Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Looking behind the score: Skill structure explains sex differences in skilled video game performance

  • Kyle W. Harwell ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    harwell@psy.fsu.edu

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, United States of America

  • Walter R. Boot,

    Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, United States of America

  • K. Anders Ericsson

    Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, United States of America

Abstract

Some have explained large sex differences in visuospatial abilities by genetic adaptations to different roles in primitive hunter-gatherer societies and the interaction of innate biological differences and environmental factors. We explored the extent to which variations in behavior and acquired skills can provide alternative accounts for sex differences in the performance of a complex spatially-demanding video game (Space Fortress). Men and women with limited video game experience were given 30 hours of training, and latent curve analyses examined the development of their ship control performance and behavior. Men had significantly better control performance than women before and after training, but differences diminished substantially over the training period. An analysis of participants’ joystick behaviors revealed that initially men and women relied on different patterns of control behaviors, but changes in these behaviors over time accounted for the reduced sex differences in performance. When we controlled for these differences in behavior, sex effects after training were no longer significant. Finally, examining the development of control performance and control behaviors of men and women categorized as initially high and low performers revealed the lower-performing women may have been controlling their ship using an approach that was very different from the men and higher-performing women. The potential problems of analyzing men and women’s spatial performance as homogenous groups are discussed, as well as how these issues may account for sex differences in skilled video game performance and perhaps other domains involving spatial abilities.

Introduction

Sex differences in spatial abilities: Anthropological and biological perspectives

The origin of sex differences in spatial ability has been a topic of much academic interest over the years, and a number of studies have reported men demonstrating superior performance compared to women on spatial tasks like mental rotation [1,2]. Men have also demonstrated advantages in tasks like spatial navigation, possibly due to employing more efficient, biologically predisposed strategies for wayfinding [3,4]. Some researchers have proposed an evolutionary explanation for this advantage, notably the hunter-gatherer hypothesis of spatial sex differences, which suggests superior visuospatial abilities played a crucial role in the tracking and killing of elusive and quick-moving prey and provided a selective advantage in male humans for many thousands of years [5,6]. According to this view there are innate, biological mechanisms developed during evolutionary history that explain why men outperform women on a variety of visuospatial tasks. Other lines of research have also expanded on the anthropological hypotheses by providing additional biological evidence for differences between men and women’s spatial cognition. Recent investigations have found that women who were exposed to higher than normal levels of prenatal androgens displayed better spatial ability performance compared to women who were not, suggesting a link between the development of spatial abilities and the presence of male sex hormones [7,8]. In addition to potential biological sex differences in spatial abilities, societal and environmental factors likely play an important role in explaining the male advantage in performance of spatially-demanding tasks. For example, the biopsychosocial model proposed by [9,10] describes a positive feedback cycle in which biological dispositions and differences in brain structure and organization bias how individuals select their environments, and environmental factors then reciprocally influence biological development. The interaction with environmental elements becomes especially important when considering sex differences in light of differences in the amount of engagement with activities believed to be associated with spatial skill development.

Sex differences in spatial abilities: Sociocultural and strategic perspectives

Contrasting with the innate sex differences view, other researchers have proposed that the magnitude of performance differences between men and women may depend on a number of environmental factors and the context in which the studies take place, citing a number of studies failing to find any significant differences [11]. One compelling argument posits that sex differences in performance of tasks like mental rotation and spatial navigation may be partly attributable to cultural biases and gender norms that encourage boys and girls to engage in stereotypically “appropriate” behavior defined by their culture, resulting in girls getting less opportunity to participate in activities such as sports or video game play that promote development of the relevant skills [1214]. It is also possible that women may not find these types of tasks as engaging as men do [14], or perhaps feel less confident in their ability to perform the task [15]. A further motivational factor that likely affects performance is the possibility that women may hold gender beliefs that men are typically better at spatially-demanding tasks, and this stereotype threat may negatively impact their performance and inflate measured sex differences [16,17]. A final consideration is that observed sex differences in visuospatial tasks may not reflect deficits in spatial abilities and rather reflect difficulties converting cognitive processes into motoric responses. Most studies measure spatial cognition by the speed and accuracy of button presses, and if women have less experience with tasks like video games that require many specifically-timed button presses, the response modality itself may bias measures of performance in favor of the men in the study [18]. The motoric component of sex differences in mental rotation has been studied by neuroimaging data, where findings show differential activation in regions of motor cortex that suggests sex differences in strategy [19].

