Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Handedness is a biomarker of variation in anal sex role behavior and Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity among gay men

  • Ashlyn Swift-Gallant,

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto at Mississauga, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada

  • Lindsay A. Coome,

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto at Mississauga, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada

  • D. Ashley Monks,

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto at Mississauga, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada

  • Doug P. VanderLaan

    doug.vanderlaan@utoronto.ca

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto at Mississauga, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada, Child, Youth and Family Division, Underserved Populations Research Program, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH), Toronto, Ontario, Canada

Abstract

Developmental theories of the biological basis of sexual orientation suggest that sexually differentiated psychological and behavioural traits should be linked with sexual orientation. Subgroups of gay men delineated by anal sex roles differ according to at least one such trait: gender expression. The present study assessed the hypothesis that handedness, a biologically determined sexually differentiated trait, corresponds to differences in subgroups of gay men based on anal sex role. Furthermore, it assessed whether handedness mediates the association between gender nonconformity and male sexual orientation. Straight and gay men (N = 333) completed the Edinburgh Inventory of Handedness and the Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity Scale. Gay men also completed measures of anal sex role preference. As in previous studies, gay men showed greater non-right-handedness and gender nonconformity than straight men. Also, among gay men, bottoms/versatiles (i.e., gay men who take a receptive anal sex role, or who take on both a receptive and insertive anal sex role) were more gender-nonconforming than tops (i.e., gay men who take an insertive anal sex role). In support of the hypothesis, bottoms/versatiles were more non-right-handed than tops and handedness mediated the male sexual orientation and anal sex role differences in Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity. Together, these findings suggest that developmental processes linked to handedness underpin variation among men in sexual orientation and gender nonconformity as well as variation among subgroups of gay men that are delineated by anal sex roles.

Introduction

Handedness, or predominantly using the left or right hand for various tasks, is a marker of cerebral lateralization (for review, see [1]). Extensive research suggests that handedness is influenced by genetic factors and is also linked to sexually dimorphic endocrine and immunological mechanisms [25]. Although the factors affecting handedness are still under investigation, there is consensus that it is determined prenatally by biological factors (for review, see [6]).

Given this prenatal, biological, basis of handedness, the higher prevalence of non-right-handedness among men compared to women suggests that it also marks prenatal developmental processes influencing brain sexual differentiation (for review, see [7]). As such, handedness has been examined in relation to within-sex variation in psychological and behavioral domains that show large sexual dimorphism to provide insight into whether mechanisms underpinning handedness also influence these domains. Namely, these domains include gender expression and sexual partner preference.

In both sexes, non-right-handedness has been associated with elevated gender nonconformity. Left-handed women are more male-typical on a gender conformity scale (i.e., Bem Sex Role Inventory: [8, 9]) and highly gender-nonconforming natal male children are more non-right-handed [10]. A higher prevalence of non-right-handedness has also been reported in male-to-female and female-to-male transsexuals, regardless of sexual orientation, compared to male and female controls [1114]. Furthermore, gay men and lesbian women, who show elevated gender nonconformity [15], also show higher rates of non-right-handedness compared to straight men and women [1618].

Of note, non-right-handedness appears to be a masculine trait in that it is more common among straight men than straight women, and among lesbian women than straight women. However, non-right-handedness is also more common among gay men—who are more feminine on average—than straight men. One possible explanation for this apparent paradox is that non-right-handedness has a non-linear association with sexual differentiation among males. Specifically, among straight men non-right-handedness might be associated with greater male-typical gender expression whereas among gay men it might be associated with greater female-typical gender expression. As such, across straight and gay men, one would predict a curvilinear association between handedness and gender (non)conformity.

Handedness has yet to be examined in relation to the variation in gender nonconformity that exists both between and within sexual orientation groups. Whereas a number of studies have shown sexual orientation differences in gender nonconformity, only more recently has quantitative research begun to identify behavioral correlates of variation in gender expression among subgroups of same-sex attracted individuals. Among gay men, variation in gender nonconformity appears to correspond with variation in anal sex role behavior. Specifically, gay men with an insertive anal sex role (i.e., tops) scored higher on masculine personality traits compared to males with a receptive (i.e., bottom) anal sex role preference (i.e., bottoms; [19]). Conversely, bottoms scored higher on feminine personality traits compared to tops. Consistent with these reports, Moskowitz and Hart [20] found that self-identified tops rated themselves as more masculine compared to bottoms, and Zheng, Hart, and Zheng [21] found that tops were more likely to score higher on male-typical cognitive styles while bottoms were higher on female-typical cognitive styles.

The present study examined the hypothesis that handedness—a trait that is putatively tied to prenatal brain development, including brain sexual differentiation—is associated with variation in gender nonconformity both between and within male sexual orientation groups. One prediction tested was that, across straight and gay men, a curvilinear association exists between handedness and gender (non)conformity such that non-right-handedness is associated with greater gender conformity among straight men and greater gender nonconformity among gay men. In addition, based on research indicating a relationship between bottom anal sex role with greater female-typicality, and top anal sex role with greater male-typicality, it was predicted that these groups should also differ in handedness, with those who report bottom anal sex role being more non-right-handed. Lastly, it was examined whether handedness mediates sexual orientation as well as anal sex role-related differences in gender nonconformity.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were recruited at the 2015 Toronto Pride Festival and on Facebook. At the Toronto Pride Festival, experimenters explained to festival attendees that they were researchers from the University of Toronto conducting a study on developmental theories of sexual orientation. Experimenters offered a business card with the survey’s website, and/or recorded their email addresses as a means to send the website address for our online survey. Facebook advertisements targeted men who indicated that they were interested in men or men who indicated that they were interested in women. Only those 18 years of age and older were targeted, and the advertisements were presented only to those who spoke English. The locations selected included countries in which English is a first language (i.e., Canada, United States, New Zealand, United Kingdom, Australia). We emailed details regarding how to complete our online questionnaire to 459 people from the Toronto Pride festival, and our Facebook advertisements reached 56,155 people. A total of 598 participants (56 from Toronto Pride and 542 from Facebook) completed > 90% of the items on our questionnaire. Participants were excluded from the present study if they did not report their age, sexual orientation or, in the case of gay men, their anal sex role preference. Of the 598 participants who completed > 90% of the questionnaire, 159 did not identify their sexual orientation and 208 did not report their age. Of the remaining participants, 242 gay men indicated their preferred anal sex role, and 222 also reported their anal sex role behavior. The questionnaire was hosted on Qualtrics using their survey software. The study was approved and conducted in accordance with the guidelines from the Human Research Protections Program at the University of Toronto. The letter of information and informed consent forms were presented to participants via Qualtrics prior to the questionnaire. Participants had to click a box indicating that they read the ethics statement and agreed to participate. Consent and questionnaire data were encrypted on a computer and password protected.

