Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Lower performance of Toxoplasma-infected, Rh-negative subjects in the weight holding and hand-grip tests

  • Jaroslav Flegr ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    flegr@cesnet.cz

    Affiliation Division of Biology, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague, Viničná, Czech Republic

  • Blanka Šebánková,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Division of Biology, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague, Viničná, Czech Republic

  • Lenka Příplatová,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Biology, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague, Viničná, Czech Republic

  • Veronika Chvátalová,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Biology, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague, Viničná, Czech Republic

  • Šárka Kaňková

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Biology, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague, Viničná, Czech Republic

Abstract

Background

Toxoplasma, a protozoan parasite of cats, infects many species of intermediate and paratenic hosts, including about one-third of humans worldwide. After a short phase of acute infection, the tissue cysts containing slowly dividing bradyzoites are formed in various organs and toxoplasmosis proceeds spontaneously in its latent form. In immunocompetent subjects, latent toxoplasmosis was considered asymptomatic. However, dozens of studies performed on animals and humans in the past twenty years have shown that it is accompanied by a broad spectrum of specific behavioural, physiological and even morphological changes. In human hosts, the changes often go in the opposite direction in men and women, and are mostly weaker or non-existent in Rh-positive subjects.

Methods

Here, we searched for the indices of lower endurance of the infected subjects by examining the performance of nearly five hundred university students tested for toxoplasmosis and Rh phenotype in two tests, a weight holding test and a grip test.

Results

The results confirmed the existence of a negative association of latent toxoplasmosis with the performance of students, especially Rh-negative men, in these tests. Surprisingly, but in an accordance with some already published data, Toxoplasma-infected, Rh-positive subjects expressed a higher, rather than lower, performance in our endurance tests.

Discussion

Therefore, the results only partly support the hypothesis for the lower endurance of Toxoplasma infected subjects as the performance of Rh-positive subjects (representing majority of population) correlated positively with the Toxoplasma infection.

Introduction

About one third of the world population are infected with the protozoan parasite Toxoplasma gondii [1]. In immunocompetent subjects, after a short phase of acute toxoplasmosis promoted by tachyzoites, the disease spontaneously proceeds into its latent phase, which is characterized by the presence of tissue cysts with a slowly dividing form of the parasite, bradyzoites, in various organs and anamnestic IgG antibodies in blood [2]. Latent toxoplasmosis has been considered asymptomatic for a long time, however, during the past 20 years, about one hundred papers have been published showing that Toxoplasma seropositive and seronegative subjects differ in many personality traits [3,4], performance in certain psychomotor tests [5,6], cognitive tests [710] and also in the incidence and form of many diseases and disorders [11,12]. In some tests, however, Toxoplasma-infected subjects score better than Toxoplasma-free individuals [13,14]. Very often, these differences deepen with time since the infection [5,15,16] and the analogical changes were nearly always reported to occur in experimentally infected laboratory animals [1720]. This suggests that the infection is most probably the cause of the behavioral changes, rather than that the special combination of phenotypic traits is the cause of the infection. It has been suggested that these observed behavioral differences are either the product of the manipulation activity of the parasite aimed to increase the probability of transmission of Toxoplasma from an intermediate to a definitive host by predation, or potentially as side-effects of the pathological changes accompanying the infection. Very often, personality differences associated with toxoplasmosis differ in scope or even direction between men and women. The stress-coping hypothesis [21,22] explains the opposite reactions of men and women to Toxoplasma infection by the fact that men and women cope in an opposite way to mild chronic stress. In contrast to stressed men, who use more individualistic and antisocial (for example aggressive) forms of coping [23,24], stressed women more often seek and provide social support (Stone and Neale 1984, Rosario et al. 1988, Carver et al. 1989), join with others [24], and verbalize towards others or ones’ self [25].

Similarly, Toxoplasma infection is associated with different phenotypical outputs in Rh-positive and Rh-negative subjects. The Rh-positive subjects, especially Rh-positive heterozygotes, seem to be fully or partly protected against many of the effects of toxoplasmosis, such as personality changes [26], decreased psychomotor performance [27,28], and impaired physical and mental health [29]. The strongest effect of latent toxoplasmosis reported until now, the excessive weight gain of Toxoplasma infected women in the 16th week of their pregnancies (4.12 kg vs 2.44 kg, N = 152, 25 Toxoplasma-infected), was observed only in Rh-negative mothers [30].

