Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Emotional cues from expressive behavior of women and men with Parkinson’s disease

  • Shu-Mei Wang ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    shumei.wang@polyu.edu.hk

    Affiliation Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hung Hom, Kowloon, Hong Kong

  • Linda Tickle-Degnen

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Arts and Sciences, Tufts University, Medford, Massachusetts, United States of America

Abstract

Objective

Emotional experience of people with Parkinson’s disease is prone to being misunderstood by observers and even healthcare practitioners, which affects treatment effectiveness and makes clients suffer distress in their social lives. This study was designed to identify reliable emotional cues from expressive behavior in women and men with Parkinson’s disease.

Method

Videotaped expressive behavior of 96 participants during an interview of discussing enjoyable events was rated using the Interpersonal Communication Rating Protocol. Indices from emotional measures were represented in three components. Correlational analyses between expressive behavior domains and emotional components were conducted for the total sample and by gender separately.

Results

More gross motor expressivity and smiling/laughing indicated more positive affect in the total sample. Less conversational engagement indicated more negative affect in women. However, women with more negative affect and depression appeared to smile and laugh more.

Conclusion

This study identified reliable cues from expressive behavior that could be used for assessment of emotional experience in people with Parkinson’s disease. For women, because smiling/laughing may convey two possible meanings, that is, more positive and more negative affect, this cue needs to be interpreted cautiously and be used for detecting the intensity, not the type, of emotional experience. Healthcare practitioners should be sensitive to valid cues to make an accurate evaluation of emotion in people with Parkinson’s disease.

Introduction

Parkinson’s disease affects dopamine-generating neurons in the substantia nigra of the basal ganglia and causes movement disorders [1]. The resulting movement symptoms, including bradykinesia (slowness of movements), muscle rigidity, and tremor, hamper expressive behavior. Abnormalities of emotional expressive behavior have been extensively documented in people with Parkinson’s disease (PD) [27]. Expressive behavior refers to the acts or movements involving channels via the face, body, voice, and speech that communicate a person’s inner emotion, personality, or motives [8,9]. During interpersonal interaction, conversational partners perceive these nonverbal or verbal cues from the person’s expressive behavior and accordingly interpret the person’s internal state [1013]. Reduced expressivity easily misleads lay observers and health practitioners into identifying people with PD as being sad, hostile, or lacking emotion entirely, and into forming a possibly incorrect negative impression of them [1416]. Indeed, studies [1518] have shown that health practitioners are prone to making erroneous judgments of inner attributes in people with PD, which poses a threat to practitioner-patient rapport and effective treatment. Qualitative studies have indicated that people with PD suffer distress because of being misunderstood and negatively evaluated in every day social life [1921], which emphasizes the need to enable observers to accurately interpret these individuals’ behavior.

Despite impairment in expressive behavior, people with PD have the potential to effectively convey their emotional experience in certain contexts [6,2224], such as while they discuss enjoyable or pleasant topics and thus are facially expressive. Emotion literature [25,26] consistently indicates a two-dimension affective structure, that is, positive affect and negative affect, which are generally independent of each other (i.e. orthogonal). High positive affect includes high energy, enthusiasm, and pleasurable engagement, whereas low positive affect is characterized by lethargy [26]. Negative affect includes a variety of aversive emotions, such as distress and guilt. Low negative affect is characterized by calmness and tranquility [26]. Research findings [22,23] have shown that individuals with PD generally show more facial movement during contexts designed to elicit positive affect (e.g., talking about pleasant or enjoyable topics) compared with contexts which elicit negative affect (e.g., talking about unpleasant or frustrating topics). Additionally, it has been reported [24,27] that negative affect, compared with a calm state, exacerbates facial bradykinesia in people with PD, similar to the flat affect shown in the faces of depressed individuals. However, little is known about the association between emotional experience and expressive behaviors in the face, body, voice, and speech in people with PD. Identifying valid cues of positive affect, negative affect and depression from expressive behavior will help health professionals increase their sensitivity to valid cues, use these cues to accurately understand emotional experience in people with PD, and effectively adjust treatment plans.

It is also noteworthy that gender differences exist in expressive behavior in healthy people [2830], although there is very little relevant research on women and men with movement disorders. Women generally are more nonverbally expressive than men because gender norms in society expect women to be emotionally expressive and to smile during interpersonal interactions, and expect men to suppress their emotional expression [29,31]. We anticipate that women and men with PD would follow the pattern of healthy people and differ in expressive movement. Indeed, the few studies [32,33] that have examined gender differences in expressive behavior among people with PD showed that women are more facially expressive than men. Further examination of potential differences in emotional cues between men and women with the disease will clarify whether or not practitioners must modify their use of cues according to gender.

