Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Covariation of the endocranium and splanchnocranium during great ape ontogeny

  • Nadia A. Scott ,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    nadia_scott@eva.mpg.de

    Affiliations Department of Human Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz, Leipzig, Germany, Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research, Martinstrasse, Klosterneuburg, Austria

  • André Strauss,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Human Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz, Leipzig, Germany

  • Jean-Jacques Hublin,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Human Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz, Leipzig, Germany

  • Philipp Gunz,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Human Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz, Leipzig, Germany

  • Simon Neubauer

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Human Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz, Leipzig, Germany

Abstract

That great ape endocranial shape development persists into adolescence indicates that the splanchnocranium succeeds brain growth in driving endocranial development. However, the extent of this splanchnocranial influence is unknown. We applied two-block partial least squares analyses of Procrustes shape variables on an ontogenetic series of great ape crania to explore the covariation of the endocranium (the internal braincase) and splanchnocranium (face, or viscerocranium). We hypothesized that a transition between brain growth and splanchnocranial development in the establishment of final endocranial form would be manifest as a change in the pattern of shape covariation between early and adolescent ontogeny. Our results revealed a strong pattern of covariation between endocranium and splanchnocranium, indicating that chimpanzees, gorillas, and orangutans share a common tempo and mode of morphological integration from the eruption of the deciduous dentition onwards to adulthood: a reflection of elongating endocranial shape and continuing splanchnocranial prognathism. Within this overarching pattern, we noted that species variation exists in magnitude and direction, and that the covariation between the splanchnocranium and endocranium is somewhat weaker in early infancy compared to successive age groups. When correcting our covariation analyses for allometry, we found that an ontogenetic signal remains, signifying that allometric variation alone is insufficient to account for all endocranial-splanchnocranial developmental integration. Finally, we assessed the influence of the cranial base, which acts as the interface between the face and endocranium, on the shape of the vault using thin-plate spline warping. We found that not all splanchnocranial shape changes during development are tightly integrated with endocranial shape. This suggests that while the developmental expansion of the brain is the main driver of endocranial shape during early ontogeny, endocranial development from infancy onwards is moulded by the splanchnocranium in conjunction with the neurocranium.

Introduction

Palaeontologists have long exploited the endocranium and its negative imprint, the endocast, as a proxy with which to interrogate the size evolution of the brain in the primate fossil record [14]. Over the past two decades, these efforts have been greatly extended by employing virtual anthropological methods [520] to infer the evolution of both brain growth (an ontogenetic change in size) and development (an ontogenetic change in shape) from computed tomographic scans of extant and extinct primate endocrania in ontogenetic series [2130]. While these described endocranial developmental changes are often taken as being wholly interpretable as brain developmental changes, several authors have stressed that the endocranium is not shaped exclusively by the brain. This is because, mechanistically, developmental and evolutionary changes in facial size and morphology, cranial bone thickness, and cranial musculature must also influence endocranial shape in addition to the tempo and mode of brain growth [2425, 2728, 3034]. From this standpoint, the brain-endocranium relationship is not a true one-to-one correspondence; rather, endocranial structure is the age- and species-specific product of manifold internal (brain/meninx) and external (splanchnocranium/neurocranium/musculature) forces. Our challenge, then, is to delimit the extent of these forces in shaping the primate endocranium, a task further complicated by the likelihood that such developmental forces are liable to be bi-directional [3538].

Development is not a stochastic process but rather a highly regulated one that produces predictable results. According to Gould [3940] and Alberch et al. [4142], it is far easier to regulate a resilient ontogeny than to alter it. Fundamental to this stability is conservation or canalization: an evolved robustness of developmental pathways to genetic and environmental disturbances that ultimately results in decreased adult phenotypic variance [4346]. Yet, for novel forms to arise, these genetically optimized developmental canals cannot be wholly stable [47], but rather plastic in parts. Such plasticity has been ascribed by Gould [39] to natural selection acting upon variations in the timing and rate of development. This is of particular relevance as evidence suggests that developmental conservation (or canalization) is appreciable in the mammal cranium [48]. Within the primate order, we have shown previously that extant hominoids undergo highly conserved endocranial shape changes following the eruption of the deciduous dentition [2425, 28], a result recently corroborated by Zollikofer and colleagues [30]. While we found that endocranial developmental patterns were overall conserved in tempo and mode, great ape species differed in the amount of expansion of the anterior and posterior cranial fossae [28]. This phenomenon is especially salient for several reasons. Firstly, such differences in the amount of shape change, together with an early (prenatal) establishment of species-specific morphology, likely drive and accentuate the disparity of adult hominoid endocranial shape. According to Keith [49], such fossal expansions are corollaries of dental eruption, masticatory musculature growth and nuchal area expansion (external factors), not brain growth (internal factor). Secondly, that these species differences emerged following the complete eruption of deciduous dentition—after the cessation of brain growth (reviewed in [26])—indicates that endocranial shape continues to change after the cessation of brain growth. These results then suggest that, during infancy, the splanchnocranium succeeds the brain in the determination of final (adult) endocranial form [5052].

Thus, understanding the process of conservation itself, as driven by integration and allometry [48], is central to disentangling the relative contributions of internal and external morphological forces on the final establishment of species-specific endocranial form. Strong integration, or covariation between developmental modules, constrains evolution, whereas weakening covariation between modules permits independent specialization in response to differing selective pressures. Integration, therefore, can permit a fine mediation between species differentiation and canalization. As splanchnocranial and endocranial covariation balances dietary preferences on the one hand and encephalization on the other, such integration must be restrictive in channelling variation, yet permissive enough to act as a source of variation [5355]. In mammals, cranial development must accommodate an enormous range of morphological variation in the gnathic apparatus. Because of this, we would expect mammalian cranial covariation patterns to be conserved, as was demonstrated across multiple mammalian orders [56]. Following Cheverud [5758], such conservation of integration patterns is likely testament to extensive pleiotropy in cranial development.

As pointed out by Klingenberg [59], there is a large body of literature examining the morphological integration of the primate skull, as primates were employed as research subjects for both the revival of interest in morphological integration following Olson and Miller [60] and the development of geometric morphometrics. Using traditional and geometric methods, previous authors have consistently demonstrated that the adult primate cranium is morphologically integrated, regardless of which cranial components—e.g. face, neurocranium, cranial base, occipital bun and mandible—were subjected to analysis [37, 6172]. Furthermore, this effect has been consistently identified in studies exploring morphological integration across ontogeny [57, 70, 7375], which found that cranial developmental patterns are similar across great apes. Yet, while the literature on primate cranial developmental integration is indeed considerable, relatively little work has been done considering the endocranium itself as a developmental module. Bastir and colleagues [76] examined univariate brain and facial size as independent variables in a partial least squares analysis with basicranial shape, demonstrating that both values are important factors in determining basicranial orientation and morphology. Recently, Zollikofer and colleagues [30] utilized extant hominid virtual endocasts as a developmental module in order to determine the effects of cranial integration on endocranial morphology across ontogeny, with the aim to test the spatial packing and facial orientation hypotheses. Their results affirmed that endocranial shape is primarily moulded by cranial integration, with a correspondingly smaller proportion of variation attributable to brain morphology itself.

While the concepts of morphological integration and allometry are closely intertwined, allometry has been at the forefront of anatomical and evolutionary study for a much longer period of time. As summarized by Klingenberg and Marugán-Lobón[77], the definition and uses of allometry have evolved from the days of Huxley [78] to the geometric morphometrics era[79]. Klingenberg [80] divides allometry into two groups: (1) the Gould–Mosimann school, which defines allometry as the covariation of shape with size, and (2) the Huxley–Jolicoeur school, which defines allometry as the covariation among morphological features that all contain size information. For the former, allometry is implemented by a multivariate regression of shape variables on size, while for the latter this implementation is performed by plotting Procrustes form space [77, 80]. According to Gould [8182], ontogenetic allometric pathways operate as “channels of positive constraint”; allometry, then, like integration, enables phenotypic variation along this line of least resistance and constrains it by delimiting the adult morphospace to a narrow range of shape disparity [82]. As much of shape variation can be in fact allometric variation, thus much of morphological integration can be due to the effects of allometry [77]. Allometry, therefore, can be a confounding factor in shape covariation analyses if unaccounted for. Because allometry has emerged as an important factor in hominoid endocranial development [2125, 28], it is likely that allometry plays a crucial role in integrating endocranial development. For the present study, we correct for allometry in order to focus solely on those aspects of shape covariation that are unrelated to size variation.

