted [1] among these authors, we did not find compelling evidence that justified considering PV and PR as different entities; thus, in the present paper, we will follow Tanis et al. [1] in considering PR and PV as different manifestations of the same overarching phenomenon, and throughout the manuscript, we refer to it as PPS.

Most of the studies agreed with PPS being a common phenomenon, with various prevalence estimates; in the only review present in the literature, the prevalence of PPS ranged between 27.4% and 89% [2]. It has been mostly studied in health professionals or university students, leaving a gap in the knowledge about this phenomenon in school-aged youths [1, 3, 4, 5, 6]. This is striking because mobile phone use is basically ubiquitous in children and adolescents (>90% of adolescents [7] and 69% of children [8]); also, younger users are more likely than older users to spend much time on their mobile phones and to suffer from mobile phone-related problems [9, 10]; moreover, in the first study on the phenomenon, Rothberg et al. [3] found younger people to be more prone to the experience of PV.

The association between PSS and psychological/psychopathological factors has been studied in adults and produced mixed findings. Drouin et al. [11] showed a small but significant relation between PPS and text-message dependency and a negative relation between frequency of PPS and conscientiousness. Lin et al. [4] showed an association between the frequency and the intensity of PPS and stress due to a medical internship, but not with anxiety or depression; in a subsequent study [5], they found that interns with severe PPS had higher levels of somatic depression and subjective and somatic anxiety symptoms than did the interns with subclinical PPS. Kruger and Djerf [6] found that PPS was associated with attachment anxiety, but not with general sensation seeking and attachment avoidance. Chen et al. [12] found an association between PPS and work-related burnout syndrome symptoms, but not with general measures of anxiety and depression. It may be that, aside the above mentioned factors previously identified, mental health problems (and related to that susceptibility to mild hallucinations) explain the presence of PPS in a youth population. Thus, examining the extent to which emotional and behavioural problems are related to PPS is an important contribution to a growing literature, which covers an increasingly salient aspect of youth’s everyday life.

Based on the above mentioned considerations, we conducted a survey in a school-based population of youth aged from 9 to 14 years, to fill the gap in the prevalence of the phenomenon in this age range. We then tested the hypothesis that PPS was related to emotional problems, to shed light on the existing controversies. We also explored, for the first time, the relationship of PPS with other behavioural problems.

Materials and methods

The present data were drawn from a project whose aim is wider and encompasses several aspects of mental health and social phenomena (e.g., bullying and cyberbullying, mobile phone use, temperament and psychopathological features). It is a school-based population survey, settled in the metropolitan city of Naples, Italy. The present data were collected in a cross-sectional manner, while the longitudinal phase remains ongoing. Twelve middle schools across the area of interest (age range of students was approximately 10–14 years across three levels of instruction: 1st, 2nd and 3rd grade), six in the city of Naples and six in suburban areas, were recruited in the school year 2015/2016. Here, we present data from 2959 students (66.6% of the total students). In each school, the research team had a meeting with the classroom before the data collection to explain the research aims, procedure and motivate students to respond sincerely and accurately to obtain high-quality data. A researcher was always present during questionnaire administration to answer any queries. Subjects unable to read Italian and those with certified intellectual disabilities of any grade and type were excluded. Students with learning difficulties had the opportunity to fill in the questionnaire in the presence of a dedicated teacher. More details on the procedures and recruitment are reported elsewhere (see Catone et al., in press). For the purpose of the present study, we only included subjects who responded “yes” to the question of whether they had a smartphone. Subjects who had a smartphone made up 2859 of the 2959 students (96% of the total sample), which included 1457 males (51%) and 1402 females (49%). All parents signed a consent form after a thorough explanation of the procedures; the ethical committee of the Campania University “Luigi Vanvitelli” approved the study protocol.

Measures

Smartphone use-related items.

Three items were developed ad hoc to investigate smartphone use-related variables. Participants were asked the following questions:

PPS: “Has it ever happened that you hear or feel your smartphone ringing or vibrating, but do not see anything when you check your phone?”. Those who responded ‘‘yes” were then asked ‘‘How often did you feel it?” and they could answer on a Likert scale from 0 to 10. This item is slightly different from other studies, which asked a judgement on how bothersome the events felt to the respondent [4, 5, 6], because we tried to capture the intensity of the phenomenon in a continuous measure that may reflect the personal view of the experience, without conveying a negative meaning to the question.

Smartphone use: “has it ever happened that you use the smartphone for a time period that you have judged excessive?” Those who responded ‘‘yes” were then asked ‘‘How often?”, and they could answer on a Likert scale from 0 to 10. We used both (dichotomous and continuous) variables in the analyses, which yielded similar results; we only showed the model with the continuous version of the variable.

