Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The effects of substitute multisensory feedback on task performance and the sense of presence in a virtual reality environment

  • Natalia Cooper ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Natalia.Cooper@nrc-cnrc.gc.ca

    Affiliation Construction Research Centre, National Research Council, Ottawa, Canada

  • Ferdinando Milella,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation UKAEA, Culham Science Centre, Abingdon, United Kingdom

  • Carlo Pinto,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Resources, Software, Visualization

    Affiliation Virtual Engineering Centre, Daresbury, United Kingdom

  • Iain Cant,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Project administration, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization

    Affiliation Virtual Engineering Centre, Daresbury, United Kingdom

  • Mark White,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom

  • Georg Meyer

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom

Abstract

Objective and subjective measures of performance in virtual reality environments increase as more sensory cues are delivered and as simulation fidelity increases. Some cues (colour or sound) are easier to present than others (object weight, vestibular cues) so that substitute cues can be used to enhance informational content in a simulation at the expense of simulation fidelity. This study evaluates how substituting cues in one modality by alternative cues in another modality affects subjective and objective performance measures in a highly immersive virtual reality environment. Participants performed a wheel change in a virtual reality (VR) environment. Auditory, haptic and visual cues, signalling critical events in the simulation, were manipulated in a factorial design. Subjective ratings were recorded via questionnaires. The time taken to complete the task was used as an objective performance measure. The results show that participants performed best and felt an increased sense of immersion and involvement, collectively referred to as ‘presence’, when substitute multimodal sensory feedback was provided. Significant main effects of audio and tactile cues on task performance and on participants' subjective ratings were found. A significant negative relationship was found between the objective (overall completion times) and subjective (ratings of presence) performance measures. We conclude that increasing informational content, even if it disrupts fidelity, enhances performance and user’s overall experience. On this basis we advocate the use of substitute cues in VR environments as an efficient method to enhance performance and user experience.

Introduction

Virtual reality (VR) environments are commonly used for training, research, interpersonal communication, data visualisation and many other purposes [14] and as such a continuous optimisation and evaluation of the effectiveness of these systems is required. One of the fundamental decisions designers of VR systems have to make is to select what sensory modalities can be stimulated. Visual and auditory signals are routinely presented, but other signals, in particular vestibular cues, which require complex motion platforms, or haptic cues are often omitted.

There is substantial evidence that improvements in simulation fidelity, the degree to which VR technology delivers an objectively measurable match to real-world sensory signals, (Immersion, in Slater’s terminology [57]), improves performance in VR training environments [812]. It is therefore tempting to stimulate as many sensory modalities as possible to provide the greatest possible simulation fidelity. Practical considerations, but also financial and operational constraints, limit the degree to which sensory stimuli can be provided in many applications [13]. While there, for example, is progress in providing haptic cues [14,1426] there still are significant challenges [14, 27] that can make some tasks more difficult and decrease overall task efficiency [15] when haptic cues are presented.

Virtual immersive experiences do not just depend on sensory factors, but also include actional and symbolic components to give participants the impression that they are present in the virtual environment [9]. Ferwerda [28] in the context of image fidelity, for example, makes a distinction between physical fidelity (veridical stimulation of the sensory system), photo-realism (veridical representation) and functional fidelity (veridical representation of the symbolic information) and stresses that functional fidelity is particularly task-relevant [11, 28].

In VR environments it is entirely possible to represent functional information in arbitrary signals and modalities. In an assembly task, for example, it may be necessary to fully insert a part before the next step can be undertaken. In real life this information (part snaps into place) would be signalled by a simultaneous change in a range of sensory signals such as weight, vibration, sound or visual cues. Since haptic cues are much more difficult to realise in VR environments than visual or auditory cues [8, 9, 11, 1520], limiting information to a subset of modalities may not affect performance.

In virtual environments, however, cues are not constrained by the physical limitations of the real world; symbolic information can be presented in arbitrary modalities and as arbitrary signals. The symbolic information of a part ‘snapping into place’, for example, could be represented by a colour change in the part, arbitrary sound or vibration signals. Presenting task-relevant information using alternative signals or modalities means that sensory fidelity reduces because the presented sensory signals no longer objectively match real environments or real behaviour [57]. It might therefore be argued that substitute cues should consequently have a negative impact on user experience and performance [8].

An alternative argument is that providing task-relevant information as substitute cues, while reducing physical fidelity, adds functional information that is relevant for natural interaction, which is the focus of Witmer and Singer’s [2931] definition of immersion: being able to affect, and being affected by, the virtual environment. The provision of task-relevant information in substitute modalities, therefore, may enhance interaction and involvement in virtual environments and consequently may have a positive effect on performance and user experience.

The principal question driving this research is whether cues used in VR environments can be substituted by alternative information-bearing signals or cues in other modalities. While such substitute cues enhance information available to VR users, they negatively impact on the fidelity of the VR environment, which has also been linked to task performance [13, 3234]. We use subjective measures of user experience and objective measures of task performance to address this question. We hypothesise that substitute sensory cueing will have a positive effect on objective and subjective measures collected from participants during the completion of a realistic task in a VR environment.

In the following sections, we will review the role of multimodal feedback on task performance in VR environments. Then we will discuss previous work on the effects of sensory feedback on user’s sense of presence and immersion, specifically focusing on the relationship between the objective and subjective measures of performance.

Performance improvements via multi-sensory cues

In interacting with real world environments, we have a bias towards vision as we take most of our information about the environment through the visual modality. In VR environments it is possible to provide additional information in order to improve the ability to identify the important information relevant to the task [35]. Experimental research in multisensory stimulation suggests the potential benefits of other modalities, such as audio and tactile feedback [11, 14, 3541]. Indeed, studies have shown that visual and audio sensory cues, presented alone or in a combination, improve task performance in target localisation [35], target accuracy [14] and spatial attention tasks without affecting perceptual workload [3640]. Behavioural data from Meyer et al. [39] revealed that temporally, spatially, and semantically congruent information represented in more than one modality facilitates performance i.e. multimodal stimuli are detected faster or more accurately than incongruent bimodal stimuli [39]. This effect is not limited to one additional modality; Hecht et al. [41], for example, found that reaction times for tri-modal signals (visual, audio, haptic) were much faster than reaction times for bi-modal signals, which were in turn faster than uni-modal signals in a simple detection task. They note that although haptic perception was shown to be inferior during active movement, its combination with other modalities (visual and audio) is advantageous. Meyer et al. [11] showed that training with additional task-relevant visual and auditory cues can lead to transferrable improvements in performance.

The benefits of multisensory stimulation have been demonstrated in interface design studies in relation to improved task performance [42], user satisfaction [43] and reduced cognitive load [44, 45]. Tasks were generally performed faster, with greater efficiency, even when task difficulty was increased. For example, when assessing cognitive workload during the use of multimodal and unimodal interfaces, Shi et al. [44] and Oviatt et al. [45] showed that multimodal interfaces lead to reductions in cognitive workload reflected in objective (physiological) measures.

