Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Sympatry Predicts Spot Pigmentation Patterns and Female Association Behavior in the Livebearing Fish Poeciliopsis baenschi

  • Andrea J. Roth-Monzón ,

    rothmonzon@gmail.com

    Affiliation Evolutionary Ecology Laboratories, Department of Biology, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, United States of America

  • Laura E. Scott,

    Affiliation Evolutionary Ecology Laboratories, Department of Biology, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, United States of America

  • Ashley A. Camargo,

    Affiliation Evolutionary Ecology Laboratories, Department of Biology, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, United States of America

  • Eliza I. Clark,

    Affiliation Evolutionary Ecology Laboratories, Department of Biology, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, United States of America

  • Eric E. Schott,

    Affiliation Evolutionary Ecology Laboratories, Department of Biology, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, United States of America

  • Jerald B. Johnson

    Affiliations Evolutionary Ecology Laboratories, Department of Biology, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, United States of America, Monte L. Bean Life Science Museum, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, United States of America

Abstract

In this study, we explored the possibility that differences in pigmentation patterns among populations of the fish Poeciliopsis baenschi were associated with the presence or absence of the closely related species P. turneri. If reproductive character displacement is responsible, spotting patterns in these two species should diverge in sympatry, but not allopatry. We predicted that female P. baenschi from sympatric sites should show a preference for associating with conspecifics vs. heterospecific males, but females from allopatric sites should show no such preferences. To evaluate these predictions, we compared spotting patterns and female association behaviors in populations of P. baenschi from Central Mexico. We found that both of our predictions were supported. Poeciliopsis baenschi that co-occured with P. turneri had spotting patterns significantly different than their counterparts from allopatric sites. Using a simultaneous choice test of video presentations of males, we also found that female P. baenschi from populations that co-occured with P. turneri spent significantly more time with males of their own species than with P. turneri males. In contrast, females from allopatric populations of P. baenschi showed no differences in the amount of time they spent with either conspecific or heterospecific males. Together, our results are consistent with the hypothesis that reproductive character displacement may be responsible for behavioral and spotting pattern differences in these populations of P. baenschi.

Introduction

A variety of phenotypic cues can be used in species recognition, including visual, chemical, and auditory cues. When closely related species do co-occur, they often diverge from one another in species recognition traits relative to their conspecific counterparts that occur in allopatry. Such divergence helps minimize costly reproductive interactions between heterospecifics. This form of divergence is known as reproductive character displacement (hereafter referred to as RCD) [18].

A variety of selective pressures can favor RCD. Reproductive interactions between heterospecifics can result in a decrease in fitness, either by hybridization or through reproductive interference, wherein heterospecifics lower the efficacy of conspecific matings [118]. Hybrid offspring are potentially costly because they could be inviable, sterile, have dysfunctional genomes, or simply be less fit for their environment than either parent [4,6,1923]. When hybridization does reduce fitness [15,2428], individuals that decrease the chance of heterospecific matings will have a selective advantage. Even when hybridization does not occur, interacting with heterospecifics can have fitness costs by wasting time and energy or result in injuries [2,10,17,2931].

Studies of RCD have frequently been conducted in systems where females actively choose males using a sexual display to attract females (e.g. [9,11,15]). However, in many species, female choice is more subtle, e.g. where males attempt to force copulations [32,33]. In such systems females can exert choice by varying the extent to which they resist mating attempts, or by simply spending more time in proximity to some males versus others [3436]. Indeed, evidence thus far suggests that females play an important role in male choice in both displaying and in coercive systems [37]. In both cases a key indicator is which males the females choose to associate with. In studies of RCD, females preferentially associating with conspecific males can reduce the chance of hybridization and reproductive interference [3,3841]. Theory thus predicts that this preferential association should be strongest in sympatric populations.

The livebearing fishes Poeciliopsis baenschi and P. turneri from central Mexico provide an excellent system to examine the effects of RCD in a coercive mating system. These two species are phenotypically similar, but differ most strikingly in the number and shape of melanin-based spots along the lateral flank of their body [42]. Differences in these spotting patterns appear to be consistent, regardless of environmental stimuli [43]. Interestingly, P. baenschi also show intraspecific variation for these traits, but little is known about the factors responsible. Several studies suggest that barring or spotting patterns can be used as visual cues in species recognition [44] or identifying potential mates [4547]. Moreover, unlike other pigments, melanin is synthesized internally and can therefore be consistently displayed as a reliable signal for species recognition [4850]. Finally, the Poeciliopsis species evaluated here are members of two distinct monophyletic clades that have come into secondary contact in western Mexico [51,52], providing a set of sympatric populations that can be compared to allopatric populations in adjacent river drainages.

Here, we ask if P. baenschi show phenotypic divergence in spotting patterns and association behaviors consistent with the hypothesis of reproductive character displacement. We compared these traits using a combination of field collections and laboratory experiments. We evaluated two predictions; 1) P. baenschi from sympatric populations (those that co-occur with P. turneri) should show greater divergence in spotting traits from P. turneri than those found in allopatry, and 2) female P. baenschi from sympatric populations should have a greater tendency to associate with conspecific males over heterospecific males when compared to female P. baenschi from allopatric populations. We found that both of these predictions were supported by our data.

