Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Multiple Language Use Influences Oculomotor Task Performance: Neurophysiological Evidence of a Shared Substrate between Language and Motor Control

  • Karin Heidlmayr,

    Affiliations Laboratory Vision Action Cognition–EA 7326, Institute of Psychology, Paris Descartes–Sorbonne Paris Cité University, Paris, France, Laboratory MoDyCo–UMR 7114, CNRS, Paris Lumières–Paris Ouest Nanterre La Défense University, Paris, France

  • Karine Doré-Mazars,

    Affiliation Laboratory Vision Action Cognition–EA 7326, Institute of Psychology, Paris Descartes–Sorbonne Paris Cité University, Paris, France

  • Xavier Aparicio,

    Affiliation Laboratory CHArt–EA 4004, Paris Est Créteil University, Paris, France

  • Frédéric Isel

    fisel@u-paris10.fr

    Affiliation Laboratory MoDyCo–UMR 7114, CNRS, Paris Lumières–Paris Ouest Nanterre La Défense University, Paris, France

Abstract

In the present electroencephalographical study, we asked to which extent executive control processes are shared by both the language and motor domain. The rationale was to examine whether executive control processes whose efficiency is reinforced by the frequent use of a second language can lead to a benefit in the control of eye movements, i.e. a non-linguistic activity. For this purpose, we administrated to 19 highly proficient late French-German bilingual participants and to a control group of 20 French monolingual participants an antisaccade task, i.e. a specific motor task involving control. In this task, an automatic saccade has to be suppressed while a voluntary eye movement in the opposite direction has to be carried out. Here, our main hypothesis is that an advantage in the antisaccade task should be observed in the bilinguals if some properties of the control processes are shared between linguistic and motor domains. ERP data revealed clear differences between bilinguals and monolinguals. Critically, we showed an increased N2 effect size in bilinguals, thought to reflect better efficiency to monitor conflict, combined with reduced effect sizes on markers reflecting inhibitory control, i.e. cue-locked positivity, the target-locked P3 and the saccade-locked presaccadic positivity (PSP). Moreover, effective connectivity analyses (dynamic causal modelling; DCM) on the neuronal source level indicated that bilinguals rely more strongly on ACC-driven control while monolinguals rely on PFC-driven control. Taken together, our combined ERP and effective connectivity findings may reflect a dynamic interplay between strengthened conflict monitoring, associated with subsequently more efficient inhibition in bilinguals. Finally, L2 proficiency and immersion experience constitute relevant factors of the language background that predict efficiency of inhibition. To conclude, the present study provided ERP and effective connectivity evidence for domain-general executive control involvement in handling multiple language use, leading to a control advantage in bilingualism.

Introduction

Executive control and neuroplasticity in bilingualism

Individuals need to constantly adapt to environmental constraints and neuroplasticity allows for this adaptive capacity over the lifespan [1,2]. Flexible adaptation is amongst others required in linguistic interaction, especially if an individual has acquired competence in more than one language [3,4]. In multilingualism, significant neuroplastic changes have been observed in language learning or immersion experience, both of which are highly challenging situations, in linguistic and cognitive terms [5,6]. Structural changes found with multiple language use were, e.g. grey matter volume increases with intense language interpretation studies [7], increases of grey matter density with improving L2 proficiency in immersion [8], or white matter connectivity increases with intense classroom language training [9]; for a review, see [6]. Importantly, in sustained multiple language use, not only the neurocognitive language system but also the involved control processes need to adapt in order to meet the cognitive requirements. Indeed, second-language users of a language have different control demands than native speakers, due to the reduced automaticity of the second language [10,11], the simultaneous activation of languages [1218] and the bidirectional cross-language influences [1921]. Therefore, psycholinguistic models of language control have to discuss the different control processes and to ask to which extent these processes are shared by different cognitive functions (language, memory, attention). A longstanding debate has opposed theories, which proposed that control processes are domain-general [4,17,22] (for some psycholinguistic theories the use of these processes is limited to low proficient bilinguals [23,24]), to others postulating that these processes are at least to a large degree domain-specific [25] (for some psycholinguistic theories this is the case for bilinguals with a high proficiency in both languages [23,24]). In the present study, we asked whether some control processes are shared by both the language and the motor domain. The rationale was to examine at the neurophysiological level whether executive control processes whose efficiency is assumed to be reinforced by the frequent use of a second language can lead to a benefit for realizing a motor task involving control, i.e. the antisaccade task.

The question on the functional architecture of executive control processes across different functions is directly related to the broader theoretical framework of embodied/grounded cognition. Embodied cognition theories feed the debate on the main question of the shared vs. distinct nature of linguistic and sensory-motor processing in handling natural language. Increasing neuroimaging and neurophysiological evidence supports the view of a distributed interactive systems account (cf. “embodied cognition” or “grounded cognition”; [26,27], e.g. the coactivation of classical language and motor regions during action word processing; [28], or the influence of word reading on motor control; [29]).

Executive functions (EF) constitute “a set of general-purpose control processes that regulate one’s thoughts and behaviors” [30]; see also [31]. It has previously been suggested, that three main executive functions can be distinguished, i.e. inhibition of prepotent responses (“inhibition”), information updating and monitoring (“updating”) and mental set shifting (“shifting”) [30,31]. On a more fine-grained level of analysis these three executive functions can be further subdivided into more specific control functions [30]. For executive control, manifold empirical evidence lends support to the theoretical accounts claiming a shared nature of (domain-general) control between cognitive domains, i.e. showing overlapping neuronal activation for linguistic and non-linguistic control [3234], as well as improved performance in bi- and multilinguals in both linguistic and non-linguistic executive control tasks [3541]; there is however no unanimity in this regard, for reviews, see [40,4245]. These findings constitute major arguments in favor of models postulating domain-general control processes to be involved in the control over multiple language use.

It is important to note that the inconsistency that some studies do and others fail to show a bilingualism advantage in executive control processes may be explained by the fact that bi- or multilinguals vary on different dimensions, e.g. second language proficiency, frequency of first and second language use, or the type of interactional context (for a discussion, see [40], but also [37]), which is thought to be related to a differential recruitment of executive control processes [3,4]. As a consequence, different profiles of bilingualism may involve specific sets of control processes. Among these control processes, conflict monitoring performance in non-verbal tasks has been found to be improved in bilingualism in a behavioral study using the Attentional Network Task (ANT) [46], an advantage that has been found in a neurophysiological study to be especially strong in bilinguals with good control over their language switches [47], or in bilinguals with high second language proficiency, as observed in a neurophysiological study using a saccadic countermanding task [48]. The saccadic countermanding task is used to examine the neural response of movement initiation and suppression, i.e. the response adjustments during action control. This task involves the inhibition of an initiated saccade and the voluntary redirection of the gaze to a new location [48,49]. A behavioral advantage in response inhibition as well as task switching performance was found in a modified antisaccade task in older but not in younger adult bilinguals [50]. Using the same task, a switching advantage was also observed in bilingual children [51]. These data collected from different age groups suggest that (1) activity-dependent long-term effects on executive function capacity vary over the lifespan, and (2) activity-dependent improvements are more likely to occur in age groups with generally lower than peak executive function (EF) capacity, i.e. children and older adults. In a non-verbal task switching paradigm, Prior and MacWhinney [41] found a behavioral bilingual advantage for switching and Prior and Gollan [52] found that the benefit was strongest if bilinguals reported a high frequency of daily language switching. Concerning vector inversion/movement initiation, to our knowledge there are no previous studies that have tackled the question of an influence of bilingualism.

More recently, intermediate theoretical positions postulating a hybrid neural architecture of executive control and its interaction with the language domain have emerged, which try to account more specifically for the relation between domain-general and domain-specific processes. One account postulates, that domain-general executive control is involved in language processing, i.e. that there is an interaction between the neurocognitive networks of domain-general executive control and of language [53]. Based on network neuroscience, it is claimed that in neurocognitive networks, e.g. the language network, there is a distinction between a domain-specific network core and domain-general periphery, which is shared with other neurocognitive networks, e.g. the domain-general control network [53]. This perspective may help identifying domain-specific and domain-general processes in language processing [53]. Similarly, however for control in non-linguistic domains, one account considers that there is an interplay between both domain-general control processes, that are shared across cognitive and motor domains, and domain-specific control processes, that are specific to the control challenges in a particular cognitive or motor domain [25]. Multiple findings on language and non-linguistic control, showing evidence for an overlap across domains for some control processes while not for others support these theoretical accounts [54,55]. To date, there is no consensus on whether executive control processes are partially or fully shared between different domains (linguistic, non-linguistic, motor).

The goal of the present electroencephalographic study was to investigate whether executive control processes whose efficiency can be increased by the use of multiple languages are also involved in nonlinguistic activities such as the control of eye movements. For this purpose, we administrated to highly proficient French-German bilingual participants and to a control group of French monolingual participants an antisaccade task, i.e. a task involving motor control, in which an automatic saccade needs to be suppressed and a voluntary eye movement in the opposite direction needs to be carried out. The main hypothesis of our study is that a behavioral and/or a neurophysiological advantage of bilingualism should be observed if the control processes involved in the antisaccade task are shared between linguistic and motor domains. Else, no bilingualism advantage should be expected with the antisaccade task. Critically, we examined whether individuals who use more than one language on a daily basis (‘bilinguals’) show a better performance in controlling predominant, automatic responses in a non-linguistic motor task, i.e. the antisaccade task, than individuals who use solely one language (‘monolinguals’). To our knowledge, it is the first time that the neuronal underpinnings of oculomotor control processes and the time course of their activation were investigated in relation to language control. The paradigm chosen for the present study was a version of the antisaccade task in which the involvement of the processes of conflict monitoring, response inhibition, vector inversion/movement initiation as well as switching (task engagement and disengagement, attentional shifting) can be studied. Moreover, the role of the neuronal regions relevant for these control processes, mainly the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the prefrontal cortex (PFC), as well as their interaction were examined in order to compare neuronal dynamics underlying control processes between the two groups.