The final participant numbers were as follows for all analyses excluding the anal sex role behavior analyses: straight men: n = 91; all gay men: n = 242; bottoms: n = 91; versatiles: n = 115; tops: n = 36. For the analyses on anal sex role behavior, the total number of participants were as follows: straight men: n = 91; all gay men: n = 222; bottoms: n = 96; versatiles: n = 69; tops: n = 57. The mean (SD) age for straight men was 32.26 (15.23) years and 33.61 (13.47) years for gay men. The mean (SD) ages for anal sex role preference groups were as follows: bottoms were 33.27 (13.28), versatiles were 33.83 (13.65), and tops were 33.80 (13.77). The mean (SD) ages for anal sex role behavior groups were as follows: bottoms were 32.21 (13.35), versatiles were 34.03 (13.84), and tops were 35.47 (13.21). Racial self-identification was as follows: 301 participants classified themselves as White, 1 Black, 2 Chinese, 1 Filipino, 2 Aboriginal, 3 Latin American, 3 Southeast Asian, 1 West Asian, 2 Korean, 1 Japanese, 15 indicated “other,” and 1 participant declined to answer.

Measures

Sexual orientation was defined by both self-identification (i.e., straight, gay, bisexual or other) and sexual attraction to the opposite- (i.e., straight) or same-sex (i.e., gay) in the last year. Specifically, participants were asked the gender(s) of individuals they were sexually attracted to in the last twelve months (similar to [22]). Responses were limited to a 5-point scale: 1) only females, never to males, to 5) only males, never females. An option was also available for those who identified as asexual–“I have never engaged in sexual behaviour. Examples of sexual experience were given in parentheses “(e.g., petting, kissing, oral sex, intercourse).” Men who identified as straight and indicated an attraction to females only and never to males in the last year were categorized as straight. Men who identified as gay and indicated they were only attracted to males, never to females, in the last year were categorized as gay. All others who were not concordant for sexual attraction in the last year and their self-identified sexual orientation were excluded from analyses. In total, 39 participants were excluded: 14 were straight men (all of whom stated “more often [attracted] to females but at least once to a male”) and 25 were gay men (all of whom stated “more often [attracted] to males but at least once to a female”). These predominantly gay or straight men were excluded because of recent evidence suggesting that mostly heterosexual individuals differ from exclusively heterosexual individuals on correlates of sexual orientation (for review, see [23]), and gay participants were defined similarly to be consistent across categories.

Anal sex role preference and anal sex role behavior were self-labeled by participants. Specifically, individuals who identified as gay men were asked whether they engage in and/or fantasize about anal sex. If the participant answered “yes,” they were asked (1) which role they prefer to take when engaging in and/or fantasizing about anal sex (i.e., preference), and (2) which role they usually take when engaging in anal sex (i.e., behavior). For both questions, their response options included “top,” “versatile,” or “bottom.” For the second question (regarding which role they usually take), they were also given the option to select “Not sufficient experience with anal sex to answer.

Handedness was assessed using the 10-item Edinburgh inventory (writing, throwing, toothbrush, spoon, drawing, scissors, knife [without fork], striking a match, opening a box [lid], broom [upper hand]). Answers were provided on a 5-point likert scale: 1-always right to 5-always left. Scores for each of the 10 items were summed, which resulted in scores ranging from 10 (i.e., always use the right hand) to 50 (i.e., always use the left hand).

Gender nonconformity in the present study was assessed using the Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity Scale [24]. This 23-item questionnaire is scored such that high scores are male-typical and, therefore, low scores are gender-nonconforming. We present the data for a subset of the scale (18 items belonging to Factor 1 as described in [24]). This scale included questions such as “As a child, my best or closest friend was 1-always a boy, to 5-always a girl”, and “In fantasy or pretend play, I took the role 1-only of boys or men, to 5-only of girls or women

Statistical analyses

Gay and straight men were compared on age, Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity and Handedness using independent sample t-tests. One-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were used for the following group comparisons on Age, Handedness and Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity scale: (1) straight men and gay men categorized by anal sex role preference, and (2) straight men and gay men categorized by anal sex role behavior. As reported below, anal sex role preference groups did not differ on handedness, whereas anal sex role behavior group differences on handedness were found. Thus, further analyses compared anal sex role behavior groups, rather than preference groups. Furthermore, bottom and versatile anal sex role behavior groups were collapsed for analyses because these two groups were homogenous with respect to the focal variables of Handedness and Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity (Cohen’s d for handedness = 0.05 and for gender nonconformity = 0.04).