To sum up, the primary purpose of this study was to identify valid emotional cues by examining associations between expressive behavior in the face, body, voice, and speech, and emotional experiences, including positive and negative ones, in people with PD. A secondary purpose of the study was exploratory, to begin to identify hypothesized gender differences and address this gap in the PD literature. This study had four research hypotheses, the first three addressing our primary purpose and the fourth our secondary purpose. First, we anticipated that expressive behavior would be associated with emotional experience, that is, expressive behavior would be valid cues of emotional experience in people with PD while they discussed enjoyable topics. Second, a higher degree of expressive behavior would indicate more positive emotional experience. Third, a lower degree of expressive behavior would indicate more negative emotional experience or more severe depression. We reasoned that individuals, even when discussing enjoyable topics, would vary in the degree to which they felt negative affect or were depressed. Fourth, we hypothesized that expressive behavior would be a stronger indicator of emotional experience in women than in men with PD. Specifically, the association between expressive behavior and emotional experience would be of larger magnitude in women than in men. This final hypothesis was exploratory in nature.

Methods

Research design

A correlational study design was used. We conducted the study by analyzing the baseline database from the American research study on Self-Management Rehabilitation for PD [22,34], which contains data on emotional experience, videotaped expressive behavior during an interview, demographic characteristics, and movement symptom severity (e.g., the motor examination section score of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale) (S1 Dataset).

Participants

The participants in the current study met the following inclusion criteria, which came from the original research study: (1) a diagnosis of idiopathic PD confirmed by a neurologist; (2) ≥ 40 years old; (3) a score > 26 on the Mini-Mental Status Examination [35]; (4) Hoehn and Yahr stage 2.0–3.0 in “on” periods of responses to medications; (5) a score ≤ 20 on the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) [36]; (6) receiving stable doses of anti-Parkinson drugs for at least the 2 weeks immediately before the study began; (7) no difficulty in communicating with research personnel, assessed through direct observation of participants; and (8) no other severe diseases that might affect the participant’s movements. In order to be included in the current study, participants were those in the original research study who (1) had given written informed consent (on forms approved by the institutional review boards of Boston Medical Center and Boston University) to agree to the use of their data in studies other than the original research study; and (2) had no missing data concerning expressive behavior and emotional experience in the database.

Procedure

At baseline, participants who passed screening completed a 30-minute videotaped interview protocol that involved self-reports about quality of life and enjoyable events in the past week in response to open-ended questions. Immediately after the interview, participants completed a self-report measure of their experienced affect during the interview.

Measures of emotional experience

We included all measures from the database that addressed emotional experience. First, the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) [26] evaluated self-reported emotional experience during the interview. The PANAS includes 10 positive and 10 negative affect descriptors that require respondents to rate the extent of their experience of each specific affect descriptor on a five-point scale. A higher score reflects higher level of affective activation. Second, the GDS [36] evaluated depressive symptoms over the past week. The GDS is a self-report instrument and has 30 yes/no questions, with a higher total score being given for more severe depression. Due to the inclusion criterion (i.e., a GDS score ≤ 20) of the original database, the participants in the present study varied within the range of non-severe depression. Third, the Emotional Well-being subscale of the Parkinson’s Disease Questionnaire-39 items (PDQ-39) [37] measured self-reported emotional experience over the past month. The items of this subscale address attributes primarily related to depression and anxiety [38]. A higher score indicates more negative emotional experience. After principal component analysis (see S1 Table), the indices from all three emotional measures were divided into three emotional components: Positive Affect (PA; all positive affect indices of PANAS), Negative Affect (NA; all negative affect indices of PANAS), and Depression (all of the other indices). Three composite scores were created by averaging the participants’ z-scored responses to the indices comprising each component.

