Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Inattention in primary school is not good for your future school achievement—A pattern classification study

  • Astri J. Lundervold ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft

    astri.lundervold@uib.no

    Affiliations Department of Biological and Medical Psychology University of Bergen, 5009 Bergen, Norway, K.G. Jebsen Center for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway

  • Tormod Bøe,

    Roles Data curation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Regional Centre for Child and Youth Mental Health and Child Welfare, Uni Research Health, Bergen, Norway

  • Arvid Lundervold

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Neuroinformatics and Image Analysis Laboratory, Neural Networks Research Group, Department of Biomedicine, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway

Abstract

Inattention in childhood is associated with academic problems later in life. The contribution of specific aspects of inattentive behaviour is, however, less known. We investigated feature importance of primary school teachers’ reports on nine aspects of inattentive behaviour, gender and age in predicting future academic achievement. Primary school teachers of n = 2491 children (7–9 years) rated nine items reflecting different aspects of inattentive behaviour in 2002. A mean academic achievement score from the previous semester in high school (2012) was available for each youth from an official school register. All scores were at a categorical level. Feature importances were assessed by using multinominal logistic regression, classification and regression trees analysis, and a random forest algorithm. Finally, a comprehensive pattern classification procedure using k-fold cross-validation was implemented. Overall, inattention was rated as more severe in boys, who also obtained lower academic achievement scores in high school than girls. Problems related to sustained attention and distractibility were together with age and gender defined as the most important features to predict future achievement scores. Using these four features as input to a collection of classifiers employing k-fold cross-validation for prediction of academic achievement level, we obtained classification accuracy, precision and recall that were clearly better than chance levels. Primary school teachers’ reports of problems related to sustained attention and distractibility were identified as the two most important features of inattentive behaviour predicting academic achievement in high school. Identification and follow-up procedures of primary school children showing these characteristics should be prioritised to prevent future academic failure.

Introduction

Inattention in early childhood has been linked to a wide range of behavioural and social problems [1, 2], including poor academic achievement. This has been shown in several studies of individuals with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity disorder (ADHD, see e.g. [3, 4]), but is also widely documented in studies including community samples [511]. In most of these studies, inattention is defined as a sum score across a set of items.

Inattention is, however, a multidimensional concept, where the items reflect impairment of sustained and focused attention, impaired working memory, distractibility, forgetfulness, as well as impaired ability to organise and plan activities and tasks. These aspects of inattention have been described as independent at a biological level [12], but may be extremely difficult to disentangle behaviourally. They rather tend to occur as patterns of behaviour. For example, most children may be distracted by external stimuli in a classroom situation [13], and these distractions will probably be especially hard to handle by a child who has problems maintaining attention and engagement in a task. Thus, it may not be the total inattention score, but rather specific patterns of inattentive behaviour that have the most detrimental effect on the child’s present and future function at school. Identification of important features of inattentive behaviour will therefore be of great importance when developing remediation procedures.

Primary school teachers’ skills are crucial in the work to detect and help a child struggling with inattention. They observe their pupils on a regular basis and in a wide range of situations were inattention tends to have negative effects on performance. At the same time, one should be aware of the risk of biases. Primary school teachers may for example be more tolerant to the behaviour of a child in the lowest class levels, and previous studies have shown that teachers tend to rate girls as less impaired than boys, even when the girls exhibit problematic behaviour in the classroom [1416]. The child’s gender and age should therefore be taken into account when evaluating teacher ratings of inattentive behaviour.

The aim of the present study was to further investigate the importance of primary school teachers’ reports of inattentive behaviour. To that end, we included data from the Bergen Child Study, where primary school teachers completed a questionnaire including nine items reflecting different aspects of inattentive behaviour when the children were between 7 and 9 years old. About ten years later, when the children had become high school students, academic achievement scores from the official school registry of Norway were available for a subset of the children from the original sample. Described as a key determinant of later occupational career success and adult financial stability [17], there are strong arguments for using academic achievement as an outcome variable. Each of the nine inattention items were rated on a Likert scale with three response alternatives, and the outcome variable, academic achievement, was discretised into three intervals, including an almost equal number of participants in each category. Teacher scores on each of the nine items were used as predictors together with gender and primary school class level (a proxy for age) to answer the following questions: (1) which features of inattentive behaviour in primary school represent the strongest predictors of academic achievement in high school? (2) how well can the result be generalised to an independent data set?, and (3) are gender and the age of the child when evaluated by their primary school teachers of importance to the prediction?