Following Baron and Kenny [25], we evaluated whether handedness mediated the relationship between gender nonconformity and sexual orientation—unstandardized coefficients (B) are reported. Next, we evaluated whether handedness mediated the relationship between gender nonconformity and anal sex role behavior. Lastly, we observed a curvilinear relationship between handedness and gender nonconformity (described below), such that the relationship ran in opposite directions for gay and straight men, and therefore we performed moderated mediation for handedness on the relationship between gender nonconformity among gay and straight men (i.e., Handedness X Sexual Orientation—details below). Sobel’s tests were conducted for all mediation analyses and one-tailed p-values were used given that the direction of the effect being tested was predicted a priori and known based on the results of the mediation analysis. Fisher’s Least Significant Difference (LSD) tests were used to examine pair-wise group differences in cases where the omnibus ANOVA indicated a significant overall effect. Significance was set to α = .05.

Results and discussion

Age

No difference in age was found between gay and straight men, t(331) = 0.349, p = .727. Similarly, no differences in age were found between preferred anal sex role groups of gay men and straight men based on preference, F(3, 329) = .104, p = .958, nor when based on behavior, F(3, 309) = .877, p = .453.

Sexual orientation and Handedness

Descriptive statistics for the handedness scale of gay and straight men, as well as anal sex role preference and anal sex role behavior subgroups, are shown in Table 1. Consistent with previous research, gay men were less right-handed compared to straight men, t(331) = 2.319, p = .021.

thumbnail
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the Handedness scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170241.t001

Anal sex role and Handedness

A one-way ANOVA evaluating Handedness between straight men and gay men categorized based on their anal sex preferences was not significant, F(3, 329) = 2.094, p = .125.

A significant effect was found between straight men and anal sex behavior groups on the Handedness scale, F(3, 313) = 4.575, p = .011. Posthoc analyses indicated that gay men who reported bottom/versatile anal sex as their typical sexual behavior were less right-handed compared to straight men and top gay men, p = .022 and p = .011, respectively. Top gay men did not differ from straight gay men, p = .581.

Gender nonconformity

Descriptive statistics for the Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity scale of gay and straight men, as well as anal sex role preference and anal sex role behavior subgroups, are shown in Table 2.

thumbnail
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for the Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170241.t002

Gay men were significantly more gender-nonconforming on the Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity scale compared to straight men, t(331) = 9.492, p < .001.

Anal sex role preference groups significantly differed on the Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity scale, F(3, 329) = 23.910, p < .001. Posthoc tests indicated that preferred bottoms/versatiles were significantly more gender-nonconforming compared to tops, p = .048, and straight men, p < .001. Preferred tops were also more gender-nonconforming compared to straight men, p < .001.

Similarly, anal sex role behavior groups significantly differed on the Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity scale, F(3, 309) = 22.429, p < .001. Bottoms/versatiles were more gender-nonconforming compared to tops, p = .032, and straight men, p < .001. Tops were also more gender-nonconforming compared to straight men, p < .001.

Correlation analyses

Among gay men, a significant negative correlation was found between Handedness and Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity scores, r(242) = -.175, p = .006, indicating that increased non-right-handedness is associated with more gender nonconformity. Among straight men, these variables showed a marginally significant positive correlation that was nevertheless similar in magnitude (although notably opposite in direction) to the correlation found for gay men, r(91) = .19, p = .072. Although the correlation for straight men did not reach significance, the effect size suggests that this effect is of the same magnitude as that among gay men; thus, with a larger sample size, it is likely that this correlation would emerge as significant [for a review discussing the importance of considering both statistical significant and effect size, see 26]. Coupled with the findings reported above regarding male sexual orientation differences for these variables, these results suggest that there is a nonlinear relationship between gender nonconformity and handedness across the entire sample. Indeed, a significant curvilinear relationship was found, R2 (331) = .028, p = .009, such that participants with low and high handedness scores were more gender conforming compared to participants with mixed handedness/intermediate handedness scores (see Fig 1). This quadratic model accounted for more variance than the linear model, R2 (332) = .024, p = .003. Furthermore, the sample size composition from the present study is comprised of a majority of gay men (70.6%), thus it would be predicted that the magnitude of difference between quadratic and linear models would increase as the sample approached a sample comprised of 50% gay and 50% straight men.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Curvilinear relationship between Handedness and Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity.

A significant curvilinear relationship was found between Handedness and Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity such that participants with low and high handedness scores were more gender-conforming compared to participants with mixed handedness/intermediate handedness scores. Anal sex role preference groups are plotted as follows: circles represent top gay men, squares are versatile gay men, diamonds are bottom gay men and ‘x’ represent straight men. The solid line depicts the curvilinear relationship, whereas the dotted line represents the linear association for straight men, and the dash line represents the linear association for gay men.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170241.g001

Moderated mediation analyses

The correlations between Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity and Handedness scores indicated that the association between these variables runs in opposite directions for gay vs. straight men. As such, straight men’s Handedness scores were multiplied by -1 to create an appropriate interaction term to test for the moderated mediation effect of Handedness on the Sexual Orientation difference in Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity. The straight males were the reference group (see Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Moderated mediation of the Handedness by sexual orientation interaction on the male sexual orientation difference in Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity.

A significant moderated mediation effect was found for Handedness on the male Sexual Orientation difference in Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170241.g002

In step 1 of the mediation model, the regression of the Handedness interaction term onto Sexual Orientation was significant, B = 32.158, t(331) = 29.041, p < .001. For the second step, the regression of Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity onto Sexual Orientation was also significant, B = -9.351, t(331) = -8.115, p < .001. For the final regression, the regression of Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity onto Sexual Orientation was no longer significant when the Handedness interaction term was also included in the model, B = -3.437, t(330) = -1.606, p = .109. Handedness significantly predicted Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity in this last step, B = -.184, t(330) = -3.262, p = .001. A Sobel’s test indicated a significant moderated mediation effect of Handedness on the male Sexual Orientation difference in Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity, Sobel’s z = -3.265, one-tailed p < .001.