In 3D virtual interactions, there are limited possibilities to present haptic feedback: Force feedback conveys information to the user by the generation of forces using a mechanical interface that moves with the user and applies force to a specific point of the body, such as the finger-tip (via phantom haptic devices, [46]). As a substitute cue, vibrotactile feedback is a relatively simple mechanism for providing tactile signals [15], even though mechanical resistance to movements cannot be provided in this way [1416]. Martinez et al. [14] compared force and vibrotactile feedback in a texture detection study and showed that vibrotactile feedback is more efficient than force feedback in the recognition of textures from different patterns and shapes. Similarly, Schoonmaker and Cao [16] suggested that the distortions in force feedback devices used during minimal invasive surgery make the task more difficult for surgeons and argue that vibrotactile feedback can serve as a viable substitute for force feedback. The empirical research investigating the benefits of vibrotactile feedback has shown potential advantages of vibrotactile feedback during VR interaction [24, 1721, 26], spatial guidance [22], in teaching and learning processes of new physical activities [23, 24]. Vibrotactile feedback has also been shown to be an efficient warning signal in complex tasks such as driving a car or air traffic control [25].

Despite the supportive evidence, other studies have reported that sensory modalities, in particular tactile and audio feedback, can decrease overall performance; for example, audio and tactile cues were shown to have adverse effects in accuracy tasks, both were perceived as distracting and annoying and had a potential to cause sensory overload [26, 47, 48].

Sensory substitution has been applied successfully in helping people with deficits in one or more sensory modality [27]. Multimodal sensory substitution devices (SSDs) that provide audio and vibration cues to signal depth within the environment can successfully supplement visual information for low sighted and blind people [49]. Similarly, D’Alonzo and Cipriani [50] showed that vibrotactile sensory substitution can be used to induce self-attribution of the rubber hand during synchronous but modality conflicting visual tactile stimulation. In this study we provide additional substitute sensory cuing that is relevant to the manual task in a 3D environment.

Presence and immersion

The principal aim in designing VR systems is to immerse users to such an extent in the virtual worlds that they accept the virtual world as ‘real’. The effectiveness of VR system is consequently often considered through the constructs known as ‘presence’ and ‘immersion’ [57, 2932, 5156]. The terms ‘presence’ and ‘immersion’ have been subject to debate within the research community [57, 2932, 5156]. There is a broad consensus that the term ‘presence’ describes the subjective feeling of being present in the virtual environment, rather than the real space. The term ‘presence’ has been defined as i) sense of being in one place or environment, even when one is physically situated in another [2931, 34], ii) a state of consciousness, the (psychological) sense of being in the virtual environment [57]; iii) the sense of being physically present within a computer-generated or remote environment [32]; iv) user’s subjective, context-dependent psychological response to a VR system [13].

The definition of immersion has been subjected to a more controversial debate within the research community. Immersion is defined by some researchers, notably Witmer and Singer [2931], as a subjective experience: the psychological state where one perceives himself as being included in and interacting with an environment that provides a continuous stream of stimuli and experience [2932, 34, 55]. Other researchers, notably Slater [57], defined the term ‘immersion’ as a technological aspect of virtual environment i.e. the objective level of sensory fidelity a VR system provides. This is measurable in physical system parameters, such as the field of regard, field of view, stereoscopy, display size, display resolution, head based rendering, frame rate and refresh rate amongst others [57, 13, 33].

A variety of measures have been proposed to measure presence and immersion. The most common subjective measures are questionnaires (for a review see [56]). Objective measures include physiological measures [44, 45], time recordings [11, 39], eye movement [57] or task switching performance [58]. Visually evoked postural responses have been also proposed as an objective measure for presence in VR environment [21].

To assess user perception of the VR environment in this study, a short five-item questionnaire was used repeatedly; the items on this questionnaire were derived from the Presence Questionnaire (PQ) designed by Witmer and Singer [2931]. The PQ questionnaire is frequently used to measure user perception of VR environment because of its convenience and non-intrusiveness [2931, 53, 56, 59, 60]. PQ ratings are commonly found to the correlated with technological aspects of VR system design [61], performance and immersive tendencies of the user [53, 61] and the environment [56, 59, 60]. Empirical research has shown that VR technologies that produce a greater sense of immersion and involvement will also produce a higher sense of presence [2934, 5155]. Increased simulation realism (fidelity) is an important aspect of this and has been linked to enhanced presence and immersion, and consequently to improved performance [13, 32, 60, 62]. Some studies have shown that multimodal sensory input not only enhances the sense of presence and task performance, but also memory of objects in the VR environment [63, 64].

Witmer and Singer [2931] suggest that presence in virtual environments (VE) depends on one’s attention shifting from physical environment to virtual one; how sharply users focus their attention on the VE partially determines the extent to which they become involved and how much presence they report [2931]. Similarly, they suggest that immersion depends on one perceiving oneself as being part of the environment and being able to interact with it. Following this notion, presence would occur as a consequence of the allocation of attentional resources; immersion and involvement [2931].

Slater’s definition of immersion as’ the degree to which VR technology delivers an objectively measurable match to real-world sensory signals’ [57] focuses on sensory fidelity [9]. In the experiments reported here, sensory fidelity is deliberately reduced by presenting increased levels of substitute cues, which provide task-relevant information. As described in the previous section, sensory substitution enhances performance in many ‘real environment’ applications and is likely to enhance subjective immersion in VR environment by providing more task-relevant cues that enable participant to interact with the virtual environment [2931]. In this paper, we therefore follow definitions of presence and immersion as suggested by Witmer and colleagues [2931]. It has been shown that the PQ measure, designed by Witmer and colleagues [2931], addresses issues related to the level of involvement and control, the naturalness of the interaction, and the quality of the interface [2931, 61, 65]. All of these aspects directly relate to our hypothesis which is focused on the evaluation of substitute sensory cues in VR environment during task performance, and as such this measure was seen as an appropriate baseline measure for our short version of presence questionnaire.

The levels of fidelity in VR environment

It is generally assumed that high fidelity VR technologies are associated with better performance [13, 62]. At the same time, these technologies are often associated with a very high cost relative to their benefits [13, 52, 66, 67]. In the research community ‘fidelity’ is understood as a degree of similarity between real and simulated environment [52, 53, 60, 6264, 66, 67]. Empirical research investigating the quantifiable benefits of high fidelity VR technologies suggests that rather than enhancing the overall VR set up, the enhancement of one or more individual components in VR can also be beneficial to performance [13, 52, 65, 66, 67]. This implies that the overall fidelity of the virtual environment may not be as important factor for overall performance. For example, McMahan et al. [66] provided empirical evidence for the benefits of using high fidelity VR technologies, but found that object manipulation can be successfully performed with lower fidelity VR technologies, such as less costly displays, with no loss of efficiency. Similarly, Dahlstrom et al. [67] found that a high level of simulator fidelity has little or no effects on skill transfer. They suggested that lower fidelity simulation can reduce complexity and enhance focus on training and should be used to complement higher fidelity simulations.

Summary of experimental aims

In this study, task-relevant multisensory information is provided to represent cues that are not available in conventional VR simulations. The term ‘substitute cues’ is used to describe these sensory signals. This information may be presented in alternative modalities and may use cues that would not occur in real environments. We examine whether the available substituting sensory information improves task performance and users’ subjective experience, in particular their sense of presence in VR environment [57, 2931, 5156]. We hypothesize that substitute sensory cues will not be perceived as distraction; instead that substitute cueing will facilitate task performance and overall subjective experience of the users. We examine which sensory cues (audio, tactile, visual) are most effective for objective and subjective performance measures. Additionally, we also examine the role of modes of feedback (multisensory, bimodal, unimodal) on users performance. Furthermore, we establish whether a relationship between the users’ subjective experience and overall objective task performance exists.