The antisaccade task: control processes and neuroanatomical regions

The saccade task [56] is a task that allows for studying the voluntary control of action [57]. Participants are instructed to carry out either an automatic eye movement towards a visual target (prosaccade) or suppress this automatic eye movement and effectuate a saccade into the opposite direction (antisaccade), which depends on the color of the instructional cue preceding the target stimulus. Miyake and Friedman [30] classify the antisaccade task as a representative task to study inhibition, defined as the “deliberate overriding of dominant or prepotent responses”; more specifically the antisaccade task may require response inhibition [58]; for theoretical accounts claiming a separation between response inhibition and interference suppression, see [59,60]; see however [61]. Munoz and Everling [57] claimed that the antisaccade task does not only require response inhibition of the automatic prosaccade but also vector inversion (i.e. direction inversion), that is the stimulus vector must be inverted into the saccade vector in order to initiate a voluntary antisaccade (see also [62]). Furthermore, it has been suggested that conflict monitoring is also a relevant control process for successful antisaccade performance [63,64]. In general, conflict monitoring has been defined as the processes of monitoring for the occurrence of conflict in information processing and is on the evaluative side of cognitive control. In the antisaccade task, conflict monitoring is required because the requirement to look away from a visual stimulus creates a conflict between two opposing saccade commands, an automatic (sensory-driven) saccade toward the stimulus and a voluntary (internally driven) saccade away from the stimulus [65]. Conflict monitoring serves to translate the occurrence of conflict into compensatory adjustments in control, i.e. the conflict monitoring system evaluates the levels of conflict and communicates this information to systems responsible for control implementation [63]. In addition to response inhibition, vector inversion and conflict monitoring, control adjustment is also required in the transition between trials of different conditions, i.e. switching-related control processes, in the antisaccade task. In linguistic and non-linguistic tasks, the ability to switch between different task-sets reflects the flexibility and ease of transitioning to new task-set representations [30]. The switching process involves task disengagement, task engagement, suppression of previous task sets [4,66,67], overcoming of inhibition and attentional shifting [68]. When the direction of switch is from a more difficult towards an easier task, previously applied sustained inhibition needs to be overcome, which is not the case in switching from the easier to the more difficult task, producing the robustly observed asymmetrical switching cost [68]; for a review and alternative accounts to explain asymmetrical switching costs, see [67].

At the neuroanatomical level, the suppression and/or generation of saccadic eye movements involves activation in a number of cortical and subcortical structures, i.e. the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the lateral intraparietal area (LIP), the supplementary eye fields (SEF), the frontal eye fields (FEF), the superior colliculus (SC), the substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNpr) [57], the striatum [65,69] and the thalamus; for reviews, see [57,69,70]. During antisaccades, the automatic activation of saccade neurons contralateral to the visual target needs to be inhibited while saccadic activity ipsilateral to the stimulus (contralateral to the target movement) is required. The inhibition of saccade neurons is thought to be carried out by fixation neurons and interneurons in the FEF and SC, which receive the information to do so probably from the PFC, the SEF, or the SNpr. The neuronal underpinnings of vector inversion, which is required for carrying out correct antisaccades beside response inhibition, are not yet very well understood but there is evidence that the LIP–which is at the interface between sensory and motor processing–and the FEF play a role in this process. Moreover, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) has been found to play a role in reflexive saccade suppression [70,71] and is thought to be active during conflict monitoring processes involved in antisaccade trials [63,70,72,73]. Once a pro- or antisaccade is initiated, fixation neurons in the FEF and SC cease to fire and there is a buildup of activity in saccade neurons. Control-related ERPs and oscillatory activity in the antisaccade task, thought to reflect conflict monitoring, response inhibition, vector inversion/motor planning and switching, will be presented as follows and the ERP markers are schematized in Fig 1.

thumbnail
Fig 1. ERP components.

Overview of the different ERP components and the oscillatory marker reported in the preparation, implementation and execution phases of saccadic eye movements in cue-locked, target-locked and saccade-locked epochs. PSP: presaccadic positivity; LPP: late parietal positivity; A: antisaccade; P: prosaccade.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.g001

ERP and oscillatory markers of control processes in an antisaccade task

The above-mentioned control processes in an antisaccade task are associated with distinct ERP and oscillatory markers. For presenting these processes and their associated neurophysiological markers, we decided to follow their hypothesized chronological order: (1) conflict monitoring, (2) response inhibition, (3) vector inversion/motor planning and lastly, however concerning the transition between trials: (4) switching-related processes. Moreover, for each component, the phase in which it occurs, i.e. the ‘preparation’ (cue-locked), ‘implementation’ (target-locked) and ‘execution’ (saccade-locked), will be indicated. Conflict monitoring has previously been ascribed to a stimulus-locked fronto-central N2 component that is larger in the condition requiring control (i.e. antisaccade or nogo condition) as compared to a control condition (i.e. prosaccade or go condition; [7476]; for a different view on the N2 in the antisaccade task, see [68]). The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is considered as a principal neuronal generator of the fronto-central N2 associated with conflict monitoring [72,77]. The process of response inhibition of the automatic prosaccade in an antisaccade task is thought to be reflected by a target-locked parietal positivity at around 300 ms post-target onset (P3) which was shown to be reduced in anti- compared to prosaccades [68] and the prefrontal cortex (PFC) has been found as one of its main neuronal generators [78]. Although such a reduction of the P3 amplitude for a task involving inhibition can sound counterintuitive, however, it makes sense when, as it was suggested, the P3 modulation may reflect the decision to withhold a response [79]. Reduced P3 amplitudes have also been observed in the Eriksen Flanker task [80], the go/no-go task [79], or in a partially incongruent categorization task [78] and the processes underlying the reduction of the P3 amplitude may not be located in one single neuronal generator but may consist of a combination of the activity in inhibitory control regions and/or the outcome of high-level inhibitory control at target sites, e.g. the motor cortex [75]. Given that the increased N2 and reduced P3 frequently occur together they are frequently also considered as an ‘N2/P3 complex’ [68,81,82]. Another inhibitory component, but occurring during the preparation stage, is the frontal cue-locked positivity around 200–300 ms post-cue onset that is smaller in anti- than prosaccades [68] which probably reflects preparatory processes for the decision to withhold an automatic response. A third component that has been suggested to reflect inhibitory processes occurs during the saccade execution stage, i.e. a saccade-locked central presaccadic positivity (PSP) over a period of 250–50 ms prior to saccade onset that is smaller before anti- than prosaccades [83,84]. Vector inversion and motor planning have been found to be reflected by a fronto-central and occipital power decrease in the beta band (13–26 Hz) [85,86]. As for switching-related components, a target-locked late parietal positivity (LPP) for switch vs. repetition trials at approximately 500–600 ms has been found to be larger for switch as compared to repetition trials, for both antisaccades and prosaccades, and is thought to reflect attentional shifting to the relevant task [68].

The present study

At present, there is behavioral and neuroimaging evidence in the literature that multiple language use involves domain-general control mechanisms which are also involved in non-linguistic control. Different evidence, however, gives support to hybrid accounts postulating that there are partially domain-general and partially language domain-specific control processes involved. The present study investigates the relation between language and oculomotricity and particularly asks whether bilinguals show a benefit in realizing a motor task involving control. The contribution of our study is to examine the neurodynamics of different control processes and subprocesses involved in an antisaccade task at the preparation (cue-locked), implementation (target-locked) and execution (saccade-locked) stages. EEG is a technique that allows to track phases of neuronal activation on a temporally fine-grained scale and therefore is a particularly useful technique for disentangling activity associated with different neurocognitive control processes and subprocesses. This should allow us to disentangle the processes for which an impact of bilingualism can occur. Moreover, source localization analyses will enable to identify neuronal generators associated with EEG markers thought to reflect these processes. Finally, by conducting DCM (dynamic causal modelling) analyses we aim at identifying effective connectivity dynamics at the network level between these neuronal regions. Effective connectivity concerns how activity in one brain region influences activity in another region [87]. In the present study, the investigation of effective connectivity will allow for learning more about the interplay of ACC and PFC in conflict monitoring and response inhibition processes in bilinguals and monolinguals. Note that despite the attested role of subcortical structures and of fronto-subcortical connections in bilingualism [88,89] as well as in saccade control [57,65,69] these sources were not included in our analyses due to the low precision for subcortical structures in source reconstruction analyses for ERPs.

In the present study, we aim to test if an impact of bilingualism on control performance can be observed in non-verbal oculomotor task. If control processes that are supposed to be involved in and trained by multiple language use show higher efficiency in a non-linguistic task, this can be considered as evidence in favor of accounts claiming domain-generality of control processes. If however, a more mitigated picture emerges, with only some processes being influenced by bilingualism while others are not, this can be taken as evidence supporting hybrid accounts claiming partially domain-general and partially domain-specific processes to be involved in language control. Moreover, in the present study, we aimed at investigating the predictive power of, e.g. second language proficiency, immersion experience, frequency of second language use, and frequency of language switching, on eye movement control capacity. By doing so, we aimed at giving a more fine-grained account of the multifaceted nature of bilingualism than has been previously done.

For behavioral measures we expect to find longer saccade latencies and higher error rates for antisaccades than for prosaccades. Saccade latencies were measured as the duration from target onset until saccade onset and are given in milliseconds. As erroneous responses were considered expected saccade direction errors as well as saccade omissions (absence of saccade initiation) and are given as a percentage. Moreover, saccade latencies are predicted to be longer for switch compared to repetition trials. This transition effect (switching effect) is predicted to be larger for prosaccades than for antisaccades (AP > PP; >; PA > AA). Both, the saccade task effect (‘Antisaccade effect’) and the transition effect (switching effect) are expected to be larger for monolinguals than for bilinguals. At the neurophysiological level, a bilingual advantage for conflict monitoring should be reflected by a larger N2 effect size. This, advantage is expected to be modulated by the self-reported degree of control over and the frequency of daily language switching. A bilingual advantage for response inhibition should be reflected by smaller effect sizes of the cue-locked positivity, the P3 and PSP in bilinguals as compared to monolinguals. The processes of vector inversion and motor planning should be associated with a target-locked fronto-central and occipital beta (13-26Hz) power decrease in anti- compared to prosaccades. Finally, higher switching capacity should be reflected by smaller LPP effect sizes in bilinguals, reinforced in bilingual individuals with better controlled and more frequent daily language switching.

Methods

Approval for the study was given by the local Ethics Committee (Conseil d’évaluation éthique pour les recherches en santé at Paris Descartes–Sorbonne Paris Cité University) and the participants gave their informed written consent prior to participating in the study.