Handedness was evaluated as a moderated mediator for Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity differences among anal sex role behavior groups. We used anal sex role behavior rather than anal sex role preference for these analyses because (as reported above) group differences in Handedness reached the critical alpha of < .05 when compared between anal sex role behavior, but not preference, groups. Again, straight males’ Handedness scores were multiplied by -1, but this time bottoms/versatiles were the reference group because they differed from tops and straight men on Handedness whereas these latter groups showed no such difference (See Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Moderated mediation of the Handedness by sexual orientation interaction on the anal sex role behavior difference in Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity.

Bottom/versatile groups were used as the reference group. Results support a partial moderated mediation effect for Handedness on the anal sex role behavior difference in Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170241.g003

In step 1 of the moderated mediation model, the regressions of Handedness onto anal sex role behavior groups were significant: straight men, B = -32.658, t(310) = -28.772, p < .001 and tops, B = -3.426, t(310) = -2.565, p = .011. For the second step, the regressions of Gender Nonconformity onto anal sex role behavior groups were significant: straight men, B = 9.993, t(310) = 8.216, p < .001, and tops, B = 3.097, t(310) = 2.157, p = .032. For the final regressions, with Handedness included in the model, straight men, B = 4.637, t(309) = 2.011, p = .045, and tops, B = 2.526, t(309) = 1.764, p = .079, were less significantly different on Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity compared to bottoms/versatiles, suggesting partial moderated mediation. Handedness significantly predicted Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity in this last step, B = -.164, t(309) = -2.722, p = .007. The Sobel’s test was significant for straight men, Sobel’s z = 2.721, one-tailed p = .003, and tops, Sobel’s z = 1.87, one-tailed p = .035. These findings again suggest a moderated mediation effect of Handedness on Sexual Orientation differences in Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity, and also Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity differences among subgroups of gay men delineated by anal sex role behavior.

Conclusions

Consistent with previous research, male sexual orientation and anal sex role were associated with Recalled Childhood Gender Nonconformity scale scores in our sample. Gay men were more gender-nonconforming compared to straight men and gay men with a bottom/versatile anal sex role were more gender-nonconforming compared to both gay men with a top anal sex role and straight men. We also replicated the finding that gay men are more non-right-handed compared to straight men. We evaluated handedness among gay men grouped by their anal sex roles, and found that bottoms/versatiles were more non-right-handed compared to tops and straight men. Of particular importance, the present study suggests that differences in gender nonconformity between straight and gay men and among anal sex role subgroups of gay men are influenced by prenatal mechanisms linked to handedness.

Across both gay and straight men, a curvilinear relationship was found between handedness and gender (non)conformity. High (left) and low (right) handedness scores were associated with more recalled childhood gender conformity relative to participants with mixed/intermediate handedness scores; this pattern appeared to occur because the association between gender conformity and non-right-handedness is positive among straight men and negative among gay men (see Fig 1). Furthermore, moderated mediation analyses indicated that handedness mediated the overall male sexual orientation difference as well as anal sex role subgroup differences in gender (non)conformity (see Figs 2 and 3). To our knowledge, these are the first results to suggest that developmental processes related to handedness explain variation in gender expression both between straight and gay men as well as within these groups.

The biological mechanisms underlying handedness are not entirely clear. One prominent hypothesis has been that the sex difference is related to sex differences in prenatal testosterone [2]. If this hypothesis holds true, it would correspond well with the neurohormonal theory [27, 28] of sexual orientation development, which postulates that the prenatal surge in testosterone promotes male-typical brain development, giving rise to male-typical behaviors, including male-typical gender patterns and sexual partner preference (i.e., a preference for women). In males, female-typical gender expression and same-sex attraction are thought to result from a relative lack of prenatal testosterone exposure, resulting in female-typical brain development and, thus, female-typical behavior and sexual partner preference (i.e., a preference for men).

Evidence suggests that higher levels of prenatal testosterone are associated with more male-typical play patterns in early childhood in boys and girls [29, 30]. In addition, a putative marker of prenatal testosterone exposure (i.e., second-to-fourth digit ratio; for review, see [31]) was found to be more female-typical in males with a female gender identity, again suggesting a relationship between low testosterone and female-typical gender development [32]. The evidence-base is further strengthened by studies examining samples of individuals affected by disorders of sex development for which there is relative certainty regarding the prenatal sex hormone milieu. For example, female-typical development and gender identity is common among XY-individuals with androgen insensitivity syndrome, a condition that limits the action of androgens [33]. Likewise, females with conditions associated with elevated prenatal androgens such as congenital adrenal hyperplasia display more male-typical gender patterns than females not affected by the condition [34].

Findings relating prenatal testosterone to male sexual orientation or to handedness have been more mixed. For male sexual orientation, bio-correlates of prenatal testosterone have been found to be more female-typical (e.g., size of the third interstitial nucleus of the anterior hypothalamus: [35]), more male-typical (e.g., penis size: [36]) or not different from straight men (e.g., second-to-fourth digit ratio: [37]). Similarly, non-right-handedness has been linked to high prenatal testosterone (e.g., [3840]) and low prenatal testosterone [4143], while others did not find a relationship between testosterone and handedness [44, 45]. Interestingly, a recent study found evidence for a curvilinear relationship between handedness and markers of androgens, and between markers of androgens and androphilic attraction; quadratic effects are not frequently evaluated, which may account for the inconsistencies in this literature [46]. Specifically, Ellis, Skorska, and Bogaert [46] found: (a) a curvilinear relationship between handedness and markers of androgen in women, and a linear relationship between these variables in men (although the authors suggest that the relationship appears curvilinear for men as well, but the linear model, not the quadratic, was significant), and (b) for both men and women a quadratic relationship between androphilic attraction and markers of androgen.