The relations between sociocultural factors and performance on spatially-demanding tasks are necessarily correlational in nature, making it difficult to infer any causal mechanisms. To gain information about the mechanisms, researchers have started to investigate the behavior men and women engage in during the tasks themselves. If men and women were found to employ different strategies while performing tasks that measure spatial abilities, then at least some of the male performance advantage may be accounted for by women’s selection of different strategies and patterns of behavior. Studies have found that women were more likely than men to use a less efficient piecewise or analytic strategy rather than a holistic or rotational strategy on mental rotation tasks [2022], and that sex differences in performance can be significantly reduced through training and manipulations of the instructions or task [17,23]. Similar differences in selected strategies have been found in examinations of men and women’s navigation of virtual environments [24,25]. This pattern of results have led some researchers to suggest the differences in selected strategies may arise from women’s tendency to process spatial information locally, whereas men tend to process spatial features globally or holistically [22,26]; thus, differences in how men and women allocate spatial attention while performing tasks may bias their selection among available strategies.

While studies such as these have provided important insights into potential strategy differences between men and women, many are limited by their assessment of participants’ strategies. For example, strategy use has been mostly inferred indirectly through analyses of overall task performance measures like accuracy and reaction time, aggregated across entire groups [2023]. Other studies have relied on post-hoc measures of strategy like behavior on a critical trial in the task [24] or post-task self-reports of strategy use [25], and these two approaches are insensitive to changes in behavior participants may employ over the course of many trials. These strategy evaluations also fail to relate task performance to specific behaviors, making it difficult to determine what behavioral differences distinguish high and low performance within and across groups using different strategies. Research on men and women’s developmental trajectories, where strategy use may change over time as task-specific knowledge and skills are gained, requires a different approach. Such an assessment of strategies should analyze participants’ detailed behavior on individual trials and explicate how differences in behavior influences task performance. In sum, a description of men and women’s behavior while performing spatial tasks will be critical for understanding the source of observed sex differences and inform the design of more effective training procedures.

Training spatial abilities with video games

Video games have become an increasingly important paradigm for studying complex skills, including those involving visuospatial abilities, and they offer a rich context within which to study sex differences. One area of interest concerns identifying the source of large sex differences seen across various game genres, where men demonstrate an advantage in performance for action video games like first-person shooters [27]. This has led some researchers to propose a causal relationship between lower spatial abilities and lower performance of women in spatially-demanding tasks like video games [25,2831]. Action video games, which require players to monitor multiple features and/or track multiple targets while making time-sensitive decisions in highly dynamic environments, have been proposed as a domain well suited for studying development of generalizable spatial abilities in both non-gamers, generally [32,33], and women, specifically [30,34].

Many studies examining differences in visuospatial ability and the effects of training to reduce them [35], including those involving games, make the unverified assumption that men and women are performing the tasks using the same or similar sequences of cognitive processes. Thus, men are assumed to outperform women due to their greater capacities for spatial abilities. However, it is also possible men and women approach tasks using fundamentally different patterns of behavior. According to the expert-performance framework [36,37], there is evidence skilled performers employ qualitatively different approaches compared to unskilled performers, due to qualitatively different representations of the demands of the tasks. The lower performance seen among women may be in part attributable to less efficient patterns of behavior, which can only be identified by recording and analyzing detailed sequences of behaviors executed during performance of the tasks. Unfortunately, many studies analyze only summary scores of overall performance or focus on simple behavioral measures like reaction time which can obscure meaningful differences in participant behavior. Further, most video game studies rely on data summarized across entire gaming sessions, likely because they utilize commercial video games designed primarily for entertainment which make it difficult or impossible to extract detailed behavioral data [38]. Without the analysis of detailed sequences of behavior, the strategies mediating skilled performance may go unnoticed [39], and the skill mechanisms underlying sex differences will remain elusive.

The present study

One skill that may be especially important to consider when evaluating player performance is the ability to manipulate the player’s avatar within the game. Given the complex demands action video games impose on players, players would not be expected to achieve a high overall score if they lack the prerequisite skills necessary to maneuver around the game space. Thus, measurements of how well players are controlling their avatars and what behaviors discriminate good versus bad control would likely account for individual differences in development of skilled action video game performance in these types of games. Additionally, examination of control-relevant behaviors could provide important details regarding the intermediate stages that participants progress through as they develop or alter their control strategies as a result of training.

The current investigation was motivated by the possibility of extending beyond the existing sex differences literature by examining how online measures of participant behavior may provide insight into the relations between sex and differences in assessed strategies and task performance. We analyzed sex differences in development of control performance in an action video game (Space Fortress; [40]) and how participants’ control behaviors changed over the course of training. We also evaluated whether the sex effects before and after training persisted after statistically controlling for players’ control behaviors. Finally, we compared development of control performance and control behaviors among participants within each sex for individuals identified as low and high performers based on their performance before training.