Participants

Forty right-handed participants (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory) were recruited. By their own account, participants had no history of current or past neurological or psychiatric diseases, they had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and normal color vision. Twenty of the participants were native speakers of French (L1) and highly proficient second language speakers of German (L2). One bilingual participant was excluded from the analyses due to a high degree of motor artifact contamination of the recorded EEG. The second group of 20 participants were native speakers of French (L1) with reduced use of other languages than their mother tongue (Frequency of daily non-native language use: 1.4 ± 1.2%). All participants were selected after having completed a language history questionnaire. Data on linguistic and environmental background measures can be found in Table 1. The average age did not differ between bilinguals (n = 19; 12 females, 7 males; 22.5 ± 2.6 years, range 19–30) and monolinguals (n = 20; 10 females, 10 males; 23.8 ± 5.1 years, range 19–37; F < 1). The two groups were matched for socio-economic status using the educational and professional status, i.e. all bilingual and monolingual participants were students or young university graduates. Bilinguals were late learners of German who studied the language at secondary school in France as their first non-native language (L2; Age of acquisition (AoA): 10.4 ± 0.8 years). They all had a regular use of their L2 (L2 daily frequency of use: 21.2 ± 16.9%) and 84% of the bilingual participants (16 individuals) also frequently used an additional L3 (L3 daily frequency of use: 6.5 ± 6.4%). The bilinguals’ self-reported proficiency of 1.3 ± 0.6 [scale: 1: good– 5: poor] was high, which was also confirmed by the percentage of correct responses on a standardized test of German as a foreign language (DAF–Deutsch als Fremdsprache): 84.1 ± 7.4%.

thumbnail
Table 1. Background data.

Linguistic and environmental background measures as assessed by a questionnaire are reported in the table. The mean and standard deviation (SD) is indicated for each category.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.t001

Stimuli and Procedure

Stimuli were displayed on an Iiyama HM240DT monitor with a refresh rate of 160 Hz and a resolution of 800×600 pixels. The experimental sessions took place in a dimly lit room. Participants were seated 22 inches away from the screen and their head kept stable with a chin and forehead rest. Eye movements were recorded with an Eyelink® 1000 (SR Research, Ontario, Canada), with a temporal resolution of 1000 Hz, and a spatial resolution of 0.15°. Viewing was binocular but only movements of the right eye were monitored. Each session began with a 9 points calibration over the entire screen. Before each trial, central fixation was checked and compared to the calibration. If the distance between the fixation check and the calibration was greater than 0.75°, fixation was refused and the trial was reinitiated. When successful calibration was detected, the trial began. Online saccade detection corresponded to above-threshold velocity (30°/s) and acceleration (8000°/s2).

Our experimental design was taken from the one used by Mueller et al. [68]. Each trial began with a blank screen, followed by a 1.5° x 1.5° black fixation cross presented in the center of the screen on a grey background. The combination of blank screen and fixation cross lasted 2100 ms with four different timings (1600+500 ms; 1400+700 ms; 1200+900 ms; 100+ 1100 ms respectively) randomized from trial to trial (see Fig 2), in order to avoid anticipation of cue onset. The fixation cross then turned into two different cues, a green or a red cross presented during 300 ms on the screen. The white target box (1.5° x 1.5°) then appeared on either the left or the right side of the screen, and was displayed for 700 ms on two possible eccentricities (6° and 10°) randomized across trials.

Participants were instructed to make an eye movement towards the target box (prosaccade trial) if the cue was green, and an eye movement away from the target box, in a mirror symmetric location (antisaccade trial) when the cue was red. They were instructed to hold their fixation until the disappearance of the target box, and then, look back to the center of the screen. The experimental session (768 trials) was divided into three parts. In the first part of the experiment (pre), participants monitored a single task session of 96 prosaccade trials, and then 96 antisaccade trials (the order was counterbalanced across participants of each group). In the second part (mixed task session), they monitored 384 trials with pro- and antisaccade trials mixed. Then, in the third and last part (post) they monitored again a single task session of 96 prosaccade trials and a single task session of 96 antisaccade trials (in the same order as the first part). Before each task (prosaccades or antisaccades single task sessions or mixed task sessions), a block of training was proposed in order to familiarize the participants with the task. A short break was allowed to the participants between each block. All together (including single task and mixed task sessions), there was the same number of pro- and antisaccades (384). In the mixed task session, there were as many switches (defined as when the current saccade task differed from the task in the previous trial) as repetition trials (defined as when the previous saccade task was identical to the saccade task in the current trial). These dyads, taking into account n-1 trials, were used in order to examine the switching-related processes in comparing prosaccades and antisaccades as a function of repetition and switching trials in the mixed task session. The second trial of a dyad was considered as the target element for which the influence of the preceding trial was aimed to be studied. In total, 96 trials per dyad type (PP, AP, AA, PA) were implemented over the total of 12 mixed blocks. To set up the lists of 32 dyads per mixed block, we applied amongst others the constraint that the same type of saccade was presented no more than three times in a row. For statistical analyses, only those dyads with accurate performance on n as well as n-1 trials were taken into consideration.

Behavioral data analysis

Saccades with latencies below 50 ms were considered as early starts and removed from the analysis, as well as latencies above 500 ms, and trials containing eye blinks. Trials with latencies exceeding the threshold of mean ± 2 SD per condition and participant were considered as outliers and hence excluded from the analysis. Overall, 5.8 ± 1.6% of outlier trials were removed (single and mixed task blocks collapsed), which did not differ between saccade tasks (p > .05) or groups (F < 1). We averaged the saccade latencies for correct answers (i.e. correct saccade direction in regard to instructions) with data from both eccentricities (6° and 10°) and from both target locations (left and right). A trial was considered erroneous when the participant carried out a prosaccade if the instructional cue indicated an antisaccade trial (red cue), and if an antisaccade was carried out when the cue indicated a prosaccade (green cue). Error rates were averaged for both eccentricities and target locations.

Statistical analyses were carried out with SPSS 17.0. For analysis of the behavioral measures (saccade latencies and error rate), a three-way repeated measures ANOVA with Group as a between-subjects factor (monolingual vs. bilingual), and Saccade task (prosaccade vs. antisaccade) and Block type (mixed task blocks vs. single task blocks) as within-subjects factors was conducted. Planned comparisons were made to examine task effects for each Group. Moreover, a three-way repeated measures ANOVA with Group as a between-subjects factor (monolingual vs. bilingual), and Saccade task (prosaccade vs. antisaccade) and Transition (switch vs. repetition) as within-subjects factors was performed to examine switching-related performance on the two behavioral measures (saccade latencies and error rates).

EEG acquisition and preprocessing

EEG preprocessing has been conducted using Brain Vision Analyzer 2.1.0 (Brain Products). EEG was recorded from 33 channels placed according to the international 10–20 system (Fp1, Fp2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, FC5, FC1, FCz, FC2, FC6, T7, C3, Cz, C4, T8, TP9, CP5, CP1, CP2, CP6, TP10, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, PO9, O1, Oz, O2, PO10), mounted in an elastic cap (ActiCap, Brain Products) and recorded with the Brain Vision Recorder, Brain Products. All channels were referenced online against FCz. For data analysis, channels were re-referenced to an average reference after having removed any bad channels. Electrode impedances were kept below 25 kΩ. Data were recorded at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. An online band-pass filter of 0.01–100 Hz was used. Then, the data were filtered offline (IIR-Butterworth filter) with a high-pass filter at 0.05 Hz (slope 12 dB/octave) and a notch filter for 50 Hz (slope 24 dB/octave). On the continuous data, automatic artifact detection for non-ocular artifacts was conducted (Maximum amplitude difference in interval of 200 ms: 300 μV, maximum gradient voltage step: 50 μV/ms, lowest allowed activity in interval of 100 ms: 0.5 μV). Then, in order to remove artifacts from horizontal eye movements or from eye blinks from the continuous EEG signal, an ocular correction ICA (unbiased extended Infomax) was run using Fp1 as a VEOG channel and F7 and F8 as HEOG channels. Blinks were detected using the Value Trigger Algorithm (Blink trigger value: 97%, blink correlation trigger: 70%). The ocular correction ICA was applied using the whole data. Components relevant for vertical and horizontal activity were identified based on the relative variance in the respective channel(s) and the percentage of variance to delete was set to 30%. For further analyses, only trials on which the participants carried out a correct oculomotor response and which were not contaminated by anticipatory eye movements (saccade latencies faster than 50 ms), saccade latencies of more than 500 ms (indicating that the participant did not correctly follow the instructions) or other movement artifacts, were included. The continuous EEG was segmented into epochs relative to three events: (1) cue-locked segments were segmented into epochs from 500 ms pre-cue until 1300 ms post-cue onset, (2) target-locked segments were segmented into epochs from 800 ms pre-target until 1000 ms post-target onset and (3) saccade-locked segments were segmented into epochs from 1100 ms pre-saccade until 700 ms post-saccade onset.

On the percentage of trials included in ERP analyses after preprocessing and data cleaning we conducted a three-way repeated measures ANOVA with Group as a between-subjects factor (monolingual vs. bilingual), and Saccade task (prosaccade vs. antisaccade) and Block type (mixed task blocks vs. single task blocks) as within-subjects factors. There was a main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 53.31, MSE = 183.18, p < .001, η2p = .590), indicating that the percentage of included trials was higher in prosaccades (70.2 ± 14.7%) than antisaccades (54.4 ± 15.7%). Moreover, there was a Saccade task by Block type interaction (F(1, 37) = 43.99, MSE = 45.34, p < .001, η2p = .543), reflecting that for prosaccades a higher percentage of trials was kept in mixed (73.4 ± 14.7%) compared to single block sessions (67.0 ± 16.5%; p < .001), while in antisaccades a smaller percentage was kept in mixed (50.4 ± 18.1%) than single task sessions (58.4 ± 15.2%; p < .001). There was no other main effect or interaction.

