Figures
Abstract
Visual mismatch negativity (vMMN), an event-related signature of automatic detection of events violating sequential regularities is traditionally investigated at the onset of frequent (standard) and rare (deviant) events. In a previous study we obtained vMMN to vanishing parts of continuously presented objects (diamonds with diagonals), and we concluded that the offset-related vMMN is a model of sensitivity to irregular partial occlusion of objects. In the present study we replicated the previous results, but in order to test the object-related interpretation we applied a new condition with a set of separate visual stimuli: a texture of bars with two orientations. In the texture condition (offset of bars with irregular vs. regular orientation) we obtained vMMN, showing that the continuous presence of objects is unnecessary for offset-related vMMN. However, unlike in the object-related condition, reappearance of the previously vanishing lines also elicited vMMN. In principle reappearance of the stimuli is an event with probability 1.0, and according to our results, the object condition reappearance was an expected event. However, the offset and onset of texture elements seems to be treated separately by the system underlying vMMN. As an advantage of the present method, the whole stimulus set during the inter-stimulus interval saturates the visual structures sensitive to stimulus input. Accordingly, the offset-related vMMN is less sensitive to low-level adaptation that differs between the deviant and standard stimuli.
Citation: Czigler I, Sulykos I, File D, Kojouharova P, Gaál ZA (2019) Visual mismatch negativity to disappearing parts of objects and textures. PLoS ONE 14(2): e0209130. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209130
Editor: Manuel S. Malmierca, Universidad de Salamanca, SPAIN
Received: November 20, 2018; Accepted: January 26, 2019; Published: February 7, 2019
Copyright: © 2019 Czigler et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: All relevant data are available from German Neuroinformatics Node/G-Node (GIN) at https://web.gin.g-node.org/gaalzs/vanish_vMMN.
Funding: Funding was provided by National Research, Development and Innovation Office (NKFIH), Grant 119587 to IC. The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
The visual information processing system is sensitive to events violating the regularity of stimulus sequences, even if the events are unrelated to the ongoing task (unattended). The automatic detection of violating regularities can be revealed by the visual mismatch negativity (vMMN) components of event-related brain potentials (ERPs). VMMN is the difference between the ERPs elicited by the deviant events and the regular ones. VMMN is elicited by deviant visual features (color, orientation, movement direction, etc.), object-related deviancies, facial emotions, handedness, numerosity, sequential regularities, familiarity, language-related and other deviances, etc. (for reviews see [1, 2, 3]).
In our previous study [4] we obtained vMMN to the offset of irregularly vanishing parts of objects. In particular, diamonds with diameters were presented during the inter-event interval. From time to time two parallel sides of the diamonds disappeared. One of the parallel sides disappeared infrequently, the other pair disappeared frequently. Importantly, diamonds were unrelated to the ongoing tracking task. VMMN, as a difference potential between those elicited by the infrequent and frequent offset emerged over the occipital location within the 120–202 ms range. However, no vMMN appeared after the reappearance of the whole object. We interpreted our result as showing that the infrequent occlusion of the represented objects elicited vMMN, whereas the reappearance of the object was a predicted event, and accordingly these events did not elicit vMMN. This interpretation is in accord with a prevailing theory of auditory MMN and vMMN. The predictive coding theory considers the mismatch potentials as error signals. The memory representation of the frequent (standard) stimuli generates an expectation about the likely properties of future events. In cases where there is a match between the input and the expected representation (i.e., without new information) the perceptual system may ignore the event. Further processing only occurs when there is a discrepancy between the input and the expectancy. The mismatch components are signatures of the mutual adjustment between the input and the expected events only. According to the predictive coding view, reappearance of the whole pattern (i.e. an event with 1.0 probability) does not elicit vMMN [2, 3, 5, 6]. Furthermore, this interpretation of the previous study [4] was closely connected to object-related representation, because we considered that the environmental model consisted of representations of whole diamonds. The aim of the present study was to replicate this result, and to investigate the object-related aspect of our interpretation. Beside the object-related condition, we examined a texture condition: in the inter-event period we presented unconnected bars with two orientations. One set of bars with a particular orientation vanished infrequently, the other frequently. We hypothesized that without the object-related representation stimulus offset does not elicit vMMN, but stimulus onset, as an orientation-related deviancy does.