Variation in androgen sensitivity is another possible explanation for these inconsistent findings (e.g., [47]). The X-chromosome linked androgen receptor gene (AR) has been associated with handedness [47]. Handedness has been associated with the polymorphic number of polyglutamine (CAG) repeats on the AR gene. Individual differences in CAG repeat lengths have been associated with the efficiency of the receptor such that AR variants with longer CAG repeat tracts are thought to function less efficiently (e.g., [48]).

Medland et al., [49] found that longer repeats were associated with left-handedness in females, and that shorter repeats were associated with left-handedness in males. This latter finding suggests that left-handedness is associated with more efficient AR in males and, thus, dovetails with the present data on straight men. Among straight men, a tendency toward non-right-handedness was associated with greater male-typical childhood behavior, possibly due to greater prenatal action of testosterone on brain masculinization.

Conversely, mixed handedness (i.e., the use of right hand for some tasks and left for others) was associated with longer CAG repeats in males in the study by Hampson and Sankar [43]. Arning et al., [47] found similar results: longer CAG-repeats were associated with mixed handedness in both men and women. These findings suggest that mixed handedness is associated with less efficient AR and, thus, dovetails with the present data on gay men, particularly those with a bottom or versatile anal sex role. It may be that this subset of men is particularly likely to have less efficient AR, leading to both non-right-handedness and more female-typical gender expression and sexual partner preference.

In sum, AR appears to be associated with handedness such that relative sensitivity of AR to testosterone might account for variation in handedness. If such is the case, then it is possible that subgroups of gay men delineated by handedness may differ from each other and from straight men in terms of their sensitivity to androgens. Interestingly, sexual orientation has been associated with other X-chromosome loci such as Xq28 (for review, see [50, 51]), although no link has yet been reported with regard to male sexual orientation and AR CAG repeat length [52]. Further research is needed to understand the individual and additive or interactive contributions of endocrine and genetic mechanisms underlying handedness, and this research may indicate these mechanisms as also underlying sexual orientation development.

These mechanisms may also be relevant to heterogeneity among gay men with respect to sexual behavior. Here, handedness significantly differed among anal sex role behavior groups (i.e., tops were more likely to be right-handed), and notably not among anal sex role preference groups. These patterns suggest that handedness is somehow associated with one or more behavioral mechanisms that influence whether a gay man is likely to perform a bottom/versatile vs. top anal sex role. Given that the handedness differences among anal sex role subgroups of gay men corresponded with parallel differences in recalled childhood gender (non)conformity (i.e., tops reported greater gender conformity), gender expression may be one such behavioral mechanism. Indeed, masculinity and stereotypes surrounding masculinity appear to influence decisions around anal sex role identity and sexual positioning practices among men who have sex with men [53]. As such, mechanisms linked with handedness may have an indirect influence on sexual behaviors such as anal sex role via their effects on gender expression, which is putatively important for influencing decisions about sexual positioning.

Although the precise contributions of genetic and hormonal processes to handedness and their relation to sexual orientation remain unclear, it should be noted that it is unlikely that these processes interact with mechanisms related to the fraternal birth order effect (i.e., the well-replicated finding that each additional older brother increases the odds of same-sex sexual attraction among males; [54]). This effect interacts with handedness such that only right-handed gay men exhibit it, whereas mixed- or left-handed gay men do not [16, 5558]. Bogaert [56] suggested that extremely right-handed gay men also do not show the fraternal birth order effect—only moderately right-handed gay men showed the effect. Furthermore, two additional studies did not find an association between the fraternal birth order effect and gender nonconformity [59, 60], whereas handedness mediated variation in gender nonconformity in the present study. Thus, it appears that mechanisms linking male sexual orientation to handedness differ from those linking it to the fraternal birth order effect (for review, see [61]).

In addition to the genetic and hormonal theories for the development of handedness, others have proposed that handedness and sexual orientation may be related to developmental instability [17]. Traits related to developmental instability such as minor physical anomalies and fluctuating asymmetry are more prominent in left-handers and extreme right-handers [62], and sexual orientation is associated with left-handedness and extreme right-handedness [17, 55, 56, 57, 58]. Although two studies [63, 64] did not find support for increased fluctuating asymmetry in gay men, both Miller, Hoffmann, and Mustanski [65] and Hall and Schaeff [66] did find increased fluctuating asymmetries, such as asymmetries in the ear, elbow, knee, ankle and finger length, among gay men compared to straight men. It is possible that developmental instability affects sexual differentiation of the brain and behavior and, thus, might also account for subgroups of gay men who differ in terms of gender nonconformity and/or anal sex role. This hypothesis could be tested by evaluating traits associated with developmental instability (e.g., minor physical anomalies, fluctuating asymmetry) in relation to sexual orientation, gender expression, and anal sex role. If this hypothesis is accurate, one would predict—based on patterns found in the present study—that gay men who are more non-right-handed, gender nonconforming, and bottom/versatiles would show increased traits of developmental instability.

Limitations and future directions

The present study found that handedness mediates the relationship between male sexual orientation and childhood gender nonconformity. Thus, sexual differentiation mechanisms or mechanisms related to developmental instability that influence handedness may also regulate same-sex sexual orientation and gender nonconformity. It would be of interest, therefore, to evaluate the relationship between the prenatal endocrine environment and the AR gene (CAG repeats/sensitivity of the receptor) in relation to handedness, sexual orientation, anal sex role, and gender nonconformity. Although similar studies have been conducted on some of these variables individually or in pairs (e.g., handedness and AR [47], sexual orientation and handedness [16], anal sex role and gender nonconformity [19]), the present study suggests that this line of questioning merits a more comprehensive evaluation of these variables together, and that nonlinear relationships should be considered. It would also be of interest to investigate whether these relationships hold true for females and female same-sex sexual orientation.