ERP analysis

ERP preprocessing and analyses have been conducted using Brain Vision Analyzer 2.1.0 (Brain Products) and EEGLAB toolbox (version 13.2.2) [90] for MATLAB (version 7.12.0, R2011a) and SPSS 17.0. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were computed for each participant in each experimental condition for cue-, target- and saccade-locked epochs. Cue-locked segments were baseline corrected with the baseline set from 100 ms pre-cue onset until cue onset. Target-locked segments were baseline corrected with the baseline set from 400 ms to 300 ms pre-target onset (which is equivalent to the 100 ms baseline before cue onset). Saccade-locked segments were baseline corrected with a baseline set from 700 ms to 600 ms pre-saccade onset (which covers a time window that is on average before cue-onset). Then, in each experimental condition, the ERP activity was averaged over trials and over participants (i.e. grand average ERP). Statistical analyses of the ERP effects were performed for cue-, target- and saccade-locked ERPs in selected time windows based on previous studies [68,83,84] and adjusted by visual inspection of the grand averages (Preparation phase: Cue-locked positivity: Cue-locked 150–250 ms; Implementation phase: N2: target-locked 160–200 ms, P3: target-locked 200–400 ms, Late parietal positivity (LPP): target-locked: Antisaccades: 400–650 ms, Prosaccades: 400–550 ms; Execution phase: Presaccadic positivity (PSP): saccade-locked -250 to -50 ms). Mean amplitudes were calculated for each time window. All analyses were quantified using the multivariate approach to repeated measurement and followed a hierarchical analysis schema. In order to allow for an examination of hemispheric differences, the data recorded at the midline electrode sites were treated separately from the data recorded from lateral recording sites. Four-way repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted for the analyses for the lateral electrodes, including the within-subjects factors Saccade task (prosaccade vs. antisaccade) as well as two topographical variables Region (anterior vs. posterior) and Hemisphere (left vs. right) and the between-subjects factor Group (monolingual vs. bilingual). Four regions of interest (ROIs) resulting from a complete crossing of the Region and Hemisphere variables were defined: left anterior (F3, FC1, FC5), right anterior (F4, FC2, FC6), left posterior (CP5, CP1, P3), and right posterior (CP6, CP2, P4). The data from the midline electrodes were analyzed with a three-way repeated measures ANOVA including the within-subjects factors Saccade task (prosaccade vs. antisaccade) as well as Electrode (Fz, Cz, Pz) and the between-subjects factor Group (monolingual vs. bilingual). Note that for one bilingual and for one monolingual participant the Fz electrode provided noisy data and these two participants were consequently excluded from the analyses on midline electrodes, which explains the reduced degrees of freedom in these analyses. To investigate switching-related activity, separate analyses were conducted for antisaccades and prosaccades, respectively. Four-way repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted for the analyses on the lateral electrodes, including the within-subjects factors Transition (switch vs. repetition) as well as two topographical variables Region (anterior vs. posterior) and Hemisphere (left vs. right) and the between-subjects factor Group (monolingual vs. bilingual). Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted for the analysis of data from the midline electrodes, including the within-subjects factors Transition (switch vs. repetition) as well as Electrode (Fz, Cz, Pz) and the between-subjects factor Group (monolingual vs. bilingual). The dependent variable was the voltage amplitude [μV] averaged over the relevant interval of each ERP component of interest. The Greenhouse–Geisser correction [91] was applied when evaluating effects with more than one degree of freedom in the numerator. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons at single electrode sites were performed using a modified Bonferroni procedure [92]. A significance level of 0.05 was used for all statistical tests and only significant results are reported. For visual presentation of the midline electrodes in detail figures, curves were smoothed (1:50 points).

Dynamic causal modelling (DCM) analysis

The dynamic causal modelling (DCM) analysis was conducted in SPM12 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK) and SPSS 17.0. DCM is a method that allows to test hypotheses of dynamics in a neuronal network which need to be defined as effective connectivity models. Effective connectivity allows us to examine how activity in one brain region influences activity in another region [87]. Here, based on (1) previous models of both saccade control [93] and the antisaccade task [57], as well as (2) fMRI evidence with an antisaccade task [9496] and (3) TMS-MEG evidence of frontal top-down control on occipito-parietal excitability [97], a highly plausible effective connectivity model was created (Fig 3) and tested for effective connectivity differences between groups. Note that source reconstruction analyses for ERPs have sufficient precision only for cortical sources and consequently our modeling did not include any subcortical structures, despite their evident role in saccade control. Nine cortical sources, modeled as equivalent current dipoles (ECDs), were included in our effective connectivity model: left and right primary visual cortex (LV1, RV1), left and right lateral intraparietal area (LLIP, RLIP), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), left and right prefrontal cortex (LPFC, RPFC), left and right frontal eye field (LFEF, RFEF). Table 2 presents the MNI coordinates and Fig 3B the locations of these cortical neuronal generators taken from two fMRI studies on an antisaccade paradigm, i.e. Ford [96] and Aichert, et al. [94], and transformed from Talairach to MNI space using the tal2mni tool (http://imaging.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/imaging/MniTalairach). The source locations were specified while the dipole orientation parameters were left free and were individually adjusted during the model inversion process. During the model inversion process, DCM optimizes for each participant the information provided concerning the electromagnetic forward model and the neuronal sources (i.e. the number, locations and connections of the neuronal sources), aiming at minimizing free energy [98]. As forward model for the ERP data of the present study, the boundary element model (BEM) implemented in SPM12 was used.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Effective connectivity model tested in a DCM analysis and equivalent current dipole locations.

A The neuronal sources in the model are connected with forward (black), backward (dark grey) or lateral (light grey) connections. Connections that are modelled to vary between experimental conditions are depicted with dotted lines. Connections between V1 and FEF as well as LIP and FEF also connect to the contralateral side but are depicted only for the ipsilateral side for the sake of clarity of the figure. B Locations of the equivalent current dipoles included in the model are depicted in an MRI of a standard brain in MNI space. ACC, Anterior cingulate cortex; LFEF/RFEF, left and right frontal eye field; LLIP/RLIP, left and right lateral intraparietal area; LPFC/RPFC, left and right prefrontal cortex; LV1/RV1, left and right primary visual cortex.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.g003

thumbnail
Table 2. Coordinates of neuronal sources used in the DCM analysis.

Source coordinates have been taken from *Ford [96] and ΔAichert et al. [94] and were transformed from Talairach to MNI space using the tal2mni tool (http://imaging.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/imaging/MniTalairach).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.t002

The DCM model was designed as follows: Given that our paradigm involved visual stimulation, the left and right primary visual cortex were defined as input regions. The time window used for model adjustment started with cue-onset and ended at 400 ms after target-onset and consequently modeled source activity underlying the cue-locked positivity, the N2, the P3 and the PSP effects. The visual stimulation by the presentation of the cue was modeled at starting at 0 ms of the time window and reaching the input region 64 ms after cue onset (this delay is an approximation of when the stimulation activates the cortical areas of the model, see [99]). The target onset was modeled at time point 300, reaching the input region 64 ms after target onset. The following DCM was modeled: The visual input (cue, target) entered bilaterally to the primary visual cortex (LV1/RV1), which were connected to ipsi- and contralateral lateral intraparietal areas (LLIP/RLIP), which again were connected to the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in the frontal lobe. The ACC was bilaterally connected to the prefrontal cortex (LPFC/RPFC), which again had connections to ipsi- and contralateral frontal eye fields (LFEF/RFEF). Lateral connections were assumed between the left and right V1, left and right LIP, left and right PFC and left and right FEF. All connections were reciprocal and connected with the ipsi- as well as the contralateral site.

Here, DCM analyses were conducted for pro- and antisaccades issued from mixed task blocks. In the DCM model, the connections that are hypothesized to be modulated by experimental condition (‘modulatory connections’), i.e. Saccade task (prosaccade vs. antisaccade), are those from ACC to PFC and backward from PFC to ACC, from PFC to FEF and from ACC to FEF. After inversion of the model on each subject’s data set, the values of the effective connectivity in each modulatory connection, i.e. connections between neuronal sources that were allowed to vary between prosaccades vs. antisaccades, were extracted for each participant. The value for each modulatory effective connection was subsequently submitted to an independent samples t-test with Group as a between-subjects factor.

Correlation analyses with language background factors

Finally, the information on the individual language background of the bilingual participants in our study were put into relation with the experimental measures on which group differences were observed. These analyses were done using SPSS 17.0. Most of the language background factors included in our language background questionnaire, e.g. language switching experience, immersion in L2 environment, L2 proficiency, and the motivation to improve L2 proficiency, were addressed with more than one question or test in order to obtain a precise picture. For each language background factor, the relevant sub-scores in the questionnaire were identified and included in the principle component analysis (PCA) if the responses showed reasonable variance, i.e. for instance were sub-scores not included if all participants responded with the highest score on the scale. The included variables were standardized using a correlation matrix, an orthogonal rotation method (Varimax with Kaiser normalization) was applied and the principle component analysis was constrained to extract only one component. The language background factors and the corresponding questions are presented in Table 3. Correlations involving these five language background factors, i.e. language switching experience, immersion in L2 environment, L2 proficiency, motivation to improve L2 proficiency, and the frequency of daily L2 use [%] and the ERP effect sizes and DCM modulatory connections for which group differences had been found, were conducted for the bilingual group.

thumbnail
Table 3. Language background factors.

One principal component for each of the language background factors of interest was extracted from the responses to the corresponding questions in the questionnaire.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.t003

Time-frequency analysis

Time-frequency analyses were performed using the EEGLAB toolbox (version 13.2.2) [90] for MATLAB (version 7.12.0, R2011a). The original sampling rate of 1000Hz was kept throughout the analyses. In order to assess event-related spectral perturbations (ERSP) [100], i.e. event-related shifts in the power spectrum, event-related synchronization (ERS) and desynchronization (ERD) was calculated for target- and saccade-locked segments in the mixed task blocks. ERS and ERD represent the relative power increase (ERS) or decrease (ERD) in a post-stimulus interval relative to a pre-stimulus baseline. A baseline of 500 ms was used in order to obtain at least two oscillatory cycles as a baseline reference even in the lowest frequency involved, i.e. 4 Hz. For target-locked segments a baseline from 800 ms to 300 ms pre-target onset (which is equivalent to the 500 ms baseline before cue onset) and for saccade-locked segments from 1100 ms to 600 ms before saccade onset (which covers a time window that is on average before cue-onset) were chosen. For the frequencies from 4 to 40 Hz, the number of cycles for Morlet wavelets was set to 2 for the lowest frequency but was set to increase with increasing frequencies while allowing to the same degree for an adjustment of the time window (medium between FFT and wavelet analysis (FFT: keeping the time window constant for all frequencies; Wavelet: keeping the number of cycles constant for all frequencies)). Statistical analyses included the factors Saccade task (prosaccade vs. antisaccade) and Group (bilingual vs. monolingual). The analyses conducted for the ERSP data consisted of parametric t-tests (False Discovery Rate (FDR) corrected for multiple comparisons) to test for simple and main effects of these factors and Bootstrapping to test for an interaction between these factors. A significance level of 0.05 was used for all statistical tests and only significant results are reported.

Results

Behavioral results

Behavioral data are presented in Table 4 and inferential statistics on behavioral data in Table 5.

thumbnail
Table 4. Behavioral data for single and mixed task sessions (‘Saccade task’) and for assessing transition effects (‘Transition’).

Error rates (ERR) are given in percentage [%] and saccade latencies (SL) in milliseconds [ms].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Inferential statistics for behavioral data in single and mixed task sessions (‘Saccade task’) and for assessing transition effects (‘Transition’).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.t005

Error rates.