Methods

Participants

The current study was approved by the University of Liverpool's Institute of Psychology and Health Sciences Ethics Committee (PSYC-1112–049A). The individual portrayed in one of the pictures in this manuscript and the individual appearing in the video (S1 Video) have given their written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details. Seventeen participants were recruited via opportunity sampling (12 male, 5 female, aged between 18 and 48, M = 26.7, SD = 12.4). All participants gave informed consent and reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision and normal hearing.

Apparatus

The experiment was conducted at the Virtual Engineering Centre (VEC) facility located in the Science and Technologies Facilities Council (STFC) in Daresbury, UK. The task was to interact within the virtual environment by holding a pneumatic tool (impact wrench) and perform a wheel change on a virtual racing car whilst substitute cues, presented in the form of visual, tactile and audio sensory feedback, provided additional task-relevant information.

Virtual reality set up

The virtual reality set up consisted of a planar display screen 6m in length and 2.1m in height, behind which were two active stereo projectors that create 3390 x 1200 resolution images at a rate of 120Hz. 3D stereo images were produced using an NVIDIA Quadro K6000 GPU. Participants wore wireless LCD shutter glasses that were synchronized with the projectors to provide stereoscopic images. 16 high-spec infrared cameras (VICON Bonita B10 with 250 fps capture speed, motion resolution of 0.5mm of translation and 0.5 degrees of rotation in a 4m x 4m volume using 9mm markers) were used to track object motion in the VR environment. Position data, computed using VICON Tracker software, was broadcast in real-time across the internal network using a VRPN protocol at a rate of 200Hz and used to update the virtual environment. The following objects were tracked in order to provide the required interaction within the virtual immersive environment: LCD shutter glasses (for head tracking and POV adjustment), haptic gloves on subject’s hands (to drive the subjects’ virtual hands) and the impact wrench (PLC Prestige 1/2”, weight = 1.3kg, 15.2cm long), the tool used to remove the bolts from the wheel (Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Virtual tools and set up.

Apparatus used in the experiment: (A) 3D shutter glasses, (B) impact wrench and (C) haptic gloves. Picture D shows the position of the wheel when the task is completed (first wheel on the stand, second wheel on the racing car).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191846.g001

The wheel change simulation was designed using the 3DVia system running at a constant speed of 75fps across all possible combinations of cues to ensure an accurate time recording in all experimental conditions. The virtual simulation consisted of a virtual racing car that was positioned on a stand in the middle of the screen. The virtual scene also contained two stands that are positioned 68cm in x direction, 14cm in y direction and 8cm in z direction from the centre point of the wheel on the virtual car. The stand on the right side of the car held the spare tyre and the stand on the left side of the car was free for participants to put the car tyre on. In order to pick the wheel up participants had to stand directly in front of the wheel, thus every participant had to make similar postural adjustments to get in contact with the wheel. A faithful digital mock-up of the impact wrench was used to interact with the bolts. Through accurate calibration, both hands and the impact wrench overlapped with their virtual counterparts from the participant’s perspective (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. View during the task.

The simulation view from a participants’ perspective: (E) Participants hand and tool were co-located with their virtual counterparts i.e. participants were not able to see virtual hands or tool (F) Participant wore 3D shutter glasses, headphones, vibration gloves and were holding impact wrench whilst performing the task.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191846.g002

Tactile feedback was provided by “tactile gloves” which had vibration motors attached to the back of the palm of each of the VICON hand tracking kits. The tactile gloves provided variable vibration cues, ranging from 15Hz (when participants hold the bolt) to 250Hz (the strongest vibration experienced when bolt is in), via a wireless interface with the simulation CPU. The vibration cues were designed to mimic the intensity of vibration generated by the impact wrench when performing the wheel change task. For example, the subject felt an intermediate level of vibration when screwing a bolt out or back in place (85Hz), which increased to the maximum level (250Hz) as soon as the bolt is completely screwed in, or reduced to zero when it is completely removed.

Audio feedback was presented via SONY headphones (RF811RK Wireless Headphones) with the frequency responses from 20 to 20 kHz. When audio cues were enabled they provided task-appropriate sounds, such as sampled recording of a torque wrench. When audio cues were disabled, a white noise was played to mask the sound generated by the vibration motors in the haptic gloves.

Task procedure

The experiment was conducted in a dedicated VR laboratory with no distraction, which is consistent with previous research where participants are engaged in their activity but not aware of the observers or other distractions [34]. Participants were explained the main definitions of the constructs that are being evaluated during the study. For the task, participants wore 3D shutter glasses, vibration gloves and headphones that played either audio cues (when they were on) or a continuous white noise to mask any vibration noise from the haptic gloves (Fig 2). The task was to change the wheel on the virtual racing car in the 3D environment as fast as possible. The substitute sensory cues were presented as unimodal, bimodal and multimodal feedback in a counterbalanced quasi-random order. Every participant started with two practice trials.

The time started when participants got in contact with the physical tool (impact wrench). First, they had to unscrew five bolts from the wheel on the virtual racing car. Then they had to put down the impact wrench, pick up the wheel and place it on the stand located on the right side of the racing car axle (distance 68cm). After this, they had to go and take the replacement wheel from the stand on the left side of the racing car, attach it on the racing car, take hold of the impact wrench and screw the bolts back in. The overall recording stopped when the participants placed the tool back on the table, which was located on the right side, approximately 1.5m from the projection display. Between each condition, participants’ subjective perceptions of the VR environment were recorded on a short questionnaire adapted from PQ measure [2931]. The video recording of the whole task can be found in S1 Video.

Multisensory feedback cues

A projection-based VR system, where visual information is always present, was used in this study. The substitute cues (visual, tactile, audio) provided additional information to compensate for other cues that are not readily available in VR environments, such as weight or torque.

Substitute visual cues consisted of bolts turning yellow when in contact with the tool and red when the bolts are completely in or out; the wheel turning yellow when in contact and red when in the correct position; the virtual hands of the participant turning yellow when in contact with virtual objects. Tactile cues were presented as vibration when: the tool was in contact with the bolt and a more intense vibration when the bolt was completely in or out; when the virtual hands were in contact with the wheel (carrying the wheel from the car to the stand and from the stand on to the car). Audio cues included a sampled impact wrench noise that was played when the virtual wrench was in contact with the bolt and a ‘snap’ sound when the wheel was placed on the stand and on the car.

Objective and subjective performance measures

In this study overall task performance was used as an objective measure. Participants were instructed to perform the task as fast as they can. The mean completion times for each condition were used in the statistical analysis. As an incentive, a leader board was displayed within the virtual scene where the participants’ fastest times were shown and updated after each participant had completed the task.