Here, we explore which factors determine great ape endocranial shape variation at different stages of ontogeny. More specifically, by defining the endocranium and splanchnocranium as developmental modules, we analyze their covariation shape patterns among great ape species and across postnatal ontogeny. If splanchnocranial development determines endocranial structure following the end of brain growth, then we would expect to see a change in covariation patterns around this juncture. Based on the findings described in our previous work[28] and Zollikofer and colleagues[30], we hypothesize the following: (1) a shared ontogenetic pattern of covariation describes endocranial and splanchnocranial shape change in great apes; (2) species-specific variation in the direction and magnitude of shape change contributes to adult endocranial and splanchnocranial morphologies; (3) allometry alone is insufficient to describe the observed covariation between endocranial and splanchnocranial shape change; (4) endocranial shape changes are not driven solely by the development of the splanchnocranium and its associated changes in the cranial base.

Firstly, we use partial least squares analysis to establish whether morphological covariation between the splanchnocranial module and the endocranial module varies across ontogeny and among chimpanzees, gorillas and orangutans. Secondly, we aim to determine to what degree allometry is manifest in the observed ontogenetic shape covariation pattern, as per Klingenberg [80]. We will accomplish this by comparing centroid sizes for each developmental module, plotting Procrustes form space and by re-running our partial least squares analysis accounting for allometry, i.e. by analyzing the shape residuals for each module following regression on centroid size. Lastly, we will determine whether endocranial shape is exclusively driven by developmental integration with the splanchnocranium via the cranial base. Using thin-plate spline warping, we will test this by standardizing each vault for the effect of its accompanying cranial base, the idea being that the cranial base acts as the interface between splanchnocranium and endocranium during development [32, 8387]. Thus, any residual vault shape variation would be indicative of forces that are independent of cranial base, and therefore splanchnocranial, development.

Material and methods

Sample

Our cross-sectional sample comprised computed tomographic scans of museum-housed dried crania from Gorilla gorilla (n = 75), Pan troglodytes (n = 69, including scans of two whole frozen neonates) and Pongo pygmaeus (n = 77), covering the lifespan from infancy to adulthood (Table 1). No permits were required for the described study, which complied with all relevant regulations. Sex attribution was drawn from museum records where available, while age groups were based on molar eruption (1 = no erupted deciduous dentition; 2 = incompletely erupted deciduous dentition; 3 = completely erupted deciduous dentition; 4 = ≥ one erupted M1; 5 = ≥ one erupted M2; 6 = ≥ one erupted M3). For full specimen information, including provenance, please see S1 Table.

Segmentation and measurement

Endocasts were produced virtually using two- and three-dimensional semi-automated segmentation [14]. To measure the endocasts, we placed endocranial surface landmarks that encompassed the geometry of the endocranial cavity as delineated by the cranial fossae (anatomical landmarks = 29, plus semilandmarks on curves, based on our previous work [24]). We then measured on these same individuals anatomical landmarks (n = 23) and semilandmarks on curves that encompassed the geometry of the splanchnocranium; such division of the ectocranium into modules is inherently arbitrary and our definition here of the splanchnocranium does not comprise the mandible. For splanchnocranial and endocranial landmark information, please see Table 2; for full landmark data, see S2 and S3 Tables. Semilandmarks on curves were resampled to equivalent point count, and endocranial surface semilandmarks were calculated following the measurement protocol developed by us previously [24], in which all semilandmarks were slid iteratively so as to minimize the thin-plate bending energy of each specimen against the sample mean shape [8889]. Generalized Procrustes analyses [9092], whereby coordinates of the 309 endocranial and 202 facial anatomical landmarks and slid semilandmarks are transformed into Procrustes shape variables, were performed separately for endocranial and splanchnocranial landmark blocks.

Analyses

To assess covariation, two-block partial least squares (PLS) analyses of Procrustes shape variables were performed. As an iterative method, this analysis yields vector pairs—singular warps, as per Rohlf and Corti [93]—ranked according to the maximum covariance between the two blocks of Procrustes shape data, here the endocranium and the splanchnocranium. In this paper, we use the terms PLS and singular warps interchangeably. Landmarks and semilandmarks were superimposed separately using Procrustes superimposition. The Procrustes shape variables were then mean-centred (standardized) by subtracting the species-specific age group mean from the shape data of each individual prior to computation of the covariance matrix, as recommended by Mitteroecker and Bookstein [94]; for species-specific analyses, this covariance matrix was composed solely of the species under investigation. A PLS analysis was performed firstly with all species combined (Fig 1 and S1 Fig). Following Bookstein and colleagues [74], individual PLS axes are termed singular warps. In this paper, we use both terms interchangeably.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Two-block, age group mean-centred PLS analysis between endocranium and the splanchnocranium: Pooled species, singular warp 1.

(A) Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 2–6 for each species; age group labels denote age group means, while lines are B-spline curves of the average species-specific trajectories. Colours: green = chimpanzee; dark grey = gorilla; orange = orangutan. (B) Sagittal (left) and coronal (right) visualizations of the shape changes corresponding to PLS 1 in the endocranium (top) and splanchnocranium (bottom), from negative (dashed line) to positive values (solid line).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.g001

Next, to analyze ontogenetic trajectories in shape space, Procrustes shape variables were ordinated by separate principal components analyses (PCA) for endocranial and splanchnocranial data (Figs 2 and 3). Similarly, separate principal components analyses for endocranial and splanchnocranial data were performed on Procrustes form variables to analyze ontogenetic trajectories in form space (Fig 4). Principal components (PCs) were visualized as mean shapes corresponding to the negative and positive limits of the principal components plots (Figs 2 and 3). To compare ontogenetic growth changes between modules, we plotted the centroid size of the endocranium versus the centroid size of the splanchnocranium (Fig 5).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Ontogenetic shape trajectories as ontogenetic sequences of specimens in shape space: Endocranial.

(A-B) Principal components analysis of Procrustes shape variables from the eruption of deciduous dentition (age group 2) to adulthood (age group 6). (A) Principal component 1 vs. principal component 2. (B) Principal component 1 vs. principal component 3. Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 2–6 for each species; age group labels denote age group means, while lines are B-spline curves of the average species-specific trajectories. Colours: green = chimpanzee; dark grey = gorilla; orange = orangutan. (C) Visualizations of sagittal (left) and coronal (right) shape changes corresponding to principal component 1, from negative values (dashed line) to positive values (solid line).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Ontogenetic shape trajectories as ontogenetic sequences of specimens in shape space: Splanchnocranial.

(A-B) Principal components analysis of Procrustes shape variables from the eruption of deciduous dentition (age group 2) to adulthood (age group 6). (A) Principal component 1 vs. principal component 2. (B) Principal component 1 vs. principal component 3. Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 2–6 for each species; age group labels denote age group means, while lines are B-spline curves of the average species-specific trajectories. Colours: green = chimpanzee; dark grey = gorilla; orange = orangutan. (C) Visualizations of sagittal (left) and coronal (right) shape changes corresponding to principal component 1, from negative values (dashed line) to positive values (solid line).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Ontogenetic shape trajectories in Procrustes form space.

(A-B) Principal components analysis of Procrustes shape variables from the eruption of deciduous dentition (age group 2) to adulthood (age group 6). (A) Endocranial form space; principal component 1 vs. principal component 2. (B) Splanchnocranial form space: principal component 1 vs. principal component 2. Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 2–6 for each species; age group labels denote age group means, while lines are B-spline curves of the average species-specific trajectories. Colours: green = chimpanzee; dark grey = gorilla; orange = orangutan.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Centroid size of endocranium vs. centroid size of splanchnocranium.

Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 1–6 for each species; age group labels denote age group means, while lines are B-spline curves of the average species-specific trajectories. Colours: green = chimpanzee; dark grey = gorilla; orange = orangutan.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.g005

To determine to what degree allometry affects the PLS results, we performed an additional PLS analysis on endocranial and splanchnocranial data following regression of these data on centroid size (Fig 6 and S2 Fig). Lastly, we performed PLS analyses successively with each species separately (Figs 79 and S3S5 Figs) to gain a better understanding of differences that may be obscured by pooling multiple species [9495]. It is important to note that our assessments of trajectory similarities and differences are derived from a visual assessment, rather than additional statistical testing.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Two-block, age group mean-centred PLS analysis of endocranial and splanchnocranial shape residuals after regression on size: Pooled species, singular warp 1.