In this context, statistical machine learning approaches were selected according to the following criteria: (i) the methods must handle multiple predictors with a small set of response alternatives, and with a small set of outcome categories; (ii) the methods should be generic and of interest to other similar data analysis situations and prediction challenges occurring in the behavioural sciences, and (iii) the methods should produce results that are easy to interpret at a clinical level. Based on these criteria we selected multinomial logistic regression (MLR), classification and regression trees (CART), and a random forest algorithm (RF) to assess feature importance, and a k-fold cross-validation procedure to estimate the classification accuracy, precision, and recall of a model using, in the prediction, the most important features being identified.

Materials and methods

The data included in the present study are from the Bergen Child Study (BCS), a longitudinal, population-based study on mental health and development. The first wave of the BCS was launched in October 2002, and included the total population of 9,430 children attending second to fourth grade (7-9 years old, born in 1993, 1994 and 1995) in all public, private, and special schools in Bergen. During the initial screening phase, parents and teachers were asked to complete a four-page questionnaire, including, among other scales, a somewhat modified Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham Questionnaire—Fourth Edition (SNAP-IV) [18]. Sample protocols of the first wave have been described in several previous publications from the Bergen Child Study group (e.g., [1921]).

A fourth and final study-wave was conducted when the youth were between 16 and 19 years old. The sample for this wave included all adolescents born between 1993 and 1995 living in the county of Hordaland (n = 10,222). This county includes the city of Bergen, and the BCS sample was thus nested within this Hordaland sample. Academic achievement scores from the previous semester in high school were made available from the official school registry. The BCS was approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics (REC), Western Norway (2015/800 Barn i Bergen/ung@hordaland). Parents gave written consent for participation in the first wave of the study. In accordance with the regulations from the REC and Norwegian health authorities, adolescents aged 16 years and older can make decisions regarding their own health (including participation in health studies), and thus gave consent themselves to participate in the fourth wave of the study. Parents/guardians have the right to be informed, and in the current study, all parents/guardians received written information about the study in advance. More information about the project is given at the BCS homepage: http://uni.no/en/bergen-child-study.

The sample

The sample included n = 2491 participants (n = 1192 boys). All participants were rated by their primary school teachers on all selected SNAP-IV items when they were 7 to 9 years old (primary school class levels 2, 3, or 4), and information about gender and academic achievement score were available when they attended high school (16 to 19 years old). Within this sample, the percentages of children attending 2nd, 3rd and 4th primary school class levels when evaluated by their teachers were 42.3%, 34.4% and 23.4%, respectively.

Teacher reports

Inattention items were selected from the SNAP-IV [18], a scale which describes problems used to define the inattentive symptoms of the Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) [22]. The original SNAP-IV uses four levels to evaluate each item, whereas in our study, the teachers evaluated each item on a 3-level Likert-type scale (“not true”, “somewhat true”, or “certainly true”) in order to follow the response pattern of the remaining scales included in the first wave of the BCS questionnaire. Each answer was assigned a value 0, 1, or 2. The nine inattention items from SNAP-IV are listed in Table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. SNAP items, scored as “not true” (0), “somewhat true” (1), and “certainly true” (2).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188310.t001

The percentages of children scored within the three response categories are given in Table 2, confirming that the frequency of girls reported with a “not true” response was significantly higher than in boys.

thumbnail
Table 2. Percentage of children obtaining a given response from their teachers on each inattention item (SNAP-IV).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188310.t002

Academic achievement

Academic achievement scores were provided by the official registers from the Hordaland County. In Norway, secondary schools use a scale spanning from 1 to 6, with 6 being the highest grade (outstanding competence), 2 the lowest passing grade (low level of competence), and 1 being a fail. The scores included in the present study were the mean value of the grades during the previous semester, comprising all school subjects except for physical education. The mean score for girls was statistically significant higher (μ = 4.11 (SD = 0.72)) than for boys (μ = 3.90 (SD = 0.72), p < .001). For the present study, the academic achievement scores were categorised into three levels, calculated to generate groups with a similar number of participants (see details below).

