Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Bold personality makes domestic dogs entering a shelter less vulnerable to diseases

  • Sara Corsetti,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Biology and Biotechnology Department, University of Rome Sapienza, Rome, Italy

  • Simona Borruso,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation

    Affiliation Biology and Biotechnology Department, University of Rome Sapienza, Rome, Italy

  • Mario Di Traglia,

    Roles Formal analysis, Validation

    Affiliation Public Health and Infectious Diseases, University of Rome Sapienza, Rome, Italy

  • Olga Lai,

    Roles Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Experimental Zoo-prophylactic Institute Latium and Tuscany, Rome, Italy

  • Lavinia Alfieri,

    Roles Formal analysis

    Affiliation Experimental Zoo-prophylactic Institute Latium and Tuscany, Rome, Italy

  • Agnese Villavecchia,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliation Canine consultant, free-lance, Rome, Italy

  • Giuseppe Cariola,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliation Interzonal Dog Shelter, Local Health Unit Rome 3, Rome, Italy

  • Alessandra Spaziani,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliation Interzonal Dog Shelter, Local Health Unit Rome 3, Rome, Italy

  • Eugenia Natoli

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    enatoli@tiscali.it

    Affiliation Interzonal Dog Shelter, Local Health Unit Rome 3, Rome, Italy

Correction

29 Aug 2018: The PLOS ONE Staff (2018) Correction: Bold personality makes domestic dogs entering a shelter less vulnerable to diseases. PLOS ONE 13(8): e0203399. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203399 View correction

Abstract

It is widely recognised that for vertebrate species, personalities vary along an axis with extremes represented by ‘proactive’ and ‘reactive‘ individuals. The aim of this study was to verify whether there is a relationship between personality and disease vulnerability in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) exposed to an intensely stressful situation such as entering a shelter. Twenty-eight shelter dogs participated in the study. The ethogram consisted of approximately 100 behavioural patterns. Behavioural observations of dogs in their new environment, a Novel Object and a T-maze test were used to evaluate the personality of the dogs captured as strays and entering the shelter. A blood sample from each dog was obtained at admission into the shelter and after a month to evaluate their immunological state. Based on PCA analyses of observational combined with experimental data, the dogs were ordered along the boldness-shyness axis, with the first being the boldest. Excluding one (the 6th), the first 10 dogs showed an improved health status: absence of disease symptoms during the 30 days of monitoring and improved immunological parameters; the opposite was found for shy dogs. The results of this research seem to confirm findings in other vertebrate species, i.e., bold and shy dog vulnerability to diseases might be different, especially when they must cope with a stressful and highly infectious environment such as a dog shelter.

Introduction

For vertebrate species, it is widely recognised that personalities vary in the way they cope with stress: along an axis, the extremes of which are represented by ‘proactive’ and ‘reactive’ responses [1] individuals are defined as ‘bold’ and ‘shy‘, respectively [2]. Proactive individuals are generally bold, tend to explore quickly and try to manipulate the situation even in a new context. Thus, on the one hand, they cannot be defined as “neophobic” [1]. Consequently, they are more prone to disperse. For this reason, the Novel Object test has been used in animal personality testing and, more specifically, as a way to measure the ‘proactive’ and ‘reactive’ axis, in many species (e.g., barnacle goose (Branta leucopsis), [3]; warbler (spp.) [4]; working dogs, [5]; pigs (Sus scrofa) [67]; and the rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss), [89]). On the other hand, bold individuals have been found to be less prone to make changes in established procedures or to alter their routine [1, 1011].

Conversely, reactive individuals are generally shy, tend to be more cautious and are more sensitive to external stimuli. They are slower in taking decisions and they tend and try to adapt themselves to the situation more than to manipulate it. Although this tendency makes them less exposed to the risks of being injured or killed or infected by a virus (in all the cases in which the transmission necessitates a contact between saliva and blood, that can occur during fights, see for example [12]), empirical data demonstrate that shy individuals show higher hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis reactivity in response to stress than bold individuals (e.g., [1, 1317]), resulting in an increase in circulating glucocorticoids [18]. If the stressor persists for some time, shy individuals become more vulnerable than bold ones to stress-related disorders.

It has been extensively demonstrated that, for domestic dogs (Canis familiaris), entering in a shelter represents a stressful event [1927]. Spatial and social restriction, exposure to novel environments and eventual separation from an attachment figure are some of the stressful events faced by a dog in this situation [28].

Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine whether there is a relationship between personality and disease vulnerability in domestic dogs entering a shelter. In particular, we hypothesised that boldness (as a composition of active, attentive, confident and social traits vs anxious and subordinate traits) [1, 2932] might be the most important personality dimension relevant to coping with stressful shelter conditions and that animals with a higher boldness rating would be more likely to possess the skills to cope with this challenging situation. We predicted, therefore, that compared with shy animals, bold animals would display different levels of specific physiological parameters linked to their immunological profile and a reduced risk of contracting the most common diseases.

Confirmation of such a hypothesis could help dog shelter management improve the welfare of dogs in the future.

To assess the personalities of dogs entering a shelter, we used both observational measures of their spontaneous behaviour in their novel environment and two experimental paradigms: the novel object and the T-maze test. Indeed, studies have shown that the best characterisation of the temperament of dogs is obtained from an assessment of their behaviour in multiple contexts [3334].

Materials and methods

Animals and housing

The subjects of this study were 28 domestic dogs (20 males and 8 females) aged between 1 and 7 years (estimated by veterinary standard methods, i.e., evaluation of teeth consumption, status of fur, among others) that were captured as strays, of which 20 were mixed-breed and 8 were clearly purebred-derived dogs (German shepherd, Maremma sheep dog, Pitbull, Rottweiler, Cavalier King and Belgian shepherd). Three age classes were identified: 1st from 1 year to 2 years and 11 months (15 dogs); 2nd from 3 to 5 years (9 dogs); and 3rd from 6 to 7 years (4 dogs). The dogs had to be in good health condition and negative for Leishmania infantum and Ehrlichia canis upon entry into the dog shelter. Another 24 dogs were excluded because they were unhealthy at entry (7 dogs), too aggressive (3 dogs), pregnant (1 dog), rescued by owners (8 dogs), able to climb the fence of the fenced area where the tests were conducted (1 dog) or adopted during the observational period (4 dogs).