Extreme right-handedness has been related to both male sexual orientation [16, 56] and increased gender nonconformity among gay men [58]. The sample size of the present study was insufficient to evaluate extreme right-handedness in relation to anal sex role groups and gender nonconformity. However, given that bottoms/versatiles were more non-right-handed than tops in the present study, it would be interesting to test whether bottoms/versatiles and tops similarly show differences in extreme right-handedness. These lines of investigation will help shed light on the mechanisms underlying sexual orientation, sexual behaviors such as anal sex role, and gender-related traits, as well as the developmental associations among these sexually differentiated traits in humans.

The present findings regarding gender nonconformity are consistent with previous reports suggesting that gay men who adopt a receptive anal sex role display more gender atypicality [19, 20, 21]. Men can experience erotic pleasure when their prostate gland is stimulated via receptive anal sex, and it is not uncommon for masculine straight men to enjoy being anally penetrated by a female partner through the use of objects such as a strap-on dildo [67]. Thus, findings suggesting that gay men who adopt bottom anal sex roles are more gender nonconforming do not necessarily warrant the interpretation that adopting a receptive anal sex role is itself a feminine trait.

The present study also does not necessarily provide any clear indication of how anal sex role might relate to handedness and/or gender nonconformity among straight men. Indeed, whereas non-right-handed gay men were more likely to be gender nonconforming and bottoms/versatiles behaviorally, non-right-handed straight men were more gender conforming. Given that the handedness and gender expression associations ran in opposite directions for gay vs. straight men, whether handedness and gender expression would map onto anal sex role behavior in a similar fashion among members of both groups is equivocal. Addressing this topic could be the focus of future research.

Summary

The present study found that handedness, a sexually differentiated trait, mediates the relationship between sexual orientation and gender nonconformity, suggesting that the prenatal mechanisms underlying handedness also influence male same-sex sexual orientation and gender nonconforming behaviors. Furthermore, the present findings suggest that the biological mechanisms that underlie handedness differentially apply to subgroups of gay men delineated by their anal sex role behavior. Lastly, a curvilinear relationship was found between handedness and gender nonconformity, suggesting that high and low handedness scores are associated with more gender conformity compared to participants with intermediate/mixed handedness. Together, these findings suggest that the developmental processes underlying handedness act in a nonlinear fashion to influence male same-sex sexual orientation and childhood gender nonconformity. Lastly, the observed differences in handedness and gender expression among subgroups of gay men delineated by anal sex roles support the notion that gay men constitute a heterogeneous group with respect to developmental underpinnings of their same-sex sexual orientation.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all our volunteers who assisted in participant recruitment at the Toronto Pride Festival (Mary Loka, Lindsay Melhuish, Phil Nguyen, Lauren Oakley, Martha Pokarowski Firyal Ramzan, Anna Takagi and Robert Wither). The research reported in this article was supported by a doctoral Canada Graduate Scholarship from the National Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) and an Ontario Graduate Scholarship to ASG, was well as by NSERC Discovery Grants to DAM and DPV.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: ASG DAM DPV.
  2. Data curation: ASG.
  3. Formal analysis: ASG LAC DPV.
  4. Funding acquisition: ASG DAM DPV.
  5. Investigation: ASG LAC DPV.
  6. Methodology: ASG LAC DAM DPV.
  7. Project administration: ASG LAC DPV.
  8. Resources: DAM DPV.
  9. Software: LAC.
  10. Supervision: ASG LAC DAM DPV.
  11. Validation: ASG LAC DAM DPV.
  12. Visualization: ASG LAC.
  13. Writing – original draft: ASG DPV.
  14. Writing – review & editing: ASG LAC DAM DPV.