Methods

Participants

We analyzed an archival dataset collected by a research group at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign [41] to investigate the effect of two training regimens on development of Space Fortress performance. For the present investigation, we excluded the 25 participants in the no-training control group that lacked comparable data on training to model growth. Given that the two training conditions did not differ in their overall performance (F(1, 46) = 3.78, p = .058, η2 = .076) or their Control score performance (F(1, 46) = 1.23, p > .1, η2 = .026), participants in both groups were pooled into a single sample for the current analyses. We studied the performance of 50 young adults (aged 18–30, 31 women). All participants were paid for completing approximately 60 hours of testing and training across multiple sessions. Participants had been pre-screened to ensure that none had extensive video game playing experience (defined as more than 4 hours of gameplay per week). All reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision, normal color vision, and right-handedness. The Internal Review Board of the University of Illinois approved original data collection, and all participants provided written informed consent according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. The present investigation involved no intervention or interaction with participants, and no personally identifiable or private data were used in any analyses; as such, the Florida State University Human Subjects Committee did not consider this research to require additional review.

The Space Fortress video game

Space Fortress (SF) is a video game originally designed by cognitive psychologists that combines complex motor movements, working memory for targets, multi-tasking demands, simultaneous resource monitoring, and considerable spatial attention requirements [40,41]. The player’s primary objective is to maximize their total score by firing missiles to destroy the enemy fortress (located in the center of the game environment) as many times as possible over the course of a 3-minute game. A player’s total score is the sum of their performance across several subdomains of gameplay, each with its own accompanying subscore.

The Control score is based on how well players maneuver their ship within the two-dimensional, frictionless game environment. Using the joystick they are able to rotate their ship clockwise or counterclockwise and accelerate the ship forward along its current angle of orientation by pushing the joystick forward (executing a thrust). The game samples the player’ ship position twenty times every second, and every twenty cycles it checks whether the ship is within the hexagonal boundary and updates the Control score accordingly. Players gain 7 points every time they are within the hexagonal boundary when the game updates their Control score, and they lose 7 points if they are outside the boundary. Players are also penalized for colliding with the fortress (minus 5 points), and flying beyond the edges of the game space (minus 35 points). All players start each game with a Control score of 0 and can achieve a maximum attainable Control score of 1260 (earning 7 points for all 180 Control score updates). See Fig 1 for an illustration of the SF game environment. We focus on Control score for two reasons: 1) it is the most spatial aspect of the game, making it of primary interest for studying sex differences, and 2) ship control is critical for performing all other tasks associated with SF that influence Total score. Without adequate control of the ship, destroying the fortress or efficiently dealing with mines becomes virtually impossible.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Space Fortress game environment.

Large hexagon indicates boundary for maximal Control score zone. Small hexagon indicates boundary for a fortress collision.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197311.g001

In a number of ways this study provides important extensions of previous studies of video game training, generally, and SF training, specifically (e.g., [41,42]). First, SF affords a few notable advantages over the commercial games used in other studies. SF outputs a record of game events from which we can extract player behavior on a per-game basis, in addition to calculating performance summary scores to track player development over time. Additionally, the control interface in SF is very different from modern video games and reduces the chances that participants would have significant transferrable experience using similar interfaces prior to the beginning of training. A further motivation for this study was previous SF analyses have not considered sex as a factor of primary interest, instead including it as a covariate and not reporting potentially informative sex differences in SF performance and development.

Procedure

Participants completed 15 sessions of training with SF, each lasting about 2 hours (for full details regarding the specific training methodology, see [41]). During each session, participants played a total of thirty-six 3-minute games with the first three and last three games of each session being considered “test” trials and the remaining thirty trials being considered “training” trials. Data from the SF video game were collected by computers connected to a common network, with players executing game inputs using a computer mouse and a Logitech Attack 3 Joystick®. The game was presented on color 19” LCD computer monitors. For the present investigation, all analyses focus on Control scores and control behaviors averaged across the final three 3-minute test trials of each training session. Data for a single session for 3 participants were excluded due to the absence of any recorded player behavior, leaving 747 total observations for each measure.

Statistical analyses

Analysis 1: Effect of sex on spatial control development

To gain insight into the rate of change in Control score, latent curve analysis was used to examine the development of that score in the Space Fortress video game across 15 training sessions. All models of the data were fitted with logarithmic growth curves, with a model including only the effects of training session as the base model, and then the fixed effects of sex (women as reference group) on the intercept and linear time terms were added after. Intercepts and slopes were allowed to vary freely across participants for each model, and model fit comparisons were assessed using -2 times the change in log-likelihood. All latent curve modelling in the present investigation was performed using the lme4 package [43] in the R programming language for statistical computing [44].