The study was carried out at the Municipal Interzonal Dog Shelter "La Muratella" in Rome. The dogs were housed alone or in pairs in cages (4 m2) with indoor and outdoor zones. They were fed early in the morning and late in the afternoon; the standard food provided in the shelter is both dry and wet dog food of high quality, and each dog receives an adequate quantity for its size and age. The cages were cleaned twice a day, just before food distribution. Each dog was taken out of its cage daily by the staff and/or volunteers for a walk in a fenced area inside the shelter.

Dog health monitoring

The protocol provided in the dog shelter "La Muratella", where the study was carried out, required that a dog, upon entering the shelter, was subjected to a clinical examination before housing. Thus, each dog captured as a stray entering the shelter was subjected to a clinical examination during which, among other routine checks (i.e., temperature control, visual physical examination of the ears and mouth), the animal was checked and treated for ecto- and endo-parasites. If judged in good health, it was selected for this study. In Italy, it is enforced by law a collection of blood sample (between 6 to 8 cc, from the brachial vein) of the dog entering a shelter before housing it, in order to check if the animal is positive for Leishmania infantum. In this study, several other parameters (see below) were quantified from the same blood sample. None of the selected dogs necessitated anaesthesia to keep them quiet during blood sampling. Afterwards, during the same clinical visit, each selected dog was vaccinated against distemper, Rubbarth’s hepatitis, leptospirosis and diseases due to paramyxovirus and to parvovirus (gastroenteritis). All dogs were vaccinated in the same way, with a standard veterinary protocol. Once in the shelter, the health of each dog selected for this study was accurately monitored by a veterinarian who visited the dog under observation every day for one month; on the basis of a visual inspection and, if necessary, a physical examination, any symptom that eventually appeared (e.g., cough, diarrhoea, vomit, inflammations, otitis, among others) was registered. At the end of the 30 days of monitoring, a second blood sample was collected to evaluate the same parameters (see below), a routine that falls in good veterinary practices, allowed by the Italian National Laws, to monitor the health of the animals.

We selected a 30-day interval to monitor infectious diseases because the database of this shelter underlined that the incidence of diseases increases in that time and plateaus after 30 days.

Behavioural observations

Each of the 28 dogs was observed in its home-cage for a total of 5 hours (1 hour/day for 5 consecutive days, where possible) from a distance that ranged from 2 to 5 m, within the first week of permanence in the shelter, during the daytime between 8.00 and 18.00 h. All dogs became rapidly accustomed to the presence of the observer since the latter did not interact with them, neither during the observation session nor between observation sessions: after a few minutes, they just went on performing their ordinary activities. The ethogram consisted of more than 100 behavioural patterns (Table 1, described previously by De Palma et al. [35]. Data were collected using the focal animal sampling method [36]. The observations were made with a check sheet recording of the selected behavioural patterns of one dog (the focal animal) with ‘All occurrences’ and ‘1/0’ methods (60 s interval) [36].

thumbnail
Table 1. The ethogram utilised in this study for the observational session in cages (De Palma et al. 2005).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.t001

Novel object test

We placed an unknown object in a large outdoor enclosure (approximately 320 m2) and introduced one dog a time, allowing it to explore for 30 minutes. The dog was alone during the test and familiar with the enclosure. The unknown, novel object was a big, inflatable killer whale, which is usually utilised by children on the beach. This object was chosen because i) it had a strange and unknown shape that was not linked to threatening shapes written in the phylogenetic memory of the species (e.g., a bear); ii) it had large dimensions and a dark colour and thus was potentially intimidating; iii) it was plastic and was therefore washable.

The enclosure had a canopy where the inflatable object was tied to a string so that it hung just a little bit above the dog’s head. This placement ensured the dog saw the object (Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. The inflatable killer whale tied to a string under a canopy within the enclosure where the novel object test was performed.

The dog was released and was alone in the enclosure. Printed with permission from Simona Borruso original copyright [original copyright 2013].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.g001

We transformed the dog behavioural approach to the novel object (NO) in a binary variable, where "1" identified a positive approach (sniffing, looking while wagging the tail, inviting to play), and "0" identified a negative approach (barking, growling, threatening, fleeing from) (Table 2). We measured the time latency (min) to approach to the inflatable minus the time latency to looking at the object for the first time; in fact, some dogs were more sensitive to the stimuli furnished by the environment than others, and we wanted to precisely measure the gap between the moment at which the dog had noticed the novel object and the moment at which the animal decided to approach it.

thumbnail
Table 2. The ethogram utilised in this study for the observational session in the novel object test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.t002

T-maze test

The facility utilised to carry out the T-maze test was a pre-structured enclosure with a "V" shape (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. The facility utilised to carry out the T-maze test: A pre-structured enclosure with a "V" shape.

Each arm of the structure measures 40 m, and the arms are separated by a distance of 39 m. At the end of each arm there is a reward area where some food can be placed. The dog was released at point A and was alone in the maze.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.g002

Each arm was 40 m long and positioned a distance of 39 m from the other. At the end of each arm, there was a reward area where some food, a 2-cm-long piece of sausage, could be placed. Each dog was tested 10 times, and the trials were carried out on different days within, or a few days after, the end of the month of health monitoring. The dog was released at point A and was alone in the maze. For the first 5 repetitions, the food reward was placed at the end of the right arm; from the 6th to the 10th repetitions, the food reward was placed at the end of the left arm. Individuals were tested 10 times. The test lasted 20 min/repetition. In each session, we registered the first direction undertaken by the dog and the time latency (min) it took to find the food.

Blood sampling and determination of physiological parameters

Blood samples were collected from each individual to determine the complete haemogram, white blood cell differential count, haptoglobin, lysozyme, CD4+/CD8+ ratio, serum proteins (α1-, α2-, β- e γ-globulins), leishmanial and ehrlichia diagnostic tests and oxidative status by the amount of oxidative damage (dRoms), as well as the total antioxidant capacity to cope with it. For each dog, three blood tubes were obtained, one with K3-EDTA and two without K3-EDTA, depending on the type of examination. Concerning the latter, serum was separated by centrifugation and then frozen (−20 °C) until assayed.

Haematology.

A complete blood cell count (haemogram) was performed using the automated counter Cell-Dyn 3700 (ABBOTT—12 parameters) to assess leukocyte (neutrophils, eosinophils, basophils, monocytes and lymphocytes) counts.

Phenotype of lymphocyte subpopulations.