The three-way ANOVA including the factors Saccade task, Block type and Group conducted on error rates revealed a main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 118.79, MSE = 125.64, p < .001, η2p = .763), reflecting the higher error rates for antisaccades (20.7 ± 14.4%) than prosaccades (1.1 ± 2.0%). Moreover, there was a main effect of Block type (F(1, 37) = 75.96, MSE = 27.86, p < .001, η2p = .672), reflecting the higher error rates in mixed task blocks (14.6 ± 17.1%) than single task blocks (7.3 ± 9.1%). Finally, there was a Saccade task by Block type interaction (F(1, 37) = 64.18, MSE = 28.86, p < .001, η2p = .634), indicating that for antisaccades the error rates were significantly higher in mixed task blocks (27.9 ± 15.4%) than in single task blocks (13.6 ± 9.1%; t(38) = 8.55, p < .001), whereas there was no difference for prosaccades (mixed task blocks: 1.4 ± 1.5%; single task blocks: 0.9 ± 2.3%; t(38) = 1.32, p = .195), and that the Saccade task effect was larger in mixed task blocks (d = 26.5%, t(38) = 10.96, p < .001) than in single task blocks (d = 12.7%, t(38) = 9.03, p < .001).

The three-way ANOVA including the factors Saccade task, Transition and Group to investigate the factor Transition corresponding to the transition effect (i.e. influence of the transition between trial n-1 and trial n, switch vs. repetition) revealed a main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 115.63, MSE = 230.23, p < .001, η2p = .758), reflecting the higher error rates in antisaccades (27.6 ± 15.4%) compared to prosaccades (1.4 ± 1.5%). Moreover, there was a main effect of Transition (F(1, 37) = 12.16, MSE = 17.06, p < .001, η2p = .247), reflecting the higher error rates in switch (15.7 ± 8.3%) compared to repetition trials (13.4 ± 8.0%).

Saccade latencies.

The three-way ANOVA including the factors Saccade task, Block type and Group conducted on saccade latencies revealed a main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 228.60, MSE = 782.81, p < .001, η2p = .861) reflecting the longer latencies for antisaccades (346 ± 43 ms) than prosaccades (278 ± 26 ms). Moreover, there was a main effect of Block type (F(1, 37) = 11.27, MSE = 191.70, p < .01, η2p = .234), indicating on average longer latencies in single task blocks (316 ± 51 ms) than in mixed task blocks (308 ± 47 ms). Finally, there was a Saccade task by Block type interaction (F(1, 37) = 27.33, MSE = 124.91, p < .001, η2p = .425) revealing that on average saccade latencies for antisaccades were significantly longer in single task blocks (354 ± 43 ms) than in the mixed ones (338 ± 43 ms; t(38) = 5.77, p < .001), while there was no difference for the prosaccade latencies between the two blocks (single task blocks: 277 ± 21 ms; mixed task blocks: 279 ± 30 ms; t(38) = -0.69, p = .496), and that the Saccade task effect was larger in single task blocks (d = 77 ms, t(38) = 14.20, p < .001) than in mixed task blocks (d = 58 ms, t(38) = 14.30, p < .001).

In the analysis of saccade latencies in dyads, the three-way ANOVA including the factors Saccade task, Transition and Group revealed a main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 165.38, MSE = 752.04, p < .001, η2p = .817) indicating that the averaged saccade latencies were longer for antisaccade (339 ± 45 ms) than for prosaccade trials (282 ± 27 ms). Moreover, there was a main effect of Transition (F(1, 37) = 25.98, MSE = 89.90, p < .001, η2p = .412) reflecting the longer latencies in switch (314 ± 46 ms) compared to repetition trials (306 ± 47 ms). Finally, there was Saccade task by Transition interaction (F(1, 37) = 4.22, MSE = 175.22, p < .05, η2p = .102) indicating that there was a significant Transition effect in prosaccades (d = 12 ms, t(38) = 5.89, p < .001) but not in antisaccades (d = 3 ms, t(38) = 1.08, p = .289). Moreover, there was a tendency towards a Transition by Group interaction (F(1, 37) = 3.96, MSE = 89.90, p = .054, η2p = .097), reflecting a tendency towards a larger Transition effect in bilinguals (d = 11 ms; t(18) = 4.08, p < .001) than in monolinguals (d = 5 ms; t(19) = 3.00, p < .01), whereas there was no difference between groups for repetition trials (t(37) = 0.80, p = .427) or switch trials (t(37) = 1.31, p = .198).

ERP results

In the following, first the results of the mixed task blocks and subsequently those of the single task blocks will be presented. Table 6 and Table 7 display an overview of the statistics for lateral and midline electrodes for the two task blocks.

thumbnail
Table 6. Analyses of ERP data for the lateral electrodes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.t006

thumbnail
Table 7. Analyses of ERP data for the midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.t007

Preparation phase (cue-locked).

Cue-locked positivity (cue-locked 150–250 ms): The four-way ANOVA including the factors Saccade task, Region, Hemisphere and Group conducted on lateral electrodes revealed a main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 8.85, MSE = 0.193, p < .01, η2p = .193) reflecting a reduced positivity in the antisaccade compared to the prosaccade condition (Cue-locked positivity effect; Fig 4 and Fig 5). Moreover, there was a significant Saccade task by Group interaction (F(1, 37) = 4.37, MSE = 0.193, p < .05, η2p = .106), reflecting that the cue-locked positivity effect was present in the monolingual group (t(19) = 3.59, p < .01) but not in the bilingual group (t(18) = 0.62, p = .540), whereas the amplitudes did not differ between the two groups in either antisaccades (t(37) = 1.05, p = .301) or prosaccades (t(37) = -0.06, p = .953). The three-way ANOVA including the factors Saccade task, Electrode and Group conducted on midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz) revealed a Saccade task by Electrode interaction (F(2, 70) = 4.03, MSE = 0.38, p < .05, η2p = .103; Fig 5), indicating that the cue-locked positivity effect was significantly larger on the Cz compared to the Pz electrode (F(1, 35) = 8.03, MSE = 0.43, p < .01, η2p = .187).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Cue- and target-locked ERPs in the mixed task session on midline and lateral electrodes.

The ERPs are presented with -300 ms at cue onset and 0 ms set at target onset, A for all bilingual and monolingual participants confounded, B for bilingual participants, and C for monolingual participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Cue- and target-locked ERPs in the mixed task session on the three midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz).

The left panel shows the main effect of Saccade task and the right panel the difference waves (antisaccades minus prosaccades) in the two groups. Grey bars mark the time windows used for investigating the cue-locked positivity effect, as well as the target locked N2 and P3 components.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.g005

Implementation phase (target-locked).

N2 (target-locked 160–200 ms): The four-way ANOVA including the factors Saccade task, Region, Hemisphere and Group conducted on lateral electrodes revealed a main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 12.07, MSE = 0.440, p < .001, η2p = .246), reflecting a larger negativity in the antisaccade than in the prosaccade condition (N2 effect; Fig 4 and Fig 5). For midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz), the three-way ANOVA including the factors Saccade task, Electrode and Group revealed a significant Saccade task by Group by Electrode interaction (F(2, 70) = 6.73, MSE = 1.35, p < .01, η2p = .161; Fig 5). Post-hoc analyses revealed that there was a significant Saccade task by Group interaction at the Fz electrode (F(1, 35) = 5.39, MSE = 1.36, p < .05, η2p = .133), indicating that the N2 effect was present in the bilingual group (t(17) = 3.23, p < .01) but not in the monolingual group (t(18) = 0.35, p = .734), whereas the amplitudes did not differ between the two groups in either antisaccades (t(35) = -1.34, p = .190) or prosaccades (t(35) = 0.01, p = .992). Moreover, the Saccade task by Group interaction was also significant at the Pz electrode (F(1, 35) = 4.83, MSE = 1.13, p < .05, η2p = .121), where the N2 effect was present in the monolingual group (t(18) = 2.77, p < .05) but not in the bilingual group (t(17) = -0.54, p = .594), whereas the amplitudes did not differ between the two groups in either antisaccades (t(35) = 1.80, p = .080) or prosaccades (t(35) = 0.46, p = .652). Given that the control monitoring-related N2 is characterized as having a fronto-central distribution [77], only the group differences for the N2 effect on the Fz electrode will be discussed subsequently.

P3 (target-locked 200–400 ms): The four-way ANOVA with the factors Saccade task, Region, Hemisphere and Group run on lateral electrodes revealed a significant main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 15.56, MSE = 0.695, p < .001, η2p = .296) reflecting a reduced positivity in the antisaccade compared to the prosaccade condition (P3 effect; Figs 4 and 5). Moreover, there was a Saccade task by Region interaction (F(1, 37) = 16.57, MSE = 0.809, p < .001, η2p = .309), indicating that the P3 effect was only significant over the posterior region (t(38) = 6.86, p < .001) but not over the anterior one (t(38) = -0.28, p = .781). For the midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz), the three-way ANOVA with the factors Saccade task, Electrode and Group showed a Saccade task by Group by Electrode interaction (F(2, 70) = 4.60, MSE = 0.99, p < .05, η2p = .116; Fig 5). However, the post-hoc analyses only revealed a marginal Saccade task by Group interaction at the Pz electrode (F(1, 35) = 2.70, MSE = 0.75, p = .109, η2p = .072), indicating a descriptively smaller P3 effect in bilinguals than in monolinguals.

Late parietal positivity (LPP; target-locked; Antisaccades: 400–650 ms, Prosaccades: 400–550 ms): The LPP effect was tested in the two Saccade tasks separately and after visual inspection, the target-locked time window 400–650 ms was selected for antisaccades while in prosaccades, the time window 400–550 ms was selected. In antisaccades, the four-way ANOVA with the factors Transition, Region, Hemisphere and Group run on lateral electrodes revealed a main effect of Transition (switch vs. repetition; F(1, 37) = 20.56, MSE = 0.19, p < .001, η2p = .357), reflecting a larger LPP in the antisaccade switch condition (antisaccade trial preceded by a prosaccade trial) than in antisaccade repetition condition (antisaccade trial preceded by an antisaccade trial; Antisaccade LPP effect; Figs 6 and 7). For prosaccades, the four-way ANOVA with the factors Transition, Region, Hemisphere and Group run on lateral electrodes revealed a main effect of Transition (switch vs. repetition; F(1, 37) = 10.98, MSE = 0.30, p < .01, η2p = .229) reflecting a larger LPP in the prosaccade switch condition (prosaccade trial preceded by an antisaccade trial) than in the prosaccade repetition condition (prosaccade trial preceded by a prosaccade trial; Prosaccade LPP effect; Fig 6 and Fig 7). Moreover, there was a Transition by Region interaction (F(1, 37) = 6.63, MSE = 0.47, p < .05, η2p = .152) indicating that the prosaccade LPP effect was only present over the anterior region (t(38) = -3.86, p < .001) but not the posterior one (t(38) = -0.06, p = .956).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Target-locked ERPs for the Transition types switch and repetition on midline and lateral electrodes.

The ERPs are presented A for antisaccades, and B for prosaccades, for all bilingual and monolingual participants confounded.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.g006

thumbnail
Fig 7. Target-locked ERPs for the Transition types switch and repetition on the three midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz).