One of the earliest reported effects of latent toxoplasmosis in humans capable of enhancing the transmission of the parasite by predation in infected animals was the phenomenon of lower endurance of infected individuals [5,27,31]. In three independent questionnaire surveys, infected subjects, especially men, responded affirmatively to six of ten statements of the Toxo-92 questionnaire significantly more often, including the following item: “When I am attacked, physically or otherwise, or when I should fight for something important, I stop fighting at one moment. It is not a result of a rational decision not to fight, as in fact I know that I should continue fighting and I would like to do so, but my own subconsciousness betrays me and I lose the will to fight back”[31]. It was suggested that the reason of this premature surrender could be the lack of endurance in Toxoplasma-infected subjects. However, no experimental data or other empirical evidence, possibly except an increased rate of suicides or suicide-attempts in the infected subjects [3236], have thus far been published.

The aim of present study was to search for empirical evidence for a decreased will to fight in the Toxoplasma-infected subjects. In double-blind experiments, we measured the endurance of Toxoplasma-infected and Toxoplasma-free university students using two tests–a weight-holding tests and a hand-grip test. As the effects of toxoplasmosis very often differ in men and women and in Rh-positive and Rh-negative subjects, we included the variables sex and Rh phenotype into our statistical models.

Material and methods

Population

The population consisted of 347 women (age: 22.8, Std. Dev. 3.95, weight: 62.6, Std. Dev. 9.65, height: 167.4, Std. Dev. 6.18, BMI: 22.3, Std. Dev. 3.18) and 208 men (age 24.1, Std. Dev. 5.37, weight: 74.7, Std. Dev. 11.15, height: 180.4, Std. Dev. 6.82, BMI: 23.0, Std. Dev. 3.02), mostly students of biology of the Faculty of Science, Charles University. They were recruited during undergraduate and graduate level courses of Evolutionary Biology and Methodology of Science between 2010–2015. Usually, more than 70% of students attending these coursed accepted the invitation to participate in projects studying the manipulation activity of the parasite Toxoplasma and all except those with visible physical injuries or disabilities were accepted into the endurance tests. They were offered the opportunity to enrol in such experiments during a course entitled Methods of Evolutionary and Experimental Psychology, and compensated for their participation with one credit toward their assessment. The project, including the method of recruitment of participants and obtaining informed consent, was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Faculty of Science, Charles University (No. 2014/21).

Registered probands signed an informed consent, completed several questionnaires including an anamnestic questionnaire, and provided 3 ml of blood for testing for the presence of anamnestic anti-Toxoplasma antibodies and for testing of Rh phenotype. Experiments were separated to three sessions and took about ten hours altogether. Probands participated in all the sessions within several weeks. Both of the tests of performance were completed in the first session, which took place once a week and always started at 9:00 am. After finishing the whole battery of tests, they were informed about results of their serological tests for toxoplasmosis and Rh factor and rewarded with 400 CZK for their participation in the experiment.

First, probands got an email invitation to attend a session ahead of time and chose a date according to their preferences. They were asked to fulfil some requirements: to sleep sufficiently during the night before testing; to refrain from physical exertion for several days beforehand; to cancel their attendance in case of an illness, physical or mental problems or convalescence; to not drink coffee, tea, alcohol, and other energy drinks the day of the experiment. They were, however, offered a snack to replenish their energy during the long experimental session.

Grip test

Hand grip dynamometry is one of the screening procedures measuring fitness in a normal population. Hand grip strength can predict vitality, the nutritional state of an individual, as well as possible functional bodily failure [37,38]. A Collin dynamometer, a type of closed steel spring dynamometer, was used for testing grip strength. Hand grip strength was measured in kg.