In order to investigate participants’ experience, participants were required to fill in a short questionnaire after each experimental condition. The selected dimension for the presence measure was a continuous scale. The questionnaire contained seven items measuring the sense of involvement and immersion (5 questions), enjoyment (1 question) and general discomfort (1 question). The questions concerning the perceived sense of involvement and immersion were adapted from Presence Questionnaire (PQ) developed by Witmer and Singer [2931] to collectively capture the ‘the sense of being present inside’ the VR environment. All adapted questions were transformed into statements and participants were asked to mark on the line (0-not at all, to 10-completely) how strongly they agree or disagree with the statements after each sensory condition. Higher responses on the questions concerning the levels of involvement and immersion indicated an increased sense of presence [2931]. The statements were as follows:

The interaction with the environment seems natural (control factor);

I felt that sensory cues helped me in completing the assigned task (sensory factor);

The sense of immersion inside the virtual environment was compelling (realism/immersion factor);

The virtual environment felt real (realism/ immersion factor);

The virtual environment was responsive to my actions (distraction factor).

To capture immediate attitudes about presented sensory cues two additional statements were included:

Doing the task felt enjoyable (enjoyment);

I experienced general discomfort (discomfort).

Although no questions directly addressed the state of ‘being in the virtual environment’ we assume that they collectively measure the construct known as ‘presence’, as the questions were adopted from a 4-factor model of presence derived from the factor analysis of the Presence Questionnaire (PQ) [2931]. Reliability analysis of the questionnaire is reported in the results section.

Experimental design

This study adopted a repeated measures within-subject factorial (2x2x2, presentation of substitute audio x tactile x visual) design. A single group of participants performed the task in all conditions in a quasi-random sequence. There were eight possible combinations of sensory feedback provided: audio (A), visual (V), tactile (T), audio-visual (AV), audio-tactile (AT), tactile-visual (TV) and audio-visual-tactile (AVT) as well as condition where no additional cues were presented (NONE). The task completion times were logarithmically transformed to satisfy the normality assumption (Shapiro-Wilk) for statistical analysis. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed on mean completion times to investigate the effects of each sensory modality: visual, audio and tactile cues were set as binary factors that were either present or absent. The Mauchly test of sphericity was applied and when significant, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were adopted. Partial eta squared is reported for effect sizes. Paired sample t tests, with correction for multiple comparisons (Sidak), were conducted to investigate whether the availability of sensory modality affected participants’ behaviour and performance. When the direction of the relationship was predicted, one tailed test results were reported. The significance level for all statistical tests was set at 0.05. For paired sample t tests, Cohen’s d [68] was chosen as a measure of effect size, which is calculated as the difference between two means divided by pooled standard deviation. The accepted suggestions for the magnitude of effect sizes are: 0.2 = small effect, 0.3 = medium effect and 0.5 = large effect [68].

Results

Objective measures

It took participants 50.3 seconds (SE = 1.9) to complete the virtual wheel change task on average. The mean task completion times in each sensory cue condition are shown on Fig 3. The order of conditions that facilitated performance the most is: ATV (M = 46.9, SE = 2.03), AV (M = 49.1, SE = 1.54), AT (M = 49.5, SE = 2.9), TV (M = 50.2, SE = 2.36), A (M = 49.7, SE = 2.17), T (M = 49.9, SE = 2.06), V (M = 52.3, SE = 3.17) and NONE (M = 55.1, SE = 3.4).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Objective performance data.

Means and standard errors in each condition for mean completion times. Participants were asked to perform the task as fast as they can; therefore shorter completion times indicate better performance. The data indicate that as the amount of sensory cues in the simulation increased, the task was completed faster.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191846.g003

A (2x2x2) repeated measures ANOVA revealed that there were significant main effects in objective performance caused by the presentation of audio (F(1,16) = 5.87, p = 0.028, η2 = 0.27) and tactile cues (F(1,16) = 4.48, p = 0.043, η2 = 0.24), but not for additional visual cues (F(1,16) = 0.433, p = 0.52, η2 = 0.03). No significant interactions between modalities were observed. Planned comparisons using Sidak adjustment (p < 0.02) were performed on data that were separated into groups where each sensory modality was either present or absent. The analysis revealed that the task was performed significantly faster when audio cues were present (M = 48.8, SE = 1.61; t(16) = -2.324, p = 0.015, d = 0.36, one tailed) compared to conditions when white noise was played (M = 51.9, SE = 2.42). Significant differences were also observed for tactile modality; the mean time to complete the task was significantly faster when the tactile cues were present (M = 49.6, SE = 1.85; t(16) = -2.044, p = 0.025, d = 0.29, one tailed) as opposed to trials when tactile cues were absent (M = 51.6, SE = 2.21). No significant difference was observed on mean completion times for additional information bearing visual cues (M = 49.6, SE = 1.86; t(16) = -1.027, p = 0.15, d = 0.17, one tailed) vs (M = 51.1, SE = 2.23) (Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Performance data for each sensory cue.

To visualise the data easily, all sensory cues were separated into groups when each of the cues were present or absent. The graphs display the mean completion times and standard errors for (A) audio, (B) tactile and (C) visual cues. Paired sample t-tests with Sidak correction revealed a significant effect of audio (p = 0.015) and tactile cues (p = 0.025). No significant differences were found for visual cues (p = 0.15).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191846.g004

To further investigate which form of sensory feedback was most beneficial to the overall task performance, pairwise comparisons on mean completion times in each mode of feedback were conducted. The data were grouped into four modes of feedback (unimodal, bimodal, multimodal and no feedback) and the means across the groups were used for the analysis (Fig 5). Repeated measures ANOVA with Greenhouse-Geisser correction revealed a significant effect of mode of feedback (F(3,48) = 5.164, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.24). Planned comparisons with Sidak adjustment (p < 0.01) revealed that the task was performed significantly faster with multimodal feedback (M = 46.9, SE = 2.04) as opposed to no feedback (M = 55.2, SE = 3.4; t(16) = -2.884, p = 0.005, d = 0.7). There was no significant difference between multimodal feedback (M = 46.9, SE = 2.04) and unimodal feedback (M = 50.7, SE = 2.26, p = 0.02, d = 0.5) or bimodal feedback (M = 49.7, SE = 1.8; p = 0.03, d = 0.47). The difference in mean times between the no feedback condition (M = 55.2, SE = 3.4) and bimodal feedback (M = 49.7, SE = 1.8) was recorded significant after adjustment (t (16) = -2.361, p = 0.015, d = 0.57); no significance was obtained with unimodal feedback (M = 50.7, SE = 2.26; p = 0.03, d = 0.48) or between bimodal (M = 49.7, SE = 1.8) and unimodal feedback (M = 50.7, SE = 2.26; p = 0.25, d = 0.16).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Effect of feedback modes on objective data.