References

  1. 1. Sun T, Walsh CA. Molecular approaches to brain asymmetry and handedness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006 Aug 1;7(8):655–62. pmid:16858393
  2. 2. Geschwind N, Galaburda AM. Cerebral lateralization: Biological mechanisms, associations, and pathology: I. A hypothesis and a program for research. Archives of Neurology. 1985 May 1;42(5):428–59. pmid:3994562
  3. 3. Galaburda AM, Corsiglia J, Rosen GD, Sherman GF. Planum temporale asymmetry, reappraisal since Geschwind and Levitsky. Neuropsychologia. 1987 Jan 1;25(6):853–68.
  4. 4. Geschwind N, Behan P. Left-handedness: Association with immune disease, migraine, and developmental learning disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1982 Aug 1;79(16):5097–100.
  5. 5. Yeo RA, Gangestad SW, Daniel WF. Hand preference and developmental instability. Psychobiology. 1993 Jun 1;21(2):161–8.
  6. 6. Gutwinski S, Löscher A, Mahler L, Kalbitzer J, Heinz A, Bermpohl F. Understanding left-handedness. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2011 Dec 16;108(50):849–53. pmid:22259638
  7. 7. Papadatou-Pastou M, Martin M, Munafo MR, Jones GV. Sex differences in left-handedness: a meta-analysis of 144 studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2008 Sep;134(5):677. pmid:18729568
  8. 8. Casey MB, Nuttall RL. Differences in feminine and masculine characteristics in women as a function of handedness: Support for the Geschwind/Galaburda theory of brain organization. Neuropsychologia. 1990 Dec 31;28(7):749–54. pmid:2215887
  9. 9. Nicholls ME, Forbes S. Handedness and its association with gender-related psychological and physiological characteristics. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1996 Dec 1;18(6):905–10. pmid:9157114
  10. 10. Zucker KJ, Beaulieu N, Bradley SJ, Grimshaw GM, Wilcox A. Handedness in boys with gender identity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2001 Sep 1;42(6):767–76. pmid:11583249
  11. 11. Green R, Young R. Hand preference, sexual preference, and transsexualism. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2001 Dec 1;30(6):565–74. pmid:11725455
  12. 12. Orlebeke JF, Boomsma DI, Gooren LJ, Verschoor AM, Van Den Bree MJ. Elevated sinistrality in transsexuals. Neuropsychology. 1992 Oct;6(4):351.
  13. 13. Watson DB, Coren S. Left-handedness in male-to-female transsexuals. JAMA. 1992 Mar 11;267(10):1342–42.
  14. 14. Wisniewski AB, Prendeville MT, Dobs AS. Handedness, functional cerebral hemispheric lateralization, and cognition in male-to-female transsexuals receiving cross-sex hormone treatment. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2005 Apr 1;34(2):167–72. pmid:15803250
  15. 15. Lippa RA. Sexual orientation and personality. Annual Review of Sex Research. 2005 Mar 1;16(1):119–53.
  16. 16. Blanchard R, Lippa RA. Birth order, sibling sex ratio, handedness, and sexual orientation of male and female participants in a BBC Internet research project. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2007 Apr 1;36(2):163–76. pmid:17345165
  17. 17. Lalumiere ML, Blanchard R, Zucker KJ. Sexual orientation and handedness in men and women: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2000 Jul;126(4):575. pmid:10900997
  18. 18. Lippa RA. Handedness, sexual orientation, and gender-related personality traits in men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2003 Apr 1;32(2):103–14. pmid:12710825
  19. 19. Zheng L, Hart TA, Zheng Y. The relationship between intercourse preference positions and personality traits among gay men in China. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2012 Jun 1;41(3):683–9. pmid:21837518
  20. 20. Moskowitz DA, Hart TA. The influence of physical body traits and masculinity on anal sex roles in gay and bisexual men. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2011 Aug 1;40(4):835–41. pmid:21465269
  21. 21. Zheng L, Hart TA, Zheng Y. Top/Bottom Sexual Self-labels and Empathizing—Systemizing Cognitive Styles Among Gay Men in China. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2015 Jul 1;44(5):1431–8. pmid:25663239
  22. 22. Bogaert AF, Friesen C. Sexual orientation and height, weight, and age of puberty: New tests from a British national probability sample. Biological Psychology. 2002 Mar 31;59(2):135–45. pmid:11911936
  23. 23. Savin-Williams RC, Vrangalova Z. Mostly heterosexual as a distinct sexual orientation group: A systematic review of the empirical evidence. Developmental Review. 2013 Mar 31;33(1):58–88.
  24. 24. Zucker KJ, Mitchell JN, Bradley SJ, Tkachuk J, Cantor JM, Allin SM. The recalled childhood gender identity/gender role questionnaire: Psychometric properties. Sex Roles. 2006 Apr 1;54(7–8):469–83.
  25. 25. Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator—mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986 Dec;51(6):1173. pmid:3806354
  26. 26. Hojat M, Xu G. A visitor's guide to effect sizes—statistical significance versus practical (clinical) importance of research findings. Advances in Health Sciences Education. 2004 Sep 1;9(3):241–9. pmid:15316274
  27. 27. Ellis L, Ames MA. Neurohormonal functioning and sexual orientation: A theory of homosexuality—heterosexuality. Psychological Bulletin. 1987 Mar;101(2):233. pmid:2882536
  28. 28. Gooren L. The endocrinology of transsexualism: a review and commentary. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1990 Dec 31;15(1):3–14. pmid:2195578
  29. 29. Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Ashwin E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G, et al. Fetal testosterone predicts sexually differentiated childhood behavior in girls and in boys. Psychological Science. 2009 Feb 1;20(2):144–8. pmid:19175758
  30. 30. Hines M, Golombok S, Rust J, Johnston KJ, Golding J, Parents and Children Study Team. Testosterone during pregnancy and gender role behavior of preschool children: a longitudinal, population study. Child Development. 2002 Nov 1;73(6):1678–87. pmid:12487486
  31. 31. Breedlove SM. Minireview: organizational hypothesis: instances of the fingerpost. Endocrinology. 2010 Sep;151(9):4116–22. pmid:20631003
  32. 32. Kraemer B, Noll T, Delsignore A, Milos G, Schnyder U, Hepp U. Finger length ratio (2D: 4D) in adults with gender identity disorder. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2009 Jun 1;38(3):359–63. pmid:17906922
  33. 33. Mazur T. Gender dysphoria and gender change in androgen insensitivity or micropenis. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2005 Aug 1;34(4):411–21. pmid:16010464
  34. 34. Berenbaum SA, Bailey JM. Effects on gender identity of prenatal androgens and genital appearance: evidence from girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 2003 Mar 1;88(3):1102–6.
  35. 35. LeVay S. A difference in hypothalamic structure between heterosexual and homosexual men. Science. 1991 Aug 30;253(5023):1034–37. pmid:1887219