The test was performed first in a battery of experiments. Each proband was requested to stand upright, with arms kept freely along their trunk, and to look straight ahead. They took the gauge by their dominant arm and the experimenter explained how they should assumed a fixed position in their hand. The proband was asked to exert the maximal practicable effort on the grip without moving their trunk and shoulders. After the performance, the experimenter recorded the measurement and asked the proband to repeat the sequence two more times with the same hand, and then three times with their nondominant hand. After the test was over, the experimenter informed the proband of his results and told him which of his or her hands was stronger. This part of the experiment was executed individually in the same room with other probands, but in seclusion.

Weight-holding test

A weight-holding performance test was prepared as a secondary experiment to be implemented in the same session as the grip test. It was placed at the end of a day-long experimental session, preceded by an intelligence and memory test, and several questionnaires distributed in paper and electronic forms. The probands were accommodated with two breaks and a snack in between the parts of the session. Each proband was requested to stand upright, not to lean over or backward, and to look straight ahead with experimenters outside his or her visual field. The proband was fitted with a hinged weight (5 kg) and asked to grasp it with both hands, to tense their arms in a levelled position with their shoulders, and with the backs of their hands up. The timer (measurement in seconds) was started by an experimenter the moment the appropriate position was engaged. The test was finished by the proband or by the experimenter when the proband was no longer capable of stabilizing their arms in a levelled position with shoulders. The probands were informed of their results after completing the test.

Testing for toxoplasmosis and Rh phenotype

Testing for toxoplasmosis was performed at the National Reference Laboratory for Toxoplasmosis, National Institute of Public Health, Prague. The complement-fixation test (CFT), which determines the overall levels of IgM and IgG antibodies of particular specificity, and Enzyme-Linked Immunosorbent Assays (ELISA) (IgG ELISA: SEVAC, Prague) were used to detect the T. gondii infection status of the subjects. ELISA assay cut-point values were established using positive and negative standards according to the manufacturer’s instructions. In CFT test, the titre of antibodies against Toxoplasma in sera was measured in dilutions between 1:4 and 1:1024. The subjects with CFT titres between 1:8 and 1:128 were considered Toxoplasma infected (the subjects with higher titres, who were suspected to be in the acute phase of the infection, were excluded from the study). Only subjects with clearly positive or negative results of CFT and IgG ELISA tests were diagnosed as Toxoplasma-infected or Toxoplasma-free, whilst subjects with different results on these tests, or ambiguous results, were retested or excluded from the study. A standard agglutination method was used for Rh factor examination. A constant amount of anti-D serum (human monoclonal antiD reagent; SeracloneH, Immucor Gamma Inc.) was added to a drop of blood on a white glass plate. Red cells of Rh-positive subjects were agglutinated within 2–5 min.

Statistics

Statistic analyses were performed with program Statistica v. 10.1. The effect of Toxoplasma seropositivity and Rh on participants’ performance in the weight-holding test was measured with ANCOVA (modul General Linear Models), with the independent binary variables Toxoplasma seropositivity (toxo), Rh phenotype (Rh), and sex of a subject (sex), the covariate age of a subject in time of testing (age), and toxo-Rh, toxo-sex, Rh-sex, and Rh-toxo-sex interactions as the independent variables. The effect of toxoplasmosis and Rh on participants’ performance in the hand-grip test was measured with repeated measures ANCOVA, with the performance of particular subjects in six trials (3 dominant hand trials and 3-nondominant hand trials) as the repeated measures; the independent variables were toxo, Rh, sex, and two nested factors: hand (dominant/nondominant) and trial (1st, 2nd, and 3rd trial for each hand). The model also included all interactions of toxo, Rh, sex, hand, and trial. In the follow-up analyses, the same tests (without the factor sex and without all interactions with sex) were performed separately for men and women.

All raw data are available at Figshare https://figshare.com/s/692c3a7d08c3df860c9e

Terminological notes

For the sake of clarity, we abbreviated “Toxoplasma seropositivity” to “toxoplasmosis”in the Discussion and to “toxo” in the description of our statistical models. Also, the statistical relations between (formally) dependent and (formally) independent variables are called “effects,” despite the fact that the real causal relation between these variables can be different or even non-existent (as reminded in the Discussion).