Measures of expressive behavior

The Interpersonal Communication Rating Protocol (ICRP) [39] was used to rate facial, bodily, vocal, and verbal expressive behavior in people with PD. Several studies [18,22,40] have validated the use of ICRP items to evaluate expressive behavior in people with PD. The ICRP involved three measurement steps. First, a video camera filmed the face and upper body of people with PD, who were seated, during interviews. Second, short clips were extracted from the last 20 seconds of the interview videotapes when the participant described recent enjoyable events. The third step involved four trained raters separately rating each participant’s behavior during the clip. Participants’ clips were placed in random order on a DVD for viewing. Raters viewed the video channels of clips to rate facial and bodily behaviors, listened to the audio channels to rate vocal and two verbal behaviors (positive content and negative content), and used combined video and audio channels to rate the third verbal behavior “topic control”. Raters used a 5-point Likert scale (score 1 = low and 5 = high) to score their gestalt impression of the frequency, duration, and intensity of each ICRP item. Subsequently, the raters’ scores for each participant were averaged for each ICRP item. Effective reliability [41,42] was used to indicate how reliable the average score of four raters’ ratings was. The equation of effective reliability [41,42] is: where RSB is effective reliability for the average score, n is the number of raters, and r is the average of the interrater reliability of all possible pairs of raters in a group of raters (called mean reliability). The average score from the four raters achieved adequate effective reliability on ICRP items (a mean of RSB = 0.77) for the current study. Raters were blind to participants’ emotion scores to prevent rating biases. After principal component analysis (see S1 Supporting Information), all ICRP items were divided into six domains (Table 1) and integrated into one composite factor: Expressive Activation. Domain scores were created by averaging the scores of items that loaded on the same component. The Expressive Activation score was formed by averaging all ICRP scores after first reverse-scoring the forward slouching and tremors items, which were negatively correlated with the composite factor.

Data analysis

Pearson’s correlation coefficients were calculated to examine the relationships between expressive behavior scores and emotional experience scores for the total sample, as well as for women, and men separately. Expressive behavior scores included Expressive Activation and the six expressive behavior domains. Emotional experience scores included Positive Affect, Negative Affect, and Depression. The Z test [43] was used to compare correlation coefficients between women and men. The alpha level of the statistical significance tests (two-tailed) for the correlational analysis and the Z test was set at 5% for statistical significance, and between 5% and 10% for trends toward statistical significance. We referred to Cohen’s criteria [44] for interpreting the magnitude of correlations, with the absolute value of 0.10 being considered “small”, 0.30 being considered “moderate”, and 0.50 being considered “large”.

Results

Participant characteristics

Among the original 116 participants in the original Self-Management Rehabilitation for PD database, 96 people (Table 2) met the inclusion criteria and were used in correlational analysis. The ratio of women (n = 26) to men (n = 70) in this study was 1:2.7, which is similar to the ratio (approximately 1: 2) of the PD population [45]. Men had more severe movement symptoms than women.

thumbnail
Table 2. Demographic and clinical data for the total sample (N = 96) and separately for women (n = 26) and men (n = 70).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.t002

Expressive behavior correlates of Positive Affect

Descriptive statistics of emotional experience and expressive behavior variables are displayed in S2 and S3 Tables. Table 3 shows the correlations between expressive behavior and Positive Affect. Expressive Activation was a small-to-moderate correlate of Positive Affect in the total sample (Fig 1). More Expressive Activation was indicative of more Positive Affect. Among the expressive behavior domains, Gross Motor Expressivity and Smile-Laugh were small-to-moderate correlates of Positive Affect in the total sample (Figs 2 and 3). More Gross Motor Expressivity and more Smile-Laugh were indicative of more Positive Affect.

thumbnail
Table 3. Correlation between expressive behavior and Positive Affect for the total sample (N = 96) and separately for women (n = 26) and men (n = 70).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.t003

thumbnail
Fig 1. Correlation between Positive Affect and Expressive Activation in the total sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Correlation between Positive Affect and Gross Motor Expressivity in the total sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Correlation between Positive Affect and Smile-Laugh in the total sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.g003

Although no significant gender differences were found in the correlations, the results showed that women’s Expressive Activation was indicative of Positive Affect at a moderate-to-large magnitude of positive correlation (Fig 4), while this correlation in men was at a small-to-moderate magnitude. For women, Gross Motor Expressivity and Smile-Laugh were moderate correlates of Positive Affect (Figs 5 and 6). More Gross Motor Expressivity and Smile-Laugh in women were indicative of more Positive Affect. For men, however, these two expressive behavior domains were small correlates of Positive Affect.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Correlation between Positive Affect and Expressive Activation in women.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Correlation between Positive Affect and Gross Motor Expressivity in women.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.g005

thumbnail
Fig 6. Correlation between Positive Affect and Smile-Laugh in women.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.g006