Statistical analysis

The data analysis was divided into three parts: (a) data preparation, including discretising the average academic achievement into three levels, (b) casting the data analysis problem into a machine learning classification task assessing feature importances using both a multinomial logistic regression (MLR), classification and regression trees (CART), and a random forest (RF) algorithm, and (c) a pattern classification procedure using k-fold cross-validation with five different linear and nonlinear classifiers (MLR, MLP, XGB, SVM, KNN, each described below) and incorporation of a voting classifier across these five. All these steps were implemented in Jupyter notebooks using Python (3.5.4), Numpy (1.12), Pandas (0.20), Statsmodels (0.8), XGBoost (0.6), Scikit-learn (0.19), rpy2 (2.8.5) and Matplotlib (2.0) for producing Figs 13. Our Jupyter notebook for computing feature importances and classification with k-fold cross-validation will be available on GitHub (https://github.com/arvidl/inattention-populationsample).

thumbnail
Fig 1. The predictor data (explanatory variables), the academic achievement outcome, and the types of classification analyses being performed.

Data values are represented as grey level heat maps. MLR = multinomial logistic regression, MLP = multi-layer perceptron, XGB = extreme gradient boost, SVM = support vector machine, KNN = k-nearest neighbor, Voting = voting (argmax) classifier across MLR, …, KNN.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188310.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Fitted classification tree (CART analysis), including the predictor variables SNAP-IV items 1 to 9 (0 = “not true”; 1 = “somewhat true”; 2 = “certainly true”); gender (0 = girl; 1 = boy); grade (primary school class level 2, 3, 4) and the academic achievement outcome (L = low, M = medium, H = high).

The percentage in each node box denote the percentage of samples routed to that particular node—where the root node will contain 100% of the samples, and a leaf node will contain the least number of samples along a rooted path in the decision tree. The node numbers are given on top of each node box. For each split decision, True denotes that the corresponding statement is true and then pointing to the left child node (that is either a new internal decision node or a final leaf node), and False denotes that the corresponding statement is false and then pointing to the right child node (that is either a new internal decision node or a final leaf node).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188310.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. The ranked importance of SNAP2, SNAP8, gender and grade (primary school class level) in predicting academic achievement outcome according to the random forest algorithm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188310.g003

(a) Data preparation and explorative data analysis.

The original data, provided to us as a SPSS-file, were imported into the Jupyter notebook environment via rpy2 and the r-foreign packages. For the analysis we used the sample of n = 2491 children having complete data on the 11 predictor variables and mean academic achievement as outcome variable, cfr. Fig 1.

For classification purposes, the average academic achievement scores (ave) were discretised into three intervals (level of academic achievement) using Pandas qcut(), to include about the same number of participants in each of the categories: low (ave ∈ [1.000 − 3.714〉, n = 834), medium (ave ∈ [3.714 − 4.375〉, n = 831), and high (ave [4.375 − 6.000], n = 826). The distribution across the three levels—from low to high—was 40.3%, 33.3% and 26.4% for boys, and 27.3%, 33.4% and 39.3% for girls, confirming the overall higher academic scores achieved by the girls.

Depiction of the complete dataset is given in Fig 1, using gray scale heatmap columns for the n = 2491 participants comprising the predictor variables gender, grade, SNAP1, …, SNAP9, and the outcome variable academic achievement. In Fig 1 we have also listed the six classifiers being used for prediction in a k-fold cross validation scheme. The observations above the horizontal dotted line represent girls and below the dotted line are the boys.

(b) Assessment of feature importance.

To assess feature importances of the 11 candidate variables for predicting low, medium, and high academic achievement in the whole cohort, we performed three types of analyses: (i) Multinomial logistic regression with consideration of each parameter, i.e. the magnitude of its coefficient, the standard error of the corresponding parameter, and the odds ratio, (ii) a CART analysis with assessment of the top important decision nodes, and (iii) a random forest classification using a forest of 10000 trees (“weak learners”) and ordering of features importance according to the ‘gini’ information criterion.

Multinomial logistic regression model (MLR).

The multinomial logistic regression analysis included the following set of variables on a nominal level: the three levels of academic achievement scores as outcome variable, and gender, primary school class level (grade), and teacher reports on the nine inattention items SNAP1,…,SNAP9 as predictors. Generally, the multinomial logistic regression model relates a set of explanatory variables x1, …, xp to a set of log-odds, log(π2/π1), … log(πJ/π1) according to (1) for j = 2, …, J. Here, j = 1 represents the base level category, πj = P(academic achievement level = j), πj/πj denotes the odds of category j relative to j′ (i.e. odds ratio, OR), and (see e.g. [23] for details). In our case, we let the base level category j = 1 be the low mean academic achievement, meaning that the low was compared separately to the medium and high categories. For computations we used mnlogit() from the statsmodels.formula.api.

Classification trees (CART).