The left panel shows the ERPs for antisaccade trials and the right panel the ERPs for prosaccade trials, confounded over the two groups. The grey bar marks the time window used for investigating the late parietal positivity (LPP) component.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.g007

Execution phase (saccade-locked).

Presaccadic positivity (PSP; saccade-locked -250 to -50 ms): The four-way ANOVA including the factors Saccade task, Region, Hemisphere and Group conducted on lateral electrodes revealed a main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 8.75, MSE = 0.335, p < .01, η2p = .191), reflecting a reduced PSP in the antisaccade compared to the prosaccade condition (PSP effect; Figs 8 and 9). Moreover, there was a Saccade task by Region interaction (F(1, 37) = 9.87, MSE = 0.718, p < .01, η2p = .211) indicating that the PSP effect was present over the anterior region (t(38) = 4.05, p < .001), but not the posterior one (t(38) = -0.90, p = .375). The three-way ANOVA including the factors Saccade task, Electrode and Group conducted on midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz) revealed a Saccade task by Group interaction (F(1, 35) = 4.68, MSE = 0.952, p < .05, η2p = .118; Fig 9), indicating that the PSP effect was present in the monolingual (t(18) = 6.22, p < .001) but not in the bilingual group (t(17) = 1.78, p = .093), whereas the amplitudes did not differ between the two groups in either antisaccades (t(35) = 1.87, p = .069) or prosaccades (t(35) = 0.31, p = .759). Moreover, there was a Saccade task by Group by Electrode interaction (F(2, 70) = 5.66, MSE = 0.93, p < .05, η2p = .139). Post-hoc analyses revealed a Saccade task by Group interaction at the Cz electrode (F(1, 35) = 5.09, MSE = 0.597, p < .05, η2p = .127), indicating that the PSP effect was present in monolinguals (t(18) = 6.14, p < .001) but not in bilinguals (t(17) = 1.41, p = .177), and, moreover, that monolinguals showed a marginally significant reduced PSP amplitude compared to bilinguals in antisaccades (t(35) = 2.04, p = .049), but not in prosaccades (t(35) = 0.76, p = .453).

thumbnail
Fig 8. Saccade-locked ERPs in the mixed task session on midline and lateral electrodes.

The ERPs are presented A for all bilingual and monolingual participants confounded, B for bilingual participants, and C for monolingual participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.g008

thumbnail
Fig 9. Saccade-locked ERPs in the mixed task session on the three midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz).

The left panel shows the main effect of Saccade task and the right panel the difference waves (antisaccades minus prosaccades) in the two groups. The grey bar marks the time window used for investigating the presaccadic positivity (PSP) component.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.g009

Single task blocks.

Moreover, analyses on the single task blocks were conducted because we were interested if a bilingual advantage would show to the same degree if the two saccade tasks were presented in separate blocks. The data and statistics for mixed and single task blocks are presented in Table 6 for lateral electrodes and Table 7 for midline electrodes. ERPs for single task blocks are presented in Fig 10 for cue- and target-locked ERPs and in Fig 11 for saccade-locked ERPs. In the preparation phase, for the cue-locked positivity (cue-locked 150–250 ms), the four-way ANOVA with the factors Saccade task, Region, Hemisphere and Group conducted on lateral electrodes revealed a main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 23.44, MSE = 0.216, p < .001, η2p = .388) reflecting a reduced positivity in the antisaccade compared to the prosaccade condition (Cue-locked positivity effect). The three-way ANOVA with the factors Saccade task, Electrode and Group conducted on midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz) revealed a Saccade task by Electrode interaction (F(2, 70) = 7.58, MSE = 0.383, p < .01, η2p = .178), indicating that the cue-locked positivity effect was significantly larger on the Cz as compared to the Fz electrode (F(1, 35) = 13.31, MSE = 0.521, p < .001, η2p = .275) and the Pz electrode (F(1, 35) = 14.32, MSE = 0.583, p < .001, η2p = .290). In the implementation phase, for the N2 (target-locked 160–200 ms), the four-way ANOVA with the factors Saccade task, Region, Hemisphere and Group conducted on lateral electrodes revealed a main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 19.39, MSE = 0.841, p < .001, η2p = .344), reflecting a larger negativity in the antisaccade than in the prosaccade condition (N2 effect). Moreover, there was a Saccade task by Region interaction (F(1, 37) = 5.75, MSE = 0.822, p < .05, η2p = .135), indicating that the N2 effect was present over the posterior region (t(38) = 6.64, p < .001) but not the anterior one (t(38) = 1.19, p = .242). For the P3 (target locked 200–400 ms), the four-way ANOVA with the factors Saccade task, Region, Hemisphere and Group run on lateral electrodes showed a significant main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 18.82, MSE = 0.863, p < .001, η2p = .337), reflecting a reduced positivity in the antisaccade compared to the prosaccade condition (P3 effect). Moreover, there was a Saccade task by Region interaction (F(1, 37) = 9.92, MSE = 0.838, p < .01, η2p = .211), reflecting that the P3 effect was present over the posterior region (t(38) = 6.87, p < .001) but not the anterior one (t(38) = 0.71, p = .482). In the execution phase, for the PSP (saccade-locked -250 to -50 ms prior to saccade onset), the four-way ANOVA with the factors Saccade task, Region, Hemisphere and Group run on lateral electrodes revealed a main effect of Saccade task (F(1, 37) = 39.95, MSE = 0.965, p < .001, η2p = .519) reflecting a reduced PSP in the antisaccade compared to the prosaccade condition (PSP effect). Moreover, there was a Saccade task by Region interaction (F(1, 37) = 6.35, MSE = 0.703, p < .05, η2p = .146) indicating that the PSP effect was larger over the anterior region (t(38) = 5.13, p < .001) than the posterior one (t(38) = 4.70, p < .001). Finally, neither a main effect of Group nor an interaction involving Group was found.

thumbnail
Fig 10. Cue- and target-locked ERPs in single task blocks on midline and lateral electrodes.

The ERPs are presented with -300 ms at cue onset and 0 ms set at target onset, for all bilingual and monolingual participants confounded.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.g010

thumbnail
Fig 11. Saccade-locked ERPs in single task blocks on midline and lateral electrodes.

The ERPs are presented for all bilingual and monolingual participants confounded.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.g011

Correlation analyses between language background factors and ERP effect sizes.

Correlation analyses between language background factors (language switching experience, immersion in L2 environment, L2 proficiency, motivation to improve L2 proficiency, and frequency of daily L2 use) and the ERP effect sizes and electrode sites for which group differences had been found [Cue-locked positivity effect (Cz), N2 effect (Fz), P3 effect (Pz), PSP effect (Cz)] were conducted (Table 8). At the execution phase (saccade-locked), our correlation analyses showed a negative correlation between immersion in the L2 environment and PSP effect size on the Cz electrode (r(17) = -.550, p < .05). This correlation indicates that the more L2 immersion experience the bilingual participants had, the smaller was their PSP effect size. Moreover, there was a negative correlation between L2 proficiency and PSP effect size on the Cz electrode (r(13) = -.742, p < .01) indicating that higher L2 proficiency was related to a smaller PSP effect size.

thumbnail
Table 8. Pearson correlations between behavioral, ERP and DCM effect sizes on the one hand and language background factors on the other hand, in bilinguals.

Data are presented for mixed task blocks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.t008

Dynamic causal modelling (DCM).

DCM analyses were conducted for pro- and antisaccades issued from mixed task blocks. A highly plausible model was constructed and inverted for each participant, i.e. during the model inversion process, DCM optimizes for each participant the information provided concerning the electromagnetic forward model and the neuronal sources, aiming at minimizing free energy [98]. The DCM included modulatory connections—i.e. which were modelled to vary between the experimental conditions antisaccade vs. prosaccade—between anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), bilateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and bilateral frontal eye fields (FEF).

DCM parameter estimates for modulatory connections.

Parameter estimates were extracted for each modulatory effective connection, i.e. connections that were modelled to vary between the experimental conditions antisaccade vs. prosaccade. Independent samples t-tests with Group as a between-subjects factor were run on the parameter estimates of each modulatory connection. Moreover, independent samples t-tests were conducted to examine if the effect was significantly different from 0 in either of the groups. Descriptive and inferential statistics are shown in Table 9. Effective connectivity measures reflect how activity in one brain region influences activity in another region [87]. The independent samples t-test on modulatory parameters revealed a group difference for the RPFC to LFEF modulatory connection (t(37) = 2.75, p = .01 (FDR-corrected p = .096)), indicating that in monolinguals there was a more negative effective connectivity in RPFC to LFEF (-.25 ± .37; t(19) = -3.07, p = .006 (FDR-corrected p = .06)) in antisaccades compared to prosaccades, while this effective connectivity modulation was not significant in bilinguals (.13 ± .50; t(18) = 1.18, p = .253). Moreover, groupwise analyses showed that in bilinguals there was tendency towards a more negative effective connectivity in ACC to RPFC (-.19 ± .46; t(18) = -1.85, p = .081 (FDR-corrected p = .81)) in antisaccades compared to prosaccades, but not in monolinguals (.03 ± .42; t(19) = 0.28, p = .78).

thumbnail
Table 9. Dynamic causal modelling (DCM): modulatory parameter estimates.

Parameter estimates for modulatory connections, i.e. connections that were modelled to vary between the experimental conditions antisaccade vs. prosaccade, are presented for bilingual and monolingual participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.t009

Correlation analyses between language background factors and DCM modulatory connections.

Correlation analyses between language background factors and the DCM modulatory connection that were found to be relevant in the bilingual group, i.e. the modulatory effective connection from ACC to RPFC, were conducted for the bilingual group (Table 8). However, there was no significant correlation between any of the five language background factors and the strength of this modulatory effective connection.

Time-frequency results

In order to test for vector inversion and motor planning, we tested for target-locked fronto-central and occipital beta (13-26Hz) power changes in anti- compared to prosaccades. There was a significant (p < .05, FDR-corrected) power decrease in the antisaccade compared to the prosaccade condition around 150 ms after target onset in the beta band over the vertex and occipital scalp (Fig 12). There was no main effect of or interaction with Group.

thumbnail
Fig 12. Time-frequency analysis.

A Event-related spectral perturbations (ERSPs) time-locked to target onset are plotted for the Cz electrode for antisaccades and prosaccades and a panel for significant ERSP differences between Saccade tasks is displayed on the right side. B The beta power decrease at around 150 ms after target onset in antisaccades compared to prosaccades over the central and posterior scalp is plotted at frequency 24 Hz. A panel showing the electrodes with a significant ERSP difference between Saccade tasks in red is plotted on the right side.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165029.g012

Discussion

In the present study, we investigated the neurodynamics of control processes involved in performing an antisaccade task in two groups differing in language use, i.e. bilinguals and monolinguals. The rationale was to examine whether executive control processes whose efficiency is reinforced by the frequent use of a second language can lead to a benefit in the control of eye movements, i.e. a non-linguistic activity. While the two groups performed similarly at the behavioral level, at the neuronal level clear differences emerged in the ERP measures and dynamic causal modelling (DCM) between bilinguals and monolinguals.