Results

Weight-holding test

This test was performed by 289 women, 59 (20.4%) Toxoplasma-seropositive and 176 men, 29 (16.5%) Toxoplasma-seropositive. The seroprevalence of toxoplasmosis did not differ between women and men (p = 0.339, Fisher exact test). Similarly, frequency of Rh-negative subjects, 21.1% (61) in women and 19.3% (34) in men, did not differ between men and women or between Toxoplasma-seropositive and Toxoplasma-seronegative male or female subjects (Chi2 = 1.31, df = 4, p = 0.860, all partial p values > 0.30). The age of Toxoplasma-seropositive subjects was higher than age of seronegative subjects (women: 24.6, SD 6.5 vs 22.6, SD = 3.30, t287 = -3.39, p = 0.001, men: 26.4, SD 9.00 vs 23.9, SD = 4.56, t174 = -2.19, p = 0.029). The effect of Toxoplasma-seropositivity and Rh phenotype in the weight-holding test was measured with General Linear Models (GLM) with time of holding as the dependent variable and toxo, Rh, age, sex, toxo-Rh, toxo-sex, Rh-sex, toxo-Rh-Sex as independent variables. Age and especially sex had very significant effects on participants’ performance in the test (age: eta2 = 0.023, p = 0.0004; sex: eta2 = 0.20, p < 0.0005). The effect of toxo depended on the Rh phenotype of subjects (toxo-Rh interaction: eta2 = 0.011, p = 0.026). Toxoplasma-seropositive, Rh-negative subjects expressed lower performance and Toxoplasma-seropositive Rh-positive subjects expressed higher performance than their Toxoplasma-seronegative peers, see the Fig 1. Separate GLM analyses performed for men and women showed that the effect was significant neither for men, nor for women (p values > 0.10).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Effects of toxoplasmosis and Rh phenotype on performance in the weight holding test.

Points show arithmetic means of endurance computed for covariate at its mean and spreads denote 95% confidence intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200346.g001

Hand-grip test

The hand-grip test was performed by 343 women, 66 (19.2%) Toxoplasma-seropositive and 207 men, 35 (16.9%) Toxoplasma-seropositive. The seroprevalence of toxoplasmosis did not differ between women and men (p = 0.570, Fisher exact test). Similarly, the frequency of Rh-negative subjects, 21.9% in women and 18.8% in men, did not differ between men and women or between Toxoplasma-seropositive and Toxoplasma-seronegative male or female subjects (Chi2 = 1.48, df = 4, p = 0.830, partial p values > 0.11). The age of Toxoplasma-seropositive subjects was higher than age of seronegative subjects (women: 24.2, SD 6.3 vs 22.48, SD = 3.09, t345 = -3.24, p = 0.001, men: 25.8, SD 8.38 vs 23.7, SD = 4.45, t206 = -2.12, p = 0.035). The effects of Toxoplasma-seropositivity and Rh phenotype in the hand-grip test were measured with repeated measures GLM with the performance in three dominant hand-trials and in three non-dominant hand-trials as nested repeated measures, and toxo, Rh, sex, hand, trial, age, and all interactions of toxo, Rh, sex, hand, and trial as independent variables. Table 1 shows the results of this analysis and analyses performed separately for men and women. Fig 2 shows that the effect of trial-Rh-toxo is stronger in men than in women and that the toxoplasmosis had negative effects on the performance of the Rh-negative subjects. Separate analyses performed for men and women showed that the effects were significant for men, but not for women, see Table 1.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Effects of toxoplasmosis and Rh phenotype on performance in the hand-grip test.

Points show arithmetic means of endurance computed for covariate at its mean and spreads denote 95% confidence intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200346.g002

thumbnail
Table 1. Effects of toxoplasmosis, Rh phenotype, hand and trial on performance in the hand-grip test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200346.t001

Discussion

The results of the present study suggest that Toxoplasma seropositivity was related to the performance of infected subjects in two tests that primarily measure the isometric (weight-holding test) and isotonic (hand-grip test) strength of participants. This relation was different in Rh-positive and Rh-negative subjects. Toxoplasma-seropositivity was always associated negatively with the performance of Rh-negative subjects, but it had a positive association with endurance in the weight holding test of the Rh-positive subjects, especially the men.