Analysis 2: Effect of sex on development of detailed control behaviors

We hypothesized the reduction in sex differences in control performance might reflect a modification of how men and women approach ship control over time and result in changes in execution of detailed control behaviors across training. Comparisons between latent curve models were used to test the effects of sex on the developmental trajectory of the three ship control behaviors (i.e., thrusts, clockwise rotations, and counterclockwise rotations) across the 15 training sessions.

Analysis 3: Sex differences after training are accounted for by differences in control behaviors

We hypothesized the three types of detailed control behavior would account for all reliable variance in Control score. Two hierarchical regression analyses were conducted, the first predicting Control score for the first training session (where sex differences were largest), and the second focusing on Control score for the final training session (where sex differences were smallest). For both models, ship thrusts, clockwise rotations, and counterclockwise rotations were entered at the first level, and then sex was entered at the second level.

Analysis 4: Effect of sex differs for high and low performers

Our final question examined whether sex differences in control skill development are influenced by different patterns of control behaviors executed by men and women. Specifically, we hypothesized the large sex differences seen prior to training were attributable to some women employing less efficient patterns of control behavior, and the reduction in sex differences after training was related to their adoption of behavior more consistent with the men’s behavior. Towne, Boot, and Ericsson [45] previously described participants employing vastly different patterns of behavior for controlling the SF ship (strategies) with relationships to differential game performance, and Destefano and Gray [46] pointed to skilled players’ ability to develop highly-specialized strategies to exploit particular game mechanics to improve their scores. If lower-performing players are employing qualitatively different patterns of behavior, then analyses looking at performance and behaviors aggregated across high and low performers may not be appropriate for explaining the sex effects.

Men and women were categorized as high or low initial performers based on their performance for Session 1 by a median split of their Control scores for each sex. Latent curve analyses were conducted on the development of Control score and the three ship control behaviors following the same procedures as above, this time adding an additional model that included the effect of initial performance (low performers as reference group), its interactions with sex and training session, and the three-way initial performance by sex and training session interaction.

Results

Analysis 1: Effect of sex on spatial control development

The effect of sex significantly improved model fit (Δχ2(2) = 17.16, p < .001), and examination of parameter estimates of the final model revealed a significant effect of session (b = 3.43, t(49.94) = 11.03, p < .001), indicating a significant improvement in Control score across training sessions. There was also a significant effect of sex on the intercept (b = 868.17, t(49.96) = 4.52, p < .001), indicating that men started training with significantly higher Control performance (Session 1 Cohen’s dsex = -1.50). In addition, the interaction between sex and session was significant for the slope (b = -586.94, t(49.92) = -4.14, p < .001), suggesting women improved significantly faster than men and the magnitude of the sex difference significantly decreased over time. Rerunning the models with the intercept set at the final training session revealed the effect of sex was still significant but substantially smaller (b = 177.88, t(49.92) = 2.49, p = .016, dsex = -0.95). See Fig 2 for a graphical depiction of Control score over time.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Control scores by sex across training session.

Error bars represent standard error of the mean. Dotted line represents maximum achievable Control score.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197311.g002

Analysis 2: Effect of sex on development of detailed control behaviors

The models including the effects of sex displayed significant improvement of fit beyond the base models for clockwise rotations, Δχ2(2) = 7.16, p = .028, and counterclockwise rotations, Δχ2(2) = 15.81, p < .001. Sex did not significantly improve fit beyond the base model for ship thrusts, but the difference approached significance, Δχ2(2) = 4.94, p = .085. There was a significant sex by training session interaction for the developmental trajectory of both types of ship rotations (clockwise rotations, b = 173.74, t(49.90) = 2.76, p = .008; counterclockwise rotations, b = 180.14, t(49.98) = 2.39, p = .021). Women began their training executing significantly more rotations per game than men (clockwise rotations, b = -178.24, t(49.99) = -2.07, p = .044, dsex = 0.62; counterclockwise rotations, b = -296.62, t(49.99) = -3.31, p = .002, dsex = 0.90), but as training progressed they reduced their numbers of ship thrusts and rotations significantly more than men did. When re-analyzed using the intercepts set at the final training session there were no significant differences in behaviors executed by men and women at the end of training (clockwise rotations, b = 26.09, t(49.90) = 0.41, p > .250, dsex = -0.08; counterclockwise rotations, b = -1.40, t(49.80) = -0.11, p > .250, dsex = 0.36; see Fig 3 for graphs of thrusts, clockwise rotations, and counterclockwise rotations across training session). These analyses show that sex differences in Control performance can be associated with differences between men and women’s Control behaviors.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Player ship control behaviors by sex across training session.