Checking the smartphone: “has it ever happened that you continuously check your smartphone in order to grasp as many conversations as possible on social media (like Facebook or Twitter)?” Those who responded ‘‘yes” were then asked ‘‘How often?” and they could answer on a Likert scale from 0 to 10. We used both dichotomous and continuous variables in the analyses, which yielded similar results; we only showed the model with the continuous version of the variable.

Psychopathology.

We used the self-report Italian version of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) [13] to assess general psychopathology. The SDQ comprised 25 items and provided five subscales: emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity-inattention problems, peer problems, and prosocial behaviour. Each item uses a three-point ordinal format to be answered with one of the following: 0 = not true; 1 = somewhat true; and 2 = certainly true. The mean score for each subscale was then calculated (range 0–10) and used in the analyses. The psychometric properties of the self-report version of the SDQ are generally good across studies [14, 15, 16, 17]. The alpha values for each subscale were 0.72 for the emotional symptoms subscale, 0.46 for the conduct problems subscale, 0.59 for the hyperactivity-inattention subscale, 0.55 for the peer problems subscale and 0.66 for pro-social behaviour subscale. The alpha values were generally consistent with those reported in the literature [17].

Data analyses

Continuous variables were reported as either the mean and standard deviation (SD), as assessed by the Shapiro-Wilk test, and compared with t-tests. Categorical variables were reported as absolute numbers and percentages and compared with Pearson’s chi-squared tests. To test the factors associated with the presence/absence of PPS, we fitted a logistic regression model with the dichotomous PPS variable as the dependent variable and age, gender, smartphone-related variables (use and check) and the scores of each of the psychopathology subscales (emotional problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity, peer problems, pro-social behaviour) as independent variables. As the alpha of conduct problems subscale was poor we tried to delete items for improving it; as we could not achieve a better alpha, we included all items separately into the model. To test for the variables that may better explain the variance in the intensity of the phenomenon a multivariable regression model was also fitted with the Likert scale of the PPS as the dependent variable and the same independent variables; we tested two models, one with only subjects who reported having experienced PPS, one with the whole sample (showed in supplementary material). Models were checked for the presence of collinearity by calculating the variance inflation factor. All analyses were run in SPSS Version 20 (IBM SPSS version 20, Armonk, NY).

Results

PPS was reported by 1685 of the students (58.9%), including 896 females (53.1%) and 789 males (46.8%) (chi squared = 28.1, p<0.000). Univariate comparisons revealed significant differences in age, gender, smartphone-related and psychopathological variables between those who reported PPS and those who did not (see Table 1).

The logistic regression model revealed that emotional problems and temper tantrums were the two psychopathological variables associated with the presence of PPS (OR = 1.06 and 1.1 respectively, p = 0.005 and p = 0.05), after controlling for age, gender and smartphone-related variables (all significant predictors). This suggested that older age, female gender, higher time spent on the smartphone and more frequent checking of the smartphone (self-reported), were all factors linked to PPS; after accounting for those factors, emotional problems and temper tantrums were still significant predictors. The model accounted for 24% of the variance. See Table 2.

thumbnail
Table 2. Logistic regression model with the dependent variable PPS (dichotomous), N = 2859.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210095.t002

More specifically, the second regression model (1685 subjects), exploring factors linked to the intensity of the phenomenon (self-reported), partially confirmed previous results: gender was not a significant predictor, however, time spent on the smartphone (B = 0.21, p<0.000), checking the smartphone (B = 0.23, p<0.000), emotional problems (B = 0.13, p<0.000) and temper tantrums (B = 0.23, p = 0.02) were still significant predictors. The model accounted for 25% of the variance. See Table 3. Model including all the subjects yielded the same pattern of results (see complementary material).

thumbnail
Table 3. Multivariate regression model with the dependent variable PPS (Likert scale), N = 1685.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210095.t003