ERP data

In our study we found a larger target-locked N2 in the antisaccade than in the prosaccade condition, assumed in the literature to reflect conflict monitoring [7476]. Moreover, the N2 effect was found to be smaller over the Fz in bilinguals than monolinguals, whereas it was larger in bilinguals over the Pz. In an antisaccade task, it is possible that at least two N2 subcomponents can be distinguished, e.g. a conflict monitoring N2 with frontal distribution and a posteriorly distributed N2 reflecting visual attention or target detection [77,101]. The observation of a larger target-locked frontal N2 effect in bilinguals than in monolinguals suggests based on previous studies that this enhancement could be associated with stronger control [102] or stronger conflict monitoring involvement [103]. However, there is currently no unanimous view on the exact functional role of the variation of the N2 amplitude and/or effect size in terms of conflict monitoring capacity [104]. In addition, our ERP data also revealed a group effect for markers of inhibition. A smaller effect size was observed in bilinguals compared to monolinguals for the cue-locked positivity, the target-locked P3 and the saccade-locked presaccadic positivity (PSP), i.e. three ERP markers thought to reflect inhibitory processes / decision to withhold an automatic response [68,83,84]. Moreover, in bilinguals, negative correlations were observed but only between the PSP effect size and (1) L2 proficiency and (2) immersion experience suggesting that these two linguistic factors are good indicators of inhibitory control improvement. Higher L2 proficiency and immersion experience may, due to the continuous need to control over the L1 and the increasingly automatic L2, lead to strengthened inhibitory control. Bilinguals with high compared to those with low second language proficiency have previously been shown to have a behavioral advantage in an oculomotor control task [48], an advantage that had however been attributed to improved performance monitoring rather than inhibitory control. Taken together, the increased N2 effect size in bilinguals, thought to reflect their more efficient conflict monitoring, combined with the reduced effect sizes on markers reflecting inhibitory control, i.e. cue-locked positivity, the target-locked P3 and the saccade-locked presaccadic positivity (PSP), may reflect a dynamic interplay between strengthened conflict monitoring leading to subsequently reduced cost for inhibitory control in realizing the task. While neurophysiological differences between bilinguals and monolinguals were observed for conflict monitoring and inhibition there were no differences on the behavioral level. One reason may be the higher sensitivity of neurophysiological and neuroimaging techniques for disentangling cognitive sub-processes while behavioral measures in many cases allow for assessing a performance that is a product of different sub-processes [105,106]. Finally, no difference between groups was observed for the marker of switching-related activity, i.e. the target-locked late parietal positivity (LPP). That means that bilinguals and monolinguals performed similarly to change from one task to the other in both direction of switch. The type of bilinguals tested in the present study, i.e. late bilinguals immersed in their L1 environment who regularly use their L2, however without frequent switches between the two languages, may have an expertise in conflict monitoring and inhibitory control, but less so in switching-related processes. Hence, further investigation should try to clarify if different types of bilinguals with respect to their language-switching activity show different profiles of control enhancement. To sum up, the present evidence suggests that while bilinguals–at least for the type of bilinguals tested here–do present more efficient neuronal conflict detection and inhibitory control, it was not the case for task switching.

DCM data

Our study of effective connectivity used a dynamic causal model (DCM) of the executive control network supposed to be involved in eye movement control. The model included modulatory forward connections from the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) to the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and to the frontal eye fields (FEF), as well as backward from PFC to ACC. For parameter estimates of modulatory connections, differences between the two groups emerged. In DCM, the notion of causality is used in a control theory sense and means that activity in one brain area causes dynamics in another, and that these dynamics cause the observations [87]. In this sense, a positive effective connection indicates that the activity in the source region’s neural population-level activity leads to an increase in the neural population-level activity in the target region, while a negative effective connection indicates that the activity in the source regions leads to a decrease of the activity in the target region. Positive effective connections are thought to reflect an excitatory effect from the source to the target region [107]. Negative effective connections have been interpreted to reflect, either a top-down inhibitory influence from the source to the target region [107,108], or an increase of the response threshold in the target region (leading to a decreased activation in the target region; [108]). Our main interest focused on the effective connections from the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)–thought to be a core neuronal region involved in conflict monitoring–and the bilateral prefrontal cortex (PFC)–thought to be a crucial region involved in inhibitory control implementation. Group differences were found for the effective connectivity from RPFC to LFEF, with a more negative effective connectivity in antisaccades compared to prosaccades in monolinguals but not in bilinguals. The more negative effective connectivity in RPFC to LFEF in monolinguals but not in bilinguals may reflect a stronger top-down inhibitory influence from PFC to FEF in monolinguals. Moreover, for the connection from ACC to RPFC, we found a more negative effective connectivity in antisaccades compared to prosaccades in bilinguals but not in monolinguals. The more negative effective connectivity from ACC to PFC in bilinguals may reflect a stronger reliance on ACC-directed control (stronger reliance on conflict monitoring) in bilinguals and the more negative effective connectivity from RPFC to LFEF in monolinguals may reflect a stronger reliance on PFC-directed control (stronger involvement of inhibitory control) in monolinguals in performing the antisaccade task. The more negative effective connectivity from ACC to PFC in bilinguals may reflect their strong reliance on highly efficient conflict monitoring in the ACC and its–possibly facilitatory—influence on the subsequent inhibitory control implementation in the PFC, which might indicate that bilinguals resolve conflict in a less costly way than monolinguals. Moreover, the negative connection from ACC to PFC may reflect an inhibitory effect from ACC to PFC activity or in contrast an increase of the response threshold in the PFC, reflected by its reduced activity. Furthermore, this pattern of source activation may also be reflected on the ERP level, on which the increased N2 effect in bilinguals may reflect the stronger reliance on conflict monitoring, which might be causally related to the subsequently smaller effects on markers of inhibitory control, i.e. the cue-locked positivity, the target-locked P3 and the saccade-locked PSP effects. It has previously been shown that individuals with a low (vs. highly) persistent cognitive style show better conflict monitoring ability, and on the neuronal level higher ACC activity and frontal N2 effect size, in performing a Stroop task [103]. Similarly, in the present study, the increased frontal N2 effect size and the ACC-driven neuronal processes might reflect strongly conflict monitoring-driven control in bilingual participants, while in monolinguals inhibitory-driven control appears to be dominant. These group differences in control processes may be linked to the experience with error and uncertainty in second language learning, but further investigation would give precious insight into this question.

To sum up, in bilinguals, the negative effective connectivity from ACC to PFC may indicate a preference in tackling the conflict in an antisaccade task via conflict monitoring. This may hence lead to less costly reactive and inhibitory control implementation, while in monolinguals, the negative effective connectivity from PFC to FEF may indicate that monolinguals preferentially tackle the antisaccade conflict by PFC-based inhibitory control. Moreover, the smaller effect sizes for ERP-markers of inhibition in the present study may also indicate that the reduced cost for inhibitory processes is also due to basically enhanced inhibitory capacities in bilinguals. A key conclusion from the present study is that the investigation of control processes and their neural substrate in isolation might not reveal the most accurate picture but that they should always be considered within their tight interrelation with other control processes. Further investigation should also take into consideration the role of subcortical structures in cognitive and motor control, given their involvement in the antisaccade task [57] as well as in multiple language control [88,89,109], for which neuroimaging techniques such as fMRI or NIRS could provide useful information. Moreover, in EEG studies, a higher number of electrodes would increase the clarity of source and DCM analyses and hence the strength of the effects, which should be taken into consideration in future studies. Moreover, different profiles of bilinguals may show different patterns of control use, hence the study of bilinguals showing a high degree of language switching or mixing as well as bilinguals in early stages of immersion experience may provide further insight on the patterns of neuroplastic control adaptations.

Domain-general control in multiple language use

The present findings support psycholinguistic theories postulating domain-general control involvement in multiple language control. The sustained coactivation of a bilingual’s languages [1216] and the bidirectional cross-language influences [1921] require efficient top-down control in order to allow for the adaptation to speakers of one of the two–or both—languages and for fluid communication. Due to frequent situations of language interference and conflict, bilinguals may in the long run develop more efficient conflict monitoring and inhibitory control in order to prevent and resolve situations of conflict. The observation that ERP and effective connectivity differences between bilinguals and monolinguals are observed in a completely non-linguistic task, i.e. the antisaccade task, is strong evidence that the control processes recruited by bilinguals to control the use of their languages are domain-general and hence shared between different cognitive domains. Moreover, there is evidence that the interplay between conflict monitoring and inhibition processes is optimized in bilinguals in order to reduce the cost in conflict processing.

Time-frequency data

In order to test for vector inversion and motor planning, we tested for target-locked fronto-central and occipital beta (13-26Hz) power changes in anti- compared to prosaccades. There was a significant power decrease in the antisaccade compared to the prosaccade condition around 150 ms after target onset in the beta band over centro-posterior scalp but there was no main effect of or interaction with Group. Note that this effect occurred before saccade onset in prosaccades (279 ± 30 ms) and antisaccades (338 ± 43 ms) and is hence not linked to the muscular activity during motor execution itself. For the change of a motor program, power modulations in the alpha (8–12 Hz) and beta (13–26 Hz) frequency bands have been shown to be of relevance. Alpha and beta oscillations play a role in holding a status quo and for inhibiting movements. Alpha oscillations have been suggested to play a role in the suppression of the excitability in sensory and motor areas, e.g. regulating the receptiveness/readiness in saccadic control network circuits, and to serve a top-down control function for suppressing externally driven saccades in favor of internal goals [110]. Beta oscillations are important for the maintenance of a current motor state, and beta power is highest during holding periods after movements [86]. Beta attenuation however occurs during voluntary movements as well as during preparation and execution of movements, where beta activity is replaced by faster rhythms in the gamma-band (30–100 Hz) [86]. Cordones et al. [85] found stronger fronto-central and occipital beta power decreases in antisaccades compared to nogo trials during the instructive period, which may indicate that these beta power variations do not reflect inhibitory processes but motor planning and preparation. Moreover, beta power decreases have been found in the somatosensory cortex contralateral to the stimulus in an antisaccade task, which has been claimed to reflect somatosensory gating, i.e. increasing the excitability of the cortical region that shows a beta decrease [111]. In accordance with these previous findings, we find beta suppression relatively early, and slightly earlier than proposed in our model (Fig 1), where we suggest vector inversion to be temporally overlapping with conflict monitoring and inhibition processes. This indicates that motor planning processes are also strongly reflected by beta suppression. The occurrence of beta suppression already slightly before conflict monitoring is in accordance with the fact that vector inversion processes are related to preparation and motor planning processes, that take place with the use of the color cue indicating if a vector inversion will have to take place or not, in the present study. Given that the presence or absence of a conflict between movement directions can be prepared, it is hence likely that the motor planning and vector inversion processes start earlier than hypothesized in our model, i.e. already before conflict monitoring, and continue largely in parallel with conflict monitoring as well as with inhibitory processes. To sum up, power variations in the beta band seem to reflect processes of vector inversion and motor planning in the antisaccade task, but these control processes do not seem to differ between bilinguals and monolinguals.