(A) Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 2–6 for each species; age group labels denote age group means, while lines are B-spline curves of the average species-specific trajectories. Colours: green = chimpanzee; dark grey = gorilla; orange = orangutan. (B) Sagittal (left) and coronal (right) visualizations of the shape changes corresponding to PLS 1 in the endocranium (top) and splanchnocranium (bottom), from negative (dashed line) to positive values (solid line).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.g006

thumbnail
Fig 7. Two-block, age group mean-centred PLS analysis between endocranium and the splanchnocranium: Chimpanzees, singular warp 1.

(A) Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 1–6; age group labels denote age group means, while line is B-spline curve of the average chimpanzee-specific trajectory. (B) Sagittal (left) and coronal (right) visualizations of the shape changes corresponding to PLS 1 in the endocranium (top) and splanchnocranium (bottom), from negative (dashed line) to positive values (solid line).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.g007

thumbnail
Fig 8. Two-block, age group mean-centred PLS analysis between endocranium and the splanchnocranium: Gorillas, singular warp 1.

(A) Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 2–6; age group labels denote age group means, while line is B-spline curve of the average gorilla-specific trajectory. (B) Sagittal (left) and coronal (right) visualizations of the shape changes corresponding to PLS 1 in the endocranium (top) and splanchnocranium (bottom), from negative (dashed line) to positive values (solid line).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.g008

thumbnail
Fig 9. Two-block, age group mean-centred PLS analysis between endocranium and the splanchnocranium: Orangutans, singular warp 1.

(A) Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 1–6; age group labels denote age group means, while line is B-spline curve of the average orangutan-specific trajectory. (B) Sagittal (left) and coronal (right) visualizations of the shape changes corresponding to PLS 1 in the endocranium (top) and splanchnocranium (bottom), from negative (dashed line) to positive values (solid line).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.g009

Because the braincase is tightly integrated with the face throughout development, any size and shape changes in the face also affect the endocranium, especially at the cranial base: the interface between these two cranial modules. To explore to what degree endocranial shape changes during development are driven by cranial base changes, we analyzed developmental shape changes of the cranial vault after standardizing for shape differences in the cranial base. In other words, we used geometric morphometric methods to ask: if all individuals had the exact same cranial base shape, what would their vaults look like? To this end, we first performed a Procrustes superimposition and divided our endocranial landmark set into an endocranial base component and an endocranial vault component as used previously [24]. We then removed the contribution of the endocranial base to endocranial shape by thin-plate spline (TPS) warping [96] the endocranial base shape coordinates of each specimen to the consensus shape of the endocranial base; the respective landmarks and semilandmarks on the endocranial vault were warped using this TPS interpolation [88]. Principal components analysis was then used to ordinate the resultant shape changes attributable to the cranial vault independent of the cranial base (Fig 10). If shape changes in the cranial base linked to splanchnocranial development did account for all endocranial shape changes, then a principal components analysis of these TPS-warped endocranial vaults would only reveal noise and not an ontogenetic signal.

thumbnail
Fig 10. Principal components analysis of vault shape after standardizing for endocranial base shape as the interface between endocranial and splanchnocranial shapes.

(A-B) Principal components analysis of Procrustes shape variables from the eruption of deciduous dentition (age group 2) to adulthood (age group 6). (A) Principal component 1 vs. principal component 2. (B) Principal component 1 vs. principal component 3. Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 2–6 for each species; age group labels denote age group means, while lines are B-spline curves of the average species-specific trajectories. Colours: green = chimpanzee; dark grey = gorilla; orange = orangutan. (C) A visualization of standardized chimpanzee (left), gorilla (middle) and orangutan (right) standardized vault shapes for each individual.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.g010

Results

Ontogenetic shape covariation between species

The results of our pooled PLS analysis of covariance demonstrate that a major (r = 0.83) axis of covariation (singular warp 1) defines splanchnocranial and endocranial morphological changes during ontogeny, explaining 64% of the total covariance (Fig 1A). Taken as a whole, these data highlight a conserved pattern of ontogenetic integration among chimpanzees (green), gorillas (dark grey) and orangutans (orange) from the eruption of the deciduous dentition (age group 2) onwards to adulthood (age group 6). This vector shows that species differ in the amount of shape change experienced along this shared axis of covariation, whereby the magnitude (vector length) of endocranial and splanchnocranial shape changes between age group means is overall highest in orangutans and lowest in chimpanzees, with gorillas intermediate to both. For chimpanzees, the greatest amount of shape change occurs between age groups 4 and 5, while in gorillas, the greatest comparable changes are between age groups 3 and 4, and again to a lesser extent between age groups 4 and 5. Along this axis of covariation (singular warp 1), orangutans are distinct in this regard, undergoing large amounts of shape change both early in ontogeny (between age groups 2 and 3) and in late adolescence (age group 5 to 6). With respect to direction, gorilla and chimpanzee covariation trajectories parallel each other until age group 3 in gorillas and age group 4 in chimpanzees, whereupon they begin to diverge slightly. Conversely, orangutans are again exceptional to the overall pattern: the orangutan trajectory is entirely non-overlapping at age group 2, whereupon it overlaps with the African apes at age group 3; at this juncture, however, its direction continues at an angle to that of chimpanzees and gorillas.

The corresponding endocranial shape changes (Fig 1B) describe a lengthening of the anterior cranial fossae, an antero-inferior expansion of the temporal poles and a downwards rotation of the foramen magnum, while the splanchnocranial shape changes denote an antero-inferior extension of the maxilla, a proportional reduction of the orbits and an inferior extension of the nasal area. Because all specimens are scaled to unit centroid size during the Procrustes superimposition, the relative size of the orbits decreases in the visualization of the shape changes associated with singular warp 1, indicating that the actual size of the face is increasing along the positive direction of the y-axis. As the face grows and becomes more prognathic during postnatal ontogeny, the endocast becomes more elongated antero-posteriorly.

Higher PLS axes recover considerably weaker patterns of covariation between the endocranium and the splanchnocranium without a clear, interpretable ontogenetic signal (S1 Fig). From singular warp 2, accounting for just 11% of the sample covariation, it is apparent that this axis is predominately driven by ontogenetic changes in gorillas and chimpanzees, capturing little ontogenetic signal in orangutans (S1 Fig; middle). Singular warp 3 (S1 Fig; right) only explains 6% of the pooled sample covariation.

Ontogenetic shape variation between species

To better understand the covariation patterns revealed by our partial least squares analyses in Fig 1 and S1 Fig, we next ordinated our endocranial and splanchnocranial shape data in separate principal components analyses, allowing us to examine each concomitant shape variation pattern in isolation. In endocranial shape space, the first principal component bears the strongest ontogenetic signal, separating the youngest and oldest individuals (Fig 2A). Regarding the second principal component, the African apes overlap until late adolescence, at which point gorilla endocranial shape development exceeds that of chimpanzees; orangutans are largely distinct from this African ape pattern and instead exhibit a more curved trajectory (Fig 2A). Along the third component, gorillas diverge from the general pattern at late adolescence (Fig 2B). Overall, these plots of endocranial shape space indicate that great ape trajectories differ in degree but show a marked overall likeness [28].

Splanchnocranial shape development trajectories (Fig 3) closely mirror those of the endocranium (Fig 2), both with regards to trajectory direction and magnitude. However, while it is clear that great ape trajectories exhibit overall directional similarity, diversity in magnitude is apparent along the first principal component (Fig 3A). As trajectory magnitude corresponds to the amount of shape change, this indicates that chimpanzees undergo considerably less splanchnocranial shape change development than do orangutans and gorillas in a comparative developmental timeframe (from the incomplete eruption of deciduous dentition to adulthood). Along the second principal component, orangutans are wholly distinct from the African apes (Fig 3A), while the third principal component highlights a divergence in great ape trajectory direction following adolescence (Fig 3B).

Allometric effects

From the above PLS and Procrustes shape space results, we have observed that the great ape pattern of shape covariation (Fig 1) is manifest in the underlying similarity of the endocranial and splanchnocranial trajectories in shape space (Figs 2 and 3, respectively). However, as a considerable proportion of the ontogenetic pattern of shape development is likely attributable to allometry, that is, the proportion of shape variation due to size variation, it is interesting to remove the developmental effects that size has on shape in order to focus solely on the question at hand: how shape change alone covaries between modules with ontogeny. Therefore, to determine to what degree our PLS and PCA results are explainable by allometry, we began by examining Procrustes form, or size-shape, space whereby Procrustes shape variables and their associated centroid sizes are ordinated by principal components analysis.