Means and standard errors of overall completion times for each mode of feedback: multimodal, bimodal, unimodal, and no feedback. Data for bimodal (AV, AT, TV) and unimodal (A, T, V) category were group together and the mean scores were used for the analysis. A significant difference was observed between the multimodal and no feedback condition (p = 0.005) and the difference between no feedback and the bimodal feedback was significance after adjustment (p = 0.017).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191846.g005

Subjective measures

In order to assess user acceptability of the virtual environment, participants were asked to rate their sense of involvement and immersion using a short questionnaire after each experimental condition. The questions were adapted from a 4-factor model of presence, proposed by Witmer and colleagues [2931] who suggested that both involvement and immersion are necessary to experience an increased level of presence. The reliability of the rating scale used in this study was assessed by calculating the Cronbach coefficient—alpha [55]. The standardised alpha of rating scales showed acceptable reliability (sense of presence = .97). The overall mean rating score was 6.65 (SE = 1.74). The mean subjective ratings in each condition can be seen on Fig 6. The order of conditions that were perceived as the most compelling is as follows: ATV (M = 7.51, SE = 0.37), AT (M = 7.08, SE = 0.41), AV (M = 6.81, SE = 0.38), TV (M = 6.81, SE = 0.38), A (M = 6.68, SE = 0.38), T (M = 6.52, SE = 0.31), V (M = 6.22, SE = 0.41) and NONE (M = 4.91, SE = 0.36).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Subjective rating scores.

The mean subjective ratings and standard errors of presence ratings in all experimental conditions. Participants were asked to rate on a continuous scale from 0 (not at all) to 10 (completely) how strongly they agree or disagree with the statement. All questions were formulated in a positive manner, thus higher scores indicate an enhanced sense of presence.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191846.g006

The analysis of the subjective data was performed analogous to the analysis of the objective data. The analysis with Greenhouse-Greissler correction determined that all sensory modalities significantly influenced subjective ratings: there was a main effect of audio modality (F(1,16) = 33.45, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.68), a main effect of tactile modality (F(1,16) = 20.34, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.56) and a main effect of visual modality (F(1,16) = 15.74, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.5). Significant interactions between sensory modalities were also recorded between audio and tactile (F(1,16) = 5.19, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.25); audio and visual (F(1,16) = 6.34, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.28); tactile and visual (F(1,16) = 10.10, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.39) and audio, tactile and visual (F(1,16) = 15.07, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.49). Planned comparisons using Sidak adjustment (p < 0.01) were computed on mean scores where each modality was either present or absent (Fig 7). The results revealed that participants experienced a higher sense of presence when audio cues were present (M = 7.02, SE = 0.36) as opposed to absent (M = 6.11, SE = 0.32; t (16) = 5.774, p < 0.001, d = 1.39). The same effect was seen for tactile cues (M = 6.98, SE = 0.34) versus (M = 6.15, SE = 0.35; t(16) = 4.51, p < 0.001, d = 1.06)), and for visual cues (M = 6.84, SE = 0.35) versus (M = 6.29, SE = 0.33; t(16) = 3.961, p = 0.001, d = 1.03).

thumbnail
Fig 7. Subjective ratings for each sensory cue.

The means and standard errors for subjective ratings of presence: (A) audio, (B) tactile and (C) visual cues. Panel A, B and C shows the same data as panel D, however they are in a different order to aid a visual comparison. Significant effects in all three sensory modalities were found (D).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191846.g007

The effects of different modes of feedback on subjective ratings were also examined (Fig 8). Repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant effect of mode of feedback (F(3,48) = 37.350, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.7). Planned comparisons with Sidak adjustment (p < 0.01) revealed that the subjective ratings of presence were significantly higher when multimodal feedback was presented (M = 7.51, SE = 0.37) as compared to bimodal feedback (M = 6.91, SE = 0.37, t(16) = 3.794, p = 0.002, d = 0.86), unimodal feedback (M = 6.48, SE = 0.35; t(16) = 4.964, p < 0.001, d = 1.25) and no feedback (M = 4.89, SE = 0.37; t(16) = 7.210, p < 0.001, d = 1.79). Subjective ratings also differed significantly between no feedback (M = 4.89, SE = 0.37) and unimodal feedback conditions (M = 6.48, SE = 0.35; t(16) = 6.717, p < 0.001, d = 1.51) and between no feedback and bimodal feedback conditions (M = 6.91, SE = 0.37, t(16) = 5.809, p < 0.001, d = 1.44). The difference between bimodal (M = 6.91, SE = 0.37) and unimodal feedback conditions (M = 6.48, SE = 0.35) was also recorded significant (t(16) = 2.651, p = 0.017, d = 0.75).

thumbnail
Fig 8. Effects of feedback modes on subjective ratings.

The graph shows means and standard errors of subjective ratings in each mode of feedback.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191846.g008

The main focus of this study was to evaluate the contribution of substitute cues to the overall task performance and user experience. Previous research suggested that the experienced sense of involvement and immersion, collectively also referred to as the sense of presence [2931], might impact user’s ability to perform a task [53, 60]. One of the goals of this study was to investigate whether the user’s subjective sense of presence, as measured by the questions addressing involvement and immersion, is related to their overall task performance. In order to investigate this relationship the Pearson correlation analyses were performed. The Fig 9 shows data points for all participants in all sensory conditions.

thumbnail
Fig 9. Objective and subjective data for all participants in every sensory condition.

Reddish colour indicates that as the amount of sensory cues increased (red and orange colours) the overall performance improved.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191846.g009

A fundamental problem when investigating relationship between (subjective) self-evaluation and (objective) performance data is in the intrinsic inter-individual variability that is present in the subjective data [69]. While (objective) performance is measured relative to an external and common standard (the time to complete the task), the subjective measure (for example presence ratings) relies on internal scales that are unique to each observer. This means that individual subjective ratings can only reflect the relative changes between conditions that are experienced by the users and that there is no common subjective standard or common range of responses across participants. Individuals can, however, reliably judge relative subjective changes in their experience across the conditions they experience.

To illustrate this we selected data from three participants (Fig 10). The graph shows objective performance and subjective ratings for each of the eight conditions that were tested. A straight line fit shows the overall relationship between objective and subjective measure. This negative relationship between task completion time (in seconds) and presence judgements is seen for all participants. The subjective ratings however show significant idiosyncratic differences: participant 1 (S1, orange), for example, gave consistently low ratings for presence (around 3/10), while participant 2 (S2, green) and participant 3 (S3, blue) gave consistently much higher ratings (around 8/10) although the objective performance range was comparable. The subjective ratings for S2 and S3 are comparable although there is no overlap in their objective performance (S2 took at most 50 secs to complete the task while S3 took at least 55 secs). Participants have no access to a common standard for their subjective evaluation; it is therefore not surprising that there are significant variations in the mean ratings. While the objective conditions that each participant experiences are the same, there is no reason to expect that the range in objective differences between conditions will result in comparable ranges of subjective experience. All three subjects, in common with the majority of all of our subjects however, show a significant correlation between their individual subjective ratings and objective performance.

thumbnail
Fig 10. Correlations.

Correlational analyses on individual data collected from each participant. Participant 1 (S1), participant 2 (S2) and participant 3 (S3) show comparable performance but highly idiosyncratic differences in their subjective ratings.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191846.g010

To correlate the objective and subjective measures across the entire pool of participants the idiosyncratic differences between participants have to be resolved. We suggest that there are two ways to approach this: one approach is to normalise the individual raw data; a second approach is to correlate the pooled mean of the data across conditions. The correlations of the normalised data are presented here; correlations of the raw data and mean data (from each sensory condition) are provided in S1, S2, S3 and S4 Files. All objective and subjective data recorded in this study have been normalised so that every subject had same common mean (0) and same standard deviation (1). The results showed consistent negative regression lines between objective and subjective measures in every participant; for ten out of seventeen subjects the two measures were significantly correlated. The negative correlation implies that when participants experienced an increased sense of involvement and immersion, commonly referred to as the sense of presence [2931], their overall task performance improved i.e. they performed the task faster. A correlation analysis across all participants and all conditions shows a strong significant negative correlation (r = -0.485, p < 0.001) between the performance measures and users’ perceived sense of involvement and immersion within the VR environment (Fig 11).

thumbnail
Fig 11. Correlation.