Expressive behavior correlates of Negative Affect

Table 4 shows the correlations between expressive behavior and Negative Affect. No significant correlations were found in the total sample. In terms of gender differences, women and men differed in the Smile Laugh-Negative Affect correlation and the Confident Expressivity-Negative Affect correlation. Women’s Smile-Laugh was indicative of Negative Affect at a moderate-to-large magnitude of positive correlation (Fig 7), but this correlation in men became negative at a small magnitude. Additionally, men’s Confident Expressivity was indicative of Negative Affect at a moderate magnitude of negative correlation (Fig 8), while this correlation in women became positive at a moderate magnitude. Although no significant gender difference was found in the Conversational Engagement-Negative Affect correlation, the results showed that women’s Conversational Engagement was indicative of Negative Affect at a moderate-to-large magnitude of negative correlation (Fig 9), while this correlation in men was at a small magnitude.

thumbnail
Table 4. Correlation of expressive behavior with Negative Affect and Depression for the total sample (N = 96) and separately for women (n = 26) and men (n = 70).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.t004

thumbnail
Fig 7. Correlation between Negative Affect and Smile-Laugh in women.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.g007

thumbnail
Fig 8. Correlation between Negative Affect and Confident Expressivity in men.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.g008

thumbnail
Fig 9. Correlation between Negative Affect and Conversational Engagement in women.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.g009

Visual inspection of correlation figures in terms of Negative Affect (Figs 7 to 9) suggested the possibility of extreme outliers. In order to check if extreme outliers drove the aforementioned correlation results of Negative Affect in women and men, we conducted an extreme outlier analysis based on examination of variable boxplots in both genders in IBM SPSS Statistics 23. An extreme outlier, which was outside three times the distance of interquartile range of data [46], was found in Negative Affect of men. After we eliminated that extreme outlier participant and re-conducted the Pearson’s correlation, the Confident Expressivity-Negative Affect correlation in men became non-significant (r = -0.07, p = 0.56, n = 69). Consequently, the gender difference in the Confident Expressivity-Negative Affect correlations became non-significant (Z = 1.49, p = 0.14). In addition to eliminating the extreme outlier and re-running the Pearson’s correlation, another strategy to handle the extreme outlier is to conduct the Spearman’s correlation on the entire sample of men including the outlier (n = 70). The result was consistent with the finding when we eliminated the outlier by showing that the Confident Expressivity-Negative Affect correlation in men was non-significant (r = -.07, p = .58).

Expressive behavior correlates of Depression

No significant correlations were found in the total sample (Table 4). Although no significant gender differences were found, women’s Smile-Laugh was indicative of Depression at a moderate-to-large magnitude of positive correlation (Fig 10), while this correlation in men was near-zero.

thumbnail
Fig 10. Correlation between Depression and Smile-Laugh in women.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.g010

Discussion

The findings of this study, supporting the first hypothesis, indicate that valid cues of emotional experience exist in the expressive behavior of people with PD. The results are consistent with earlier literature [6,2224] showing that even though expressive behavior is impaired, it continues to have the potential to effectively convey attributes in PD people. We extend earlier results by finding that expressive behavior served as valid emotional cues for this population. An increase in gross motor expressivity was indicative of more positive affect in people with PD. More conversational engagement was indicative of less negative affect in women. It is noteworthy that smiling and laughing appeared to be valid indicators of more positive affect as well as more negative affect and depression in women.

In line with our second hypothesis, a higher degree of expressive behavior indicated more positive affect in people with PD while they were discussing enjoyable topics, which is consistent with earlier work [22,23,47]. An increase in gross motor expressivity, smiling and laughing were reliably indicative of more positive affect in the PD population. Based on our findings and previous studies of personality cues [17,40] showing a linkage between brow furrowing and neuroticism, it is suggested that gross motor expressivity may serve as an important cue of inner attributes in people with PD. Additionally, smiling and laughing are plausible cues of positive affect because smiles are linked to enjoyment [4850], while laughter (which co-occurs often with smiling) is a multifaceted behavior that can occur in many emotional situations including happiness, nervousness, or sadness [5153].