From Fig 4, it is evident that both endocranial (Fig 4A) and splanchnocranial (Fig 4B) primary axes are reflective of allometric shape variation. That the parallelism of the great ape trajectories in splanchnocranial form space is oriented primarily along the first principal component indicates that this axis is driven overwhelmingly by size, with higher principal components providing little explanatory value (Fig 4B). While a similar phenomenon is apparent in endocranial form space, here the effect is less marked, with the second PC also defining a proportion of the explained variance. This indicates that size alone does not account for all endocranial shape variation.

Our form analyses indicated that size increase plays a considerable role in shape development, but to dissimilar extents in endocranial versus splanchnocranial form space. To specifically compare size increases (growth) between modules, we plotted endocranial centroid size versus splanchnocranial centroid size (Fig 5). As seen in Fig 5, all great ape species follow a similar pattern of growth, defined by a dramatic endocranial size increase from age group 1 to 2 that continues more modestly from age group 2 to 3. From age group 3 onwards (the complete eruption of the deciduous dentition), endocranial size increase is less pronounced. In contrast, splanchnocranial size increases until adulthood, with relatively regular spacing between age groups, particularly after age group 3 (Fig 5). Regarding species comparisons, chimpanzee and orangutan growth patterns overlap greatly. While gorillas start at higher initial endocranial and splanchnocranial size values at age group 2, the pattern of growth itself is overall comparable to that of the smaller apes. Adolescent and adult size variation (convex hull area) in gorillas and orangutans greatly exceeds that of chimpanzees, both endocranially and splanchnocranially, likely due to prominent sexual dimorphism in these species.

Taken together, our results in Figs 4 and 5 indicate that much of endocranial and splanchnocranial shape variation is indeed allometric shape variation. Thus, to investigate great ape ontogenetic shape covariation patterns independent of the effects of size, it is imperative to correct for allometry. To determine to what degree allometry affects the PLS results, we performed an additional PLS analysis on endocranial and splanchnocranial data following regression of these data on centroid size. By analyzing the covariation of residuals, it is clear that a large amount of the major axis of covariation (r = 0.25; explained covariance = 61.5%) between splanchnocranial and endocranial shape is in fact driven by allometriceffects, as interpreted by the obvious reduction of an ontogenetic signal (Fig 6) compared to the uncorrected PLS results (Fig 1). Nevertheless, some ontogenetic signal remains, defining a similar pattern between orangutans and gorillas, with chimpanzees mirroring an abbreviated version of this covariation trajectory. From this general covariation pattern, only the earliest age group (here, age group 2; incomplete eruption of deciduous dentition) deviates, indicating that covariation between endocranium and face is different in early ontogeny, but relatively consistent among species following age group 3 (complete eruption of deciduous dentition). Specifically, splanchnocranial shape change in the transition from age group 2 to 3 exceeds that of the endocranial module, whereas subsequent to age group 3, endocranial shape changes occur with little accompanying splanchnocranial shape change.

On PLS 2 (r = 0.41; explained covariance = 6.5%), a large amount of splanchnocranial shape change occurs between age group 2 and 3 with little concomitant endocranial shape change; here, all species trajectories are parallel (S2 Fig; middle).This indicates that covariation is markedly different (weaker) in age group 2 than in subsequent age groups. PLS 3 is less informative, with little separation of age group means or species (S2 Fig; right).

Species-specific ontogenetic covariation

Thus far, it is evident that while great ape species are conserved with respect to an overarching pattern of shape change covariation between splanchnocranial and endocranial modules, each differs somewhat from one another; as noted, these deviations pertain mainly to variations in trajectory magnitude and direction. We further conducted PLS analyses on a covariance matrix of each species separately, which enables us to examine these species-specific covariation patterns in greater detail. Considering chimpanzees, this additionally allowed us to include a sample of neonates (age group 1), the inclusion of which permits the investigation of splanchnocranial and endocranial shape change covariation directly after birth. We found that chimpanzee shape covariationin neonates is entirely distinct from successive age groups (i.e. convex hull 1 is non-overlapping with convex hull 2), with the greatest overall amount of shape change occurring from age group 1 to 2 (PLS1; Fig 7A; r = 0.76). This phenomenon—a marked discontinuity between neonatal and later ontogeny—is also apparent on the third singular warp (S3 Fig; right) and is indicative of weaker covariation directly after birth: shape changes to the splanchnocranium are unmatched by comparable changes to the endocranium. On singular warp 2, covariation is also weak between age groups 1 and 2, yet here the opposite effect is observable, as a large amount of splanchnocranial shape change occurs with little corresponding shape change in the endocranium. On singular warps 1 and 3, shape covariation is greatest in adults, with relatively more shape change occurring in the splanchnocranium than in the endocranium, than in preceding age groups.

Along the first singular warp, visualizations of the shape changes of the splanchnocranium describe the difference between a relatively large-orbited splanchnocranium with no prognathism to a more prognathic adult skull with vertically compressed orbits (Fig 7B). From age group 2 to adulthood, the endocranium becomes more anterio-posteriorly elongated, the middle cranial fossae expand laterally, and the temporal poles rotate medially.

In gorillas, the ontogenetic covariation pattern along the first singular warp is both strong and positive (r = 0.9; Fig 8A), revealing a large amount of shape changes from age group 4 to 5,with a lesser amount from age group 3 to 4. Here, the covariation pattern is similar across ontogeny, with comparable amounts of shape change experienced in both the endocranium and splanchnocranium. Higher singular warps carry a clear ontogenetic signal, underscoring in particular a large amount of splanchnocranial shape change in early ontogeny (age group 2 to 3) that occurs with relatively less concomitant endocranial shape change (S4 Fig; middle and right).Shape-wise, the positive end of the first singular warp can be visualized as a narrow, oblong endocranium with an extended clivus, a splanchnocranium with angled zygomatic arches and an anteriorly extended maxilla; at the negative end, the endocranium is superiorly and laterally expanded in the middle and anterior cranial fossae, with an attenuated clivus, while the splanchnocranium has relatively larger orbits, a reduced maxilla, and a reduced angle along the zygomatic arches (Fig 8B).

In orangutans, the covariation pattern along the first singular warp (Fig 9A; r = 0.87) is more curvilinear than that of the African apes; such curvilinearity is also observable on higher singular warps (S4 Fig; middle and right). Along the first singular warp, covariation is strong, such that comparable amounts of shape change are experienced in both splanchnocranial and endocranial developmental modules. The amounts of shape change experienced between age group means is remarkably comparable across the ontogenetic signal revealed by the first singular warp (Fig 9A); on higher singular warps, this spacing is more irregular, with the greatest amounts of shape change occurring in adolescent ontogeny. As with our chimpanzee analysis, conducting a PLS of orangutans alone enabled us to include one orangutan neonate in our sample. Our results indicate that neonatal shape covariation is generally comparable to that of individuals in age group 2 on higher singular warps, but plots “off” of the general trajectory; this is likely a factor of the small sample size.

When visualized, endocranial shape variation along the first singular warp from negative to positive evinces an endocranium with a short clivus and laterally expanded anterior cranial fossae to an endocranium with inferiorly expanded temporal poles and a foramen magnum with accompanying large reductions to the lateral regions of the anterior cranial fossae (Fig 9B). In the splanchnocranium, these changes covary with a transition from relatively large orbits and a short maxilla to inferiorly elongated nasal and maxillary regions.

Standardizing the cranial base

To determine to what extent endocranial shape change occurs independently of cranial base shape change, we standardized the Procrustes shape variables of the endocranial vault to the cranial base. The resultant trajectories in shape space show a clear ontogenetic signal (i.e. they are not static) and are relatively similar between great apes (Fig 10), but to a lesser extent than in the non-standardized analysis (Fig 2). In the projection of the first two PCs, the trajectories of chimpanzees and gorillas closely overlap at age group 2, but diverge noticeably following this juncture (Fig 10A). The orangutan shape trajectory, meanwhile, is completely distinct from the African apes until age group 4, whereupon the orangutan trajectory overlaps that of chimpanzees (Fig 10A). That the orangutan age group 2 plots “off” the trajectory for age groups 3 and 4 is probably less an indicator of a biological difference than a sampling artifact. In the projection of the first and third PCs, the African ape trajectories are approximately linear (Fig 10B), converging gradually from age group 2 onwards. Orangutans, while occupying a parallel space in this projection, again exhibit a curved trajectory between age groups 2 and 4 (Fig 10B). Overall, the spacing between age group means is similar between the African ape species: a large amount of shape change between age groups two and three and remarkably little shape change between age groups five and six. In comparison, the spacing between age group means in orangutans is more evenly distributed.