Materials and Methods

Study System and Field Collections

Poeciliopsis baenschi and P. turneri (Fig 1) are members of the family Poecilidae, a group of livebearing fishes that give birth to free-swimming, precocial young. Both species are narrowly distributed along the central-western coast of Mexico (Fig 2). In some locations, P. baenschi co-occurs with P. turneri, and in other locations it occurs alone. Populations of P. turneri do not occur without P. baenschi.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Photographs of a typical male from each of the three groups used in this study.

(A) allopatric P. baenschi (B) sympatric P. baenschi, and (C) P. turneri.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170326.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Map indicating locations of allopatric and sympatric collection sites.

Numbers correspond with the collection sites as denoted in Table 1. Rivers sampled are bold with corresponding name near them. Star in inset Mexico map indicates area for the full map. Shapes for map construction were obtained from Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía (INEGI).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170326.g002

We collected P. baenschi and P. turneri from 11 localities in western Mexico during May and June 2007 (Fig 2, Table 1). Our samples included six localities where P. baenschi co-occurs with P. turneri and five localities where P. baenschi occurs alone (Fig 2). All sympatric localities were taken from the same drainage system where these species have come into secondary contact [53]. Each sympatric collection was made at least 3.5 km apart to ensure these were independent replicates. Although we do not know the extent to which gene flow occurs among these populations, our analyses of spotting patterns did not show a clinal gradient (S1 Fig), suggesting that each sympatric sampling locality is distinct.

thumbnail
Table 1. Locality information for populations of P. baenschi and P. turneri used in this study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170326.t001

All fish were collected with a hand-held seine net (1.3 m x 5 m; 8 mm mesh size). Fish used for the spotting pattern analysis were euthanized in the field, preserved in ethanol, and transported to the laboratory for data collection. Live fish used in the behavioral analysis were collected from three locations: site three (sympatric); and sites seven and 11 (allopatric; see Fig 2; Table 1). Live fish were transported to the laboratory at Brigham Young University where they were housed in 20 gallon tanks at 22°C, a temperature typical for these fishes in the wild. Fish were isolated by population, fed twice daily, and kept on a 12:12 hour LD cycle. The Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Brigham Young University approved the use of live fish for this study (IACUC protocol 06–0104) and all guidelines and recommendations in this protocol were strictly followed.

Quantifying Spots

We collected spot data from ethanol-preserved samples. Although ethanol can diminish the intensity of melanin spots, we had no difficulty measuring the shape or number of spots in our samples. We quantified spotting patterns by measuring two primary characteristics of spots of reproductively mature males; (1) the number of spots; and (2) the total pigmented area of spots. Our focus on males was to complement the female association tests described below. Previous work suggests that spotting patterns can sometimes vary between different sides of the same fish [43]. However, we found that asymmetry in spot size and color between sides was low compared to differences between populations and species, and that neither side had an inherent bias in number or shape of spots (S2 Fig). Hence, we collected all of our data from the left side of each fish. We examined 98 specimens of P. baenschi from allopatric sites, 111 specimens of P. baenschi from sympatric sites, and 67 specimens of P. turneri from sympatric sites. From these samples, we quantified the number and total area of spots using ImageJ 1.41 (http://rsbweb.nih.gov/ij/). The spots of these species were conspicuous allowing us to count the total number of spots on each fish by eye. We measured the total area of spots covering the side of the fish bounded by the opening of the operculum at the anterior margin and the end of the vertebral column at the posterior margin. We quantified area (in mm2) by transforming each image to black and white and classifying pixels as pigmented or non-pigmented using a color threshold between 30 and 50.

Behavioral Assays

Our behavioral assay allowed P. baenschi females from allopatric and sympatric locations to choose between males of their own species versus P. turneri males. We used a dichotomous-choice test with a video playback system [48,51,54,55] to determine if P. baenschi females derived from sympatric and allopatric sites showed a difference in their association times with conspecific versus heterospecific males.

To measure association time, a single P. baenschi female was simultaneously presented with two stimulus videos of males following previously published methods [56]. In brief, we created stimulus videos using a composite of video images of three males from each of our three focal populations (P. baenschi in sympatry, P. baenschi in allopatry, and P. turneri). The males used to create the stimulus tapes were of similar size (within 2 mm) and were typical in terms of spotting patterns for each population. We conservatively chose a 2 mm size difference threshold because it is smaller than the 3 mm difference shown to be necessary to have an effect on mate choice in similar studies of poeciliid fish mating preferences [5759]. In addition, males used to make the videos were chosen to be as similar as possible, with the primary difference being the number and character of spots. The composite video provided a 10 minute looped segment of a single male swimming back and forth across the screen. Males in these looped videos did not show any courtship behavior, although the swimming behavior was typical of Poeciliopsis males that approach females from the side or behind prior to mating. Hence, females were simply given an opportunity to spend time with one video male or the other. Previous observations in other poeciliid fishes has shown that the male that a female associates with is frequently a good indicator of male mating success [56,6063]. We also made control videos showing only the background with no stimulus.