It is important to note that the offset stimulation has a particular advantage. While the stimuli are present during the inter-event interval, these stimuli saturate the low-level input structures. Therefore the ERPs to deviant vs. standard difference are less susceptible to stimulus-specific adaptation, and offset-related vMMN can be considered as deviant-related additional activity (genuine vMMN; [7, 8]).
Materials and methods
Participants
Twenty adults participated in the study. All of them had normal or corrected-to-normal vision (at least 5/5 in a version of the Snellen charts). No one reported any neurological or psychiatric diseases. They were paid for their participation. One of the participants had an unusually noisy ERP, and another participant’s ERP was dominated by alpha activity. Therefore, the results were calculated for the remaining 18 participants (10 females; mean age: 22.1 years, SD: 2.3 years). Participants were paid for their contributions. Written informed consent was obtained from the participants before the experimental procedure. The study was approved by the United Ethical Review Committee for Research in Psychology (Hungary).
Stimuli and procedure
The experimental stimuli of the object condition and other aspects of the study were identical to our previous study [4]. As a summary, events were presented on a 19-inch CRT monitor (Flatron 915 FT Plus, 75 Hz refresh rate) from a 1.4 m distance using the Cogent 2000 MATLAB toolbox. Fig 1 demonstrates the task-related and vMMN-related stimuli in the two conditions and the stimulus sequence.
A: An example of the stimulus field. The vanishing stimuli were either the 45o or the 135o bars. Both orientations were standard and deviant. The green and red dots are the stimuli of the tracking task. B: the outline of the stimulus sequences (in both the onset and offset stimuli a +/- 40 ms range was presented around the 520 ms mean value). ON: the appearance of the whole stimulus set; OFF: a subset of the stimulus set vanished; S: frequent (standard) vanished subset; D: rare (deviant) vanished subset.
The task-relevant stimuli appeared on the central area of the screen and consisted of two disks. The red disk served as a fixation point, the green disk made horizontal random movements around the red disk. The task was to keep the green disk as close to the center of the red disk as possible with the left and right arrows of the keyboard. Errors occurred when the distance of the two disks exceeded 1.1 degrees. Performance (the sum of the errors in one block) was reported on the screen at the end of each block. Behavioral data were defined as the number of occasions when the ball left the target area. The performance of the two conditions were compared in a t-test.
The vMMN-related irrelevant stimuli appeared around the task-relevant stimuli. In the Object condition, diamonds and diamonds without two of their parallel lines appeared alternately. Six identical objects (75.5 cd/m2) were presented in two rows and three columns against a medium-grey background (20.1 cd/m2). There was no inter-stimulus interval between these patterns. In the offset events either the two 45-degree sides or the two 135-degree sides of the diamonds were omitted. These two patterns were presented in oddball sequences, with either the left or the right being tilted as a deviant (p = 0.2). In one block there were 95 offset events, 76 standard bow ties, and 19 deviant ones. According to the reverse control principle, both the left, and the right-tilted bow ties served as the deviant and standard (6 sequences for each). Altogether 570 stimuli were presented in each deviant-standard direction. The stimulus duration of all three patterns was 520 ms (with +/- 40 ms jitter in 13.3 ms steps).
In the Text condition there were oblique lines with both 45-degree and 135-degree orientations. The lines were randomly dispersed within the stimulus field, but the number of tilted lines, the size of the lines and the luminances were equal to those in the Object condition, and in all other respects, the two conditions were identical. Fig 1A demonstrates the screen of task-related and vMMN-related stimuli in the two conditions, and Fig 1B shows the stimulus sequence.
EEG recording, ERP acquisition and measurement
EEG was recorded with a Neuroscan recording system (SynampsRT amplifier, Compumedics Abbotsford Ltd, Australia, EasyCap, Advanced Medical Equipments Ltd, Horsham, UK; Ag/AgCl electrodes, DC-200 Hz, sampling rate: 1000 Hz). Thirty-eight electrode locations were used, in accordance with the extended 10–20 system. The ground electrode was placed on the forehead. An electrode on the tip of the nose served as a reference. HEOG and VEOG were recorded with bipolar configurations between two electrodes placed laterally to the outer canthi of the two eyes, or above and below the left eye respectively.