The main reason for doing the present study was to find direct evidence for lower endurance of Toxoplasma-infected subjects, which has been suspected to exist on the basis of several questionnaire studies [31]. Our results only partly confirmed this prediction as the lower endurance in both tests was observed only in the Rh-negative subjects. Moreover, endurance measured with the weight holding test in the Rh-positive subjects (i.e. in majority of the population) was higher in the infected than in the non-infected subjects. Our experimental setup cannot discriminate between whether the lower performance of the seropositive, Rh-negative subjects in the tests was caused by their lower endurance or by their lower physical strength. However, the fact that better rather than worse performance in the hand-grip test was observed in the first dominant-hand trial in men (see the Fig 2) suggests that lower endurance rather than lower physical strength is the more probable explanation of the result.

The existence of significant Rh-Toxo (or Trial-Rh-Toxo) interactions agreed with the results of certain performance tests [27,28] and of certain health status studies [13,29] published earlier. We confirmed that Toxoplasma-infected, Rh-negative subjects expressed worse results in performance tests, while this was not always true for Rh-positive subjects. In two previous studies performed on blood donors [27] and students [28], the effects of Rh-toxoplasmosis interaction on performance in simple reaction tests were studied. These studies showed that the Toxoplasma free, Rh-negative subjects had shorter reaction times than Toxoplasma-free Rh-positive subjects while Toxoplasma-infected, Rh-negative subjects had much worse, i.e. longer, reaction times than the Toxoplasma-infected, Rh-positive subjects. In the 2008 studies, the authors also showed, but did not discuss, that among the Rh-positive subjects, the Toxoplasma-infected ones, especially the Rh-positive heterozygotes, had better performance than the Toxoplasma-free subjects. A similar paradoxical phenomenon, namely the positive effect of Toxoplasma infection on mental health of the Rh-positive subjects, was described in a case-controls study performed on 79 women who had been tested for toxoplasmosis and Rh phenotype. The Toxoplasma-infected, Rh-negative women expressed higher neuroticism measured with the N-70 neuroticism inventory and reported more health problems than the Toxoplasma-free, Rh-negative women; the opposite was true for Rh-positive women [29]. It can be speculated that the better performance of the Toxoplasma-seropositive, Rh-positive subjects in performance tests is a result of an increased level of testosterone, which can be associated with higher levels of competitiveness [39]. However, while the increased level of testosterone was observed only in men [40,41], male mice [42] and non-castrated male rats [43], the better performance of the infected individuals when compared to non-infected individuals in Rh-positive subjects was observed in both men and women. Moreover, the higher competitiveness of Toxoplasma-infected subjects can hardly explain the better physical health of infected, Rh-positive subjects [29]. In our evolutionary past in Africa, and probably also in our recent past throughout the world, nearly all people were probably infected with Toxoplasma. In Africa 95% of humans are Rh-positive and in Asia the frequency of Rh-positive phenotypes is even higher, about 99%. A higher frequency of Rh-negative subjects, about 16%, is only among Europeans. This higher frequency of Rh-negative subjects was explained by the low abundance of felids and therefore probably the low prevalence of toxoplasmosis in Europe before the recent advent of domestic cat [28]. Among the Toxoplasma-free subjects, the Rh-negative individuals and even more the Rh-positive heterozygotes outperform the Rh-positive homozygotes in physical health and in many psychomotor and cognitive performance-related parameters [27,4446]. Therefore, the allele for Rh-negativity can spread in low toxoplasmosis prevalence areas, e.g. in ancient Europe. In contrast, in many parts of Africa, Asia and both Americas the abundance of various feline species is much higher and therefore the prevalence of toxoplasmosis in human population is (and always was) also much higher. It is possible that natural status of humans is to be infected with toxoplasma and therefore that the human body is adapted to this infection. Consequently, the performance and health of infected Rh-positive subjects is better than the performance of non-infected Rh-positive subjects.