Error bars represent standard error of the mean.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197311.g003

Analysis 3: Sex differences after training are accounted for by differences in control behaviors

For the analysis of the first session (Session 1), the final model explained a significant portion of the variance in Control scores (R2 = .62, F(4, 45) = 18.56, p < .001), and sex contributed significantly above and beyond the Control behaviors, ΔR2 = .06, F(1,45) = 7.69, p = .008. For this session the parameter estimates for thrusts and counterclockwise rotations significantly predicted Control scores (thrusts: b = 0.64, t(45) = 3.04, p = .004; counterclockwise rotations: b = -2.36, t(-5.59), p < .001). At the final session (Session 15), the final model also explained a significant portion of the variance in Control scores (R2 = .67, F(4, 45) = 22.36, p < .001). However, sex did not explain variance in Control score above and beyond the three behaviors after training (ΔR2 = < .001, F(1,45) = 0.03, p > .250). Parameter estimates revealed all three Control behaviors were predictive of performance (thrusts: b = -1.23, t(45) = -7.56, p < .001; clockwise rotations: b = 1.61, t(45) = 6.42, p < .001; counterclockwise rotations: b = 1.23, t(45) = 5.45, p < .001).

Analysis 4: Effect of sex differs for high and low performers

For all outcome measures, the addition of the models with initial performance significantly improved fit compared to the models that only included the effects of sex (Control: Δχ2(4) = 47.76, p < .001; thrusts: Δχ2(4) = 12.02, p = .017; clockwise rotations: Δχ2(4) = 9.85, p = .043; counterclockwise rotations: Δχ2(4) = 26.68, p < .001).

Examination of parameter estimates (see Table 1) of the final models revealed a significant effect of initial performance, as well as sex by initial performance and training session by initial performance interactions for Control score and both ship rotations. Additionally, the three-way interaction was also significant for Control score. Taken together, these findings suggest the large sex effects on control performance observed prior to training were different for initially high and low performing men and women, and these differences were also associated with similar differences in ship control behaviors. Moreover, the previously described sex differences in development of Control score across training was dependent upon whether players were high or low performers at the beginning of training (see Fig 4). Initially low performing women executed more of all control behaviors than high performing women and both high and low performing men prior to training, and they steadily reduced their number of actions over time (see Fig 5). This suggested some women were employing significantly different ship control behavior early on and modifying their gameplay over time. Indeed, visual inspection of the flight paths of players seemed to suggest low-performing women with high numbers of thrusts had a less systematic pattern of flight compared to more skilled women, but they became more consistent across training (see Fig 6 for example flight paths).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Control scores by sex and initial control performance across training session.

Error bars represent standard error of the mean. Dotted line represents maximum achievable Control score.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197311.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Player ship control behaviors by sex and initial control performance across training session.

Error bars represent standard error of the mean.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197311.g005

thumbnail
Fig 6. Example flightpaths of high and low-performing women across training.

Top Row, from Left: Flightpaths for final games of Session 1, 8, and 15 for the highest-performing female participant. Bottom Row, from Left. Flightpaths for final games of Sessions 1, 8, and 15 for the lowest-performing female participant. Dotted lines indicate ship position over time, and borders indicate edges of the game space. Note increased spacing between points for the low-performing player, indicating greater ship velocity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197311.g006

Discussion

Traditional theories have primarily attributed sex differences in domains requiring extensive visuospatial cognition to innate biological differences between men and women; however, the present investigation found that sex differences in control performance in a video game were associated with modifiable differences in control behaviors. Consistent with previous studies, our investigation found significant sex differences in performance in an action-video game, both prior to and following extensive training. Men displayed an advantage compared to women on Control score, but women made significantly larger gains over 30 hours of training and closed the performance gap substantially. This finding was extended by our discovery that men and women also displayed differential patterns of control behaviors across training. Additionally, the behavior of initially lower-performing women differed not only from men, but also from initially higher-performing women.

Our finding that some women were controlling their ships differently from other women raises issues for the assumption that the mechanisms underlying sex differences in action-video game performance can be accounted for by sex differences in innate spatial abilities. An argument in favor of innate sex differences would have to address differences in behavior between players of the same sex with different levels of performance. Looking at the beginning of training, where sex differences were largest, we found sex differences in behavior were dependent upon whether a player was a high or low performer within his or her sex. Initially low-performing women performed notably worse than other players, and they also executed notably more behaviors per game than other players, suggesting their behaviors may have led to the significant sex differences and obscuring the fact some women were performing and behaving similarly to the men.