We conducted a total of 10 trials for sympatric females and 11 trials for allopatric females, sample sizes sufficient to detect differences in association time in our study (see below). Females included in the study were separated from males for at least two week before starting the trials [6466]. In each trial, a female was introduced into the tank and allowed to acclimate for 10 minutes while empty tank control videos were shown on monitors abutting the opposite sides of the test tank. Following the 10-minute acclimation period, we started the 10-minute male stimulus videos. Poeciliopsis baenschi females were presented with a choice between a conspecific and heterospecific male. To control for side preferences, we randomly assigned the side to which the males were presented. A video camera placed one meter from the front glass recorded each trial. All recording was done remotely from an adjacent room. Association time was defined as the amount of time a female spent in the third of the tank closest to the stimulus video [67].

Statistical Analyses

We compared spotting patterns between groups using a general linear model framework. The number of spots and total area of spots were both analyzed by analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). In each model, we tested for differences between allopatric P. baenschi, sympatric P. baenschi, and sympatric P. turneri. The number of spots and total area of spots can covary with fish body size because larger fish have larger spots. Hence, we included “area of fish” as a covariate. We quantified “area of fish” by outlining the fish body in ImageJ over the same area for which spot pigment was measured. To meet the assumptions of the statistical models, ‘number of spots’ was square root transformed and ‘area of fish’ was natural log transformed. Our results were the same regardless of whether or not the data were transformed; hence, for ease of interpretation we present non-transformed results. The interaction term between the “groups” (allopatric P. baenschi, sympatric P. baenschi, and sympatric P. turneri) and “area of fish” was also included in each model to determine if spotting patterns changed among groups as a function of body size. Finally, we tested for differences in the two spot traits between sympatric and allopatric populations of P. baenschi (excluding P. turneri) using a one-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). These ANCOVA models used the same variables as those described above.

To analyze the association behavior data, we used a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). We compared the amount of time females spent associating with a conspecific male versus time spent with the heterospecific P. turneri male. Because females had the option of not interacting by remaining in the center of the tank, we treated the amount of time females spent with each male as an independent measure [56,62,68]. This statistical test was performed separately for the sympatric population and the allopatric population because we wanted to know if each population differed in their association with conspecifics versus heterospecifics. We also tested for a tank side-bias by comparing the amount of time spent on each side of the tank during our control treatments when the control videos were presented. In total, we present the results for four separate tests.

Statistical significance was evaluated at the P < 0.05 level. All statistical tests were conducted in R [69]. We report the least square means and standard error for the number of spots, total area of spots and association time from the behavioral assays in the results. For the number and total area of spots, the least square means were adjusted for the covariate (the differences in size of the groups), thus allowing us to compare the differences in number of spot between groups regardless of the differences in body size among individuals between the groups. For the behavioral data, we used least square means to account for differences in the number of replicates for each of the treatments (10 sympatric vs. 11 allopatric).

Results

Spotting Pattern

Allopatric P. baenschi, sympatric P. baenschi and P. turneri differed significantly from one another in their spotting patterns (Table 2; Fig 3). Consistent with our predictions, we found that sympatric populations were more divergent from P. turneri than were allopatric populations for both the number of spots and total pigmented area (Fig 3). Poeciliopsis turneri had the greatest number of spots (mean ± 1 SE, 7.58 ± 0.21), sympatric populations of P. baenschi had the fewest number of spots (4.64 ± 0.10), and allopatric populations of P. baenschi had spot numbers intermediate between the other two groups (6.45 ± 0.13; Fig 3A). Total area of spots varied significantly between the three groups. Poeciliopsis turneri had the greatest area (mm2) of spots (9.29 ± 0.30), sympatric populations of P. baenschi had the smallest area of spots (3.36 ± 0.15), and allopatric populations of P. baenschi were intermediate, although closer to sympatric P.baenschi than P. turneri (4.26 ± 0.18; Fig 3B). We also found a significant interaction between group and area of fish (fish size) for both the number of spots and total area of spots (Table 2; Fig 4), indicating that spot area and number of spots scaled with body size, but did so in different ways for each of the groups. At smaller body sizes, the total area of spots was similar between the three groups, but at larger body sizes, P. turneri had a significantly greater area of spots than both sympatric and allopatric populations of P. baenschi (Table 2; Fig 4). Area of fish was not a good predictor of number spots, as shown by the low goodness of fit of the models (R2McF = 0.004, 0.004, 0.002; Fig 4; [70]).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Comparisons of number of spots and total area of spots for allopatric P. baenschi, sympatric P. baenschi, and P. turneri.

(A) Comparisons of number of spots per lateral flank for sympatric P. baenschi, allopatric P. baenschi, and P. turneri. (B) Comparisons of total area of spots per lateral flank of each fish for sympatric P. baenschi, allopatric P. baenschi, and P. turneri. Values presented are least square means from a general linear model analyses (± 1 SE).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170326.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Relationship between total area of spots and number of spots with total area of fish (mm2) for allopatric P. baenschi, sympatric P. baenschi, and P. turneri.