The third research hypothesis was that a lower degree of expressive behavior indicated more negative affect and more severe depression in people with PD while they discussed enjoyable topics. However, our findings only partially support this hypothesis. The result of conversational engagement in women supports the hypothesis: less conversational engagement indicated more negative affect, which is in accord with earlier reports [54,55] indicating the linkage between fewer gestures and the lack of enthusiasm, as well as between slower speaking and sadness. The finding that fails to support the third hypothesis is that more smiling and laughing were also indicative of more negative affect and depression in addition to more positive affect in women, which may be explained by women’s interpersonal motivations. Literature [28,56] has suggested that women seem to be more concerned with social approval, affiliation, and making good impressions via smiling than men, thus making their smiles less reliable as an indicator of genuine enjoyment than those in men. In our study, social norms may have put pressure on people with PD to behave happily under the condition of discussing enjoyable events with an interviewer. Therefore, in order to conform to the social expectations and win social approval, women with more negative affect or depression may have disguised their negative emotional experience by demonstrating more smiles and laughter. This notion is supported by earlier studies [33,57,58] indicating that people with PD have the ability, although it is impaired compared with that in healthy people, to pose facial expression and mask negative emotion.

This fourth hypothesis was exploratory in nature. We hypothesized that expressive behavior was a stronger indicator of emotional experience in women than in men. Generally this pattern can be observed from the magnitudes of the correlational associations, which may be because women indeed are more nonverbally expressive than men [2831,33]. Moreover, we found that the genders presented contrasting smiling and laughing behavior when experiencing more negative affect, suggesting that gender differences not only appear in expressive intensity, but also appear in ways of conveying negative emotional experience.

It is noteworthy that men had more severe movement symptoms than women in this study. A possibility deserving comment is that expressive behavior showed a pattern of being a stronger cue of emotional experience in women than in men perhaps due to the former’s milder movement symptoms and not due to the gender factor that we proposed in the third hypothesis. In order to exclude this explanation, we re-examined the correlations between expressive behavior and emotional experience by computing partial correlation coefficients controlling for movement symptom severity measured by the motor examination section of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale. These partial correlation analyses did not change the findings compared to the uncontrolled correlational analyses. Therefore, the evidence supports the third hypothesis.

Limitations and future research

First, our sample was unable to include people with severe depression due to the inclusion criteria of the original database, which limited depression variation in the correlational analysis and thus the ability to detect emotional indicators from expressive behavior. Future research should include a heterogeneous sample with varying depression levels to identify possible indicators of the emotional experience. Second, difficulty in identifying feelings, which affects the self-report of emotional experience, may exist in people with PD [5961]. However, this study analyzed the already-collected data from the research database on Self-Management Rehabilitation for people with Parkinson’s disease [22,34], which did not directly assess whether the emotion-identifying difficulty existed in the participants. Nevertheless, this study used the Mini-Mental Status Examination in the screening step to ensure that participants had generally intact cognitive status, which is related to ability to identify feelings and complete self-report of emotional experience. In addition, participants in this study were in the earlier stages of the disease and functioning generally independently in the community, further supporting their ability to report their feelings. Therefore, the emotional data in this study are valid. Future studies that directly assesse emotion-identifying deficits in PD people and screen out unsuitable individuals will provide more robust evidence to justify the use of self-reported emotional data. Furthermore, this study focused on identifying valid emotional cues in people with PD. The next step is to examine the degree to which health practitioners use valid cues to evaluate these clients’ emotional experience. This study provides an exploratory report of gender differences in emotional cues in people with PD. Future research may adopt a large sample size of both women and men with PD to conduct a high-powered hypothesis test of our findings.

Implications

  • Health practitioners should be sensitive to and use gross motor expressivity to accurately evaluate positive affect in people with PD.
  • Practitioners should observe conversational engagement in women to accurately evaluate negative affect.
  • Smiling and laughing may serve as valid cues to indicate women’s higher intensity of emotional experience, regardless of positive affect or negative affect, which is crucial to activating the subsequent detailed emotional assessment. Practitioners who note more smiling and laughing in women could further probe with specific questions about positive and negative affect and gather women’s verbal disclosure to assess their emotional experience in a detail way [62]. Additionally, practitioners could rely on the combination of smile-laugh and other valid cues, such as the aforementioned gross motor expressivity and conversational engagement, to distinguish emotional types in women. Specifically, more smiling and laughing along with more gross motor expressivity reflect more positive affect. More smiling and laughing along with less conversational engagement indicate more negative affect in women.
  • Professional training of practitioners may involve sensitization to the valid emotional cues from expressive behavior in people with PD.
  • Practitioners may educate family caregivers of clients to observe the valid emotional indicators when they communicate with clients with PD. This family education helps reduce clients’ social distress because they could feel that their emotional experience is understood correctly by important others.