One possible explanation for the large sex differences could be that some women began playing SF with less efficient control behavior. For example, Towne and colleagues [45] described a strategy employed by skilled SF players in which they maneuvered their ship around the fortress in a slow clockwise orbit, maintaining a close proximity without actually crashing into the fortress. Executing this sort of plan or control strategy would require players to make very few counterclockwise rotations, as they would have to constantly rotate their ship clockwise and thrust in order to counteract the effects of centrifugal force in the frictionless game environment. The behavioral analyses revealed initially lower-performing women had a hyperactive control approach, executing more control behaviors than other players. This could be reflective of qualitatively different plans or control strategies, or perhaps more accurately the lack of a consistent plan or control strategy. However, the study providing the current data did not assess participants’ plans and strategies by collecting verbal reports on goals, so it is not possible to differentiate plans and specific strategy types among participants in the sample. Future experiments will be critical in testing the extent to which strategy selection contributes to sex differences in performance for spatially-demanding tasks like SF.

A possible alternative hypothesis is that all the participants were indeed attempting to execute a similar plan and control strategy, but individual differences in some participants’ spatial abilities rendered them unable to execute it effectively. We argue that the remarkably high number of behaviors, especially thrusts, executed by some lower-performing women makes such an argument less plausible. Some participants were executing over 1,000 ship thrusts per game, which equates to over 5 thrusts every second, on average; this is only possible if a player is continuously holding down the joystick in the forward position to keep increasing speed. In the frictionless environment of the game, this constant acceleration would be a very maladaptive gameplay style and make it difficult to gain control of the ship. An inspection of Fig 6 supports this by demonstrating that the flight path of the lower-performing woman lacks a definite path and shows essentially unpredictable flight patterns. Furthermore, this control style is most likely volitional because the joystick participants used to control the ship was spring-mounted and returned to a central neutral position if they exerted no force on it. Whereas individual differences in spatial ability may contribute to performance differences among participants using the traditional discrete ship thrusting technique, they would not account for individuals who accelerated their ship constantly. Negative transfer would seem to be a likely potential factor, with participants erroneously viewing constant acceleration as necessary for continued forward momentum, as with driving a car. Though we can only speculate on the origin of such behavior differences at this point, there appears to be some deficient understanding of the control mechanics involved, driven by qualitatively different representations of the task demands of the game. It is possible that although all the women in our sample had limited exposure to video games, perhaps some had more experience with other spatially-demanding tasks and developed spatial skills that predisposed them to selecting a control style more similar to the men, per the biopsychosocial model described by [10]. Future studies collecting detailed behavioral data and self-reported strategy information or concurrent verbal reports during gameplay could provide greater insight into the cognitive processes underlying such differences in behavior throughout training. To this end, we support the assertions of [47] and [48] that more studies examining performance involving dynamic spatial reasoning in virtual environments should look at participants’ behaviors more thoroughly rather than relying solely on performance summary information. This type of detailed behavioral data could provide better understanding of differences in the use of plans and strategies of differing effectiveness and perhaps shed some light on what specific knowledge and skills are relevant for development of superior performance in a variety of domains involving remote operation or navigation in virtual environments.

The present study has demonstrated that traditional generalizations across levels of overall performance may not fully capture the complex nature of sex differences in spatial skills and their relationship to performance in action video games. These data suggest some women were executing patterns of control behaviors that were qualitatively different from the men and other higher-performing women, casting doubt on the premise that innate differences in spatial ability primarily accounted for sex differences in control performance. Additionally, the findings show that female participants significantly changed their control behavior over time, demonstrating both the mutability of strategy with experience and the informative value of investigating participant performance at the level of frequency of individual behaviors. Much of the established literature linking spatial abilities to sex differences in domains ranging from STEM education [49] to video game play relies on accuracy or reaction time measures that provide limited insights into what men and women are actually doing and thinking while performing the tasks. Future studies of sex differences in the development of spatially-demanding complex skills should look behind summary scores and also examine differences in men and women’s behavior in order to better understand the source of these sex differences, as well as what types of training can help overcome them.

Supporting information

S1 File. Study data.

Comma delimited plain text file containing data used in the present investigation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197311.s001

(CSV)

S2 File. Study data variable coding.