(A) allopatric P. baenschi, (B) sympatric P. baenschi and (C) P. turneri.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170326.g004

thumbnail
Table 2. Analysis of covariance results comparing spotting pattern traits between the three groups (see text).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170326.t002

Our second set of analyses focused on the comparison between sympatric and allopatric populations of P. baenschi. We found that sympatric populations of P. baenschi had significantly fewer spots (4.55 ± 0.10) than allopatric populations (6.41 ± 0.11; F1,205 = 145.45, P < 0.001). We also found that the total area of spots was smaller in sympatric populations (3.01 ± 0.10 mm2) than in allopatric populations (3.81 ± 0.11 mm2; F1,205 = 11.17, P < 0.001). Interestingly, total spot area and number of spots were only weakly correlated (R = 0.24, P < 0.001) in P. baenschi, suggesting the potential for these traits to function somewhat independently.

Behavioral Assay

When given the choice between heterospecific and conspecific males, sympatric P. baenschi females showed a clear association preference for conspecific males in terms of time spent in seconds with each type of male (conspecific: 348.72 ± 54.18 s; heterospecific: 176.4 ± 54.18 s; ANOVA F1,18 = 5.06, P = 0.04; Fig 5). In contrast, allopatric P. baenschi females showed no significant difference in their association time between their own species versus P. turneri (conspecific: 197.77 ± 45.69 s; heterospecific: 257.87 ± 45.69 s; ANOVA F1,20 = 0.86, P = 0.36; Fig 5). To check whether our non-significant results were due to lack of statistical power, we conducted a post hoc power analysis using G Power 3 [71] and found that for the effect size observed in the present study (d = 0.198) with an alpha of 0.05, a sample of approximately 202 would be needed to obtain statistical power at the recommended 0.80 level [72]. Moreover, our controls revealed no evidence for a tank side effect because individuals were equally likely to spend time on either side of the tank in the absence of the male stimulus (sympatric female, right side: 205.78 ± 41.43 s; left side: 293.78 ± 41.43 s; ANOVA F1,18 = 2.23, P = 0.15 and allopatric female, right side: 227.98 ± 43.57 s; left side: 190.05 ± 43.57 s; ANOVA F1,20 = 0.38, P = 0.55). Again, power analysis revealed that for the effect size observed here for both sympatric (d = 0.334) and allopatric females (d = 0.131) with an alpha of 0.05, a sample of approximately 74 and 458 would be needed to obtain statistical power at the recommended 0.80 level [72]. All power tests showed that to achieve a level of 0.80, sample sizes that are prohibitively large for most behavioral studies are requiered. Given that our non-significant results greatly overlapped, it is reasonable to conclude that no differences were observed.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Results from the behavioral assay comparing P. baenschi female association time with a conspecific (P. baenschi) or heterospecific (P. turneri) male.

Circles refer to sympatric P. baenschi and squares refer to allopatric P. baenschi. Values presented are least square means from a general linear model (± 1 SE) for the amount of time females spent near the indicated side of the tank.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170326.g005

Discussion

Our results are consistent with the reproductive character displacement hypothesis. Spotting patterns differed more between Poeciliopsis baenschi and P. turneri where these species co-occur than when P. baenschi occurs alone. Also, P. baenschi females from sympatric sites showed preferences for males of their own species, but no such preference was observed in P. baenschi from allopatric sites. Why selection should favor reproductive character displacement in these fishes? Two plausible explanations are reinforcement and reproductive interference.

Although some species in the genus Poeciliopsis are known to hybridize [73,74], most taxa in the genus maintain distinct species boundaries. To date, no evidence of hybridization between P. baenschi and P. turneri exists, so we cannot rule out the possibility that reinforcement has led to our observed differences, but we consider it unlikely. In contrast, sexual interference is a plausible explanation given the overall phenotypic similarity between these two taxa and the similar male mating tactics of forcing copulations. Under these circumstances, mistaken mating attempts by males can be costly to females [7579]. Costs such as energy investment to avoid males or actual injuries caused by males have caused females to change their association behavior to avoid harassment in closely related species [7579]. Our data are consistent with this explanation. It is possible that the female association preferences observed here are not completely related to spotting patterns, but could be explained by confounding traits such as shape. However, we consider this unlikely given that we matched stimulus males to control for such differences. This matching accounted for coloration and male size traits that can influence visual preference in poeciliid female [9,62,8082].

Even though our data are consistent with an explanation of RCD, other factors could contribute to the observed patterns including confounding ecological variables [4,12,83] and differential fusion, where species that come into secondary contact either fuse or are maintained depending on the strength of mating discrimination present before secondary contact.

Ecological variables that affect the presence or discrimination of a phenotype could lead to changes in mating cues or association preference [18,82,8486]. It is possible that the relationship between spot number and species co-occurrence is driven by an unidentified common ecological factor. For example, both predation pressure and resource availability have been shown to affect pigmentation in poeciliid fishes [73,74,8793]. Predation can cause either an increase or decrease in pigmentation, depending on the degree to which it affects the conspicuousness of an individual. Unfortunately, there are no known differences in predation pressure among our collecting localities [94]. Environmental resources can also affect fish pigmentation where pigments are directly obtained from the diet [48,54,55,7579]. Unfortunately, this is not the case with melanin, a pigment that is not diet derived but is instead synthesized internally. Previous work [94] has demonstrated that the local environments of P. baenschi evaluated here—both sympatric and allopatric—do not differ in resource availability.