The correlational analysis was performed on the normalised data across all participants in every sensory condition that was tested (r = -0.485, n = 17, p < 0.001).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191846.g011

Discussion

This study investigated how additional substitute multisensory cues affect human objective performance and subjective evaluation during the completion of a realistic task in a VR environment. During the wheel change task different combinations of substitute sensory cues that carried task-relevant information were provided in order to aid performance.

Our principal finding is that the time to complete the task, our objective performance measure, significantly reduces when additional information-bearing cues are presented. The subjective user experience, recorded through a short questionnaire adopted from previous research [2931] also shows significant improvements in ratings. The substitute cues that were presented were information bearing cue, but not natural cue; they therefore have a negative impact on surface fidelity and, following this, might be expected to cause a reduction in the sense of presence probed in the subjective data. Our data shows that there is no direct positive relationship between surface fidelity and objective performance or subjective ratings in our task. A possible explanation is that the improvement in informational fidelity outweighs the loss in surface fidelity. Previous studies have shown that increasing the fidelity of various components of VR environment, rather than overall VR set up, could be beneficial to performance [13, 52, 66, 67]. Our findings support this research and further imply that some tasks in VR environment can be successfully performed in low fidelity VR environments with no loss on efficiency [14, 33].

The advantages of auditory and tactile feedback have been noted in previous research; for example, it was reported that audio feedback can improve task performance in terms of accuracy and spatial attention without affecting perceptual workload [11, 15, 16, 39, 4045, 49, 50]. Similarly, haptic feedback was shown to improve interaction, spatial guidance and learning in VR environments [24, 2325, 50]. The analysis of the objective measures in this study revealed significant effects of substitute cues that were presented in the audio and tactile modalities; however the effect of cue substitution in the visual modality did not reach significance. This finding may be explained by the visual information load [25, 38]: during the visual condition no meaningful audio or tactile signals were provided so that substitute cues increase the load in a single modality.

Previous research has reported some benefits of multimodal interfaces i.e. for spatial understanding [5], accuracy of performance [37, 39], in gaming environments [64] and enhanced performance in dynamic threat scenarios [14]. Similarly, in this study we show that multimodal feedback improved overall task performance as well as enhanced the levels of presence and immersion. Our data are consistent with previous findings suggesting that appropriate sensory cues can improve task performance [57, 14, 37, 39, 64]. One surprising finding stemming from this research was the effectiveness of audio and tactile feedback when presented unimodally. It has been noted that audio and tactile feedback can be perceived as distracting and annoying and can even decrease the accuracy of performance [20, 33]. However, in our study unimodal audio and tactile feedback influenced performance and subjective ratings positively. Individually, the mean time for audio condition was 49.7 seconds and for tactile it was 49.9 seconds. In comparison to bimodal conditions i.e. audio-visual (AV = 49.1s), audio-tactile (AT = 49.5s) and tactile-visual (TV = 50.2s) it can be seen that audio and tactile feedback facilitated performance in the similar manner as bimodal feedback. This suggests that the presentation of audio and tactile feedback alone was as effective in supporting task performance as bimodal feedback presentations.

The analysis of the subjective ratings of user’s perception in VR environments showed similarities to the objective measures of performance. Cue substitution in all modalities and the interactions between them had a significant effect on perceived sense of immersion and involvement, collectively known as presence [2931]. Participants rated the multimodal cues as most effective in enhancing their sense of presence, followed by bimodal and then by unimodal substitute cues. The magnitude of these effects is significant and is confirmed by the effect sizes in our analysis, thus confirming that the availability of sensory cues during the VR interaction can influence users’ perception in VR environments [34, 40, 52]. When each mode of feedback is compared, the difference between bimodal and a unimodal feedback is minimal: this may be due to the fact that data were combined to form a unimodal category. The current study was not designed to show these particular differences, but one possible explanation for the difference between the modes of feedback may be due to the insufficient power in the experiment.

To investigate whether the objective performance was related to the users’ subjective experience, mean performance times and mean subjective ratings were used for the correlational analysis. The correlational analysis performed across all normalised data revealed a significant negative relationship between the objective performance measures (time to complete the task) and the users’ subjective experience. As previous studies have suggested [24, 33, 34, 42, 45, 52, 55, 62], these results are consistent with the claim that increased presence is correlated with higher performance in VR environments.

Conclusion

In order to support training and performance in VR environments it is essential to provide necessary sensory cues that are required for the task. The results from our study show that substitute multimodal sensory feedback that might detract from the overall fidelity of VR environment can enhance overall task performance as well as the users’ perceived sense of presence. Tactile and auditory cues emerged as particularly useful substitute cues; they provided additional information in an efficient manner without being distractive, especially when participants needed to reach and grasp the objects during the VR interaction. We showed that tactile cues work well in combination with other cues (visual and audio), however as some differences between unimodal and bimodal signals were significant, a further investigation into the differential effects of these cues on user’s objective and subjective performance is encouraged. The results from correlational analyses showed that user’s subjective experience is related to the overall task performance: participants performed the task much faster and reported an increased sense of presence during the VR interaction. The correlation between the task performance and presence was expected as both of the factors have underlying cause of enhanced input. More detailed analysis, such as path analysis or partial correlation analysis, could determine the stability of this relationship; therefore future studies are encouraged to include these analyses to investigate whether the relationship between the factors is maintained. Additionally, our study showed that the use of subjective and objective measures for the evaluation of VR environments is worthwhile and is therefore recommended for future studies.

The substitute cues presented in this study were highly unrealistic; however they provided task-relevant information. The main implication stemming from this research implies that the addition of substitute sensory cues into the virtual environment can be beneficial even when the overall fidelity on the VR environment is decreased. Understanding of factors and conditions in multisensory cuing, under which VR users experience an enhanced sense of presence and increased performance, can help designers to allocate computational resources proportionally when building future designs of virtual systems with multimodal feedback [14, 33, 15, 18, 4245, 48].

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by ESRC Case award (North West DTC ES/J500094/1), funder website http://www.esrc.ac.uk/. The funders had no role in study design, data collection or analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript. The findings of this study have been previously presented at the European Conference of Virtual Reality, EURO VR 2015, Lecco, Italy.