The EEG signal was analysed with a MATLAB script developed in our lab. First, it was filtered offline with a noncausal Kaiser-windowed finite impulse response filter (low pass: 30; high-pass: 0.1 Hz). Epochs of 600 ms (including 100 ms prestimulus interval serving as baseline) were extracted for all deviants and for those standards that immediately preceded the deviants. Epochs with larger than 100 μV, or smaller than 2 μV voltage change, were considered artifacts and rejected from the further processing. ERPs were calculated by averaging the extracted epochs. According to the reverse control principle, epochs from both experimental (oddball and reverse) sequences were entered into the averaging process.
Event-related potentials were averaged separately for the two conditions (object and text), and within the conditions for the two events (offset and onset) and for the two probabilities (deviant, standard). Only those ERPs to the standard stimuli were included in the averaging that appeared before a deviant. The number of averaged epochs was 3,828 and 3,827 for deviants and last standards which was 84% of all epochs.
On the basis of the results of our previous study [4] we calculated an occipital ROI (O1, Oz, O2) from the deviant minus standard difference potentials. In the previous study vMMN emerged at the occipital locations within the 120–202 ms range, therefore in the present study we calculated the mean activity within this range. VMMN amplitudes were compared in a two-way ANOVA with factors of Condition (object, texture) and Event (offset, onset).
To control the reliability of difference between the ERPs to the deviant and standard, within the possible vMMN range we calculated series of t-tests over the 100–300 ms range at O1, Oz and O2 electrodes on the deviant minus standard difference potentials (difference from zero). As a criterion of 25 consecutive t-values (25 ms) were significant (p<0.05) at least over two locations. We obtained significant values within 116–178 ms, 139–195 ms and 155–208 ms ranges (i.e., 62 ms, 56 ms and 53 ms) for object offset, texture offset and texture onset, respectively.
As unexpected findings, in comparison to the standard stimuli, following vMMN, both offset and onset deviants elicited posterior positivity. Furthermore, over the anterior locations positive difference potentials emerged, and these positivities were larger for the offset stimuli. We measured the peak latency and the amplitude values of these positive differences in the posterior and anterior ROIs (O1, Oz, O2 and F3, Fz and F4, respectively). Latencies were measured as the largest positive component within 200–300 ms, and amplitudes were measured as the mean activity of this range. These measures were analysed in ANOVAs with factors of Condition (object, text) and Event (offset, onset).
To compare the ERPs to stimulus onset and offset on the exogenous activity, we measured the latencies and amplitudes of the posterior exogenous negative component (N1) on the occipital ROI (O1, Oz, O2). N1 component was identified in the 120–200 ms window as the highest negative-going deflection, and its latency was measured on the standard stimuli. Amplitudes were measured as the means of a +/- 5 ms range around the group average. The amplitudes and latencies were compared in ANOVAs with factors of Condition (object, texture) and Events (offset, onset). In the ANOVAs effect size was calculated as partial eta squared (ηp2).
Results
Behavioral results
Performance (errors) was characterized by the number of cases when the distance of the two discs exceeded 1.1 deg. Performance was fairly high, and the group average of errors were 5.56 (SD = 1.97) and 7.17 (SD = 4.23) in the object and texture conditions, respectively. In a t-test, the difference was not significant.
Event-related potentials
As Fig 2 shows, over the posterior locations deviant object offset, texture offset and texture onset elicited a negative deviant minus standard posterior difference potential, but object onset did not elicit posterior negativity. To replicate the results [4] we calculated vMMN amplitude within the range of significant difference of the previous study (120–202 ms). We calculated an occipital ROI from O1, Oz and O2 locations. In an ANOVA with factors of Condition (object, texture) and Event (offset, onset). We obtained significant main effect of Event, F(1,17) = 5.23, p = 0.035, ηp2 = 0.24, and interaction F[1,17] = 73.28, p = 0.029, ηp2 = 0.25). Following the negative difference potentials, for the deviant offset events a positivity emerged. As Fig 3 shows, over the anterior locations the difference potentials for the offset stimuli were also positive. We conducted separate ANOVAs for the posterior (O1, Oz, O2) and anterior (F3, Fz, F4) ROIs with factors of Condition and Event for the 200–300 ms range. For the posterior ROI the main effect of Event was significant, F(1,17) = 8.39, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.33. In a similar ANOVA for the anterior positivity the main effect of Event was also significant, F(1,17) = 8.26, p = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.30. Table 1 shows the amplitude values of the negativities and positivities.