The hypothesis of differential fusion suggests that when species come in secondary contact, they fuse or persist as distinct species depending on the strength of mating discrimination that existed in allopatry [12]. Our results show that allopatric P. baenschi females had no association preference and thus had no effect on male mating potential. Therefore differential fusion is an unlikely explanation. Our best explanation is that the presence or absence of heterospecific P. turneri has influenced spotting patterns and female association preferences in P. baenschi.

In conclusion, our results are consistent with the explanation that species recognition behavior has evolved in sympatric sites where there are potential fitness risks caused by the presence of a heterospecific, but is absent in allopatric sites where there are no such risks. Consistent with our findings, several other studies suggest that barring or spotting patterns on fish can be used as visual cues in species recognition [44]. Differences in spotting patterns and species association behaviors observed here suggest that sympatric populations of P. baenschi express phenotypes that reduce the possibility of reproductive interference. Whether these differences are genetically based or environmentally induced remains unknown. More research is needed to directly determine what fitness benefits sympatric populations of P. baenschi achieve in their habitat relative to allopatric populations. Nonetheless, our study points to the presence of the closely related fish P. turneri as an important factor for shaping phenotypic divergence in both spotting patterns and association behavior among P. baenschi populations.

Supporting Information

S1 Fig. Boxplot of both number of spots and total area of spots for sympatric P. baenschi sampled localities.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170326.s001

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Photographs of a typical male from each of the three groups used in this study from both left and right side.

(A) allopatric P. baenschi (B) sympatric P. baenschi, and (C) P. turneri.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170326.s002

(TIF)

Acknowledgments

J. J. Zuniga-Vega helped secure permits and logistical support during fieldwork. We thank K. Stilson for his help with spot quantification. M. Belk and two anonymous reviewers read the manuscript and provided helpful feedback. This work was funded by a Mentoring Environment Grant and a grant from the Roger and Victoria Sant Educational Endowment for a Sustainable Environment at Brigham Young University to JBJ. AJRM is supported by fellowships from Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnologia, México and a Brigham Young University High Impact Doctoral Research Award (HIDRA).

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: LES JBJ.
  2. Formal analysis: AJRM.
  3. Funding acquisition: JBJ.
  4. Investigation: LES AAC EES EIC.
  5. Methodology: LES JBJ.
  6. Project administration: JBJ.
  7. Resources: LES EES JBJ.
  8. Supervision: JBJ.
  9. Visualization: AJRM.
  10. Writing – original draft: AAC EIC LES AJRM JBJ.
  11. Writing – review & editing: AJRM JBJ.