The SNAP1,…,SNAP9 items were included together with demographics (gender and primary school class level (grade)) as predictor variables in a CART analysis [24] used to predict level of academic achievement score {low, medium, high}. In brief, the root of the classification tree is the top node and input patterns are passed down the tree such that decisions are made at each node until a terminal node (a leaf) is reached. At each non-terminal node a question is posed on which a binary split is made such that the “child” nodes are on average “purer” than their “parent”. A measure of “impurity” is low (i.e. close to 0) if the probability of the occurrence of a class at a given node for all subsets of patterns reaching that node is concentrated on that class. The “impurity” is maximal (i.e. close to 1) if the class probabilities at that node is uniform.

In our analysis we used the DecisionTreeClassifier() from sklearn.tree with impurity criterion = ‘gini’ and max_depth = 2 for growing the classification tree.

Random forest ensemble learning (RF).

Random forest (RF) is an ensemble learning method for classification that constructs a multitude of decision trees at training time and output the mode class among the generated classes. The RF algorithm involves the construction of n trees and ensures that each tree uses a different set of data (bootstrapping) and a different set of variables (“feature bagging”) at each candidate split. Thus, RF is less prone to overfitting compared to CART, and will therefore produce more generalisable results [24]. Moreover, the order of decisions in the hierarchies of trees will reflect the importance of the corresponding feature variables being involved. In our setting, the variables gender, grade, and the SNAP1,…,SNAP9 items were included as predictors of the outcome level of academic achievement: low (L), medium (M), or high (H). In the analysis we used the RandomForestClassifier() from sklearn.ensemble with impurity criterion = ‘gini’, n_estimators = 10000, bootstrap = True, max_features = None, and max_depth = None. After fitting the forest with the 2491 × 11 predictor matrix X and academic achievement outcome y ∈ {L, M, H}, i.e. forest.fit(X,y), the Scikit-learn RF algorithm enables the calculation of forest.feature_importances_.

(c) Prediction using k-fold cross-validation.

From the feature importance step, the top ranked predictors of academic achievement scores were selected for a comprehensive classification study using k-fold cross-validation to assess prediction properties (accuracy, precision, and recall). In this procedure we used both linear classifiers (multinomial logistic regression = MLR) and non-linear classifiers (multi-layer perceptron = MLP, extreme gradient boosting = XGB, a radial basis function kernel support vector machine = SVM, and k-nearest neighbours = KNN).

For the k-fold cross-validation we used StratifiedKFold() from sklearn.model_selection with n_splits = 10 and shuffle = True, where the folds (splits) are made by preserving the percentage of samples for each class. For a given fold 1, …, k = 10, fixed pairs of (X_train, y_train) and (X_test, y_test) datasets were provided for each of the six classifiers using the Pipeline mechanism in Scikit-learn, and the feature vectors were standardized to zero mean and unit variance using the StandardScaler() from sklearn.preprocessing. For the individual classifications we used the LogisticRegression() with solver = ‘saga’ and multi_class = ‘multinomial’ from sklearn.linear_model; the MLPClassifier() with hidden_layer_size = 3, activation = ‘relu’, and solver = ‘adam’ from sklearn.neural_network; the XGBClassifier with n_estimators = 1000 and max_depth = 3 from xgboost; the SVC() with C = 1.0, kernel = ‘rbf’, and degree = 3 from sklearn.svm; and the KNeighborsClassifier() with n_neighbors = 5 and metric = ‘minkowski’ from sklearn.neighbors. To obtain an ensemble voting across the five different classifier we used the VotingClassifier() with estimators = [MLR, MLP, XGB, SVM, KNN] and voting = ‘soft’ from sklearn.ensemble, predicting the class label based on the argmax of the sums of the predicted probabilities.

For the performance assessment on each (X_test, y_test) we used accuracy_score (the ratio of correct classifications), precision_score (the ratio tp/(tp + fp), where tp is the number of true positives and fp number of false positives), recall_score (sensitivity, the ratio tp/(tp + fn) where fn is the number of false negatives), and f1_score (harmonic mean of the precision and recall) from sklearn.metrics. Finally we computed the mean and standard deviations of these classifier-specific performance measures across the k folds.

Results

We first report the results from the analysis of feature importance, then the prediction results from k-fold cross validation using the six different classifiers.

Assessment of feature importance

Multinomial logistic regression model (MLR).

Performing MLR on the complete dataset, gender significantly predicted whether a child obtained a low rather than a high academic achievement score in high school (OR = 0.60, p < .002) as well as a low rather than a medium score (OR = 0.79, p < 0.001). This shows that the boys (1) were overall more likely to obtain a low academic achievement score in high school than the girls (0) (Table 3).