The results of the reaction time and the endurance tests, however, contrast strongly with the results of intelligence tests performed on a cohort of 502 male soldiers [13]. This study also showed a significant Rh-toxoplasmosis interaction, however, the infected Rh-negative soldiers scored higher and infected Rh-positive subjects scored lower in two different intelligence tests than corresponding Toxoplasma-free subjects. We have no explanation for the results of this study, however, the better performance of the infected Rh-negative subjects in the IQ tests suggest that the effects of Rh-toxoplasmosis interaction on performance are context-dependent and rather specific.

Limitation

The main limitation of the present study is that the participants have been screened for the Rh phenotype, not Rh genotype. All available data, however, suggest that the Rh-positive heterozygotes, not homozygotes, are resistant to physiological effects of toxoplasmosis. More informative data will probably be obtained when the Rh-positive participants are tested for their Rh genotype. The number of Rh-negative, Toxoplasma seropositive male subjects, the rarest subpopulation in general population, is relatively low. This is probably the reason for the absence of a significant effect of the Rh-Toxoplasma interaction in males, despite a relatively large effect size. The general design of a study, the cross-sectional cohort study, can prove the existence of statistical association between two or more variables–here Toxoplasma seropositivity, Rh phenotype and endurance–but cannot solve the question of causality. Of course, neither Toxoplasma seropositivity nor Rh phenotype could be affected by the endurance of a human. However, for example, both the probability of the infection and the endurance can be influenced by some unknown third variable, such as the health status of subjects.

Conclusions

Our results partly support the hypotheses that latent toxoplasmosis has negative effects on the endurance of infected subjects, and Rh positivity protects people against some negative effects of toxoplasmosis. It must, however, be stressed that all evidence is only indirect and should be supported by the results of manipulative studies performed on laboratory-infected animals.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Charlie Lotterman for his help with the final version of the paper. This work has been supported by Czech Science Fundation 18-13692S and Charles University Research Centre program No. 204056.