References

  1. 1. Brown WL, Wilson EO. Character displacement. Syst Biol. 1956;5:49–64.
  2. 2. Rafferty NE, Boughman JW. Olfactory mate recognition in a sympatric species pair of three-spined sticklebacks. Behav Ecol. 2006;17(6):965–70.
  3. 3. Campbell DLM, Hauber ME. Spatial and behavioural measures of social discrimination by captive male zebra finches: implications of sexual and species differences for recognition research. Behav Processes. 2009;80(1):90–8. pmid:19010401
  4. 4. Pfennig KS, Pfennig DW. Character displacement: ecological and reproductive responses to a common evolutionary problem. Q Rev Biol. 2009;84(3):253–76. pmid:19764283
  5. 5. Pfennig DW, Pfennig KS. Character Displacement and the Origins of Diversity. Am Nat. 2010;176 Suppl 1:S26–44.
  6. 6. Yamaguchi R, Iwasa Y. Reproductive character displacement by the evolution of female mate choice. Evol Ecol Res. 2013;15(1):25–41.
  7. 7. Macarthur R, Levins R. Competition, Habitat Selection, And Character Displacement In A Patchy Environment. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1964; 51:1207–10. pmid:14215645
  8. 8. Lemmon EM. Diversification Of Conspecific Signals In Sympatry: Geographic Overlap Drives Multidimensional Reproductive Character Displacement In Frogs. Evolution. 2009;63(5):1155–70. pmid:19210538
  9. 9. Kozak HL, Cirino LA, Ptacek MB. Female mating preferences for male morphological traits used in species and mate recognition in the Mexican sailfin mollies, Poecilia velifera and Poecilia petenensis. Behav Ecol. 2008;19(3):463–74.
  10. 10. Gröning J, Hochkirch A. Reproductive interference between animal species. Q Rev Biol. 2008;83(3):257–82. pmid:18792662
  11. 11. Rundle HD, Schluter D. Reinforcement of Stickleback Mate Preferences: Sympatry Breeds Contempt. Evolution. 1998;52(1):200–8.
  12. 12. Noor MAF. Reinforcement and other consequences of sympatry. Heredity. 1999;83(5):503–8.
  13. 13. Gabor CR, Ryam MJ. Geographical variation in reproductive character displacement in mate choice by male sailfin mollies. Proc Biol Sci. 2001;268(1471):1063–70. pmid:11375091
  14. 14. Servedio MR. The what and why of research on reinforcement. PLoS Biol. 2004;2(12):e420. pmid:15597115
  15. 15. Höbel G, Gerhardt HC. Reproductive Character Displacement In The Acoustic Communication System Of Green Tree Frogs (Hyla cinerea). Evolution. 2003;57(4):894–904. pmid:12778558
  16. 16. Servedio MR, Noor MAF. The Role of Reinforcement in Speciation: Theory and Data. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 2003;34(1):339–64.
  17. 17. Dyer KA, White BE, Sztepanacz JL, Bewick ER, Rundle HD. Reproductive Character Displacement Of Epicuticular Compounds And Their Contribution To Mate Choice In Drosophila subquinaria and Drosophila recens. Evolution. 2014;68(4):1163–75. pmid:24351014
  18. 18. Richards-Zawacki CL, Cummings ME. Intraspecific Reproductive Character Displacement In A Polymorphic Poison Dart Frog, Dendrobates pumilio. Evolution. 2010;65(1):259–67. pmid:20825478
  19. 19. Seddon N, Tobias JA. Character displacement from the receiver's perspective: species and mate recognition despite convergent signals in suboscine birds. Proc Biol Sci 2010;277(1693):2475–83. pmid:20375056
  20. 20. Ortiz-Barrientos D, Grealy A, Nosil P. The Genetics and Ecology of Reinforcement. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1168(1):156–82.
  21. 21. Chek AA, Bogart JP, Lougheed SC. Mating signal partitioning in multi-species assemblages: a null model test using frogs. Ecol Letters. 2003;6(3):235–47.
  22. 22. Morris M. Sexual difference in signal-receiver coevolution. Anim Behav 1996;52(5):1017–24.
  23. 23. Gerhardt HC. Reproductive character displacement of female mate choice in the grey treefrog, Hyla chrysoscelis. Anim Behav. 1994;47(4):959–69.
  24. 24. Barton NH. The role of hybridization in evolution. Mol Ecol. 2001;10(3):551–68. pmid:11298968
  25. 25. Kirschel ANG, Blumstein DT, Smith TB. Character displacement of song and morphology in African tinkerbirds. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(20):8256–61. pmid:19420223
  26. 26. Arnold ML, Martin NH. Hybrid fitness across time and habitats. Trends Ecol Evol. 2010;25(9):530–6. pmid:20598770
  27. 27. Seehausen O. Hybridization and adaptive radiation. Trends Ecol Evol. 2004;19(4):198–207. pmid:16701254
  28. 28. Burke JM, Arnold ML. Genetics and the Fitness of Hybrids. Annu Rev Genet. 2001;35(1):31–52.
  29. 29. Pillay N, Rymer TL. Behavioural divergence, interfertility and speciation: A review. Behav Processes. 2012;91(3):223–35. pmid:22982487
  30. 30. Bacquet PMB, Brattström O, Wang HL, Allen CE, Löfstedt C, Brakefield PM, et al. Selection on male sex pheromone composition contributes to butterfly reproductive isolation. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2015;282(1804):20142734. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.2734
  31. 31. Heubel KU, Plath M. Influence of male harassment and female competition on female feeding behaviour in a sexual–asexual mating complex of mollies (Poecilia mexicana, P. formosa). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2008;62(11):1689–99.
  32. 32. Chapman T, Arnqvist G, Bangham J, Rowe L. Sexual conflict. Trends Ecol Evol 2003;18(1):41–7.