Flow cytometric immunophenotyping of leucocytes was performed by analyses of whole blood. The concentration and percentage of CD4 and CD8 T cell populations were measured using a mAb against canine CD4 FITC/CD8 RPE (DC048) purchased from AbD Serotec (Bio-Rad) (UK). Aliquots of 100 μl of whole blood containing approximately 1x106 white blood cells were added to 10 μl of mAbs and incubated for 1 hour in the dark at 4°C. Next, the erythrocytes were lysed, and the leucocytes were fixed by addition of FACSLyse (Becton Dickinson Biosciences, San Josè, CA, USA). After two washes with PBS, the cells were analysed by Cell Quest Pro software on a FACSCalibur flow cytometer (Becton Dickinson Biosciences) equipped with a 488 nm wavelength argon laser and a 635 nm wavelength red diode laser. The results were reported as the percentage of gated cells positive for each cell surface marker.

Oxidative stress.

The oxidative status was assessed by the amount of oxidative damage (dRoms) as well as the total antioxidant capacity to cope with it. Reactive oxygen metabolites (dRoms) were determined by a colorimetric method. The results of the dRoms test were expressed in arbitrary units called “Carratelli units” (CARR U), that is 1 CARR U = 0.08 mg of H2O2/100 ml (Diacron Adsorbent Test, Grosseto, Italy; limit of detection = 11 CARR; limit of quantification = 40 CARR; linearity 40–1000).

The Total Antioxidant Status test (Randox Lab. UK) was performed by a colorimetric method, and the results are expressed in mmol/l (Diacron Adsorbent Test, Grosseto, Italy; limit of detection = 25 micromol/ml; limit of quantification = 80 micromol/ml; linearity 80–500 micromol/ml).

Innate immune response.

Lysozyme in canine serum was determined according to established procedures by the bacteriological assay [3739]. Protein electrophoresis was performed using an automated analyser (Interlab G26).

Acute phase protein.

Haptoglobin was determined by an immunoenzymatic test (Tridelta Development Ltd. Ireland).

Statistical analysis

Behavioural patterns utilised to collect data during the observations of dogs when in their cage were grouped into categories (Table 3). The personalities of the dogs were determined by principal component analysis (PCA) utilising the categories of behavioural patterns that were displayed by at least two-thirds of the dogs in the study (see Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. The behavioural categories utilised for the PCA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.t003

The PCA factors were named based on variables showing correlations with them (significance set at 0,40); furthermore, we utilised the dog’s individual score for each factor to calculate a numerical index for each dog. To obtain that index, the individual scores for each factor were divided by the square root of the eigenvalue of that factor to obtain new scores with homogenous variability. The scores were then ordered in a decreasing sequence for the 28 dogs. The final personality index (PI) was obtained by dividing the individual score for the factor named “Insecurity” by the sum of the individual scores for two factors named “Self-confidence and Sociality twd humans”. A low ratio indicated a bold individual.

We ranked the dogs from the lowest to the highest ratio, i.e., from the boldest to the shyest.

To explain individual variability in vulnerability to diseases, we ran a generalised linear model (logistic regression) using the personality index as a predictor variable, the dichotomous variable “health improvement = 1, worsening = 0”, as a dependent variable and sex, age classes, breed and housing conditions (alone vs. with partner) as factors.

The model considers both simple and interactive effects (saturated model). Given that no relationship was found between the independent variables mentioned and the dependent variables, we ran a stepwise procedure. We checked the linearity of the log ODDs (probability of events and probability of non-events ratio), and the personality index was linear.

To identify the relationship between the personality index and the reaction to the novel object, we again applied a generalised linear model (logistic regression) using the personality index as a predictor variable, the binomial variable “fear towards the novel object” = 0, “non-fear towards the novel object” = 1”, as the dependent variable and sex, age classes, breed and housing conditions as independent factors. Based on a positive and negative approach to the inflatable object, two groups emerged: dogs that did not show any fear, and dogs that were frightened by the novel object. The same model was applied to the two groups, separately. Due to the small sample size of one of the two groups, we did not take into account sex, age classes, breed and housing conditions as factors. The same assumptions were checked.

Furthermore, we applied a linear regression model using the personality index as the predictor variable and the latency to approach the novel object as the dependent variable.

To identify the relationship between the personality index and the latency to find the food in the T-maze test, we applied a linear regression model using the personality index as the predictor variable and the average latency to find the food for each session as the dependent variable. Due to the small sample size, we did not take into account the sex, age classes, breed and housing conditions as factors. Gauss-Markov conditions were verified through the residual analysis.

The difference in mean latency of approaching the inflatable object between the groups was tested by a Mann-Whitney U test because of the different sizes.

The Spearman rank correlation coefficient was applied to assess the relationship among the parameters in the first and, separately, in the second blood sample.

Finally, we also applied a mixed model ANOVA—a between-subjects variable (bold vs. intermediate vs. shy classification) and a within-subjects variable (different parameters at time 1 vs. time 2).

All statistical analyses were carried out using Statistica release 8 (StatSoft Inc., Tulsa, OK, U.S.A.).

Ethics statements

We did not need an institutional or governmental permission to carry on the study since it was an observational study that involved two blood samples, the first enforced by the Italian National Law on dogs entering a shelter, and the second falling in good veterinary practice allowed by the National and International Laws.

Neither anesthesia nor euthanasia, or any kind of animal sacrifice, was part of the study.

Results

Dog personality assessment

The first three factors in the PCA together explained approximately 56,31% of the total variance of the data. For the first factor, any correlation of 0.50 or above was deemed relevant for the variable loading (Table 4). The first factor (F1) was defined “Self-confidence” since it was highly correlated with measures of “dominance, physical activity, attention, vocalisation, generic sociability"; for the second factor (F2), since it was the only one related to anxiety, we considered relevant any correlation of 0.40 or above. It was defined as "Insecurity” since it was highly correlated with measures of anxiety and subordination towards humans"; the third factor (F3) was defined “Sociality towards humans” since it was highly correlated with measures of sociability towards humans" (see Table 4). Thus, dogs with high F1 and F3 values were individuals with a bold personality because they were active, confident and sociable; in other words, they showed some degree of confidence in themselves. In contrast, dogs with high F2 values were individuals with a shy personality because they showed a high frequency of displacement activities and stereotypical behaviour, as well as subordinate attitudes towards humans; in other words, they were insecure.