Discussion

In our analysis of endocranial and splanchnocranial morphological integration, we have shown that great ape species are typified by similar developmental covariation patterns. Hence, in corroboration of the work of Zollikofer and colleagues [30], our results signify that conservation, or canalization, is indeed a hallmark of endocranial/splanchnocranial integration. This adds to the growing body of work demonstrating (a) that the primate cranium is highly integrated [61, 6372]; and (b) that developmental integration patterns are comparable across primate species [57, 7374, 94].

Species-specific variation

Within this overarching pattern of similar developmental trajectories, some species-specific variation in magnitude and direction exists. However, as our partial least squares and principal components analyses indicate that developmental trajectories are curvilinear, this precludes the calculation of angles between trajectories to determine whether apparent species variation in vector direction is statistically significant [28]. Based on visual inspection, however, it is apparent that chimpanzees and gorillas are more similar to each other with regards to trajectory direction than either is to orangutans; this phenomenon is evident both for shape covariation and shape variation. Indeed, that chimpanzee and gorillas overlap to a considerable degree highlights that they differ in vector length between age group means: the amount of shape change covariation experienced between comparable dental-age stages. Thus, while the African apes overlap in age group means at the incomplete eruption of deciduous dentition, gorillas undergo much greater amounts of shape change such that the gorilla age group 5 mean overlaps with that of the chimpanzee age group 6 mean. Our results therefore add further substantiation to evidence showing that many aspects of the overall cranial shape differences between gorillas and chimpanzees can be explained by ontogenetic allometric scaling [97103]. Meanwhile, it is intriguing that the amount of shape change covariation is remarkably consistent across orangutan ontogeny, as this would suggest that any growth spurt is not particularly marked. A more likely explanation, however, is that the substantial amount of covariation observed in orangutans (large convex hull area) masks multiple species, sexual bimaturism [104108], and the development of secondary sexual characteristics [109], all of which contribute to the enormous shape disparity of orangutans [110112]. It is likely that the observed discrepancy between orangutan and African ape trajectories is attributable to the differing facial architecture of orangutans [32, 52, 113116].

Effects of allometric variation

Our form space analyses indicated that size increase plays a considerable role in shape development, with this effect differing in extent in endocranial versus splanchnocranial form space. Specifically, that splanchnocranial trajectories in form space are parallel to the first axis of variation indicates that shape variation in the splanchnocranium is predominated by size variation. Such a pronounced effect is not observed in endocranial form space, implying that endocranial shape variation is not entirely determined by allometric variation [29]. By regressing out size and subsequently analyzing the covariation of residuals (Fig 6), we demonstrated that a large amount of the major axis of covariation between splanchnocranial and endocranial shape is driven by allometric effects. Despite this, an ontogenetic signal remains. This residual signal evinces comparable trajectories between great apes, particularly between orangutans and gorillas, with chimpanzees again displaying an abbreviated version of the gorilla trajectory.

Differences between early and adolescent ontogeny

Based on virtual endocasts, we [28] and Zollikofer and colleagues [30] previously showed that great ape endocranial shape continues to develop past a point when brain growth would be expected to cease. This phenomenon is manifest in earlier studies relying solely on cranial cross-sections [32, 5052, 117118], and is indicative of the successive influence of the face on the continuing development of the primate endocranium. In this paper, we hypothesized that this transition between brain growth and splanchnocranial growth and development would be reflected in a change in the ontogenetic shape covariation pattern between endocranial and splanchnocranial modules. From our partial least squares analyses (Fig 1 and Figs 69), we demonstrated that our youngest age groups (i.e. prior to the complete eruption of the deciduous dentition) deviate to some extent from the pattern adhered to by older age groups. This corroborates our hypothesis that covariation between endocranium and face is different in early ontogeny. However, sample sizes are too small to permit statistical testing of differences in angle between early and late ontogeny for each species.

Influence of the splanchnocranium on endocranial shape development

If endocranial shape change were primarily attributable to the development of the face and its attendant effects on the cranial base, then one would expect minimal shape change in the endocranial vault past cessation of brain growth after accounting for the shape changes of the cranial base. When we standardize endocranial shape variables to the cranial base, however, all great apes display continued shape change from the eruption of the deciduous dentition to adulthood (Fig 10). These trajectories are similar in direction between great apes, indicating a conservation of shape change in these expansionary structures following the complete eruption of the deciduous dentition (Fig 10). The differences between our cranial base-standardized (Fig 10) and non-standardized analyses (Fig 2) suggest that several of the endocranial shape changes visualized here, notably, the inferior expansion of the clivus region, the posterior migration of opisthion, and the superior rotation of the cribriform plate (Fig 2B), are corollary to the development of the face. Furthermore, our cranial base-standardized results show that while gorillas and chimpanzees follow similar trajectories in terms of direction, they differ in magnitude such that gorillas change shape more during the period of development from age groups two to three alone than do chimpanzees from age groups two to six. The ontogenetic signal from age group 3 to 6 along the major axis of covariation between endocranial and facial shape therefore takes place during a time when the relationship of low endocranial size increase and high splanchnocranial size increase is quite constant.

From the eruption of the deciduous dentition to adulthood, endocranial shape becomes more elongated as the splanchnocranium grows and become more prognathic (Fig 1 and Figs 69). We interpret this consistent pattern of covariation as ontogenetic integration between the splanchnocranium and the endocranium. That endocranial shape development persists to adulthood in close conjunction with splanchnocranial shape development would suggest that the latter drives the former. However, not all splanchnocranial shape changes are tightly integrated with endocranial shape. Indeed, in all of our PLS analyses here, only the first singular warp can be linked to covariation between splanchnocranium and endocranium, while higher singular warps indicate that some splanchnocranial changes during ontogeny do not affect endocranial shape. In particular, during early postnatal ontogeny, from age group 1 to 2, and from age group 2 to 3, the covariation between facial growth and development and the shape of the endocranium is weak. This time-window coincides with a period of high brain growth rates, therefore indicating that, during early postnatal development, the shape of the endocranium is primarily driven by the tempo and mode of brain expansion [21, 2325]. At later stages of development, splanchnocranial and endocranial shape development were found to covary more strongly in all examined great ape species, particularly from the complete eruption of the deciduous dentition onwards.

Conclusion

In summary, we have shown that covariation between the hominid splanchnocranium and endocranium is weak during early infancy but strong in subsequent age groups. This suggests that whereas the developmental expansion of the brain is the main driver of endocranial shape during early ontogeny, the adult endocranium is shaped by strong integration of the neurocranium with the splanchnocranium in later developmental periods. Throughout hominoid evolution, endocranial shape has accordingly been modified to accommodate the adaptive diversification of the face, suggesting that final endocranial form is a product of multiple developmental pathways and multiple selective pressures.

As our partial least squares analyses of orangutans and chimpanzees alone indicate, neonatal shape covariation differs from that of later age groups. In chimpanzees, for which we have more than one neonatal specimen, it is apparent that neonates are entirely distinct from the general pattern of covariation. By improving the sample composition of neonatal specimens in future research, it will be possible to ascertain the degree to which neonates differ in their morphological integration. Including prenatal data would further illuminate whether our neonatal results found here represent a pattern established prior to birth.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Two-block, age group mean-centred PLS analysis between endocranium and the splanchnocranium: Pooled species, singular warps 1–3.

Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 2–6 for each species; age group labels denote age group means, while lines are B-spline curves of the average species-specific trajectories. Colours: green = chimpanzee; dark grey = gorilla; orange = orangutan.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.s001

(TIFF)

S2 Fig. Two-block, age group mean-centred PLS analysis of endocranial and splanchnocranial shape residuals after regression on size: Pooled species, singular warps 1–3.

Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 2–6 for each species; age group labels denote age group means, while lines are B-spline curves of the average species-specific trajectories. Colours: green = chimpanzee; dark grey = gorilla; orange = orangutan.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.s002

(TIFF)

S3 Fig. Two-block, age group mean-centred PLS analysis between endocranium and the splanchnocranium: Chimpanzees, singular warps 1–3.

Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 1–6; age group labels denote age group means, while line is B-spline curve of the average chimpanzee-specific trajectory.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.s003

(TIFF)

S4 Fig. Two-block, age group mean-centred PLS analysis between endocranium and the splanchnocranium: Gorillas, singular warps 1–3.

Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 2–6; age group labels denote age group means, while line is B-spline curve of the average gorilla-specific trajectory.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.s004

(TIFF)

S5 Fig. Two-block, age group mean-centred PLS analysis between endocranium and the splanchnocranium: Orangutans, singular warps 1–3.

Convex hulls represent pooled sexes of age groups 1–6; age group labels denote age group means, while line is B-spline curve of the average orangutan-specific trajectory.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.s005

(TIFF)

S1 Table. Index of specimens.

Includes specimen ID and provenance information.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.s006

(XLSX)

S2 Table. Endocranial landmark data, index and specimen number.

File is an Excel spreadsheet, specimens are ordered as in index (S1 Table).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.s007

(XLS)

S3 Table. Splanchnocranial landmark data, index and specimen number.

File is an Excel spreadsheet, specimens are ordered as in index (S1 Table).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208999.s008

(XLS)

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank C. Boesch, H. Coqueugniot, W. Enard, C. Feja, F. Grine, M. von Harling, B. Herzig, J. L. Kahn, the Kyoto University Primate Research Institute, N. Lange, F. Mayer, U. Olbrich-Schwartz, K. Spanel-Borowski, F. Spoor, F. Veillon, G. Weber and A. Winter for access to specimens; P. Schönfeld (particularly for the scanning of the frozen neonates), P. Schubert, H. Temming and A. Winzer for CT scanning; and the Ivorian authorities, particularly the Ministry of the Environment and Forests and the Ministry of Research, the Swiss Centre for Scientific Research Abidjan, and the University of Zurich for their support of the Taï chimpanzee study. The authors are especially grateful to the editor and three anonymous reviewers, who greatly assisted in expanding the scope of this paper.