References

  1. 1. Tenter AM, Heckeroth AR, Weiss LM (2000) Toxoplasma gondii: from animals to humans. International Journal for Parasitology 30: 1217–1258. pmid:11113252
  2. 2. Pappas G, Roussos N, Falagas ME (2009) Toxoplasmosis snapshots: Global status of Toxoplasma gondii seroprevalence and implications for pregnancy and congenital toxoplasmosis. International Journal for Parasitology 39: 1385–1394. pmid:19433092
  3. 3. Flegr J, Hrdý I (1994) Influence of chronic toxoplasmosis on some human personality factors. Folia Parasitologica 41: 122–126. pmid:7927062
  4. 4. Flegr J, Preiss M, Klose J, Havlíček J, Vitáková M (2003) Decreased level of psychobiological factor novelty seeking and lower intelligence in men latently infected with the protozoan parasite Toxoplasma gondii. Dopamine, a missing link between schizophrenia and toxoplasmosis? Biological Psychology 63: 253–268. pmid:12853170
  5. 5. Havlíček J, Gašová Z, Smith AP, Zvára K, Flegr J (2001) Decrease of psychomotor performance in subjects with latent 'asymptomatic' toxoplasmosis. Parasitology 122: 515–520. pmid:11393824
  6. 6. Pearce BD, Hubbard S, Rivera HN, Wilkins PP, Fisch MC, Hopkins MH, et al. (2013) Toxoplasma gondii exposure affects neural processing speed as measured by acoustic startle latency in schizophrenia and controls. Schizophrenia Research 150: 258–261. pmid:23953218
  7. 7. Flegr J, Guenter W, Bielinski M (2012) Toxoplasma gondii infection affects cognitive function–corrigendum. Folia Parasitologica 59: 253–254.
  8. 8. Ferreira EC, Marchioro AA, Guedes TA, Mota D, Guilherme ALF, de Araújo SM (2013) Association between seropositivity for Toxoplasma gondii, scholastic development of children and risk factors for T-gondii infection. Transactions of the Royal Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene 107: 390–396. pmid:23598948
  9. 9. Dickerson F, Stallings C, Origoni A, Katsafanas E, Schweinfurth L, Savage C, et al. (2014) Antibodies to Toxoplasma gondii and cognitive functioning in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and nonpsychiatric controls. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 202: 589–593. pmid:25010110
  10. 10. Pearce BD, Kruszon-Moran D, Jones JL (2014) The association of Toxoplasma gondii infection with neurocognitive deficits in a population-based analysis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 49: 1001–1010. pmid:24477344
  11. 11. Flegr J, Prandota J, Sovickova M, Israili ZH (2014) Toxoplasmosis—A global threat. Correlation of latent toxoplasmosis with specific disease burden in a set of 88 countries. PLoS ONE 9.
  12. 12. Flegr J, Escudero DQ (2016) Impaired health status and increased incidence of diseases in Toxoplasma-seropositive subjects—an explorative cross-sectional study. Parasitology 143: 1974–1989. pmid:27719690
  13. 13. Flegr J, Preiss M, Klose J (2013) Toxoplasmosis-associated difference in intelligence and personality in men depends on their Rhesus blood group but not ABO blood group. PLoS ONE 8.
  14. 14. Stock AK, von Heinegg EH, Kohling HL, Beste C (2014) Latent Toxoplasma gondii infection leads to improved action control. Brain Behavior and Immunity 37: 103–108.
  15. 15. Flegr J, Zitkova S, Kodym P, Frynta D (1996) Induction of changes in human behaviour by the parasitic protozoan Toxoplasma gondii. Parasitology 113: 49–54. pmid:8710414
  16. 16. Flegr J, Kodym P, Tolarová V (2000) Correlation of duration of latent Toxoplasma gondii infection with personality changes in women. Biological Psychology 53: 57–68. pmid:10876065
  17. 17. Webster JP (2001) Rats, cats, people and parasites: the impact of latent toxoplasmosis on behaviour. Microbes and Infection 3: 1037–1045. pmid:11580990
  18. 18. Vyas A, Sapolsky R (2010) Manipulation of host behaviour by Toxoplasma gondii: what is the minimum a proposed proximate mechanism should explain? Folia Parasitologica 57: 88–94. pmid:20608470
  19. 19. Vyas A (2015) Mechanisms of host behavioral change in Toxoplasma gondii rodent association. PLoS Pathogens 11.
  20. 20. Hodková H, Kodym P, Flegr J (2007) Poorer results of mice with latent toxoplasmosis in learning tests: impaired learning processes or the novelty discrimination mechanism? Parasitology 134: 1329–1337. pmid:17445326
  21. 21. Lindová J, Novotná M, Havlíček J, Jozífková E, Skallová A, Kolbeková P, et al. (2006) Gender differences in behavioural changes induced by latent toxoplasmosis. International Journal for Parasitology 36: 1485–1492. pmid:16978630
  22. 22. Lindová J, Kuběna AA, Šturcová A, Křivohlavá R, Novotná M, Rubešová A, et al. (2010) Pattern of money allocation in experimental games supports the stress hypothesis of gender differences in Toxoplasma gondii-induced behavioural changes. Folia Parasitologica 57: 136–142. pmid:20608476
  23. 23. Carver CS, Scheier MF, Weintraub JK (1989) Assessing coping strategies—A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 56: 267–283. pmid:2926629