The individual personality index, calculated as described in the Methods section and considering the ‘weight’ of each factor, provided a gradient along which each dog was classified from ‘bold’ to ‘shy‘.

We were able to identify 3 different categories: bold dogs (n = 9), intermediate dogs (n = 10), shy dogs (n = 9). The categories were formed by the observation of the qualitative behaviour of each individual dog matched with the personality index. The first category consisted of dogs that had an index between 0 and 0.3, and the observed behaviour confirmed that class. The second category comprised dogs that had an index value of 0.3 to 0.9, but observation of the qualitative behaviour of two subjects (Pedro and Bistecca) with 0.68 and 0.7 indexes led us to classify them in the third group with index values from 0.9 to 2.25. In fact, Pedro and Bistecca corresponded more to a shy personality description: Pedro was terrified by people and by other dogs; Bistecca was also subordinate to humans and showed fear aggression towards other dogs; this was highlighted, for example, by barking and growling at other dogs whilst holding the tail tucked between the legs.

Novel object test

Many different kinds of reactions to the novel object were recorded: some dogs were clearly frightened (9 individuals), and some others (19 individuals) were curious and willing to investigate the “strange” object. Thus, once the novel object was noticed, the dogs that showed fear clearly avoided it, ran around in the enclosure with ears and tail held low, tried to maintain a large distance from the inflatable object and barked at it. In contrast, dogs that showed curiosity approached the inflatable object, sniffed it with a relaxed body posture and one dog invited it to play.

The personality index and other factors (sex, age class, breed and condition of housing) were not good predictors of the quality (positive or negative) and latency to approach the novel object because, indeed, a number of “bold” dogs showed fear towards the inflatable killer whale (odds ratio = 8,17, N = 28, power = 0,74, p < 0,05) and were not quick to approach and investigate it. Nevertheless, if we distinguished between dogs that had a negative approach and those that had a positive approach, personality seemed to play a role. The two groups differed in mean latency to approach (dogs without fear: 40,53 sec: fearful dogs: 470,67 sec; test U Mann-Whitney: Z = -2,78; Zagg = -2,79; p = 0,005). Among the dogs that showed fear, bolder dogs were clearly faster (bold = 1,23 min; intermediate = 6,48 min; shy = 22,40 min) (r2 = 0,72; df = 1,7; p = 0,0041) (Fig 3); however, among the dogs that did not show any fear, the behaviour of bold and shy dogs did not differ (r2 = 0,07; df = 1,17; p = 0,29) (Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 3. The time latency of approach to the novel object of dogs who showed fear towards the unknown object.

Dogs are ordered from left to right from boldest to shyest.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. The time latency of approach to the novel object of dogs who did not show fear towards the unknown object.

Dogs are ordered from left to right from boldest to shyest.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.g004

T-maze test

The latency to find food tended to decrease from the 1st to the 10th trial (Fig 5). In the first five trials, when food was placed at the end of the right arm of the maze, the average of all dogs together ranged from 8,54 (1st) to 2,44 (5th) minutes.

thumbnail
Fig 5. The average time latency (minutes) that the 28 dogs spent searching food in the T-maze test in the 10 trials.

In the 6th trial, the location of the food was changed from the right to the left arm. Bars indicate the standard error.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.g005

In the 6th trial, when the position of the food was moved to the left arm for the first time, the mean time latency increased to 4,60 min and again dramatically decreased during the 9th and 10th trials (2,04 and 2,78 min, respectively) (see Fig 5).

The personality index was not a good predictor of the latency to find the food (r2 = 0,028, p = 0,398).

The first 9 dogs, belonging to the “bold” category, from the 7th to the 10th trials showed the highest mean latency to find the food when compared to intermediate and shy categories of dogs (3,44, 2,22 and 2,59 min, respectively).

Although the latency to find the food decreased dramatically from 8,54 min (mean of the first trial) to 2,78 min (mean of the tenth trial), the choice of direction taken by the dogs seemed totally random (Fig 6). In other words, neither bold nor shy dogs learned to go in the right direction to find the food; in contrast to findings in other mammal species, in which bold animals were usually slower to learn because they were less prone to routine changes during the reversal learning task, we could not find any relationship between the direction chosen in the T-maze test and dog personality.

thumbnail
Fig 6. The direction chosen by the 28 dogs in the 10 trials during the T-maze test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.g006

Dog personality and health status

The generalised linear model (logistic regression) developed for the dependent variable ‘health improvement/worsening’ significantly showed that dog personality index was a good predictor of health trend (p = 0,019; personality index odds ratio = 9,72), especially for crossbred vs. purebred dogs (Table 5).

thumbnail
Table 5. Modelled probability of the dog health status trend as personality index increases.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.t005

The model showed a good fit to the data (Fig 7).

thumbnail
Fig 7. Scatterplot of observed and expected probability against the personality index.

Health improvement = 1, worsening = 0.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.g007

Thus, the bolder the dogs were, the lower was the probability of contracting a disease and/or the higher was the probability of improving their health status, especially for crossbred dogs. The results shown in Table 5 confirm that as the personality index (PI) increased towards higher shyness trait scores, the probability of an improvement in health status decreased (logistic regression coefficient for personality index = -2,33).

These findings indicated that the symptoms detected during the 30 days of intensive monitoring of the 28 dogs’ health were confirmed by physiological parameters (Table 6).

thumbnail
Table 6. Dog health status trend during the 30 days of accurate monitoring.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.t006

The physiological evidence that bolder individuals reacted better to the stressful shelter environment was confirmed by comparison of specific parameter values in the first and second blood samples. For example, bolder dogs had the highest mean level of haptoglobin in the first blood sample (B = 3,71 mg/ml; I = 2,05 mg/ml; S = 1,52 mg/ml), but it decreased dramatically after 30 days, compared to the other classes of dog personality (B = 2,12 mg/ml; I = 3,18 mg/ml; S = 1,98 mg/ml), and this difference tended to be significant (ANOVA for repeated measures: personality*haptoglobin: F2,24 = 3,001; p = 0,06) (Fig 8). The same was true for the level of dRoms (ANOVA for repeated measures: personality*dRoms: F2,13 = 3,29; p = 0,06) (Fig 9), despite the small number of dogs, further reducing the number of samples due to technical problems during the blood analysis (Table 7).

thumbnail
Fig 8. Mixed model ANOVA—A between-subjects variable.