Conclusion

The major contribution of this study is that it identifies valid emotional cues from different domains of expressive behavior in people with PD while this population discussed enjoyable topics. The cues included (1) gross motor expressivity for positive affect in people with PD, and (2) conversational engagement for women’s negative affect. This study also suggests that smiling and laughing may be used to assess emotional intensity, but not different types of emotional experience, in women because more smiling and laughing in women may reflect more positive affect as well as more negative affect and depression. These results provide a basis for future studies examining whether practitioners indeed use these valid cues to accurately evaluate emotional experience in people with PD. Accurate evaluation presumably would contribute to quality improvement of treatment. Health practitioners may increase their sensitivity to using these valid cues to facilitate effective emotional evaluation and meaningful intervention for these clients.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Rotated component loadings for each emotional index in the three-component solution of the principal component analysis with varimax rotation for people with Parkinson’s disease (N = 105a).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Descriptive statistics of emotional measures for the total sample (N = 96) and separately for women (n = 26) and men (n = 70).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Descriptive statistics of expressive behavior variables for the total sample (N = 96) and separately for women (n = 26) and men (n = 70).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.s003

(DOCX)

S1 Supporting Information. Steps to develop expressive behavior domains.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199886.s004

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Sarah D. Gunnery, Ph.D., a postdoctoral fellow of Health Quality of Life Lab in Tufts University, for reviewing the manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Olanow CW, McNaught K. The etiopathogenesis of Parkinson’s disease: Basic mechanisms of neurodegeneration. In: Hallett M, Poewe W, editors. Therapeutics of Parkinson’s disease and other movement disorders. West Sussex (UK): Wiley-Blackwell; 2008. p. 3–23.
  2. 2. Argaud S, Verin M, Sauleau P, Grandjean D. Facial emotion recognition in Parkinson’s disease: A review and new hypotheses. Mov Disord. 2018;33: 554–567. pmid:29473661
  3. 3. Ricciardi L, Visco-Comandini F, Erro R, Morgante F, Bologna M, Fasano A, et al. Facial emotion recognition and expression in Parkinson’s disease: An emotional mirror mechanism? PLoS ONE. 2017;12: 1–16.
  4. 4. Bologna M, Berardelli I, Paparella G, Marsili L, Ricciardi L, Fabbrini G, et al. Altered kinematics of facial emotion expression and emotion recognition deficits are unrelated in Parkinson’s disease. Front Neurol. 2016;7: 1–7.
  5. 5. Schroder C, Nikolova ZT, Dengler R. Changes of emotional prosody in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci. 2010;289: 32–35. pmid:19732910
  6. 6. Simons G, Pasqualini MCS, Reddy V, Wood J. Emotional and nonemotional facial expressions in people with Parkinson’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10: 521–535. pmid:15327731
  7. 7. Pitcairn TK, Clemie S, Gray JM, Pentland B. Non-verbal cues in the self-presentation of Parkinsonian patients. Br J Clin Psychol. 1990;29: 177–184. pmid:2364195
  8. 8. Allport GW, Vernon PE. Studies in expressive movement. New York (NY): MacMillan; 1933.
  9. 9. Ambady N, Bernieri FJ, Richeson JA. Toward a histology of social behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices of the behavioral stream. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2000;32: 201–271.
  10. 10. Frith CD, Frith U. Interacting minds: A biological basis. Science. 1999;286: 1692–1695. pmid:10576727
  11. 11. Poletti M, Enrici I, Adenzato M. Cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in neurodegenerative diseases: Neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neurochemical levels. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36: 2147–2164. pmid:22819986
  12. 12. Frith C, Frith U. Theory of mind. Curr Biol. 2005;15: R644–R645. pmid:16139190
  13. 13. Bodden ME, Mollenhauer B, Trenkwalder C, Cabanel N, Eggert KM, Unger MM, et al. Affective and cognitive theory of mind in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2010;16: 466–470. pmid:20538499