  33. 33. Clutton-Brock TH, Parker GA. Sexual coercion in animal societies. Anim Behav. 1995;49:1345–65.
  34. 34. Gavrilets S, Arnqvist G. The evolution of female mate choice by sexual conflict. Proc Biol Sci. 2001;268:531–9. pmid:11296866
  35. 35. Soltis J, Mitsunaga F, Shimizu K, Yanagihara Y, Nozaki M. Sexual selection in Japanese macaques I: female mate choice or male sexual coercion? Anim Behav. 1997;54(3):725–36. pmid:9299056
  36. 36. Magurran AE. Sexual Coercion. In: Evans JP, Pilastro A, Schlupp I. Ecology and Evolution of Poeciliid Fishes. The University of Chicago Press; 2010.
  37. 37. Head ML, Vega-Trejo R, Jacomb F, Jennions MD. Predictors of male insemination success in the mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki). Ecol Evol. 2015;5(21):4999–5006. pmid:26640677
  38. 38. Kodric-Brown A, Nicoletto P. Female choice in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata): the interaction between male color and display. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2001;50(4):346–51.
  39. 39. Kodric-Brown A. Female Choice of Multiple Male Criteria in Guppies: Interacting Effects of Dominance, Coloration and Courtship. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1993;32:415–20.
  40. 40. Bastiaans E, Bastiaans MJ, Morinaga G, Castañeda Gaytán JG, Marshall JC, Bane B, et al. Female Preference for Sympatric vs. Allopatric Male Throat Color Morphs in the Mesquite Lizard (Sceloporus grammicus) Species Complex. PLoS One. 2014;9(4):e93197. pmid:24718297
  41. 41. Williams TH, Mendelson TC. Behavioral Isolation Based on Visual Signals in a Sympatric Pair of Darter Species. Ethology. 2010;116(11):1038–49.
  42. 42. Miller RR, Minckley WL, Norris SM,. Freshwater Fishes of México. University of Chicago Press; 2005.
  43. 43. Lyons J, Navarro-Perez S. Fishes of the Sierra de Manantlan, West-Central Mexico. Southwest Nat. 1990;35(1):32–46.
  44. 44. Katzir G. Visual Aspects of Species Recognition in the Damselfish Dascyllus aruanus L (Pisces, Pomacentridae). Anim Behav. 1981;29:842–9.
  45. 45. Fisher HS, Mascuch SJ, Rosenthal GG. Multivariate male traits misalign with multivariate female preferences in the swordtail fish, Xiphophorus birchmanni. Anim Behav. 2009;78:265–9.
  46. 46. Morris MR, Mussel M, Ryan MJ. Vertical bars on male Xiphophorus multilineatus: a signal that deters rival males an attracts females. Behav Ecol. 1995;6:274–9.
  47. 47. Engeszer RE, Wang G, Ryan MJ, Parichy DM. Sex-specific perceptual spaces for a vertebrate basal social aggregative behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(3):929–33. pmid:18199839
  48. 48. Endler JA. Natural and sexual selection on color patterns in poeciliid fishes. Environ Biol Fish. 1983;9:173–90.
  49. 49. Endler JA. Natural Selection on Color Patterns in Poecilia reticulata. Evolution. 1980;34(1):76–91.
  50. 50. Olson VA, Owens IPF. Costly sexual signals: are carotenoids rare, risky or required? Trends Ecol Evol. 1998;13(12):510–4. pmid:21238418
  51. 51. Rosenthal GG. Using Video Playback to Study Sexual Communication. Environ Biol Fish. 1999;56:307–16.
  52. 52. Mateos M. Comparative phylogeography of livebearing fishes in the genera Poeciliopsis and Poecilia (Poeciliidae: Cyprinodontiformes) in central Mexico. J Biogeogr. 2005;32(5):775–80.
  53. 53. Mateos M, Sanjur OI, Vrijenhoek RC. Historical Biogeography Of The Livebearing Fish Genus Poeciliopsis (Poeciliidae: Cyprinodontiformes). Evolution. 2002;56(5):972–84. pmid:12093032
  54. 54. Grether GF, Hudon J, Endler JA. Carotenoid scarcity, synthetic pteridine pigments and the evolution of sexual coloration in guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Proc Biol Sci. 2001;268(1473):1245–53. pmid:11410150
  55. 55. Kodric-Brown A. Dietary carotenoids and male mating success in the guppy: an environmental component to female choice. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1989;25:393–401.
  56. 56. Johnson JB, Basolo AL. Predator exposure alters female mate choice in the green swordtail. Behav Ecol. 2003;14(5):619–25.
  57. 57. Franck D, Müller A, Rogmann N. A colour and size dimorphism in the green swordtail (population Jalapa): female mate choice, male-male competition, and male mating strategies. Acta Ethol. 2003;5(2):75–9.
  58. 58. Basolo AL. Variation between and within the sexes in body size preferences. Anim Behav. 2004;68(1):75–82.
  59. 59. Schlupp I, Riesch R, Tobler M, Plath M, Parzefall J, Schartl M. A novel, sexually selected trait in poeciliid fishes: female preference for mustache-like, rostral filaments in male Poecilia sphenops. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2010;64:1849–55.
  60. 60. Head ML, Jacomb F, Vega-Trejo R, Jennions MD. Male mate choice and insemination success under simultaneous versus sequential choice conditions. Anim Behav. 2015;103:99–105.
  61. 61. Culumber ZW, Ochoa OM, Rosenthal GG. Assortative Mating and the Maintenance of Population Structure in a Natural Hybrid Zone. Am Nat. 2014;184(2):225–32. pmid:25058282
  62. 62. Bisazza A, Vaccari G, Pilastro A. Female mate choice in a mating system dominated by male sexual coercion. Behav Ecol. 2001;12(1):59–64.