Two of the teacher reported inattention items significantly predicted a low rather than a medium academic achievement score. The strongest effect was found for an item reflecting problems related to sustained attention, SNAP2 (p = 0.001). An odds ratio of .54 tells us that for each unit change in the score given by the teacher, the child was almost two times less likely to obtain a medium compared to a low academic achievement score (1/.54 = 1.9). The second item reflects distractibility, SNAP8 (p = 0.02, OR = 0.75), leaving the child with a somewhat increased odds (1.3) of obtaining a low score.

Predictions from the two inattention items were even stronger when comparing low to high academic achievement scores, with the highest estimate on SNAP2 (p < 0.001) followed by SNAP8 (p < 0.001). The odds ratios show that the child was 2.5 times more likely to obtain a low than high score in high school for each more severe step in problems reported on SNAP2 (OR = 0.40) and 1.8 times more likely for each step on SNAP8 (OR = 0.55). The prediction of low rather than high academic achievement score was also significant for two other items reflecting problems related to sustained attention, SNAP1 (p = 0.05) and SNAP6 (p = 0.001). With ORs of 0.61 and 0.48, the increase was around twofold (1.6 and 2, 1, respectively). SNAP5 (p = 0.009) gave a more surprising result, with a higher likelihood to obtain a high academic achievement level if reported with disorganised behaviour by your primary school teacher.

To sum up the results from the MLR, inattentive behaviour associated with problems related to sustained attention and distractibility predicted low rather than medium or high academic achievement levels in high school, with an overall higher odds-ratio in boys than in girls (Table 3).

Classification trees (CART).

The CART analysis, using maximum depth = 2, generated four terminal nodes (Fig 2).

The first and most important split (the top node #0) was on SNAP2, assessing problems related to sustained attention. The “False” branch at this node, i.e. teachers reporting “somewhat true” or “certainly true” on this item, arriving at node #4 (11.5% of the sample), were mainly associated with a low academic achievement score in high school. In this subsample, primary school class level (grade) did matter. A higher portion of those those with “somewhat true” or “certainly true” reports on SNAP2 in the 3rd and 4th grades (node #6) obtained lower academic scores than those in the 2nd grade (node #5), 71% and 57%, respectively.

If primary school teachers reported “not true” on SNAP2, then the reports on problems related to distractibility (SNAP8) was important for prediction, i.e. node #1 comprising 88.5% of the sample. Reporting “somewhat true” or “certainly true” on SNAP8, i.e. the “false” branch from node #1 to node #3, led to the highest percentages towards a low academic achievement score (42%), while a “not true” report (node #2, 74.5% of the sample) was associated with the highest percentages towards the high score (39%).

To sum up the results from the CART analysis, problems related to sustained attention (SNAP2) or distractibility (SNAP8) were important predictors of low school academic achievement scores in 25.5% (nodes #3 and #4) of the children. Primary school class grade did also matter, with a higher percentage of children obtaining a low score when assessments were done at 3rd or 4th grade.

Random forest ensemble learning (RF).

The random forest algorithm with 10000 trees further explored feature importances in the cohort. Fig 3 shows the ranked importance of the 11 predictor variables, confirming the main findings from the MLR and CART analyses, where to top three most important features were SNAP2 > SNAP8 > grade.

Prediction using k-fold cross-validation

The cross-validation procedure was performed separately for the three top features selected by the RF analysis. Gender was included due to its effect upon both the SNAP-IV items and the academic achievement score, and the statistically significant effect revealed by the MLR. Table 4 shows the results from the selected classifiers and the overall voting on the accuracy, precision and recall measures. All values were above chance level (> 33%) for the three categories of academic achievement scores.

thumbnail
Table 4. Classification performance using k-fold cross-validation (k = 10).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188310.t004

Discussion

Summary of results

The present study asked if specific features of inattentive behaviour in primary school—as reported by teachers—act as predictors of academic achievement in high school. Different types of multivariate analyses were used to handle the set of categorical variables. Overall, items reflecting problems related to sustained attention and distractibility were selected as the two most important features of inattention in predicting the achievement score. Gender and a proxy for age (primary school class level) were added as important features by the MLR analysis. The CART analysis showed that as many as 25.5% of the children were reported with either of the two inattention problems, and that these children had a high risk of obtaining a low academic achievement score. Age when assessed by their primary school teachers was of some importance, in that the chance of obtaining a low achievement score was somewhat lower when reported with problems in the 2nd than in higher grades (3rd and 4th grades). Age and the items reflecting sustained attention and distractibility were also identified with the highest importance by the RF analysis, suggesting that these results are expected to generalise to other samples. This was confirmed by the k-fold cross-validation analyses.