References

  1. 1. Dart RA. Australopithecus africanus the man-ape of South Africa. Nature. 1925; 195.
  2. 2. Falk D. Hominid paleoneurology. Annual Review of Anthropology. 1987 Oct;16(1):13–28.
  3. 3. Falk D, Hildebolt C, Smith K, Morwood MJ, Sutikna T, Saptomo EW, et al. LB1’s virtual endocast, microcephaly, and hominin brain evolution. Journal of Human Evolution. 2009 Nov 1;57(5):597–607. pmid:19254807
  4. 4. Holloway RL, Broadfield DC, Yuan MS. The human fossil record. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2005. 315 p. https://doi.org/10.1002/0471663573.
  5. 5. Amano H, Kikuchi T, Morita Y, Kondo O, Suzuki H, Ponce de León MS, et al. Virtual reconstruction of the Neanderthal Amud 1 cranium. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2015 Oct;158(2):185–97. pmid:26249757
  6. 6. Bienvenu T, Guy F, Coudyzer W, Gilissen E, Roualdès G, Vignaud P, et al. Assessing endocranial variations in great apes and humans using 3D data from virtual endocasts. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2011 Jun;145(2):231–46. pmid:21365614
  7. 7. Carlson KJ, Stout D, Jashashvili T, De Ruiter DJ, Tafforeau P, Carlson K, et al. The endocast of MH1, Australopithecus sediba. Science. 2011 Sep 8:1203922.
  8. 8. Conroy GC, Falk D, Guyer J, Weber GW, Seidler H, Recheis W. Endocranial capacity in Sts 71 (Australopithecus africanus) by three‐dimensional computed tomography. The Anatomical Record. 2000 Apr 1;258(4):391–6. pmid:10737857
  9. 9. Falk D. Interpreting sulci on hominin endocasts: old hypotheses and new findings. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014 May 1;8:134. pmid:24822043
  10. 10. Grine FE, Gunz P, Betti-Nash L, Neubauer S, Morris AG. Reconstruction of the late Pleistocene human skull from Hofmeyr, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution. 2010 Jul 1;59(1):1–5. pmid:20546848
  11. 11. Holloway RL, Hurst SD, Garvin HM, Schoenemann PT, Vanti WB, Berger LR, et al. Endocast morphology of Homo naledi from the Dinaledi Chamber, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2018 May 29;115(22):5738–43.
  12. 12. Hublin JJ, Neubauer S, Gunz P. Brain ontogeny and life history in Pleistocene hominins. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2015 Mar 5;370(1663):20140062. pmid:25602066
  13. 13. Hublin JJ, Ben-Ncer A, Bailey SE, Freidline SE, Neubauer S, Skinner MM, et al. New fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco and the pan-African origin of Homo sapiens. Nature. 2017 Jun;546(7657):289. pmid:28593953
  14. 14. Neubauer S, Gunz P, Mitteroecker P, Weber GW. Three-dimensional digital imaging of the partial endocranium MLD 37/38. Canadian Association of Radiologists Journal. 2004 Oct 1;55(4):271. pmid:15362352
  15. 15. Neubauer S, Gunz P, Weber GW, Hublin JJ. Endocranial volume of Australopithecus africanus: new CT-based estimates and the effects of missing data and small sample size. Journal of Human Evolution. 2012 Apr 1;62(4):498–510. pmid:22365336
  16. 16. Neubauer S, Gunz P, Leakey L, Leakey M, Hublin JJ, Spoor F. Reconstruction, endocranial form and taxonomic affinity of the early Homo calvaria KNM-ER 42700. Journal of Human Evolution. 2018 Apr 26.
  17. 17. Neubauer S, Hublin JJ, Gunz P. The evolution of modern human brain shape. Science Advances. 2018 Jan 1;4(1):eaao5961. pmid:29376123
  18. 18. Ponce de León MS, Golovanova L, Doronichev V, Romanova G, Akazawa T, Kondo O, et al. Neanderthal brain size at birth provides insights into the evolution of human life history. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2008 Sep 16;105(37):13764–8.
  19. 19. Profico A, Schlager S, Valoriani V, Buzi C, Melchionna M, Veneziano A, et al. Reproducing the internal and external anatomy of fossil bones: Two new automatic digital tools. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2018 Apr 21.
  20. 20. Zollikofer CP, de León MS, Lieberman DE, Guy F, Pilbeam D, Likius A, et al. Virtual cranial reconstruction of Sahelanthropus tchadensis. Nature. 2005 Apr;434(7034):755. pmid:15815628
  21. 21. Gunz P, Neubauer S, Maureille B, Hublin JJ. Brain development after birth differs between Neanderthals and modern humans. Current Biology. 2010 Nov 9;20(21):R921–2. pmid:21056830
  22. 22. Gunz P, Neubauer S, Maureille B, Hublin JJ. Virtual reconstruction of the Le Moustier 2 newborn skull. Implications for Neandertal ontogeny. PALEO. Revue d'archéologiepréhistorique. 2011 Dec 1(22):155–72.
  23. 23. Gunz P, Neubauer S, Golovanova L, Doronichev V, Maureille B, Hublin JJ. A uniquely modern human pattern of endocranial development. Insights from a new cranial reconstruction of the Neandertal newborn from Mezmaiskaya. Journal of Human Evolution. 2012 Feb 29;62(2):300–13. pmid:22221766
  24. 24. Neubauer S, Gunz P, Hublin JJ. The pattern of endocranial ontogenetic shape changes in humans. Journal of Anatomy. 2009 Sep 1;215(3):240–55. pmid:19531085
  25. 25. Neubauer S, Gunz P, Hublin JJ. Endocranial shape changes during growth in chimpanzees and humans: a morphometric analysis of unique and shared aspects. Journal of Human Evolution. 2010 Nov 1;59(5):555–66. pmid:20727571
  26. 26. Neubauer S, Hublin JJ. The evolution of human brain development. Evolutionary Biology. 2012 Dec 1;39(4):568–86.
  27. 27. Neubauer S. Endocasts: possibilities and limitations for the interpretation of human brain evolution. Brain, behavior and evolution. 2014;84(2):117–34. pmid:25247826
  28. 28. Scott N, Neubauer S, Hublin JJ, Gunz P. A shared pattern of postnatal endocranial development in extant hominoids. Evolutionary Biology. 2014 Dec 1;41(4):572–94.
  29. 29. Zollikofer CP, De León MS. Pandora's growing box: Inferring the evolution and development of hominin brains from endocasts. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 2013 Jan;22(1):20–33.
  30. 30. Zollikofer CP, Bienvenu T, Ponce de León MS. Effects of cranial integration on hominid endocranial shape. Journal of Anatomy. 2017 Jan 1;230(1):85–105. pmid:27503252
  31. 31. Bruner E, Amano H, la Cuétara JM, Ogihara N. The brain and the braincase: a spatial analysis on the midsagittal profile in adult humans. Journal of Anatomy. 2015 Sep 1;227(3):268–76. pmid:26200138
  32. 32. Duckworth WL. Morphology and anthropology: a handbook for students. University Press; 1904.
  33. 33. Richtsmeier JT, Aldridge K, DeLeon VB, Panchal J, Kane AA, Marsh JL, et al. Phenotypic integration of neurocranium and brain. Journal of Experimental Zoology Part B: Molecular and Developmental Evolution. 2006 Jul 15;306(4):360–78.
  34. 34. Richtsmeier JT, Flaherty K. Hand in glove: brain and skull in development and dysmorphogenesis. Acta neuropathologica. 2013 Apr 1;125(4):469–89. pmid:23525521
  35. 35. Bastir M, Rosas A, Stringer C, Cuétara JM, Kruszynski R, Weber GW, et al. Effects of brain and facial size on basicranial form in human and primate evolution. Journal of Human Evolution. 2010 May 1;58(5):424–31. pmid:20378153
  36. 36. Cheverud JM, Midkiff JE. Effects of fronto‐occipital cranial reshaping on mandibular form. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 1992 Feb;87(2):167–71. pmid:1543243
  37. 37. Profico A, Piras P, Buzi C, Di Vincenzo F, Lattarini F, Melchionna M, et al. The evolution of cranial base and face in Cercopithecoidea and Hominoidea: Modularity and morphological integration. American Journal of Primatology. 2017 Dec;79(12):e22721.
  38. 38. Rhode MP, Arriaza BT. Influence of cranial deformation on facial morphology among prehistoric South Central Andean populations. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2006 Aug 1;130(4):462–70. pmid:16444729
  39. 39. Gould SJ. Ontogeny and phylogeny. Harvard University Press; 1977.
  40. 40. Gould SJ. Change in developmental timing as a mechanism of macroevolution. In: Evolution and Development. 1982 (pp. 333–346). Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg.
  41. 41. Alberch P, Gould SJ, Oster GF, Wake DB. Size and shape in ontogeny and phylogeny. Paleobiology. 1979 Jul;5(3):296–317.
  42. 42. Alberch P. Problems with the interpretation of developmental sequences. Systematic Biology. 1985 Mar 1;34(1):46–58.
  43. 43. Debat V, David P. Mapping phenotypes: canalization, plasticity and developmental stability. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2001 Oct 1;16(10):555–61.
  44. 44. Stearns SC, Kaiser M, Kawecki TJ. The differential genetic and environmental canalization of fitness components in Drosophila melanogaster. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 1995 Sep;8(5):539–57.
  45. 45. Waddington CH. Canalization of development and the inheritance of acquired characters. Nature. 1942 Nov;150(3811):563.
  46. 46. Wilkins AS. Canalization: a molecular genetic perspective. BioEssays. 1997 Mar;19(3):257–62. pmid:9080776
  47. 47. Bradshaw AD. Evolutionary significance of phenotypic plasticity in plants. In: Advances in genetics. 1965 Jan 1 (Vol. 13, pp. 115–155). Academic Press.
  48. 48. Marroig G, Shirai LT, Porto A, de Oliveira FB, De Conto V. The evolution of modularity in the mammalian skull II: evolutionary consequences. Evolutionary Biology. 2009 Mar 1;36(1):136–48.
  49. 49. Keith A. Description of a new craniometer and of certain age changes in the anthropoid skull. Journal of Anatomy and Physiology. 1910 Apr;44(Pt 3):251. pmid:17232849
  50. 50. Krogman WM. Studies in growth changes in the skull and face of anthropoids. III. Growth changes in the skull and face of the gorilla. American Journal of Anatomy. 1931 Jan;47(1):89–115.
  51. 51. Krogman WM. Studies in growth changes in the skull and face of anthropoids. IV. Growth changes in the skull and face of the chimpanzee. American Journal of Anatomy. 1931 Mar;47(2):325–42.
  52. 52. Krogman WM. Studies in growth changes in the skull and face of Anthropoids. V. Growth changes in the skull and face of the Orang‐utan. Developmental Dynamics. 1931 Mar 1;47(2):343–65.
  53. 53. Cardini A, Polly PD. Larger mammals have longer faces because of size-related constraints on skull form. Nature Communications. 2013 Sep 18;4:2458. pmid:24045342
  54. 54. Godinho RM, Spikins P, O’Higgins P. Supraorbital morphology and social dynamics in human evolution. Nature Ecology and Evolution. 2018 Jun;2(6):956. pmid:29632349
  55. 55. Raia P, Boggioni M, Carotenuto F, Castiglione S, Febbraro M, Vincenzo F, et al. Unexpectedly rapid evolution of mandibular shape in hominins. Scientific Reports. 2018 May 9;8(1):7340. pmid:29743608
  56. 56. Porto A, de Oliveira FB, Shirai LT, De Conto V, Marroig G. The evolution of modularity in the mammalian skull I: morphological integration patterns and magnitudes. Evolutionary Biology. 2009 Mar 1;36(1):118–35.