The results of the mixed model ANOVA—a between-subjects variable (bold vs. intermediate vs. shy personality class) and a within-subjects variable (haptoglobin values of the 1st blood vs. the 2nd blood sample).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.g008

thumbnail
Fig 9. Mixed model ANOVA—A within-subjects variable.

The results of the mixed model ANOVA—a between-subjects variable (bold vs. intermediate vs. shy personality class) and a within-subjects variable (dRoms values of the 1st blood vs. the 2nd blood sample).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.g009

thumbnail
Table 7. Blood parameter average values of blood samples in the different personality classes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.t007

Finally, several blood parameters correlated amongst each other in the first blood sample (Table 8), and their number increased after 30 days, i.e., in the second blood sample (Table 9). It is interesting to note that some correlations between blood parameters were moderate for all 28 dogs, but increased in significance for bold dogs and decreased for intermediate and shy dogs, when personality categories were considered separately (e.g., albumin/globulin ratio and haptoglobin. All dogs: rho = - 0,41, N. = 28, p < 0,05; bold dogs: rho = - 0,70, N. = 9, p < 0,05; intermediate dogs: rho = - 0,08, N. = 10, NS; shy dogs: rho = - 0,53, N. = 9, NS).

thumbnail
Table 8. Significant correlations (p < 0.05) among blood parameters in the first blood sample (Spearman rank correlation test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.t008

thumbnail
Table 9. Significant correlations (p < 0.05) among blood parameters in the second blood sample (Spearman rank correlation test).

*Indicates correlations that were not present in the first blood sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.t009

Discussion

Dog personality and health status

Direct observation of dog behaviour using traditional ethological methods [36] yielded a dataset that, analysed by means of PCA, identified three personality dimensions: "self-confidence”; "Insecurity” and "sociality towards humans”. None of the three identified dimensions alone was sufficient to assess the personality of the dogs in this study along the proactive and reactive axis since a bold individual is much more than an individual who is aggressive, dominant or fast in taking decisions; on the other hand, a shy individual is much more than an individual who is quiet, subordinate or slow in taking decisions [31]. For example, in contrast to potential expectations, researchers observed that bold individuals of some species are less able to solve problems caused by a routine change, whereas shy individuals can adapt more quickly to changes [1, 4041]. For these reasons, we considered the variables that featured high on the first factor as important variables that featured high on the third factor in relation to boldness, and, conversely, of the variables that featured high on the second factor; thus, we wanted to create a (weighted) index that combined the three factors, providing a broader personality dimension. Such an index allowed us to assess dog personality along the boldness-shyness axis, in accordance with the literature (see for example [29, 35, 4245]).

In fact, most of the literature on the subject extract primary factors that are subsequently combined in a broader dimension along the bold-shy continuum. For example, Svartberg and Forkman ([29] p. 152) state the following: <<The higher-order factor, which relates to playfulness, exploration, interest in chase and sociability, indicates that to a great extent it is one single dimension that influences the dogs’ behaviour during the whole test>> and (p. 153) <<Shy individuals are generally cautious, timid and evasive in novel situations—both in social and in non-social situations—while bolder individuals are more spontaneous, social, and exploratory>>. Moreover, Svartberg ([30] p. 16) note the following: <<I calculated a boldness score, by averaging the scores for playfulness, curiosity/fearlessness and sociability.

Furthermore, the results presented herein suggest that dogs rated higher in ‘boldness’ are more likely to possess the skills to cope with a stressful environment, which potentially carries a high risk of infection, such as a dog shelter. This finding is in accordance with previous findings reported for other mammalian species (for mice see, for example, [1, 17, 4647]). In other words, the results of this study suggest that there is a relationship between personality and stress-related disease vulnerability. In fact, the health of bold dogs even improved after a stay of one month in the shelter, while intermediate or shy dogs tended to worsen because the latter have an inadequate stress response, due to a scant capacity to control the environment. It has been suggested ([1, 17, 4650]; [51] reviewed in [52]) that physiological and behavioural changes occur when an organism is not able to control the environment, leading to an increased vulnerability to psychosomatic diseases, e.g. cardiovascular, gastrointestinal, and immunological disturbances [52]. Thus, since the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis is an important mediator in communication between the brain and the immune system [2425, 5356], lowering of the immune system defences in stressed shy individuals leads to an increased vulnerability to diseases.

Furthermore, the present results confirm that one personality dimension that is particularly important for determining disease vulnerability seems to be ‘sociability’, in agreement with the study by Capitanio et al. [57] on rhesus macaques. Sociability in dogs housed in a shelter has been found to be related to a higher total antioxidant capacity [58] and a lower concentration of faecal cortisol metabolites [35], both of which are potentially associated with higher levels of welfare.

An added value of the current results is that dogs were tested in a "natural setting" (referring to the infective agents), such that the observed dogs were presented with a stressful situation replete with infectious agents that pre-existed in the dog shelter, rather than artificially inoculating them with infective agents. Especially in Italy, where the “no kill policy” has been enforced by law since 1991 (and since 1988 in the Lazio Region) (see [58], p. 228), shelter dog populations might be an ideal subject for studying the effects of ‘naturally occurring stress’ on physiology and immunology. One point of the same law states the following: <<Shelter dogs cannot be used as experimental animals>> (National Italian Law No. 281 come out in 1991). Thus, we must stress that we are not suggesting the use of shelter dogs as experimental subjects, but from a perspective of animal welfare, we suggest that the physiological status of dogs housed in shelters should be monitored closely to study their reaction to stress, as an alternative to officially authorized laboratory experiments on dogs with the same purpose.

As suggested by Capitanio et al. [57] for adult male rhesus monkeys, there are physiological correlates of personality factors, and the relationship between personality dimensions and physiological measures reflect the particular demands of the situation experienced by the animals. The blood parameter values found in this study support this perspective. First, it is well known that in dogs, as well as in other species, general leucocytosis, neutrophilia, lymphopenia, and eosinopenia occur during acute stress [25, 53, 5960], as well as increases in serum and urine lysozyme levels during infections [37, 39]. Leukocyte, neutrophil, lymphocyte and eosinophil numbers, as well as the lysozyme value and CD4/CD8 ratio, displayed substantial temporal consistency in the present study.

Furthermore, some correlations between blood parameters that appeared in the second blood sample were not present in the first one. In some respects, the 30 days of permanence in the shelter provided a clearer picture of the dog’s health.