The scalp distributions are calculated for the ranges with significant deviant minus standard differences.
As Figs 2 and 3 show, positive difference potentials emerged over the posterior and anterior locations. To explore the appearance of the positivities in the two conditions to the two events, we conducted ANOVAs on the mean amplitudes within the 200–300 ms latency range. For both ROIs the Event main effect was significant: F(1,17) = 8.38, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.33 for the occipital (O1, Oz, O2) ROI, F(1,17) = 9.75, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.33 and F(1,17) = 8.26, p = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.30 for the anterior ROI (F3, Fz, F4), respectively, indicating larger positivity to the offset events.
To compare the ERPs in the texture and object conditions to the onset and offset events, ANOVAs with factors of Condition and Event were calculated for the peak latency and the mean amplitude values (+/- 5 ms around the group average). Latency values were fairly similar, 160 ms, 157 ms, 158 ms and 162 ms for object offset, object onset, texture offset and texture onset, respectively. Accordingly, neither the main effects, nor the interaction were significant. As Table 1 shows, onset events elicited larger N1 than offset events. In the ANOVA the Condition main effect was significant, F(1,17) = 22.31, p<0.001, ηp2 = 0.57. According to the significant interaction, F(1,17) = 20.71, p<0.001, ηp2 = 0.55, the difference was due to the larger N1 to the object onset.
Discussion
On the basis of the object-related representation of environmental events we expected vMMN emergence to object offset, but we were uncertain whether the offset of visual textures could elicit vMMN. Furthermore, we expected no onset-related vMMN with object onset deviancy. According to the results object as well as and texture offset elicited vMMN. Concerning the reappearance (onset) of objects, there were no detectable ERP differences between the onset after the frequently and infrequently vanishing lines of the diamonds. In other words, in the object condition we did not register vMMN. This result replicated our previous finding [4]. However, emergence of vMMN to texture offset and onset requires the revision of the view suggested by Sulykos and colleagues [4]. In the discussion of this study we claimed that the memory system underlying vMMN represented objects as wholes (Gestalts), and the procedure was a model of partial occlusion of the objects. Therefore VMMN emerged when frequent occlusions were replaced by rare ones. Furthermore, reappearance of the object, irrespective of the previous (deviant or standard) offset was a fully predictable event, therefore this event did not elicit vMMN. However, as the offset-related vMMN of the texture condition of the present study shows, vanishing of particular bar orientations were sufficient for eliciting vMMN. Importantly, there was an obvious difference between the object and texture conditions, i.e., appearance of onset-related vMMN in texture condition. To preserve an aspect of the object-related representation, we claim that in the object condition the system underlying vMMN treated the offset and onset events as units (disappearance and reappearance of parts of the objects). However, the system underlying vMMN treats texture offset and onset separately, i.e., rare vs. frequent offset of particular line orientations, and rare vs. frequent onset of particular line orientations. In other words, whereas the representation of the object survived the offset period, texture onset and offset were treated as separate events. This explanation preserves the notion that vMMN is a surprise-related component elicited by non-reinforced predictions (e.g. [4, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14]), even if in principle, onset is a fully expected event in both conditions.
Posterior positivity following the vMMN appeared in previous studies [15, 16]. In the present study this positivity appeared only to the offset events. Similarly, anterior positivity appeared in some studies [7, 17] to deviant stimuli. However, connection of these positivities to the processes underlying vMMN and their functional significance is unclear. Furthermore, some recent studies reported positive mismatch responses emerged in later latency ranges [9, 18]. Due to the lack of a priory expectation, as a speculative explanation, the positivities are connected to a further processing of the more salient offset stimulation; in this case anterior structures are involved in the processing of deviant events. The predictive coding view [5] is capable of explain these ERP effects as a modification of the environmental model. However, relations between vMMN and the subsequent positivities require further research.
Onset events usually elicit ERPs with larger amplitudes than offset events [19, 20]. We obtained similar results. Onset-related N1 was larger in the object condition. While the reappearing bars were similar in the two conditions, we have no post-hoc explanation for this unexpected result.