Early predictors of academic achievement in high school

The present results showed that problems related to sustained attention and distractibility in primary school are important drivers of poor academic performance in high school. By this, the results partly overlapped with findings previously reported in a study by Holmberg et al. [9], where teacher reports of failure to finish a task were found to be one of the main factors explaining academic outcome. Our study add to this by revealing the importance of problems related to distractibility. The MLR analysis showed that this problem was associated with an almost two-fold increase in OR of an achievement score in the lower than higher end of the scale. Its importance as a predictor of poor achievement scores was also supported by the CART analysis, with the strongest effect when reported as a problem in the 3rd and 4th grades. In a class situation, the relation between the two is obvious. A child with the ability to stay focused on a task over a longer period of time is expected to be less disturbed by habits and cues in the environment than a child with poor vigilance. This enables the child to obtain the basic skills and knowledge that are of importance to the academic achievement scores as the curriculum becomes more complex at higher grade levels.

Inclusion of information about nine aspects of inattentive behaviour separately in the statistical analyses revealed their relative importance to high school academic performance. Most previous studies have defined inattention as a sum-score from reports of problems reflecting a range of different behaviours. A significant relation between such a sum score and academic achievement was shown in one of our previous studies, including subsamples from BCS and the Berkeley Girls with ADHD Longitudinal Study (BGALS). Inattention was found to be significant across these two culturally and diagnostically diverse groups, and the effect was over and above the effect of demographics and intellectual function [25]. The present results indicate that the effect on academic achievement is driven by a few features defined within the full inattention score.

The cross-diagnostically effect of inattention was confirmed by the present study. Although inattention is one of the core symptoms of ADHD, the importance of inattentive behaviour in explaining future academic success is definitely not restricted to a diagnostic category; the present study documented this effect in a population-based sample. However, although a high proportion of children obtaining a low academic achievement score were reported as inattentive by their primary school teachers, the cross-validation analyses revealed that more information about the child is needed to obtain an improved validation of the prediction. This probably reflects both the instability of inattentive behaviour and the large number of co-existing and new challenges influencing a child through childhood and adolescence. Previous studies have for example shown the importance of socio-economic factors in general (e.g., [26]), with some cultural differences regarding the importance of its subcomponents [27] and consequences [28]. Further studies should thus include a larger number of predictors and a more diverse sample than in the present study.

Taken together, the present results should inspire assessment and treatment efforts in primary school children vulnerable to distractibility and with problems to sustain their attention in school-related work. The close relationship between inattentive behaviour and cognitive function [29, 30] has lead to increased popularity of presenting cognitive training programs to school children with ADHD (see e.g., [31, 32]). A sole focus on cognitive training of the child is, however, not expected to lead to successful alleviation of the inattentive behaviour described in the present paper. This was supported by the results from the meta-analysis presented by Cortese and collaborators [33], showing that cognitive training procedures had limited effects on ADHD symptoms. Positive contributions from parents and teachers seem to be essential (see e.g., [34]). Whereas parent-focused training produces improvements in negative parenting and impairment at home, incorporation of child skill training and teacher consultation may be necessary to produce improvements at school [35].

Gender turned out to be another important predictor. Girls were reported by their primary school teachers to have less inattention symptoms and to obtain higher academic achievement in high school than boys. Although gender was identified as one of the main predictors of academic achievement scores in the feature extraction by the MLR analysis, it was not selected among the top features of importance by the the CART and RF analyses. Further gender balanced longitudinal studies of functional outcomes of early inattentive behaviour are warranted.

Strengths and limitations

The large population-based sample of high school students followed from childhood, inclusion of a standardised questionnaire assessing inattention, and inclusion of academic achievement scores from official National registers are main strengths of the present study. Another strength is the inclusion of several statistical methods to assess patterns in the data and to perform predictions—the use of the MLR, CART, and RF algorithms to assess feature importances and select features, and the comprehensive k-fold cross-validation procedure. We believe that the relevance of the present analytic approach is not restricted to the topic of the present study, in that questionnaire data with a few response categories are commonly used in psychological research.