  14. 14. Pell MD, Cheang HS, Leonard CL. The impact of Parkinson’s disease on vocal-prosodic communication from the perspective of listeners. Brain Lang. 2006;97: 123–134. pmid:16226803
  15. 15. Pentland B, Pitcairn TK, Gray JM, Riddle WJ. The effects of reduced expression in Parkinson’s disease on impression formation by health professionals. Clin Rehabil. 1987;1: 307–313.
  16. 16. Tickle-Degnen L, Zebrowitz LA, Ma HI. Culture, gender and health care stigma: Practitioners’ response to facial masking experienced by people with Parkinson’s disease. Soc Sci Med. 2011;73: 95–102. pmid:21664737
  17. 17. Tickle-Degnen L, Lyons KD. Practitioners’ impressions of patients with Parkinson’s disease: The social ecology of the expressive mask. Soc Sci Med. 2004;58: 603–614. pmid:14652056
  18. 18. Lyons KD, Tickle-Degnen L, Hemy A, Cohn E. Impressions of personality in Parkinson’s disease: Can rehabilitation practitioners see beyond the symptoms? Rehabil Psychol. 2004;49: 328–333.
  19. 19. Caap-Ahlgren M, Lena L, Ove D. Older Swedish women’s experiences of living with symptoms related to Parkinson’s disease. J Adv Nurs. 2002;39: 87–95. pmid:12074755
  20. 20. Miller N, Noble E, Jones D, Burn D. Life with communication changes in Parkinson’s disease. Age Ageing. 2006;35: 235–239. pmid:16540492
  21. 21. Thordardottir B, Nilsson MH, Iwarsson S, Haak M. “You plan, but you never know”–participation among people with different levels of severity of Parkinson’s disease. Disabil Rehabil. 2014;36: 2216–2224. pmid:24670191
  22. 22. Takahashi K, Tickle-Degnen L, Coster WJ, Latham NK. Expressive behavior in Parkinson’s disease as a function of interview context. Am J Occup Ther. 2010;64: 484–495. pmid:20608279
  23. 23. Brozgold AZ, Borod JC, Martin CC, Pick LH, Alpert M, Welkowitz J. Social functioning and facial emotional expression in neurological and psychiatric disorders. Appl Neuropsychol. 1998;5: 15–23. pmid:16318462
  24. 24. Griffin WA, Greene SM. Social interaction and symptom sequences: A case study of orofacial bradykinesia exacerbation in Parkinson’s disease during negative marital interaction. Psychiatry. 1994;57: 269–274.
  25. 25. Watson D, Tellegen A. Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychol Bull. 1985;98: 219–235. pmid:3901060
  26. 26. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54: 1063–1070. pmid:3397865
  27. 27. Knapp ML, Hall JA. The effects of the face on human communication. In: Knapp ML, Hall JA, editors. Nonverbal communication in human interaction. Belmont (CA): Thomson Wadsworth; 2006. p. 295–337.
  28. 28. Depaulo BM. Nonverbal behavior and self-presentation. Psychol Bull. 1992;111: 203–243. pmid:1557474
  29. 29. Dodd DK, Russell BL, Jenkins C. Smiling in school yearbook photos: Gender differences from kindergarten to adulthood. Psychol Rec. 1999;49: 543–553.
  30. 30. Wang SF, Liu ZL, Wang ZY, Wu GB, Shen PJ, He S, et al. Analyses of a multimodal spontaneous facial expression database. IEEE Trans Affect Comput. 2013;4: 34–46.
  31. 31. Hall JA. Women’s and men’s nonverbal communication: Similarities, differences, stereotypes, and origins. In: Manusov V, Patterson ML, editors. The SAGE handbook of nonverbal communication. Thousand Oaks (CA): Sage; 2006. p. 201–218.
  32. 32. Gunnery SD, Naumova EN, Saint-Hilaire M, Tickle-Degnen L. Mapping spontaneous facial expression in people with Parkinson’s disease: A multiple case study design. Cogent Psychology. 2017;4: 15.
  33. 33. Simons G, Ellgring H, Pasqualini MCS. Disturbance of spontaneous and posed facial expressions in Parkinson’s disease. Cogn Emot. 2003;17: 759–778.
  34. 34. Tickle-Degnen L, Ellis T, Saint-Hilaire MH, Thomas CA, Wagenaar RC. Self-management rehabilitation and health-related quality of life in Parkinson’s disease: A randomized controlled trial. Mov Disord. 2010;25: 194–204. pmid:20077478
  35. 35. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12: 189–198. pmid:1202204
  36. 36. Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1983;17: 37–49.
  37. 37. Peto V, Jenkinson C, Fitzpatrick R, Greenhall R. The development and validation of a short measure of functioning and well being for individuals with Parkinson’s disease. Qual Life Res. 1995;4: 241–248. pmid:7613534