Discussion

The present study explored the phenomenon of PPS in a large and representative sample of school-aged youth. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study investigating this phenomenon in 10- to 14-year old Italian students. Data showed that the prevalence of experiencing phantom ringing or vibrating was relatively common (58.9% of the subjects), was more frequent in females and tended to be more common with increasing age. The prevalence rate is not far from that reported in studies with older subjects (67% reported by Rothberg [3] and 78% by Lin [4]). Thus, the high frequency has also been confirmed in youth, and these data are relevant considering the widespread use of smartphones in pre-adolescence. Mohammadbeigi [18] reported a higher female prevalence for vibration and male prevalence for ringing; other studies did not find differences across genders (taking vibration and ringing together [1]). Future studies should elucidate whether the higher prevalence in females in our study is an age-related result or whether the result could be explained by our larger sample size; that is, other studies did not have enough statistical power to detect the gender difference. Epidemiological findings are highly relevant in light of the subsequent results regarding the relationship between PPS and emotional symptoms. We tried to elucidate previous controversies in the literature studying a larger sample size with a well-established measure of psychopathology. Our regression models revealed an association between the presence of PPS and both emotional problems and temper tantrums; these associations survived the control with age, gender, smartphone use and check and other psychopathology scales. Additionally, both models with dichotomous and dimensional variables showed converging results, thereby strengthening the findings. Thus, we confirmed and extended to a younger population, previous data from the literature reporting an association with PPS and affective symptoms [4, 5, 6]. Lin et al. [5] demonstrated an association between somatic anxiety and depression and severe PPS in medical interns during an internship. The authors discussed the cognitive mechanism from which auditory hallucinations may arise in non-schizophrenic subjects (top-down mechanisms, abnormalities in executive inhibition, and negative emotions [19]). We noted the relevance of the latter factor, negative emotions, which seems to provide the emotional background to the phenomenon. This is in line with recent studies on psychotic-like experiences in clinical and non-clinical adolescents that revealed a clear association between “psychotic” (e.g., paranoia, hallucinations) and emotional disturbances, which challenged the historical divide between them [20, 21, 22]. From another point of view, Kruger’s paper [6] argued PPS was linked to insecurity in interpersonal relationships, a convincing argument that was indirectly supported, from another point of view, by our data. The observed link between PPS and emotional problems may be due to personality factors not explored in the present papers, but highlighted into a previous one [11]; for example, it is known that conscientiousness is negatively related to frequency of PPS and neuroticism is positively related to bothersomeness of PPS; future studies may systematically analyze a model with personality factors and metal health problems in relationship to PPS. Back to the previous results on gender, it makes sense that as females are more prone to experience internalizing symptoms [23], they are more prone to display PPS as well.

Regarding other results, even if we used smartphone-related variables (phone use and phone check, which were self-reported) as control variables, we confirmed that both are factors that explain part of the individual differences in the experience of PPS, as reported by other authors [1, 3]. More doubtful is the relationship with conduct problems, as our data were basically exploratory and need replication. The alpha value of the SDQ conduct problem subscale was poor and thus it may not be the best way to assess non-clinical conduct problems. Anyway, temper tantrums seem associated to PPS in both models, thus it seems useful to explore the relationship between externalizing symptoms and PPS in future studies. Reward processing-related circuits, which are activated by social media use [24], are activated by aggression as well [25] and thus a link may be postulated and justified, but specific data on PPS are lacking.

The theoretical implications of our findings are related to the conceptualization of PPS. Considering PPS a hallucination (auditory or tactile) that is not schizophrenic in nature, but rooted in a background that contains emotional, stress-related [4, 5] and attachment/interpersonal [6] difficulties seems the best way to conceptualize the phenomenon. From a clinical point of view, it seems important to detect PPS when present as it may represent an alarm for possible emotional problems, but it is far from being a signal of severe mental illness. Whether it is a precursor/risk factor or a simple correlate needs to be determined.

The results should be interpreted in light of some limitations. First, all data are self-reported which may be a source of bias (for example, a social desirability effect, which may cause a subject to answer positively to several questions, could have inflated the observed correlation). While it may be suitable to assess emotional problems from teachers’ or parent’s reports allowing future studies can overcome this limitation, it seems difficult to find a different way to assess PPS; in this regard, our intensity question referred to “how often do subjects feel the phenomenon” without assessing the bother or impact on everyday life, which may have limited the study. A more comprehensive measure of PPS would be welcome in the future. Also, we had not measured any stress-related variables or socio-economic assessment; thus, the results may be, at least in part, driven by these other variables. Finally, alphas of SDQ-subscales resulted to be poor to moderate. Replication with more reliable instruments is needed.

In conclusion, the established link of PPS with emotional psychopathology opens the routes for future studies that could examine the phenomenon in clinical samples or in relation to more specific dimensions of psychopathology (e.g., externalizing symptoms, obsessive-impulsive spectrum). More importantly, future studies could address these issues longitudinally. For now, we advise mental health professionals, as well as caregivers, that PPS is prevalent in youths, and may be a signal of emotional problems.

Acknowledgments

GC, SP, VPS, MC, MS, SS, AG, AP are all part of a research group whose aim is to study the relationship between psychosocial phenomena and psychopathological symptoms (The Bullying and Youth Mental Health Naples Study, BYMHNS).