Plain text file describing study data variable coding.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197311.s002

(TXT)

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Dr. Hyunkyu Lee for her assistance with obtaining files related to player behavior and Dr. Leonard Hill for his comments on the manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Levine SC, Foley A, Lourenco S, Ehrlich S, Ratliff K. Sex differences in spatial cognition: advancing the conversation: Sex differences in spatial cognition. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2016;7: 127–155. pmid:26825049
  2. 2. Maeda Y, Yoon SY. A Meta-Analysis on Gender Differences in Mental Rotation Ability Measured by the Purdue Spatial Visualization Tests: Visualization of Rotations (PSVT:R). Educ Psychol Rev. 2013;25: 69–94.
  3. 3. Saucier DM, Green SM, Leason J, MacFadden A, Bell S, Elias LJ. Are sex differences in navigation caused by sexually dimorphic strategies or by differences in the ability to use the strategies? Behav Neurosci. 2002;116: 403–410. pmid:12049321
  4. 4. Sandstrom NJ, Kaufman J, Huettel SA. Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task. Cogn Brain Res. 1998;6: 351–360.
  5. 5. Silverman I, Choi J, Peters M. The Hunter-Gatherer Theory of Sex Differences in Spatial Abilities: Data from 40 Countries. Arch Sex Behav. 2007;36: 261–268. pmid:17351740
  6. 6. Silverman I, Eals M. Sex Differences in Spatial Abilities: Evolutionary Theory and Data. In: Barkow JH, Cosmides L, Tooby J, editors. The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York: Oxford Press; 1992. pp. 531–549.
  7. 7. Heil M, Kavšek M, Rolke B, Beste C, Jansen P. Mental rotation in female fraternal twins: Evidence for intra-uterine hormone transfer? Biol Psychol. 2011;86: 90–93. pmid:21094200
  8. 8. Puts DA, McDaniel MA, Jordan CL, Breedlove SM. Spatial Ability and Prenatal Androgens: Meta-Analyses of Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia and Digit Ratio (2D:4D) Studies. Arch Sex Behav. 2008;37: 100–111. pmid:18074217
  9. 9. Halpern DF. Sex Differences in Cognitve Abilties. 4th ed. Psychology Press; 2012.
  10. 10. Miller DI, Halpern DF. The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18: 37–45. pmid:24246136
  11. 11. Hyde JS. The Gender Similarities Hypothesis. Am Psychol. 2005;60: 581–592. pmid:16173891
  12. 12. Hyde JS. Sex and cognition: gender and cognitive functions. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2016;38: 53–56. pmid:26953847
  13. 13. Cherney ID, Neff NL. Role of strategies and prior exposure in mental rotation. Percept Mot Skills. 2004;98: 1269–1282. pmid:15291215
  14. 14. Lucas K, Sherry JL. Sex Differences in Video Game Play: A Communication-Based Explanation. Commun Res. 2004;31: 499–523.
  15. 15. Estes Z, Felker S. Confidence Mediates the Sex Difference in Mental Rotation Performance. Arch Sex Behav. 2011;41: 557–70. pmid:22130691
  16. 16. Moe A, Pazzaglia F. Following the Instructions! Effects of Gender Beliefs in Mental Rotation. Learn Individ Differ. 2006;16: 369–377.
  17. 17. Moè A. Teaching motivation and strategies to improve mental rotation abilities. Intelligence. 2016;59: 16–23.
  18. 18. Terlecki MS, Newcombe NS. How Important Is the Digital Divide? The Relation of Computer and Videogame Usage to Gender Differences in Mental Rotation Ability. Sex Roles. 2005;53: 433–441.
  19. 19. Jordan K, Wüstenberg T, Heinze H-J, Peters M, Jäncke L. Women and men exhibit different cortical activation patterns during mental rotation tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40: 2397–2408. pmid:12417468
  20. 20. Geiser C, Lehmann W, Eid M. Separating “Rotators” From “Nonrotators” in the Mental Rotations Test: A Multigroup Latent Class Analysis. Multivar Behav Res. 2006;41: 261–293. http://dx.doi.org.proxy.lib.fsu.edu/10.1207/s15327906mbr4103_2
  21. 21. Geiser C, Lehmann W, Corth M, Eid M. Quantitative and qualitative change in children’s mental rotation performance. Learn Individ Differ. 2008;18: 419–429. http://dx.doi.org.proxy.lib.fsu.edu/10.1016/j.lindif.2007.09.001
  22. 22. Heil M, Jansen-Osmann P. Sex differences in mental rotation with polygons of different complexity: Do men utilize holistic processes whereas women prefer piecemeal ones? Q J Exp Psychol. 2008;61: 683–689. http://dx.doi.org.proxy.lib.fsu.edu/10.1080/17470210701822967
  23. 23. Boone AP, Hegarty M. Sex differences in mental rotation tasks: Not just in the mental rotation process! J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2017;43: 1005–1019. pmid:28125253
  24. 24. Astur RS, Purton AJ, Zaniewski MJ, Cimadevilla J, Markus EJ. Human sex differences in solving a virtual navigation problem. Behav Brain Res. 2016;308: 236–243. pmid:27108050