References

  1. 1. Pietschmann D, Rusdorf S. Matching levels of task difficulty for different modes of presentation in a VR table tennis simulation by using assistance functions and regression analysis. In International Conference on Virtual, Augmented and Mixed Reality 2014 Jun 22 (pp. 406–417). Springer International Publishing.
  2. 2. Van Erp JB, Van Veen HA. Vibrotactile in-vehicle navigation system. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour. 2004 Sep 30;7(4):247–56.
  3. 3. Ramsamy P, Haffegee A, Jamieson R, Alexandrov V. Using haptics to improve immersion in virtual environments. In International Conference on Computational Science 2006 May 28 (pp. 603–609). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
  4. 4. Burke JL, Prewett MS, Gray AA, Yang L, Stilson FR, Coovert MD, et al. Comparing the effects of visual-auditory and visual-tactile feedback on user performance: a meta-analysis. In Proceedings of the 8th international conference on Multimodal interfaces 2006 Nov 2 (pp. 108–117). ACM.
  5. 5. Slater M, Linakis V, Usoh M, Kooper R, Street G. Immersion, presence, and performance in virtual environments: An experiment with tri-dimensional chess. In ACM virtual reality software and technology (VRST) 1996 Jul 1 (pp. 163–172). New York, NY: ACM Press.
  6. 6. Slater M. Measuring presence: A response to the Witmer and Singer presence questionnaire. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments. 1999 Oct;8(5):560–5.
  7. 7. Slater M. A note on presence terminology. Presence connect. 2003 Jan;3(3):1–5.
  8. 8. Coulter R, Saland L, Caudell T, Goldsmith TE, Alverson D. The effect of degree of immersion upon learning performance in virtual reality simulations for medical education. InMedicine Meets Virtual Reality 2007 Jan 18 (Vol. 15, p. 155).
  9. 9. Dede C. Immersive interfaces for engagement and learning. Science. 2009 Jan 2;323 (5910):66–9. pmid:19119219
  10. 10. Gurusamy K, Aggarwal R, Palanivelu L, Davidson BR. Systematic review of randomized controlled trials on the effectiveness of virtual reality training for laparoscopic surgery. British Journal of Surgery. 2008 Sep 1;95(9):1088–97. pmid:18690637
  11. 11. Meyer GF, Wong LT, Timson E, Perfect P, White MD. Objective fidelity evaluation in multisensory virtual environments: auditory cue fidelity in flight simulation. PloS one. 2012 Sep 5;7 (9):e44381. pmid:22957068
  12. 12. Seymour NE, Gallagher AG, Roman SA, O’Brien MK, Bansal VK, Andersen DK, et al. Virtual reality training improves operating room performance: results of a randomized, double-blinded study. Annals of surgery. 2002 Oct;236(4):458. pmid:12368674
  13. 13. Bowman DA, McMahan RP. Virtual reality: how much immersion is enough? Computer. 2007 Jul;40 (7):36–43.
  14. 14. Richard P, Chamaret D, Inglese FX, Lucidarme P, Ferrier JL. Human-Scale Virtual Environment for Product Design: Effect of Sensory Substitution. IJVR. 2006 Jun;5(2):37–44.
  15. 15. Martínez J, Martínez D, Molina JP, Garcia A. Comparison of force and vibrotactile feedback with direct stimulation for texture recognition. In Cyberworlds (CW), 2011 International Conference on 2011 Oct 4 (pp. 62–68). IEEE.
  16. 16. Schoonmaker RE, Cao CG. Vibrotactile feedback enhances force perception in minimally invasive surgery. InProceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 2006 Oct 1 (Vol. 50, No. 10, pp. 1029–1033). SAGE Publications.
  17. 17. Akamatsu M, MacKenzie IS, Hasbroucq T. A comparison of tactile, auditory, and visual feedback in a pointing task using a mouse-type device. Ergonomics. 1995 Apr 1;38(4):816–27. pmid:7729406
  18. 18. Hopp PJ, Smith CA, Clegg BA, Heggestad ED. Interruption management: The use of attention-directing tactile cues. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. 2005 Mar 1;47(1):1–1.
  19. 19. Adams RJ, Klowden D, Hannaford B. Virtual training for a manual assembly task. Haptics-e, 2001, 2(2), 1–7
  20. 20. Feintuch U, Raz L, Hwang J, Josman N, Katz N, Kizony R, et al. Integrating haptic-tactile feedback into a video-capture-based virtual environment for rehabilitation. CyberPsychology & Behavior. 2006 Apr 1;9(2):129–32
  21. 21. Meyer GF, Shao F, White MD, Hopkins C, Robotham AJ. Modulation of visually evoked postural responses by contextual visual, haptic and auditory information: a ‘virtual reality check’. PloS one. 2013 Jun 28;8(6):e67651. pmid:23840760
  22. 22. Weber B, Schätzle S, Hulin T, Preusche C, Deml B. Evaluation of a vibrotactile feedback device for spatial guidance. In World Haptics Conference (WHC), 2011 IEEE 2011 Jun 21 (pp. 349–354). IEEE.
  23. 23. Spelmezan D, Jacobs M, Hilgers A, Borchers J. Tactile motion instructions for physical activities. InProceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems 2009 Apr 4 (pp. 2243–2252). ACM.
  24. 24. Van Der Linden J, Johnson R, Bird J, Rogers Y, Schoonderwaldt E. Buzzing to play: lessons learned from an in the wild study of real-time vibrotactile feedback. InProceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human factors in Computing Systems 2011 May 7 (pp. 533–542). ACM.
  25. 25. Ho C, Tan HZ, Spence C. Using spatial vibrotactile cues to direct visual attention in driving scenes. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour. 2005 Nov 30;8(6):397–412
  26. 26. Vitense HS, Jacko JA, Emery VK. Multimodal feedback: an assessment of performance and mental workload. Ergonomics. 2003 Jan 1;46(1–3):68–87 pmid:12554399
  27. 27. Richard P, Coiffet P. Human perceptual issues in virtual environments: sensory substitution and information redundancy. In Robot and Human Communication, 1995. RO-MAN'95 TOKYO, Proceedings., 4th IEEE International Workshop on 1995 Jul 5 (pp. 301–306). IEEE.
  28. 28. Ferwerda JA. Three varieties of realism in computer graphics. InHuman Vision and Electronic Imaging 2003 Jan 21 (pp. 290–297).
  29. 29. Witmer BG, Singer MF. Measuring presence in virtual environments. ARMY RESEARCH INST FOR THE BEHAVIORAL AND SOCIAL SCIENCES ALEXANDRIA VA; 1994 Oct.
  30. 30. Witmer BG, Singer MJ. Measuring presence in virtual environments: A presence questionnaire. Presence: Teleoperators and virtual environments. 1998 Jun;7(3):225–40.
  31. 31. Witmer BG, Jerome CJ. Singer MJ. The factor structure of the presence questionnaire. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 2005 14(3): 298–312.
  32. 32. Ma R, Kaber DB. Presence, workload and performance effects of synthetic environment design factors. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies. 2006 Jun 30;64 (6):541–52.
  33. 33. Schuchardt P, Bowman DA. The benefits of immersion for spatial understanding of complex underground cave systems. In Proceedings of the 2007 ACM symposium on Virtual reality software and technology 2007 Nov 5 (pp. 121–124). ACM.
  34. 34. Faas D, Bao Q, Frey DD, Yang MC. The influence of immersion and presence in early stage engineering designing and building. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing. 2014 May 1;28(02):139–51.