  57. 57. Cheverud JM. Phenotypic, genetic, and environmental morphological integration in the cranium. Evolution. 1982 May 1;36(3):499–516. pmid:28568050
  58. 58. Cheverud JM. A comparative analysis of morphological variation patterns in the papionins. Evolution. 1989 Dec 1;43(8):1737–47. pmid:28564330
  59. 59. Klingenberg CP. Cranial integration and modularity: insights into evolution and development from morphometric data. Hystrix, the Italian Journal of Mammalogy. 2013 May 13;24(1):43–58.
  60. 60. Olson EC, Miller RL. Morphological integration. University of Chicago Press; 1999 Oct 1.
  61. 61. Cheverud JM. Morphological integration in the saddle-back tamarin (Saguinus fuscicollis) cranium. The American Naturalist. 1995 Jan 1;145(1):63–89.
  62. 62. Gunz P, Harvati K. The Neanderthal “chignon”: variation, integration, and homology. Journal of Human Evolution. 2007 Mar 1;52(3):262–74. pmid:17097133
  63. 63. Marroig G, Cheverud JM. A comparison of phenotypic variation and covariation patterns and the role of phylogeny, ecology, and ontogeny during cranial evolution of New World monkeys. Evolution. 2001 Dec;55(12):2576–600. pmid:11831671
  64. 64. Marroig G, Vivo MD, Cheverud JM. Cranial evolution in sakis (Pithecia, Platyrrhini) II: evolutionary processes and morphological integration. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2004 Jan 1;17(1):144–55. pmid:15000657
  65. 65. Neaux D, Guy F, Gilissen E, Coudyzer W, Ducrocq S. Covariation between midline cranial base, lateral basicranium, and face in modern humans and chimpanzees: a 3D geometric morphometric analysis. The Anatomical Record. 2013 Apr 1;296(4):568–79. pmid:23382005
  66. 66. Neaux D, Gilissen E, Coudyzer W, Guy F. Integration between the face and the mandible of Pongo and the evolution of the craniofacial morphology of orangutans. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2015 Nov;158(3):475–86. pmid:26174601
  67. 67. Neaux D. Morphological integration of the cranium in Homo, Pan, and Hylobates and the evolution of hominoid facial structures. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2017 Apr 1;162(4):732–46. pmid:28035660
  68. 68. Polanski JM, Franciscus RG. Patterns of craniofacial integration in extant Homo, Pan, and Gorilla. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2006 Sep 1;131(1):38–49. pmid:16552733
  69. 69. Singh N, Harvati K, Hublin JJ, Klingenberg CP. Morphological evolution through integration: a quantitative study of cranial integration in Homo, Pan, Gorilla and Pongo. Journal of Human Evolution. 2012 Jan 31;62(1):155–64. pmid:22178399
  70. 70. Stelzer S, Gunz P, Neubauer S, Spoor F. Hominoid arcade shape: Pattern and magnitude of covariation. Journal of Human Evolution. 2017 Jun 30;107:71–85. pmid:28526290
  71. 71. Stelzer S, Gunz P, Neubauer S, Spoor F. Using the covariation of extant hominoid upper and lower jaws to predict dental arcades of extinct hominins. Journal of Human Evolution. 2018 Jan 31;114:154–75. pmid:29447757
  72. 72. Stelzer S, Neubauer S, Hublin JJ, Spoor F, Gunz P. Morphological trends in arcade shape and size in Middle Pleistocene Homo. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2018 Oct 23.
  73. 73. Ackermann RR. Ontogenetic integration of the hominoid face. Journal of Human Evolution. 2005. Feb 28;48(2):175–97. pmid:15701530
  74. 74. Bookstein FL, Gunz P, Mitteroecker P, Prossinger H, Schafer K, Seidler H. Cranial integration in Homo: singular warps analysis of the midsagittal plane in ontogeny and evolution. Journal of Human Evolution. 2003 Feb 28;44(2):167–87. pmid:12662941
  75. 75. Bookstein FL, Gunz P, Mitteroecker P, Prossinger H, Chafer KS, Seidler H. Cranial integration. Homo. 2003:167–87.
  76. 76. Bastir M, Rosas A, Stringer C, Cuétara JM, Kruszynski R, Weber GW, et al. Effects of brain and facial size on basicranial form in human and primate evolution. Journal of Human Evolution. 2010 May 31;58(5):424–31. pmid:20378153
  77. 77. Klingenberg CP, Marugán-Lobón J. Evolutionary covariation in geometric morphometric data: analyzing integration, modularity, and allometry in a phylogenetic context. Systematic Biology. 2013 May 31;62(4):591–610. pmid:23589497
  78. 78. Huxley JS. Constant differential growth-ratios and their significance. Nature. 1924 Dec;114(2877):895.
  79. 79. Mitteroecker P, Gunz P, Windhager S, Schaefer K. A brief review of shape, form, and allometry in geometric morphometrics, with applications to human facial morphology. Hystrix, the Italian Journal of Mammalogy. 2013;24(1):59–66.
  80. 80. Klingenberg CP. Size, shape, and form: concepts of allometry in geometric morphometrics. Development, genes and evolution. 2016 Jun 1;226(3):113–37.
  81. 81. Gould SJ. Allometry and size in ontogeny and phylogeny. Biological Reviews. 1966 Nov;41(4):587–638. pmid:5342162
  82. 82. Gould SJ. The structure of evolutionary theory.Belknap Press: Harvard University Press, 2002. p. 1464.
  83. 83. Ashton EH. Age changes in the basicranial axis of the Anthropoidea. Journal of Zoology. 1957Aug 1;129(1):61–74.
  84. 84. Biegert J. The evaluation of characteristics of the skull, hands and feet for primate taxonomy. Classification and Human Evolution. 1963:116–45.
  85. 85. Björk A. Cranial base development: a follow-up x-ray study of the individual variation in growth occurring between the ages of 12 and 20 years and its relation to brain case and face development. American Journal of Orthodontics. 1955 Mar 1;41(3):198–225.
  86. 86. Enlow DH, Hans MG. Essentials of facial growth. Needham Press; 2008.
  87. 87. Scott JH. The growth of the human skull. J Dent Ass S fr. 1958;13:133.
  88. 88. Gunz P, Mitteroecker P, Bookstein FL. Semilandmarks in three dimensions. In: Slice, Dennis E., editor. Modern morphometrics in physical anthropology. Springer: Boston, MA; 2005. p. 73–98.
  89. 89. Gunz P, Mitteroecker P. Semilandmarks: a method for quantifying curves and surfaces. Hystrix, the Italian Journal of Mammalogy. 2013;24(1):103–9.
  90. 90. Dryden IL, Mardia KV. Statistical analysis of shape. Wiley; 1998.
  91. 91. Gower JC. Generalized procrustes analysis. Psychometrika. 1975 Mar 27;40(1):33–51.
  92. 92. Rohlf FJ, Slice D. Extensions of the Procrustes method for the optimal superimposition of landmarks. Systematic Biology. 1990 Mar 1;39(1):40–59.
  93. 93. Rohlf FJ, Corti M. Use of two-block partial least-squares to study covariation in shape. Systematic Biology. 2000 Dec 1;49(4):740–53. pmid:12116437
  94. 94. Mitteroecker P, Bookstein F. The evolutionary role of modularity and integration in the hominoid cranium. Evolution. 2008 Apr 1;62(4):943–58. pmid:18194472
  95. 95. Mitteroecker P, Gunz P, Neubauer S, Müller G. How to explore morphological integration in human evolution and development?. Evolutionary Biology. 2012 Dec 1;39(4):536–53.
  96. 96. Bookstein FL. Thin-plate splines and the atlas problem for biomedical images. In: Biennial International Conference on Information Processing in Medical Imaging. 1991 Jul 7 (pp. 326–342). Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg.
  97. 97. Ackermann RR, Krovitz GE. Common patterns of facial ontogeny in the hominid lineage. The Anatomical Record. 2002 Jun 15;269(3):142–7. pmid:12124900
  98. 98. Berge C, Penin X. Ontogenetic allometry, heterochrony, and interspecific differences in the skull of African apes, using tridimensional Procrustes analysis. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2004 Jun;124(2):124–38. pmid:15160366
  99. 99. Giles E. Cranial allometry in the great apes. Human Biology. 1956 Feb 1;28(1):43. pmid:13318690
  100. 100. Mitteroecker P, Gunz P, Bernhard M, Schaefer K, Bookstein FL. Comparison of cranial ontogenetic trajectories among great apes and humans. Journal of Human Evolution. 2004 Jun 1;46(6):679–98. pmid:15183670
  101. 101. Mitteroecker P, Gunz P, Bookstein FL. Heterochrony and geometric morphometrics: a comparison of cranial growth in Pan paniscus versus Pan troglodytes. Evolution &Development. 2005 May;7(3):244–58.
  102. 102. Shea BT. Allometry and heterochrony in the African apes. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 1983 Nov;62(3):275–89. pmid:6660286
  103. 103. Shea BT. Size and diet in the evolution of African ape craniodental form. Folia Primatologica. 1983;40(1–2):32–68.
  104. 104. Fooden J, Izor RJ. Growth curves, dental emergence norms, and supplementary morphological observations in known‐age captive orangutans. American Journal of Primatology. 1983;5(4):285–301.
  105. 105. Leigh SR. Patterns of variation in the ontogeny of primate body size dimorphism. Journal of Human Evolution. 1992 Jul 1;23(1):27–50.
  106. 106. Leigh SR, Shea BT. Ontogeny and the evolution of adult body size dimorphism in apes. American Journal of Primatology. 1995;36(1):37–60.
  107. 107. MacKinnon J. The behaviour and ecology of wild orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus). Animal behaviour. 1974 Feb 1;22(1):3–74.
  108. 108. Rijksen H. D. A field study on Sumatran orang utans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii, Lesson 1827): Ecology, behaviour and conservation. 1978. Netherlands: H. Veenman.
  109. 109. Thompson ME, Zhou A, Knott CD. Low testosterone correlates with delayed development in male orangutans. PLoS One. 2012 Oct 15;7(10):e47282. pmid:23077585
  110. 110. Hrdlicka A. Anatomical observations on a collection of orang skulls from western Borneo, with a bibliography. Proceedings of the United States National Museum. 1907 31: 539–568.
  111. 111. Selenka E. Studien über Entwickelungsgeschichte der Thiere. Wiesbaden: CW Kreidels Verlag, 1898.
  112. 112. Röhrer-Ertl O. Cranial growth. In Schwartz J. L., editor, Orang-utan Biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1988.p. 201–223).
  113. 113. Blaney SP. An allometric study of the frontal sinus in Gorilla, Pan and Pongo. Folia Primatologica. 1986;47(2–3):81–96.
  114. 114. Cave AJ, Haines RW. The paranasal sinuses of the anthropoid apes. Journal of Anatomy. 1940 Jul;74(Pt 4):493. pmid:17104832
  115. 115. Koppe T, Röhrer-Ertl O, Hahn D, Reike R, Nagai H. Growth pattern of the maxillary sinus in orang-utan based on measurements of CT scans. Okajimas folia anatomica Japonica. 1995 May 20;72(1):37–43. pmid:7566878
  116. 116. Senck S, Coquerelle M. Morphological integration and variation in facial orientation in Pongo pygmaeus pygmaeus: a geometric morphometric approach via partial least squares. International Journal of Primatology. 2015 Jun 1;36(3):489–512.
  117. 117. Biegert J. (1957). Der Formwandel des Primatenschädels und seine Beziehungen zur ontogenetischen Entwicklung und den phylogenetischen Spezialisationen der Kopforgane. Gegenbaursmorphologisches Jahrbuch, 98, 77–199.
  118. 118. Welcker H. Kraniologische Mittheilungen. Friedrich Vieweg und Sohn; 1866.