In spite of the strengths and the importance of the present study, several limitations must be mentioned. Inclusion of very few features when predicting an outcome about 10 years ahead, is an obvious limitation of the present study. A stronger model could have been obtained by including results from a psychometric test assessing vigilance and distractibility, similar to the one developed by Cassuto et al. [36], or a more ecological valid virtual reality test as the one described by Pelham et al. [37]. Inclusion of teacher reports only may also be considered as a limitation. Furthermore, stronger conclusions could have been obtained by including information from repeated inattention reports to understand the trajectory from early symptoms of inattention to function in adolescence and adulthood. The importance of the latter was demonstrated in a study by Pingault and collaborators [7], showing that increase in symptoms of inattention during childhood really matters when it comes to school graduation failure. Such studies are important and should include analysis of behavioural patterns, because a specific pattern of vigilance and distraction was suggested by the present study. Finally, academic achievement level did not reflect overall high school achievement, in that it was operationalised as the mean of grades for one semester only.

Acknowledgments

The Bergen Child study (BCS) was supported by the Centre for Child and Adolescent Mental Health and Welfare, Uni health, Uni Research, Bergen, Norway, and was also funded by the University of Bergen, the Norwegian Directorate for Health and Social Affairs, and the Western Norway Regional Health Authority. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. We are grateful to the children, parents and teachers participating in the BCS and members of the BCS project group for making the study possible. A special thank to Professor Christopher Gillberg, who initiated this study.