For example, it is interesting to note the negative correlation between lymphocytes and dRoms (as indicators of free radicals): it is well known that stress provokes decreases in lymphocytes [25] and increases in dRoms [61]. Additionally, the positive correlation in the second blood sample between the albumin/globulin ratio and the CD4/CD8 ratio indicated that both defence systems (humoral and cell-mediated) were engaged because of an infection (predominantly viral).

Still, haptoglobin, an acute phase protein, is correlated to the degree of severity of an infection and is considered a predictive marker of inflammation because it indicates inflammation earlier than the immune responses and the appearance of clinical symptoms [62]. In this study, bolder dogs tended to have higher levels of haptoglobin in the first blood sample, whereas this difference disappeared in the second blood sample; still, there was a negative correlation between the albumin/globulin ratio and the haptoglobin in the second blood sample, especially when personality categories were considered separately.

These observations would further support the idea that the dogs were reacting to infectious agents encountered during the first 30 days of their stay at the shelter and, furthermore, that bold dogs coped with this insult better than shy ones.

Thus, several pieces of evidence showed that the highly infectious environment had an impact on the dogs. Nevertheless, the health of some dogs improved, the majority of which were bold.

The only exception was Pippo, who despite being included in the ‘bold personality’ category, showed a deterioration in his health condition with obvious symptoms of urinary infection emerging during the 30 days of observation.

Novel object test

Wilson et al. [63] suggested that boldness can be defined as an individual’s general tendency to approach novel objects and willingness to take risks, and indeed, this measure has been used in a number of species (e.g., [6466]). However, in contrast to such studies, the current results from the Novel Object test did not seem to provide useful information about the personalities of the dogs because bold and shy dogs did not react differently to the inflatable killer whale. King et al. [67] showed in their study that, while not specifically measured, dogs that exhibited postural signs of fear often showed increased latency times to the novel object approach, based on anecdotal observations. Our results, in which the latency time of approach was measured, confirmed this observation but showed that, among dogs that showed fear, personality seemed to again play a crucial role because bolder dogs, although clearly frightened, clearly were faster to approach the unknown object.

T-maze test

It has been reported for many animal species that bold animals are usually less thorough explorers and rely more on routines [9, 41, 64, 6869]. In the present study, bold dogs were slower to find the food than intermediate and shy ones, but the difference was not statistically significant. Overall, the performance of dogs in the maze improved over time since the latency to obtain food decreased across trials, but the dogs never learned the correct the rule regarding the direction to take to find the food more quickly, either during the acquisition or during the reversal phase. Thus, it would seem that dogs learned that there was food somewhere, and hence, their speed of movement in the maze increased, but they did not acquire the knowledge of the location in which to find the food.

Conclusions

In the present study, dogs were monitored for thirty days after their entry into the shelter. The observed increase in leukocytes, neutrophils and eosinophils, as well as the increase and/or decrease in other blood immunological parameters, revealed an immediate reaction to stress and to exposure to diseases present in the shelter that, depending on the personality of the dog, led to the improving or worsening health status.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Raw data regarding behavioural patterns in cages, physiological parameters and reaction of dogs during the the novel object test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.s001

(PDF)

S2 Table. Raw data regarding latency for finding the food during the the T-maze test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193794.s002

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank the shelter staff for their continual cooperation in conducting this research. Particular thanks go to Sarah Marshal-Pescini for her help both in improving the language and the conceptual framework of this manuscript. We also thank Patrick Peevey for his help to improve the written English and to an anonymous referee for useful suggestions that improved the manuscript. We are grateful to John Capitanio and Simona Cafazzo, who read the first draft of the manuscript and provided helpful comments. Finally, we are indebted to Carlotta Trombetti who, between a skating lesson and the other, as the good architect as she is, produced a drawing to illustrate the equipment used for the T-maze test. This research was not supported by any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