References

  1. 1. Tanis M, Beukeboom CJ, Hartmann T, Vermeulen IE. Phantom phone signals: An investigation into the prevalence and predictors of imagined cell phone signals. Comput Human Behav. 2015;51:356–62.
  2. 2. Deb A. Phantom vibration and phantom ringing among mobile phone users: A systematic review of literature. Asia Pac Psychiatry. 2015;7:231–9. pmid:25408384
  3. 3. Rothberg M, Arora A, Hermann J, Kleppel R, St Marie P. Visintainer P. Phantom vibration syndrome among medical staff: a cross sectional survey. Br Med J. 2010;341:c6914.
  4. 4. Lin Y, Lin S, Li P, Huang W, Chen C. Prevalent hallucinations during medical internships: Phantom vibration and ringing syndromes. PloS ONE. 2013;8(6):e65152. pmid:23762302
  5. 5. Lin Y, Chen C, Li P, Lin S. A dimensional approach to the phantom vibration and ringing syndrome during medical internship. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;49:1254–8.
  6. 6. Kruger DJ, Djerf JM. High ringxiety: attachment anxiety predicts experiences of phantom cell phone ringing. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Net. 2016;19:56–9.
  7. 7. Schüz J. Mobile phone use and exposures in children. Bioelectromagnetics. 2005; Suppl 7:S45–50.
  8. 8. GSMA and the Mobile Society Research Institute within NTT DOCOMO. Children's use of mobile phone. A special report 2014.
  9. 9. Bianchi A, Phillip JG. Psychological predictors of problem mobile phone use. Cyber Psychol Behav. 2015;8:39–51.
  10. 10. Livingstone S, Smith PK. Annual research review: Harms experienced by child users of online and mobile technologies: the nature, prevalence and management of sexual and aggressive risks in the digital age. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55:635–54. pmid:24438579
  11. 11. Drouin M, Kaiser DH, Miller DA. Phantom vibrations among undergraduates: Prevalence and associated psychological characteristics. Comput Human Behav. 2012;28:1490–6.
  12. 12. Chen CP, Wu CC, Chang LR, Lin YH. Possible association between phantom vibration syndrome and occupational burnout. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2014;10:2307–14.
  13. 13. Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–6. pmid:9255702
  14. 14. Muris P, Meesters C, van den Berg F. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)—further evidence for its reliability and validity in a community sample of Dutch children and adolescents. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003; 12:1–8.
  15. 15. Yao S, Zhang C, Zhu X, Jing X, McWhinnie CM, Abela JR. Measuring adolescent psychopathology: psychometric properties of the self-report strengths and difficulties questionnaire in a sample of Chinese adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2009;45:55–62. pmid:19541250
  16. 16. Gaete J, Montero-Marin J, Valenzuela D, Rojas-Barahona CA, Olivares E, Araya R. Mental health among children and adolescents: Construct validity, reliability, and parent-adolescent agreement on the 'Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire' in Chile. PLoS One. 2018;13(2):e0191809. pmid:29401472
  17. 17. Van Roy B, Veenstra M, Clench-Aas J. Construct validity of the five-factor Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) in pre-, early, and late adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49:1304–12. pmid:19120709
  18. 18. Mohammadbeigi A, Mohammadsalehi N, Moshiri E, Anbari Z, Ahmadi A, Ansari H. The prevalence of phantom vibration/ringing syndromes and their related factors in Iranian' students of medical sciences. Asian J Psychiatr. 2017;27:76–80. pmid:28558902
  19. 19. Waters F, Allen P, Aleman A, Fernyhough C, Woodward TS, Badcock JC, et al. Auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia and nonschizophrenia populations: a review and integrated model of cognitive mechanisms. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:683–93. pmid:22446568
  20. 20. Ronald A, Sieradzka D, Cardno AG, Haworth CM, McGuire P, Freeman D. Characterization of psychotic experiences in adolescence using the specific psychotic experiences questionnaire: Findings from a study of 5000 16-year-old twins. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40:868–77. pmid:24062593
  21. 21. Pisano S, Catone G, Pascotto A, Iuliano R, Tiano C, Milone A, et al. Paranoid Thoughts in Adolescents with Social Anxiety Disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2016;47:792–8. pmid:26658937
  22. 22. Catone G, Marotta R, Pisano S, Lennox B, Carotenuto M, Gritti A, Pascotto A, Broome MR. Psychotic-like experiences in help-seeking adolescents: Dimensional exploration and association with different forms of bullying victimization—A developmental social psychiatry perspective. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2017 Dec;63(8):752–762. pmid:28990447
  23. 23. Gritti A, Bravaccio C, Signoriello S, Salerno F, Pisano S, Catone G, et al. Epidemiological study on behavioural and emotional problems in developmental age: prevalence in a sample of Italian children, based on parent and teacher reports. Ital J Pediatr. 2014 Feb 18;40(1):19. pmid:24533835
  24. 24. Sherman LE, Payton AA, Hernandez LM, Greenfield PM, Dapretto M. The power of the like in adolescence: effects of peer influence on neural and behavioral responses to social media. Psychol. Sci. 2016;27:1027–35. pmid:27247125
  25. 25. Aleyasin H, Flanigan ME, Russo SJ. Neurocircuitry of aggression and aggression seeking behavior: nose poking into brain circuitry controlling aggression. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2018;49:184–91. pmid:29524848