References

  1. 1. Bellanti CJ, Bierman KL. Disentangling the impact of low cognitive ability and inattention on social behavior and peer relationships. Conduct problems prevention research group. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 2000;29(1), 66–75. pmid:10693033
  2. 2. Connors LL, Connolly J, Toplak ME. Self-reported inattention in early adolescence in a community sample. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2012;16(1):60–70. pmid:20837982
  3. 3. Lee SS, Hinshaw SP. Predictors of adolescent functioning in girls with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the role of childhood ADHD, conduct problems, and peer status. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35(3), 356–368. pmid:16836474
  4. 4. Owens EB, Zalecki C, Gillette P, Hinshaw SP. Girls with childhood ADHD as adults: Cross-domain outcomes by diagnostic persistence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2017;85(7):723–736. pmid:28414486
  5. 5. Polderman TJ, Boomsma DI, Bartels M, Verhulst FC, Huizink AC. A systematic review of prospective studies on attention problems and academic achievement. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2010;122(4):271–284. pmid:20491715
  6. 6. Metcalfe LA, Harvey EA, and Laws HB. The longitudinal relation between academic/cognitive skills and externalizing behavior problems in preschool children. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2013;105:881–894. pmid:24039280
  7. 7. Pingault J, Cote SM, Vitaro F, Falissard B, Genolini C, Tremblay RE. The developmental course of childhood inattention symptoms uniquely predicts educational attainment: a 16-year longitudinal study. Psychiatry Research. 2014;219(3):707–709. pmid:25017615
  8. 8. Garner AA, O’connor BC, Narad ME, Tamm L, Simon J, Epstein JN. The relationship between ADHD symptom dimensions, clinical correlates, and functional impairments. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2013;34(7):469–477. pmid:24042078
  9. 9. Holmberg K, Bölte S. Do symptoms of ADHD at ages 7 and 10 predict academic outcome at age 16 in the general population? Journal of Attention Disorders. 2014;18(8):635–645. pmid:22837550
  10. 10. Gray SAO, Carter AS, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Jones SM, Wagmiller RL. Growth trajectories of early aggression, overactivity, and inattention: Relations to second-grade reading. Developmental Psychology. 2014;50(9):2255–2263. pmid:25046126
  11. 11. Salla J, Michel G, Pingault JB, Lacourse E, Paquin S, Galera C, etal. Childhood trajectories of inattention-hyperactivity and academic achievement at 12 years. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2016;25:1195–1206. pmid:27017347
  12. 12. Berry AS, Demeter E, Sabhapathy S, English BA, Blakely RD, Sarter M, Lustig C. Disposed to distraction: genetic variation in the cholinergic system influences distractibility but not time-on-task effects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2014;26(9):1981–1991. pmid:24666128
  13. 13. Rescorla L, Achenbach TM, Ivanova MY, Dumenci L, Almqvist F, Bilenberg N, etal. Epidemiological comparisons of problems and positive qualities reported by adolescents in 24 countries. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2007;75(2):351–358. pmid:17469893
  14. 14. Becker SP, Luebbe AM, Fite PJ, Greening L, Stoppelbein L. Oppositional defiant disorder symptoms in relation to psychopathic traits and aggression among psychiatrically hospitalized children: ADHD symptoms as a potential moderator. Aggressive Behavior. 2013;39(3):201–211. pmid:23436456
  15. 15. Bussing R, Zima BT, Gary FA, Garvan CW. Barriers to detection, help-seeking, and service use for children with ADHD symptoms. The journal of Behavioral Health Services & Research. 2003;30:176–189.
  16. 16. Ohan JL, Visser TAW. Why is there a gender gap in children presenting for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder services? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2009;38(5):650–60.
  17. 17. Fried R, Petty C, Faraone SV, Hyder LL, Day H, Biederman J. Is ADHD a risk factor for high school dropout? A controlled study. Journal of Attention Disorder. 2016;20, 383–389.
  18. 18. Swanson JM. School-based assessments and interventions for ADD students. KC publishing;1992.
  19. 19. Heiervang E, Stormark KM, Lundervold AJ, Heimann M, Goodman R, Posserud M, etal. Psychiatric disorders in Norwegian 8- to 10-year-olds: an epidemiological survey of prevalence, risk factors, and service use. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2007;46(4):438–447. pmid:17420678
  20. 20. Lundervold AJ, Adolfsdottir S., Halleland H., Halmøy A, Plessen K, Haavik J. Attention network test in adults with ADHD—the impact of affective fluctuations. Behavior and Brain Function. 2011;27;7:27.
  21. 21. Sivertsen B, Glozier N, Harvey AG, Hysing M. Academic performance in adolescents with delayed sleep phase. Sleep Medicine. 2015;16(9):1084–1090. pmid:26298783
  22. 22. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author; 2013.
  23. 23. Bilder CR, Loughin TM. Analysis of Categorical Data with R. CRC Press; 2015.
  24. 24. Breiman L. Random Forests. Machine Learning. 2001;45(1):5–32.
  25. 25. Lundervold AJ, Meza J, Hinshaw SP. Parent rated symptoms of inattention in childhood predict high school academic achievement across two culturally and diagnostically diverse samples. Frontiers of Psychology. 2017:8,1436.
  26. 26. Russell EA, Ford T, Russell G. Socioeconomic Associations with ADHD: Findings from a Mediation Analysis. PLoS One. 2015;10(6).
  27. 27. Bøe T, Øverland S, Lundervold AJ, Hysing M. Socioeconomic status and children’s mental health: results from the Bergen Child Study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2012;47(10):1557–66. pmid:22183690
  28. 28. Ellertsen T, Thorsen AL, Holm SE, Bøe T, Sørensen L, Lundervold AJ. A weak association between socioeconomic status and cognition in a sample of Norwegian children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2016;57: 399–405.
  29. 29. Berger I, Slobodin O, Aboud M, Melamed J, Cassuto H. Maturational delay in ADHD: evidence from CPT. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7:691. pmid:24298243
  30. 30. Berger I, Remington A, Leitner Y, Leviton A. Brain development and the attention spectrum. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2015;9:23. pmid:25698956
  31. 31. Rapport MD, Orban SA, Kofler MJ, Friedman LM. Do programs designed to train working memory, other executive functions, and attention benefit children with ADHD? a meta-analytic review of cognitive, academic, and behavioral outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review. 2013;33(8):1237–1252. pmid:24120258
  32. 32. Tamm L, Epstein JN, Loren REA, Becker SP, Brenner SB, Bamberger ME, etal. Generating attention, inhibition, and memory: A pilot randomized trial for preschoolers with executive functioning deficits. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2017;20:1–15.
  33. 33. Cortese S, Ferrin M, Brandeis D, Buitelaar J, Daley D, Dittmann RW, etal. Cognitive training for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of clinical and neuropsychological outcomes from randomized controlled trials. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015;54:164–174. pmid:25721181
  34. 34. Pfiffner LJ, Haack LM. Behavior management for school-aged children with ADHD. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2014;23:731–746. pmid:25220083
  35. 35. Haack LM, Villodas M, McBurnett K, Hinshaw S, Pfiffner LJ. Parenting as a mechanism of change in psychosocial treatment for youth with ADHD, predominantly inattentive presentation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2016;45(5):841–855.
  36. 36. Cassuto H, Ben-Simon A, Berger I. Using environmental distractors in the diagnosis of ADHD. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7:805. pmid:24319423
  37. 37. Pelham WE, Waschbusch DA, Hoza B, Gnagy EM, Greiner AR, Sams SE, etal. Music and video as distractors for boys with ADHD in the classroom: comparison with controls, individual differences, and medication effects. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39(8):1085–1098. pmid:21695447