  63. 63. Kahn AT, Livingston JD, Jennions MD. Do females preferentially associate with males given a better start in life? Biol Lett. 2012;11:1–3.
  64. 64. Cummings ME, Larkins-Ford J, Reilly CRL, Wong RY, Ramsey M, Hoffmann HA. Sexual and social stimuli elicit rapid and contrasting genomic responses. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008;275:393–402.
  65. 65. Lynch KS, Ramsey ME, Cummings ME. The mate choice brain: comparing gene profiles between female choice and male coercive poeciliids. Genes Brain Behav. 2012;11(2):222–9. pmid:22008245
  66. 66. Hughes AL. Male size, mating success, and mating strategy in the mosquitofish Gambusia affinis (Poeciliidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1985;17:271–8.
  67. 67. Wong B, Rosenthal GG. Shoal choice in swordtails when preferences conflict. Ethology. 2005;111:179–86.
  68. 68. Booksmythe I, Backwell PRY, Jennions MD. Competitor size, male mating success and mate choice in eastern mosquitofish, Gambusia holbrooki. Anim Behav. 2013;85(2):371–5.
  69. 69. R Core Development Team. R: A Language And Environment For Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2010.
  70. 70. Louviere JJ, Hensher DA, Swait JD. Choosing a choice model. In: Louviere JJ, Hensher DA, Swait JD, editors. Stated choice methods: analysis and applications. Cambridge Univ Press; 2000. pp. 34–72.
  71. 71. Franz M, van der Post D, Schülke O, Ostner J. The evolution of cooperative turn-taking in animal conflict. BMC Evol Biol. 2011;11(1):323.
  72. 72. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Revised Edition. Academic Press; 1977.
  73. 73. Mateos M, Vrijenhoek RC. Independent Origins of Allotriploidy in the Fish Genus Poeciliopsis. J Heredity. 2005;96(1):32–9.
  74. 74. Prehn LM, Rasch EM. Cytogenetic Studies Of Poecilia(Pisces). I. Chromosome Numbers Of Naturally Occurring Poeciliid Species And Their Hybrids From Eastern Mexico. Can J Genet Cytol. 1969;11(4):880–95. pmid:5392623
  75. 75. Pilastro A, Benetton S, Bisazza A. Female aggregation and male competition reduce costs of sexual harassment in the mosquitofish Gambusia holbrooki. Anim Behav. 2003;65(6):1161–7.
  76. 76. Schlupp I, McKnab R, Ryan MJ. Sexual harassment as a cost for molly females: bigger males cost less. Behaviour. 2001;138:277–86.
  77. 77. Makowicz AM, Schlupp I. The direct costs of living in a sexually harassing environment. Anim Behav. 2013;85(3):569–77.
  78. 78. Magurran AE, Seghers BH. A Cost of Sexual Harassment in the Guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci.1994;258:89–92.
  79. 79. Köhler A, Hildenbrand P, Schleucher E, Riesch R, Arias-Rodriguez L, Streit B, et al. Effects of male sexual harassment on female time budgets, feeding behavior, and metabolic rates in a tropical livebearing fish (Poecilia mexicana). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011;65(8):1513–23.
  80. 80. Culumber ZW, Bautista-Hernández CE, Monks S, Arias-Rodriguez L, Tobler M. Variation in Melanism and Female Preference in Proximate but Ecologically Distinct Environments. Ethology. 2014;120(11):1090–100.
  81. 81. Fernandez AA, Morris MR. Mate choice for more melanin as a mechanism to maintain a functional oncogene. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(36):13503–7. pmid:18757731
  82. 82. Ptacek MB, Travis J. Mate Choice in the Sailfin Molly, Poecilia latipinna. Evolution. 1997;51(4):1217.
  83. 83. Grant PR. Evolution of Character Displacement in Darwin's Finches. Science. 2006;313(5784):224–6. pmid:16840700
  84. 84. Schluter D. Character displacement and the adaptive divergence of finches on islands and continents. Am Nat. 1988;131:799–824.
  85. 85. Schluter D, McPhail JD. Character displacement and replicate adaptive radiation. Trends Ecol Evol. 1993;8(6):197–200. pmid:21236147
  86. 86. Slatkin M. Ecological Character Displacement. Ecology. 1980;61(1):163.
  87. 87. Reznick D, Butler MJ IV, Rodd H. Life-History Evolution in Guppies. VII. The Comparative Ecology of High- and Low-Predation Environments. Am Nat. 2001;157(2):126–40. pmid:18707267
  88. 88. Langerhans RB, Layman CA, Shokrollahi AM, DeWitt TJ. Predator-Driven Phenotypic Diversification In Gambusia affinis. Evolution. 2004;58(10):2305–18. pmid:15562692
  89. 89. Langerhans BR, DeWitt TJ. Shared and Unique Features of Evolutionary Diversification. Am Nat. 2006;164(3):335–49.
  90. 90. Johnson JB. Divergent life histories among populations of the fish Brachyrhaphis rhabdophora: detecting putative agents of selection by candidate model analysis. Oikos. 2002;96(1):82–91.
  91. 91. Ingley SJ, Billman EJ, Belk MC, Johnson JB. Morphological Divergence Driven by Predation Environment within and between Species of Brachyrhaphis Fishes. PLoS One. 2014;9(2):e90274. pmid:24587309
  92. 92. Horth L. Predation and the persistence of melanic male mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki). J Evol Biol. 2004;17(3):672–9. pmid:15149409
  93. 93. Endler JA. Predation, Light-Intensity and Courtship Behavior in Poecilia reticulata (Pisces, Poeciliidae). Anim Behav. 1987;35:1376–85.
  94. 94. Scott LE, Johnson JB. Does sympatry predict life history and morphological diversification in the Mexican livebearing fish Poeciliopsis baenschi? Biol J Linn Soc Lond. 2010;100(3):608–18.