References

  1. 1. Koolhaas J. M., Korte S. M., De Boer S. F., van der Vegt B. J., Van Reenen C. G., Hopster H. et al. (1999). Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress physiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 925–935. pmid:10580307
  2. 2. Wilson D. S., Clark A. B., Coleman K. & Dearstyne T. (1994). Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals. Trends Ecol. Evol. 9, 442–446.
  3. 3. Kurvers R. H. J. M., Eijkelenkamp B., van Oers K., van Lith B., van Wieren S. E., Ydenberg R. C. et al. (2009). Personality differences explain leadership in barnacle gees. Anim. Behav. 9, 1–7.
  4. 4. Mettke-Hofmann C., Ebert C., Schmidt T., Steiger S. & Stieb S. (2005). Personality traits in resident and migratory warbler species. Behaviour 142, 1357–1375.
  5. 5. Goddard M. E. & Beilharz R. G. (1986). Early prediction of adult behavior in potential guide dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 15, 247–260.
  6. 6. Wood-Gush D. G. M. & Vestergaard K. (1991). The seeking of novelty and its relation to play. Anim. Behav. 42, 599–606.
  7. 7. Ruis M. A. W., te Brake J. H. A., van de Burgwal J. A., de Jong I. C., Blokhuis H. J. & Koolhaas J. M. (2000). Personalities in female domesticated pigs: behavioural and physiological indications. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 66,31–47.
  8. 8. Wilson D. M. & Stevens E. D. (2005). Consistency in context-specific measures of shyness and boldness in rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss. Ethology 11, 849–862.
  9. 9. Thomson J. S., Watts P. C., Pottinger T. G. & Sneddon L. U. (2011). Physiological and genetic correlates of boldness: characterising the mechanisms of behavioural variation in rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss. Horm. Behav. 59, 67–74. pmid:20965192
  10. 10. Benus R. F., Den Daas S., Koolhaas J. M. & Van Oortmerssen G. A. (1990). Routine formation and flexibility in social and non-social behaviour of aggressive and non-aggressive male mice. Behaviour 112, 176–193.
  11. 11. Coppens C. M., de Boer S. F., Buwalda B. & Koolhaas J. M. (2014). Aggression and aspects of impulsivity in wild-type rats. Aggress. Behav. 40, 300–308. pmid:24464354
  12. 12. Natoli E., Say L., Cafazzo S., Bonanni R., Schmid M. & Pontier D. (2005). Bold attitude makes male urban feral domestic cats more vulnerable to Feline Immunodeficiency Virus. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29 (I), 151–157.
  13. 13. Carere C., Groothuis T. G., Mostl E., Daan S. & Koolhaas J. M. (2003). Fecal corticosteroids in a territorial bird selected for different personalities: daily rhythm and the response to social stress. Horm. Behav. 43, 540–548. pmid:12799170
  14. 14. Cavigelli S. A. & McClintock M. K. (2003). Fear of novelty in infant rats predicts adult corticosterone dynamics and an early death. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 100,16131–16136. pmid:14673078
  15. 15. Veenema A. H., Meijer O. C., de Kloet E. R. & Koolhaas J. M. (2003). Genetic selection for coping style predicts stressor susceptibility. J. Neuroendocrinol. 15, 256–257. pmid:12588514
  16. 16. Koolhaas J. M., De Boer S. F., Coppens C. M. & Buwalda B. (2010). Neuroendocrinology of coping styles: towards understanding the biology of individual variation. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 31, 307–321. pmid:20382177
  17. 17. Sgoifo A., Carnevali L. & Grippo A. J. (2014). The socially stressed heart: insights from studies in rodents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 39, 51–60. pmid:24373860
  18. 18. Costantini D., Ferrari C., Pasquaretta C., Cavallone E., Carere C., von Hardenberg A. et al. (2012). Interplay between plasma oxidative status, cortisol and coping styles in wild alpine marmots, Marmota marmota. J. Exp. Biol. 215, 374–383. pmid:22189781
  19. 19. Hennessy M. B., Davis H. N., Williams M.T., Mellott C. & Douglas C. W. (1997). Plasma cortisol levels of dogs at a county animal shelter. Physiol. Behav. 62, 485–490. pmid:9272654
  20. 20. Hennessy M.B., Williams M.T., Miller D.D., Douglas C.W. & Voith V.L. (1998). Influence of male and female petters on plasma cortisol and behaviour: can human interaction reduce the stress of dogs in a public animal shelter? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 61, 63–77.
  21. 21. Hennessy M. B., Voith V. L., Mazzei S. J., Buttram J., Miller D. D. & Linden F. (2001). Behaviour and cortisol levels of dogs in a public animal shelter, and an exploration of the ability of these measures to predict problem behaviour after adoption. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 73, 217–233. pmid:11376839
  22. 22. Hennessy M. B., Voith V. L., Hawke J. L., Young T. L., Centrone J., McDowell A. L. et al. (2002a). Effects of a program of human interaction and alterations in a diet composition on activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in dogs housed in a public animal shelter. J. Am. Vet. Med. Ass. 221, 65–71.
  23. 23. Hennessy M. B., Voith V. L., Young T. L., Hawke J. L., Centrone J. & McDowell A. L. (2002b). Exploring human interaction and diet effects on the behavior of dogs in a public animal shelter. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 5, 253–273.
  24. 24. Beerda B., Schilder M. B. H., van Hooff J.A.R.A.M., de Vries H. W. & Mol J. A. (1999a). Chronic stress in dogs subjected to social and spatial restriction. I. Behavioral responses. Phys. Behav. 66, 233–242.
  25. 25. Beerda B., Berdanidina W., Schilder M. B. H., van Hooff J.A.R.A.M., de Vries H. W. & Mol J. A. (1999b). Chronic stress in dogs subjected to social and spatial restriction. II. Hormonal and immunological responses. Phys. Behav. 66, 243–254.
  26. 26. Gazzano A., Mariti C., Himmelmann M., Calderisi L., Niccolini A., Ruggeri G. et al. (2004). Valutazione della cortisolemia in cani ospitati in canile sanitario. Ann. Fac. Med. Vet. Pisa LVII/2,189–196.
  27. 27. Rooney N. J., Gaines S. A. & Bradshaw J. W. S. (2007). Behavioural and glucocorticoid responses of dogs (Canis familiaris) to kennelling: investigating mitigation of stress by prior habituation. Physiol. Behav. 92, 847–854. pmid:17617429
  28. 28. Protopopova A. (2016). Effects of sheltering on physiology, immune function, behavior, and the welfare of dogs. Physiol. Behav. 159, 95–103. pmid:26996275
  29. 29. Svartberg K. & Forkman B. (2002). Personality traits in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 79, 133–155.
  30. 30. Svartberg K. (2005). A comparison of behaviour in test and in everyday life: evidence of three consistent boldness-related personality traits in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 91 (1–2), 103–128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2004.08.030
  31. 31. Caramaschi D., Carere C., Sgoifo A. & Koolhaas J. M. (2013). Neuroendocrine and autonomic correlates of animal personalities. In Maestripieri & Carere (Eds), ‘Animal Personalities. Behavior, physiology and evolution’. The University of Chicago Press. Chicago and London.
  32. 32. Careau V., Reale D., Humphries M. M. & Thomas D. W. (2010). The Pace of Life under Artificial Selection: Personality, Energy Expenditure, and Longevity Are Correlated in Domestic Dogs. Am. Nat. 175 (6), 753–758. pmid:20384493