  36. 36. Bogaert AF, Hershberger S. The relation between sexual orientation and penile size. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 1999 Jun 1;28(3):213–21. pmid:10410197
  37. 37. Grimbos T, Dawood K, Burriss RP, Zucker KJ, Puts DA. Sexual orientation and the second to fourth finger length ratio: a meta-analysis in men and women. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2010 Apr;124(2):278. pmid:20364887
  38. 38. Lust JM, Geuze RH, Van de Beek C, Cohen-Kettenis PT, Bouma A, Groothuis TG. Differential effects of prenatal testosterone on lateralization of handedness and language. Neuropsychology. 2011 Sep;25(5):581. pmid:21500918
  39. 39. Nass R, Baker S, Speiser P, Virdis R, Balsamo A, Cacciari E, et al. Hormones and handedness Left-hand bias in female congenital adrenal hyperplasia patients. Neurology. 1987 Apr 1;37(4):711–11. pmid:3561787
  40. 40. Kelso WM, Nicholls ME, Warne GL, Zacharin M. Cerebral lateralization and cognitive functioning in patients with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Neuropsychology. 2000 Jul;14(3):370. pmid:10928740
  41. 41. Vuoksimaa E, Eriksson CP, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ, Kaprio J. Decreased prevalence of left-handedness among females with male co-twins: Evidence suggesting prenatal testosterone transfer in humans?. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010 Nov 30;35(10):1462–72. pmid:20570052
  42. 42. Hampson E. The development of hand preference and dichotic language lateralization in males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition. 2015 Oct 27:1–8.
  43. 43. Hampson E, Sankar JS. Hand preference in humans is associated with testosterone levels and androgen receptor gene polymorphism. Neuropsychologia. 2012 Jul 31;50(8):2018–25. pmid:22579704
  44. 44. Helleday J, Siwers B, Ritzén EM, Hugdahl K. Normal lateralization for handedness and ear advantage in a verbal dichotic listening task in women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). Neuropsychologia. 1994 Jul 1;32(7):875–80. pmid:7936169
  45. 45. Mathews GA, Fane BA, Pasterski VL, Conway GS, Brook C, Hines M. Androgenic influences on neural asymmetry: Handedness and language lateralization in individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004 Jul 31;29(6):810–22. pmid:15110930
  46. 46. Ellis L, Skorska MN, Bogaert AF. Handedness, sexual orientation, and somatic markers for prenatal androgens: Are southpaws really that gay? Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition. 2016 Feb 25:1–24.
  47. 47. Arning L, Ocklenburg S, Schulz S, Ness V, Gerding WM, Hengstler JG, et al. Handedness and the X chromosome: The role of androgen receptor CAG-repeat length. Scientific Reports. 2015;5.
  48. 48. Tut TG, Ghadessy FJ, Trifiro MA, Pinsky L, Yong EL. Long Polyglutamine Tracts in the Androgen Receptor Are Associated with Reduced Trans-Activation, Impaired Sperm Production, and Male Infertility 1. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 1997 Nov 1;82(11):3777–82.
  49. 49. Medland SE, Duffy DL, Spurdle AB, Wright MJ, Geffen GM, Montgomery GW, et al. Opposite effects of androgen receptor CAG repeat length on increased risk of left-handedness in males and females. Behavior Genetics. 2005 Nov 1;35(6):735–44. pmid:16273319
  50. 50. Ngun TC, Ghahramani N, Sánchez FJ, Bocklandt S, Vilain E. The genetics of sex differences in brain and behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2011 Apr 30;32(2):227–46. pmid:20951723
  51. 51. Sanders AR, Martin ER, Beecham GW, Guo S, Dawood K, Rieger G, et al. Genome-wide scan demonstrates significant linkage for male sexual orientation. Psychological Medicine. 2015 May 1;45(07):1379–88.
  52. 52. Macke JP, Hu N, Hu S, Bailey M, King VL, Brown T, et al. Nathans J. Sequence variation in the androgen receptor gene is not a common determinant of male sexual orientation. American Journal of Human Genetics. 1993 Oct;53(4):844. pmid:8213813
  53. 53. Dangerfield DT II, Smith LR, Williams J, Unger J, Bluthenthal R. Sexual Positioning Among Men Who Have Sex With Men: A Narrative Review. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2016 May 13:1–6.
  54. 54. Blanchard R, VanderLaan DP. Commentary on Kishida and Rahman (2015), Including a Meta-analysis of Relevant Studies on Fraternal Birth Order and Sexual Orientation in Men. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2015 Jul 1;44(5):1503–9. pmid:25940737
  55. 55. Blanchard R, Cantor JM, Bogaert AF, Breedlove SM, Ellis L. Interaction of fraternal birth order and handedness in the development of male homosexuality. Hormones and Behavior. 2006 Mar 31;49(3):405–14. pmid:16246335
  56. 56. Bogaert AF. Extreme right-handedness, older brothers, and sexual orientation in men. Neuropsychology. 2007 Jan;21(1):141. pmid:17201537
  57. 57. Bogaert AF, Blanchard R, Crosthwait LE. Interaction of birth order, handedness, and sexual orientation in the Kinsey interview data. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007 Oct;121(5):845. pmid:17907817
  58. 58. Kishida M, Rahman Q. Fraternal birth order and extreme right-handedness as predictors of sexual orientation and gender nonconformity in men. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2015 Jul 1;44(5):1493–501. pmid:25663238
  59. 59. Bogaert AF. The interaction of fraternal birth order and body size in male sexual orientation. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003 Apr;117(2):381. pmid:12708534
  60. 60. Rahman Q. The association between the fraternal birth order effect in male homosexuality and other markers of human sexual orientation. Biology Letters. 2005 Dec 22;1(4):393–5. pmid:17148215
  61. 61. Blanchard R. Review and theory of handedness, birth order, and homosexuality in men. Laterality. 2008 Jan 1;13(1):51–70. pmid:18050001
  62. 62. Yeo RA, Gangestad SW. Developmental origins of variation in human hand preference. In Developmental instability: Its origins and evolutionary implications. Tempe, Arizona: Springer Netherlands.1994 (pp. 283–298).
  63. 63. Rahman Q. Fluctuating asymmetry, second to fourth finger length ratios and human sexual orientation. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005 May 31;30(4):382–91. pmid:15694118
  64. 64. Mustanski BS, Bailey JM, Kaspar S. Dermatoglyphics, handedness, sex, and sexual orientation. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2002 Feb 1;31(1):113–22. pmid:11910784
  65. 65. Miller SS, Hoffmann HL, Mustanski BS. Fluctuating asymmetry and sexual orientation in men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2008 Feb 1;37(1):150–7. pmid:18163207
  66. 66. Hall PA, Schaeff CM. Sexual orientation and fluctuating asymmetry in men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2008 Feb 1;37(1):158–65. pmid:18157740
  67. 67. Bailey JM. The man who would be queen: The science of gender-bending and transsexualism. Joseph Henry Press; 2003 Mar 24.