  35. 35. Jerome CJ. Orienting of visual-spatial attention with augmented reality: Effects of spatial and non-spatial multi-modal cues (Doctoral dissertation, University of Central Florida Orlando, Florida).
  36. 36. Jeon M, Davison BK, Nees MA, Wilson J, Walker BN. Enhanced auditory menu cues improve dual task performance and are preferred with in-vehicle technologies. In Proceedings of the 1st international conference on automotive user interfaces and interactive vehicular applications 2009 Sep 21 (pp. 91–98). ACM.
  37. 37. Jacko J, Emery VK, Edwards PJ, Ashok M, Barnard L, Kongnakorn T, et al. The effects of multimodal feedback on older adults' task performance given varying levels of computer experience. Behaviour & Information Technology. 2004 Jul 1;23(4):247–64.
  38. 38. Ho C, Spence C. Assessing the effectiveness of various auditory cues in capturing a driver's visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 2005 Sep;11(3):157. pmid:16221035
  39. 39. Meyer GF, Wuerger SM, Röhrbein F, Zetzsche C. Low-level integration of auditory and visual motion signals requires spatial co-localisation. Experimental Brain Research, 2005 166, 538–547. pmid:16143858
  40. 40. Santangelo V, Spence C. Multisensory cues capture spatial attention regardless of perceptual load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2007 Dec;33(6):1311. pmid:18085945
  41. 41. Hecht D, Reiner M, Karni A. Enhancement of response times to bi-and tri-modal sensory stimuli during active movements. Experimental Brain Research. 2008 Mar 1;185 (4):655. pmid:17992522
  42. 42. Jia D, Bhatti A, Nahavandi S. User-centered design and evaluation of an interactive visual-haptic-auditory interface: a user study on assembly. In ASME 2011 World Conference on Innovative Virtual Reality 2011 January, (pp. 263–272). American Society of Mechanical Engineers.
  43. 43. Lee M, Billinghurst M, Baek W, Green R, Woo W. A usability study of multimodal input in an augmented reality environment. Virtual Reality 2013 17(4), pp.293–305.
  44. 44. Shi Y, Ruiz N, Taib R, Choi E, Chen F. Galvanic skin response (GSR) as an index of cognitive load. In CHI'07 extended abstracts on Human factors in computing systems 2007 April. (pp. 2651–2656). ACM.
  45. 45. Oviatt S, Coulston R, Lunsford R. When do we interact multimodal? Cognitive load and multimodal communication patterns. In Proceedings of the 6th international conference on Multimodal interfaces 2004 October (pp. 129–136). ACM
  46. 46. Massie TH, Salisbury JK. The phantom haptic interface: A device for probing virtual objects. InProceedings of the ASME winter annual meeting, symposium on haptic interfaces for virtual environment and teleoperator systems 1994 Nov 20 (Vol. 55, No. 1, pp. 295–300).
  47. 47. Oakley B. The effects of multimodal feedback and age on a mouse-pointing task (Doctoral dissertation, University of Central Florida Orlando, Florida).
  48. 48. Viaud-Delmon I, Warusfel O, Seguelas A, Rio E, Jouvent R. High sensitivity to multisensory conflicts in agoraphobia exhibited by virtual reality. European Psychiatry 2006 21(7), pp.501–508. pmid:17055951
  49. 49. Chebat DR, Maidenbaum S, Amedi A. Navigation using sensory substitution in real and virtual mazes. PloS one. 2015 Jun 3;10(6):e0126307. pmid:26039580
  50. 50. D’Alonzo M, Cipriani C. Vibrotactile sensory substitution elicits feeling of ownership of an alien hand. PloS one. 2012 Nov 30;7(11):e50756. pmid:23226375
  51. 51. Bystrom KE, Barfield W, Hendrix C. A conceptual model of the sense of presence in virtual environments. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments. 1999 Apr;8(2):241–4.
  52. 52. Laha B, Sensharma K, Schiffbauer JD, Bowman DA. Effects of immersion on visual analysis of volume data. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics. 2012 Apr;18(4):597–606. pmid:22402687
  53. 53. Stevens JA, Kincaid JP. The Relationship between Presence and Performance in Virtual Simulation Training. Open Journal of Modelling and Simulation. 2015 Mar 23;3(02):41.
  54. 54. Lombard M, Ditton T. At the heart of it all: The concept of presence. Journal of Computer‐Mediated Communication. 1997 Sep 1;3(2):0-.
  55. 55. Barfield W, Hendrix C, Bjorneseth O, Kaczmarek KA, Lotens W. Comparison of human sensory capabilities with technical specifications of virtual environment equipment. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 1995 4(4): pp.329–356.
  56. 56. Ch Youngblut. Experience of presence in virtual environments. No. IDA-D-2960. Institute for Defense Analyses Alexandria, VA, Sept 2003.
  57. 57. Jennett C, Cox AL, Cairns P, Dhoparee S, Epps A, Tijs T, et al. Measuring and defining the experience of immersion in games. International journal of human-computer studies. 2008 Sep 30;66(9):641–61.58
  58. 58. Cairns P, Cox AL, Berthouze N, Jennett C, Dhoparee S. Quantifying the experience of immersion in games. InCogSci 2006 Workshop: Cognitive Science of Games and Gameplay 2006.
  59. 59. Youngblut C., Perrin BM. Investigating the relationship between presence and performance in virtual environments. Paper presented at IMAGE Conference, 2002, Arizona.
  60. 60. Youngblut C, Huie O. The relationship between presence and performance in virtual environments: Results of a VERTS study. In Virtual Reality, 2003. Proceedings. IEEE 2003 Mar 22 (pp. 277–278). IEEE.
  61. 61. Nystad E, Sebok A. A comparison of two presence measures based on experimental results. In Proceedings of the seventh international workshop on presence, 2004 (pp. 266–273
  62. 62. Dalley P, Robinson B, Weller J, Caldwell C. The use of high-fidelity human patient simulation and the introduction of new anesthesia delivery systems. Anesthesia & Analgesia. 2004 Dec 1;99(6):1737–1741.
  63. 63. Dinh HQ, Walker N, Hodges LF, Song C, Kobayashi A. Evaluating the importance of multi-sensory input on memory and the sense of presence in virtual environments. In Virtual Reality, 1999. Proceedings., IEEE 1999 Mar 13 (pp. 222–228). IEEE.
  64. 64. Nam CS, Shu J, Chung D. The roles of sensory modalities in collaborative virtual environments (CVEs). Computers in Human Behaviour. 2008 Jul 31;24(4):1404–17.
  65. 65. Carassa A, Morganti F, Tirassa M. Movement, action, and situation: Presence in virtual environments. In M. Alcañiz Raya & B. Rey Solaz (Eds.), Proceedings of Presence 2004 (pp. 7–12). Valencia, Spain: Editorial Universidad Politécnica
  66. 66. McMahan RP, Gorton D, Gresock J, McConnell W, Bowman DA. Separating the effects of level of immersion and 3D interaction techniques. In Proceedings of the ACM symposium on Virtual reality software and technology 2006 Nov 1 (pp. 108–111). ACM.
  67. 67. Dahlstrom N, Dekker S, Van Winsen R, Nyce J. Fidelity and validity of simulator training. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science. 2009 Jul 1;10(4):305–14.
  68. 68. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis. Current directions in psychological science 1992 1(3) pp.98–101.
  69. 69. Bland JM, Altman DG. Calculating correlation coefficients with repeated observations: Part 2—Correlation between subjects. British medical journal 1995. 310(6980), p.633. pmid:7703752