  25. 25. Nowak NT, Murali A, Driscoll I. Factors related to sex differences in navigating a computerized maze. J Environ Psychol. 2015;43: 136–144.
  26. 26. Pletzer B. Sex-specific strategy use and global-local processing: a perspective toward integrating sex differences in cognition. Front Neurosci. 2014;8. pmid:25565953
  27. 27. Hopp T, Fisher J. Examination of the relationship between gender, performance, and enjoyment of a first-person shooter game. Simul Gaming. 2017;48: 338–362.
  28. 28. Okagaki L, Frensch PA, others. Effects of video game playing on measures of spatial performance: Gender effects in late adolescence. Interact Video. 1996;11: 115–140.
  29. 29. Spence I, Feng J. Video games and spatial cognition. Rev Gen Psychol. 2010;14: 92–104.
  30. 30. Spence I, Yu JJ, Feng J, Marshman J. Women match men when learning a spatial skill. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2009;35: 1097–1103. pmid:19586273
  31. 31. Terlecki MS, Newcombe NS, Little M. Durable and generalized effects of spatial experience on mental rotation: gender differences in growth patterns. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2008;22: 996–1013.
  32. 32. Green CS, Bavelier D. Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature. 2003;423: 534–537. pmid:12774121
  33. 33. Green CS, Bavelier D. Action video game training for cognitive enhancement. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015;4: 103–108.
  34. 34. Feng Jing, Spence I, Pratt J. Playing an Action Video Game Reduces Gender Differences in Spatial Cognition. Psychol Sci 0956–7976. 2007;18: 850–855. pmid:17894600
  35. 35. Uttal DH, Meadow NG, Tipton E, Hand LL, Alden AR, Warren C, et al. The malleability of spatial skills: A meta-analysis of training studies. Psychol Bull. 2013;139: 352–402. pmid:22663761
  36. 36. Ericsson KA. The influence of experience and deliberate practice on the development of superior expert performance. In: Charness N, Feltovich P, Hoffman R R, editors. The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2006. pp. 685–705. Available: https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/f202/ff185048777e0544affac38bb324e92d4fce.pdf
  37. 37. Ericsson KA. Why expert performance is special and cannot be extrapolated from studies of performance in the general population: A response to criticisms. Intelligence. 2014;45: 81–103.
  38. 38. Lindstedt JK, Gray WD. Meta-T: TetrisⓇ as an experimental paradigm for cognitive skills research. Behav Res Methods. 2015;47: 945–965. pmid:25761389
  39. 39. Gray WD. Plateaus and Asymptotes: Spurious and Real Limits in Human Performance. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2017;26: 59–67.
  40. 40. Donchin E. The learning strategies project: Introductory remarks. Acta Psychol (Amst). 71: 1–15.
  41. 41. Lee H, Boot WR, Basak C, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Neider M, et al. Performance gains from directed training do not transfer to untrained tasks. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2012;139: 146–158. pmid:22133724
  42. 42. Lee H, Boot WR, Baniqued PL, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, et al. The Relationship between Intelligence and Training Gains Is Moderated by Training Strategy. Nusbaum H, editor. PLOS ONE. 2015;10: e0123259. pmid:25860978
  43. 43. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67: 1–48.
  44. 44. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Internet]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria; 2007. Available: URL https://www.R-project.org/
  45. 45. Towne TJ, Boot WR, Ericsson KA. Understanding the structure of skill through a detailed analysis of Individuals’ performance on the Space Fortress game. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2016;169: 27–37. pmid:27214790
  46. 46. Destefano M, Gray WD. Where should researchers look for strategy discoveries during the acquisition of complex task performance? The case of space fortress. Proceedings of the 38th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. 2016. pp. 668–673. Available: http://homepages.rpi.edu/~grayw/pubs/papers/2016/marc16csc.pdf
  47. 47. Gray WD. Game-XP: Action Games as Experimental Paradigms for Cognitive Science. Top Cogn Sci. 2017;9: 289–307. pmid:28296290
  48. 48. Boot WR, Sumner A, Towne TJ, Rodriguez P, Anders Ericsson K. Applying aspects of the expert performance approach to better understand the structure of skill and mechanisms of skill acquisition in video games. Top Cogn Sci. 2017;9: 413–436. pmid:27868367
  49. 49. Wai J, Lubinski D, Benbow CP. Spatial ability for STEM domains: Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. J Educ Psychol. 2009;101: 817–835.