In conclusion, offset stimuli after a longer onset period potentially saturated the input-related visual structures. However, infrequently vanishing stimulus elements elicited the signature of automatic deviance detection, the visual mismatch negativity (vMMN). In case of Gestalt-like stimuli the memory system underlying vMMN is capable of predicting the reappearance of the stimuli; therefore stimulus reappearance (onset) does not elicit vMMN. However, at texture stimuli (unconnected stimulus elements) the connection between the onset and offset events are weaker, and both kinds of stimulus events elicit vMMN. As a tentative suggestion, in a visual scene disappearance can be a more salient event than reappearance, and the more salient event may lead to further processing, as indicated by both posterior and anterior activity.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by National Research, Development and Innovation Office (NKFIH), Grant 119587.
References
- 1. Kremlaček J., Kreegipuu K, Tales A, Astikainen P, Pöldver N, Näätänen R, et al. Visual mismatch negativity (vMMN): A review and meta-analysis of studies in psychiatric and neurological disorders. Cortex. 2016: 80: 76–112. pmid:27174389
- 2. Stefanics G, Astikainen P, Czigler I. Visual mismatch negativity (vMMN): a prediction error signal in the visual modality. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015: 8, 1074. pmid:25657621
- 3. Stefanics G, Kremlaček J, Czigler I. Visual mismatch negativity: A predicting coding view. Front. Hum. Nurosci. 2014: 8, 666.
- 4. Sulykos I, Gaál Zs. A, Czigler I. Visual mismatch negativity to vanishing parts of objects in younger and older adults. Plos One, 2017: 12, e0188928.
- 5. Friston K. A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2005: 360, 815–836.
- 6. Winkler I. Interpreting the mismatch negativity. Journal of Psychophysiology 2007: 21 147–163.
- 7. Czigler I, Balázs L, Winkler I. Memory-based detection of task-irrelevant visual changes. Psychophysiology 2002: 39: 869–873. pmid:12462515
- 8. Kimura M, Katayama J, Ohira H, Schröger E. Visual mismatch negativity: New evidence from the equiprobable paradigm. Psychophysiology 2009: 46: 402–409. pmid:19207207
- 9. Amado S, Kovacs G. Does surprise enhancement or repetition suppression explain visual mismatch negativity? European Journal of Neuroscience 2016: 43: 1590–1600. pmid:27108896
- 10. Czigler I, Winkler I. Evidence from auditory and visual event-related potential (ERP) studies of deviance detection (MMN and vMMN) linking predictive coding theories and perceptual object representations. International Journal of Psychophysiology 2012: 83: 132–143. pmid:22047947
- 11. Garrido MI, Kilner JM, Stephan KE. Friston KJ. The mismatch negativity: A review of underlying mechanisms. Clini. Neurophysiol. 2008: 120: 453–463.
- 12. Kimura M, Takeda Y. Automatic prediction regarding the next state of a visual object: Electrophysiological indicators of prediction match and mismatch. Brain Research 2015: 1626: 31–41. pmid:25598206
- 13. Sel A, Harding R, Tsakiris M. Electrophysiological correlates of self-specific prediction errors in the human brain. Neuroimage, 2015: 125: 13–24. pmid:26455899
- 14. Wacongne C, Changeux J.-P, Dehaene S. A Neuronal Model of Predictive Coding Accounting for the Mismatch Negativity. J. Neurosci. 2012: 32: 3665–3678. pmid:22423089
- 15. Winkler I, Czigler I, Sussman E, Horvath J. Balazs L. Preattentive binding of auditory and visual stimulus features. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005: 17: 320–339. pmid:15811243
- 16. File D, Sulykos I, Czigler I. Automatic change detection and spatial attention: A visual mismatch negativity study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2018 First published: 15 May.
- 17.
Heslenfeld DJ. Visual Mismatch Negativity. In: Polich J. (eds) Detection of Change. Springer, Boston, MA, 2003: 41–59.
- 18. Amado S, Stoyanova P, Kovacs G. Visual mismatch responses and fMRI signal adaptation correlate in the occipital-temporal cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 2016: 347: 77–87.
- 19.
Jeffreys D.A. Component analysis of transient pattern VEPs. in: Spekreijse H, Tweel L.H van der (Eds.) Spatial Contrast, Report of a Workshop. North-Holland Publ, Amsterdam; 1977:80–84.
- 20. Shawkat FS, Kriss A. Effects of experimental scotomata on sequential pattern-onset, pattern-reversal and pattern-offset visual evoked potentials. Doc. Ophtalm. 1997: 94: 307–320.