  24. 24. Hobfoll SE, Dunahoo CL, Ben-Porath Y, Monnier J (1994) Gender and coping: the dual-axis model of coping. American Journal of Community Psychology 22: 49–82. pmid:7942644
  25. 25. Tamres LK, Janicki D, Helgeson VS (2002) Sex differences in coping behavior: A meta-analytic review and an examination of relative coping. Personality and Social Psychology Review 6: 2–30.
  26. 26. Flegr J, Novotná M, Fialová A, Kolbeková P, Gašová Z (2010) The influence of RhD phenotype on toxoplasmosis- and age-associated changes in personality profile of blood donors. Folia Parasitologica 57: 143–150. pmid:20608477
  27. 27. Novotná M, Havlíček J, Smith AP, Kolbeková P, Skallová A, Klose J, et al. (2008) Toxoplasma and reaction time: Role of toxoplasmosis in the origin, preservation and geographical distribution of Rh blood group polymorphism. Parasitology 135: 1253–1261. pmid:18752708
  28. 28. Flegr J, Novotná M, Lindová J, Havlíček J (2008) Neurophysiological effect of the Rh factor. Protective role of the RhD molecule against Toxoplasma-induced impairment of reaction times in women. Neuroendocrinology Letters 29: 475–481. pmid:18766148
  29. 29. Šebánková B, Flegr J (2017) Physical and mental health status in Toxoplasma-infected women before and three years after they learn about their infection: Manipulation or side-effects of impaired health?. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 5: 144.
  30. 30. Kaňková Š, Šulc J, Flegr J (2010) Increased pregnancy weight gain in women with latent toxoplasmosis and RhD-positivity protection against this effect. Parasitology 137: 1773–1779. pmid:20602855
  31. 31. Flegr J (2010) Influence of latent toxoplasmosis on the phenotype of intermediate hosts. Folia Parasitologica 57: 81–87. pmid:20608469
  32. 32. Arling TA, Yolken RH, Lapidus M, Langenberg P, Dickerson FB, Zimmerman SA, et al. (2009) Toxoplasma gondii antibody titers and history of suicide attempts in patients with recurrent mood disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 197: 905–908. pmid:20010026
  33. 33. Okusaga O, Langenberg P, Sleemi A, Vaswani D, Giegling I, Hartman AM, et al. (2011) Toxoplasma gondii antibody titers and history of suicide attempts in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 133: 150–155. pmid:21890329
  34. 34. Yagmur F, Yazar S, Temel HO, Cavusoglu M (2010) May Toxoplasma gondii increase suicide attempt—preliminary results in Turkish subjects? Forensic Science International 199: 15–17. pmid:20219300
  35. 35. Zhang Y, Traskman-Bendz L, Janelidze S, Langenberg P, Saleh A, Constantine N, et al. (2012) Toxoplasma gondii immunoglobulin G antibodies and nonfatal suicidal self-directed violence. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 73: 1069–1076. pmid:22938818
  36. 36. Ling VJ, Lester D, Mortensen PB, Langenberg PW, Postolache TT (2011) Toxoplasma gondii seropositivity and suicide rates in women. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 199: 440–444. pmid:21716055
  37. 37. Bohannon RW (2001) Dynamometer measurements of hand-grip strength predict multiple outcomes. Perceptual and Motor Skills 93: 323–328. pmid:11769883
  38. 38. Mafi P, Mafi R, Hindocha S, Griffin M, Khan W (2012) A systematic review of dynamometry and its role in hand trauma assessment. Open Orthop J 6: 95–102. pmid:22423305
  39. 39. Archer J (2006) Testosterone and human aggression: an evaluation of the challenge hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30: 319–345. pmid:16483890
  40. 40. Flegr J, Lindova J, Kodym P (2008) Sex-dependent toxoplasmosis-associated differences in testosterone concentration in humans. Parasitology 135: 427–431. pmid:18205984
  41. 41. Flegr J, Lindová J, Pivoňková V, Havlíček J (2008) Brief Communication:Latent toxoplasmosis and salivary testosterone concentration-important confounding factors in second to fourth digit ratio studies. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 137: 479–484. pmid:18615572
  42. 42. Kaňková Š, Kodym P, Flegr J (2011) Direct evidence of Toxoplasma-induced changes in serum testosterone in mice. Experimental Parasitology 128: 181–183. pmid:21458453
  43. 43. Lim A, Kumar V, Hari Dass SA, Vyas A (2013) Toxoplasma gondii infection enhances testicular steroidogenesis in rats. Mol Ecol 22: 102–110. pmid:23190313
  44. 44. Flegr J, Hoffmann R, Dammann M (2015) Worse health status and higher incidence of health disorders in Rhesus negative subjects. PLoS ONE 10.
  45. 45. Flegr J (2016) Heterozygote advantage probably maintains Rhesus factor blood group polymorphism: Ecological regression study. PLoS ONE 11.
  46. 46. Flegr J, Geryk J, Volny J, Klose J, Cernochova D (2012) Rhesus factor modulation of effects of smoking and age on psychomotor performance, intelligence, personality profile, and health in Czech soldiers. PLoS ONE 7: e49478. pmid:23209579