  38. 38. Jones JD, Hass C, Mangal P, Lafo J, Okun MS, Bowers D. The cognition and emotional well-being indices of the Parkinson’s disease questionnaire-39: What do they really measure? Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2014;20: 1236–1241. pmid:25260967
  39. 39. Tickle-Degnen L. The Interpersonal Communication Rating Protocol: A manual for measuring individual expressive behavior (ICRP-IEB). Parkinson’s disease version. [cited 2016 June 30]. http://ase.tufts.edu/hql/documents/researchICRP-IEB.pdf.
  40. 40. Lyons KD, Tickle-Degnen L, Henry A, Cohn ES. Behavioural cues of personality in Parkinson’s disease. Disabil Rehabil. 2004;26: 463–470.
  41. 41. Rosenthal R. Conducting judgment studies: Some methodological issues. In: Harrigan JA, Rosenthal R, Scherer KR, editors. The new handbook of methods in nonverbal behavior research. New York (NY): Oxford University Press; 2005. p. 199–234.
  42. 42. Portney LG, Watkins MP. Reliability of measurements. In: Portney LG, Watkins MP, editors. Foundations of clinical research: Applications to practice. Upper Saddle River (NJ): Pearson Education; 2009. p. 77–96.
  43. 43. Rosenthal R, Rosnow RL. Meta-analysis: Comparing and combining research results. In: Rosenthal R, Rosnow RL, editors. Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis. New York (NY): McGraw-Hill; 2008. p. 663–690.
  44. 44. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
  45. 45. Miller IN, Cronin-Golomb A. Gender differences in Parkinson’s disease: Clinical characteristics and cognition. Mov Disord. 2010;25: 2695–2703. pmid:20925068
  46. 46. Schwertman NC, Owens MA, Adnan R. A simple more general boxplot method for identifying outliers. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis. 2004;47: 165–174.
  47. 47. Wallbott HG. Bodily expression of emotion. Eur J Soc Psychol. 1998;28: 879–896.
  48. 48. Ekman P, Friesen WV, Ancoli S. Facial signs of emotional experience. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39: 1125–1134.
  49. 49. Ekman P, Friesen WV, Davidson RJ. The Duchenne smile: Emotional expression and brain physiology II. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1990;58: 342–353. pmid:2319446
  50. 50. Ekman P, Friesen W. Felt, false, and miserable smiles. J Nonverbal Behav. 1982;6: 238–252.
  51. 51. Poyatos F. The many voices of laughter: A new audible-visual paralinguistic approach. Semiotica. 1993;93: 61–81.
  52. 52. Giles H, Oxford GS. Towards a multidimensional theory of laughter causation and its social implications. Bull Br Psychol Soc. 1970;23: 97–105.
  53. 53. Szameitat DP, Alter K, Szameitat AJ, Darwin CJ, Wildgruber D, Dietrich S, et al. Differentiation of emotions in laughter at the behavioral level. Emotion. 2009;9: 397–405. pmid:19485617
  54. 54. Knapp ML, Hall JA. The effects of gesture and posture on human communication. In: Knapp ML, Hall JA, editors. Nonverbal communication in human interaction. Belmont (CA): Thomson Wadsworth; 2006. p. 225–263.
  55. 55. Scherer KR. Vocal affect expression: A review and a model for future research. Psychol Bull. 1986;99: 143–165. pmid:3515381
  56. 56. Coats EJ, Feldman RS. Gender differences in nonverbal correlates of social status. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1996;22: 1014–1022.
  57. 57. Smith MC, Smith MK, Ellgring H. Spontaneous and posed facial expression in Parkinson’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1996;2: 383–391. pmid:9375163
  58. 58. Bandini A, Orlandi S, Escalante HJ, Giovannelli F, Cincotta M, Reyes-Garcia CA, et al. Analysis of facial expressions in parkinson’s disease through video-based automatic methods. J Neurosci Methods. 2017;281: 7–20. pmid:28223023
  59. 59. Assogna F, Cravello L, Orfei MD, Cellupica N, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G. Alexithymia in Parkinson’s disease: A systematic review of the literature. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2016;28: 1–11. pmid:27086264
  60. 60. Assogna F, Palmer K, Pontieri FE, Pierantozzi M, Stefani A, Gianni W, et al. Alexithymia is a non-motor symptom of Parkinson disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;20: 133–141. pmid:22273734
  61. 61. Costa A, Caltagirone C. Alexithymia in Parkinson’s disease: A point of view on current evidence. Neurodegenerative Disease Management. 2016;6: 215–222. pmid:27230483
  62. 62. Lang F, Floyd MR, Beine KL. Clues to patients’ explanations and concerns about their illnesses: A call for active listening. Arch Fam Med. 2000;9: 222–227. pmid:10728107