  33. 33. Svartberg K., Tapper I.; Temrin H.; Radesater T. & Thorman S. (2005). Consistency of personality traits in dogs. Anim. Behav. 69, 283–291
  34. 34. Bray E. E.; Sammel M. D.; Seyfarth R. M.; Serpell J. A. & Cheney D. L. (2017). Temperament and problem solving in a population of adolescent guide dogs. Anim. Cogn. first Online: 10 July 2017. pmid:28695349
  35. 35. De Palma C., Viggiano E., Barillari E., Palme R., Dufour A. B., Fantini C. et al. (2005). Evaluating the temperament in shelter dogs. Behaviour 142, 1313–1334.
  36. 36. Altman S. A. (1974). Observational study of behaviour: sampling methods. Behaviour 49, 227–267. pmid:4597405
  37. 37. Osserman E. F. & Lawlor D. P. (1966). Serum and urinary lysozyme (muramidase) in monocytic and monocytic leukemia. J. Exp. Med. 124, 921–952. pmid:5224561
  38. 38. Bonizzi L., Amadori M., Melegari M., Ponti W., Ceccarelli A. & Bolzani E. (1989). Characterization of some parameters of non-specific immunity in dairy cattle (I). J. Vet, Med., Series B, 36, 365–373.
  39. 39. Tasca S., Furlanello T. & Caldin M. (2010). High serum and urine lysozyme levels in a dog with acute myeloid leukemia. J Vet. Diagn. Invest. 22, 111–115. pmid:20093697
  40. 40. Sih A., Bell A., Johnson J. C. & Ziemba R. E. 2004. Behavioral syndromes: An integrative overview. Quart. Rev. Biol. 79, 241–277. pmid:15529965
  41. 41. Groothuis T. G. G. & Carere C. (2005). Avian personalities: characterization and epigenesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 137–150. pmid:15652261
  42. 42. Hsu Y. & Serpell J. A. (2003). Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Ass. 223, 1293–1300.
  43. 43. Gosling S. D.; Kwan V. S. Y. & John O. P. (2003). A dog’s got personality: a cross-species comparative approach to personality judgments in dogs and humans. J. Pers. Social Psy. 85 (6), 1161–1169. pmid:14674821
  44. 44. Taylor K. D. & Mills D. S. (2006). The development and assessment of tem- perament tests for adult companion dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 1, 94–108.
  45. 45. Ley J., Coleman G. J., Holmes R. & Hemsworth P. H. (2007). Assessing fear of novel and startling stimuli in domestic dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 104, 71–84.
  46. 46. Kavelaars A., Heijnen C. J., Tennekes R., Bruggink J. E. & Koolhaas J. M. (1998). Individual Behavioral characteristics of wild-type rats predict susceptibility to experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis. Brain Behav. Immun. 13, 279–286.
  47. 47. Carnevali L., Trombini M., Porta A., Montano N., de Boer S. F. & Sgoifo A. (2013). Vagal withdrawal and susceptibility to cardiac arrhythmias in rats with high trait aggressiveness. PLoS One 8, e68316. pmid:23861886
  48. 48. Blanchard R. J., Blanchard D. C. & Flannelly K. J. (1985). Social stress, mortality and aggression in colonies and burrowing habitats. Behav. Processes 11, 209–213. pmid:24895927
  49. 49. Koolhaas J. M. & Van Oortmerssen G. A. (1988). Individual differences in disease susceptibility as a possible factor in the population dynamics of rats and mice. Neth. J. Zool. 38, 111–122.
  50. 50. Fleshner M., Laudenslager M. L., Simons L. & Maier S. F. (1989). Reduced serum antibodies associated with social defeat in rats. Physiol. Behav. 45, 1183–1187. pmid:2813542
  51. 51. Stefanski V. (2001). Social stress in laboratory rats: behavior, immune function, and tumor metastasis. Physiol. Behav. 73, 385–391. pmid:11438366
  52. 52. Sgoifo A, Koolhaas J. M., Alleva E., Musso E. & Parmigiani S. (2001). Social stress: acute and long-term effects on physiology and behavior. Physiol. Behav. 73, 253–4. pmid:11438349
  53. 53. Dhabhar F. S. & McEwen B. S. (1997). Acute stress enhances while chronic stress suppresses cell-mediated immunity in vivo: a potential role for leukocyte trafficking. Brain Behav. Imm. 11, 286–306.
  54. 54. Padgett D. A. & Glaser R. (2003). How stress influences the immune response. TRENDS Immunol. 24 (8), 444–448. pmid:12909458
  55. 55. Webster Marketon J. I. & Glaser R. (2008). Stress hormones and immune function. Cell. Immun. 252, 16–26.
  56. 56. Dudley E. S., Schiml P. A. & Hennessy M. B. (2015). Effects of repeated petting sessions on leukocyte counts, intestinal parasite prevalence, and plasma cortisol concentration of dogs housed in a county animal shelter. JAVMA 247 (11), 1289–1298. pmid:26594812
  57. 57. Capitanio J. P., Mendoza S. P. & Baroncelli S. (1999). The relationship of personality dimensions in adult male rhesus macaques to progression of simian immunodeficiency virus disease. Brain, Behav. Immunity 13, 138–154.
  58. 58. Cafazzo S., Maragliano L., Bonanni R., Scholl F., Guarducci M., Scarcella R. et al. (2014). Behavioural and physiological indicators of shelter dogs’ welfare: reflections on the no-kill policy on free-ranging dogs in Italy revisited on the basis of 15 years of implementation. Phys. Behav. 133, 223–229.
  59. 59. Capitanio J. P., Mendoza S. P. & Lerche N. W. (1998). Individual differences in peripheral blood immunological and hormonal measures in adult male rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): evidence for temporal and situational consistency. Am. J. Primat. 44, 29–41.
  60. 60. Dhabhar F. S. (2002). Stress-induced augmentation of immune function, The role of stress hormones, leukocyte trafficking, and cytokines. Brain Behav. Imm, 16. 785–798.
  61. 61. Costantini D., Carere C., Caramaschi D. & Koolhaas J. M. (2008). Aggressive and non-aggressive personalities differ in oxidative status in selected lines of mice (Mus musculus). Biol. Lett. 4, 119–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2007.0513. pmid:18042511
  62. 62. Cray C., Zaias J. & Altman N. H. (2009). Acute phase response in animals: a review. Comp. Med. 59 (6), 517–526. pmid:20034426
  63. 63. Wilson D. S., Clark A. B., Coleman K. & Dearstyne T. (1994). Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals. Trends Ecol. Evol. 9, 442–446.
  64. 64. Bourne G. R. & Sammons A. J. (2008). Boldness, aggression and exploration: evidence for a behavioural syndrome in male pentamorphic livebearing fish, Poecilia parae. Bioflux 1, 39–49.
  65. 65. Santillán-Doherty A. M., Cortés-Sotres J., Arenas-Rosas R. V., Márquez-Arias A., Cruz C., Medellín A. et al. (2010). Novelty-seeking temperament in captive stumptail macaques (Macaca arctoides) and spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi). J. Comp. Psy. 124 (2), 211–218.
  66. 66. Lendvai A. Z., Bókony V. & Chastel O. 2011. Coping with novelty and stress in free-living house sparrows. J. Exp. Biol. 214: 821–828. pmid:21307069
  67. 67. King T., Hemsworth P. H. & Coleman G. J. (2003). Fear of novel and startling stimuli in domestic dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 82, 45–64.
  68. 68. Benus R. F., Den Daas S., Koolhaas J. M. & Van Oortmerssen G. A. (1990). Routine formation and flexibility in social and non-social behaviour of aggressive and non-aggressive male mice. Behaviour 112, 176–193.
  69. 69. Coppens C. M., de Boer S. F., Buwalda B. & Koolhaas J. M. (2014). Aggression and aspects of impulsivity in wild-type rats. Aggress. Behav. 40, 300–308. pmid:24464354