Limitations

A limitation of the present study is that our groups of participants were not matched for their intelligence quotient (IQ). Some executive control processes have been found to relate to intelligence [112]. Especially, updating of working memory has been found to be correlated with intelligence measures, however less so the here tested inhibiting and shifting functions [112]. Moreover, in the present study, we used a set of 32 channels. A higher number of electrodes would be optimal for source and DCM analyses and hence the strength of our effects. In future studies, these aspects should be taken into consideration in order to increase the power of the experimental design.

Conclusion

In the present neurophysiological study examining the impact of bilingualism on event-related potentials (ERP), event-related spectral perturbation (ERSP) and the effective connectivity of the underlying neuronal generators (dynamic causal modelling; DCM) in a non-linguistic motor task, we provide evidence for the crucial role of domain-general control involvement in the control over multiple language use. Bilinguals compared to monolinguals show an increased neurophysiological effect for conflict monitoring and reduced neurophysiological effect sizes on markers of inhibitory control. Moreover, there is evidence from dynamic causal modelling that bilinguals rely more strongly on ACC-driven control and monolinguals on PFC-driven control. Finally, L2 proficiency and immersion experience appear to be good predictors of a more efficient inhibitory control.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Christelle Lemoine-Lardennois for her contribution in the experimental setup and to Thierry Baccino and Barbara Hemforth for fruitful discussions as well as to our reviewers for their helpful comments. Earlier reports of this research were presented at the annual meetings of the Society for Neuroscience 2013 (San Diego, CA. Nov 9–13, 2013) and 2014 (Washington, DC. Nov 15–19, 2014), and at the 17th European Conference on Eye Movements (ECEM) 2013 (Lund, Sweden, Aug 11–16, 2013).

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: KH KDM XA FI.
  2. Data curation: KH.
  3. Formal analysis: KH KDM FI.
  4. Funding acquisition: KDM XA FI.
  5. Investigation: KH.
  6. Methodology: KH KDM XA FI.
  7. Project administration: KH KDM FI.
  8. Resources: KDM FI.
  9. Software: KH KDM XA FI.
  10. Supervision: KDM FI.
  11. Validation: KH KDM FI.
  12. Visualization: KH.
  13. Writing – original draft: KH FI.
  14. Writing – review & editing: KH FI.

References

  1. 1. Greenwood PM, Parasuraman R. Neuronal and Cognitive Plasticity: A Neurocognitive Framework for Ameliorating Cognitive Aging. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2.
  2. 2. Li S-C. Neuromodulation and developmental contextual influences on neural and cognitive plasticity across the lifespan. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013 Nov;37(9):2201–8.
  3. 3. Green DW. Language Control in Different Contexts: The Behavioral Ecology of Bilingual Speakers. Front Psychol. 2011;2.
  4. 4. Green DW, Abutalebi J. Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. J Cogn Psychol. 2013 May 24;1–16.
  5. 5. Green DW. Individual variability and neuroplastic changes. Appl Psycholinguist. 2014 Sep;35(5):910–2.
  6. 6. Li P, Legault J, Litcofsky KA. Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: Anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex. 2014 Sep;58:301–24. pmid:24996640
  7. 7. Mårtensson J, Eriksson J, Bodammer NC, Lindgren M, Johansson M, Nyberg L, et al. Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning. NeuroImage. 2012 Oct;63(1):240–4. pmid:22750568
  8. 8. Stein M, Federspiel A, Koenig T, Wirth M, Strik W, Wiest R, et al. Structural plasticity in the language system related to increased second language proficiency. Cortex. 2012 Apr;48(4):458–65. pmid:21106192
  9. 9. Schlegel AA, Rudelson JJ, Peter UT. White matter structure changes as adults learn a second language. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012;24(8):1664–1670. pmid:22571459
  10. 10. Segalowitz N, Hulstijn J. Automaticity in Bilingualism and Second Language Learning. In: Kroll JF, De Groot AMB, editors. Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. p. 371–88.
  11. 11. Segalowitz N. Cognitive bases of second language fluency. New York: Routledge; 2010. 220 p. (Cognitive science and second language acquisition series).
  12. 12. Brysbaert M. Bilingual visual word recognition: Evidence from masked phonological priming. In: Kinoshita S, Lupker SJ, editors. Masked priming: State-of-the-art. Hove: Psychology Press; 2003.
  13. 13. Dijkstra T. Bilingual visual word recognition and lexical access. In: Kroll JF, De Groot AMB, editors. Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. p. 179–201.
  14. 14. Dijkstra T, van Heuven WJB. The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Biling Lang Cogn. 2002 Dec 18;5(3).
  15. 15. Hoshino N, Thierry G. Language selection in bilingual word production: Electrophysiological evidence for cross-language competition. Brain Res. 2011 Jan;1371:100–9. pmid:21108940
  16. 16. Van Heuven WJB, Dijkstra T, Grainger J. Orthographic Neighborhood Effects in Bilingual Word Recognition. J Mem Lang. 1998;39:458–83.
  17. 17. Green DW. Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Biling Lang Cogn. 1998;1(2):67–81.
  18. 18. Abutalebi J, Green DW. Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. J Neurolinguistics. 2007 May;20(3):242–75.
  19. 19. Blumenfeld HK, Marian V. Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. J Cogn Psychol. 2013 Aug;25(5):547–67.
  20. 20. Costa A, Albareda B, Santesteban M. Assessing the presence of lexical competition across languages: Evidence from the Stroop task. Biling Lang Cogn. 2008 Mar 6;11(1).
  21. 21. Runnqvist E, Strijkers K, Alario F-X, Costa A. Cumulative semantic interference is blind to language: Implications for models of bilingual speech production. J Mem Lang. 2012 May;66(4):850–69.
  22. 22. Aron AR, Durston S, Eagle DM, Logan GD, Stinear CM, Stuphorn V. Converging Evidence for a Fronto-Basal-Ganglia Network for Inhibitory Control of Action and Cognition. J Neurosci. 2007 Oct 31;27(44):11860–4. pmid:17978025
  23. 23. Costa A, Santesteban M, Ivanova I. How do highly proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? Inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both functional. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2006;32(5):1057–74. pmid:16938046
  24. 24. Costa A, Santesteban M. Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. J Mem Lang. 2004 May;50(4):491–511.
  25. 25. Jiang J, Egner T. Using Neural Pattern Classifiers to Quantify the Modularity of Conflict-Control Mechanisms in the Human Brain. Cereb Cortex. 2014 Jul 1;24(7):1793–805. pmid:23402762
  26. 26. Barsalou LW. Grounded Cognition: Past, Present, and Future: Topics in Cognitive Science. Top Cogn Sci. 2010 Oct;2(4):716–24. pmid:25164052
  27. 27. Pulvermüller F. Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(7):576–582. pmid:15959465
  28. 28. Hauk O, Johnsrude I, Pulvermüller F. Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron. 2004;41(2):301–307. pmid:14741110
  29. 29. Gentilucci M, Benuzzi F, Bertolani L, Daprati E, Gangitano M. Language and motor control. Exp Brain Res. 2000 Aug 4;133(4):468–90. pmid:10985682
  30. 30. Miyake A, Friedman NP. The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012 Jan 31;21(1):8–14. pmid:22773897
  31. 31. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognit Psychol. 2000 Aug;41(1):49–100. pmid:10945922
  32. 32. De Baene W, Duyck W, Brass M, Carreiras M. Brain Circuit for Cognitive Control Is Shared by Task and Language Switching. J Cogn Neurosci. 2015 Apr 22;1–14.
  33. 33. de Bruin A, Roelofs A, Dijkstra T, FitzPatrick I. Domain-general inhibition areas of the brain are involved in language switching: FMRI evidence from trilingual speakers. NeuroImage. 2014 Apr;90:348–59. pmid:24384153
  34. 34. Weissberger GH, Gollan TH, Bondi MW, Clark LR, Wierenga CE. Language and task switching in the bilingual brain: Bilinguals are staying, not switching, experts. Neuropsychologia. 2015 Jan;66:193–203. pmid:25446970
  35. 35. Gathercole VCM, Thomas EM, Jones L, Guasch NV, Young N, Hughes EK. Cognitive effects of bilingualism: digging deeper for the contributions of language dominance, linguistic knowledge, socio-economic status and cognitive abilities. Int J Biling Educ Biling. 2010 Sep;13(5):617–64.
  36. 36. Heidlmayr K, Moutier S, Hemforth B, Courtin C, Tanzmeister R, Isel F. Successive bilingualism and executive functions: The effect of second language use on inhibitory control in a behavioural Stroop Colour Word task. Biling Lang Cogn. 2014 Jul;17(3):630–45.
  37. 37. Heidlmayr K, Hemforth B, Moutier S, Isel F. Neurodynamics of executive control processes in bilinguals: Evidence from ERP and source reconstruction analyses. Front Psychol. 2015;6.
  38. 38. Hernández M, Costa A, Fuentes LJ, Vivas AB, Sebastián-Gallés N. The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Biling Lang Cogn. 2010 May 4;13(3):315–25.
  39. 39. Kovacs AM, Mehler J. Flexible Learning of Multiple Speech Structures in Bilingual Infants. Science. 2009 Jul 9;325(5940):611–2. pmid:19589962
  40. 40. Kroll JF, Bialystok E. Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. J Cogn Psychol. 2013 Aug;25(5):497–514.
  41. 41. Prior A, MacWhinney B. A bilingual advantage in task switching. Biling Lang Cogn. 2010;13(2):253–62.
  42. 42. Costa A, Hernández M, Costa-Faidella J, Sebastián-Gallés N. On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition. 2009 Nov;113(2):135–49. pmid:19729156
  43. 43. Hilchey MD, Klein RM. Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychon Bull Rev. 2011 Jun 15;18(4):625–58. pmid:21674283
  44. 44. Paap KR, Greenberg ZI. There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognit Psychol. 2013 Mar;66(2):232–58. pmid:23370226
  45. 45. Valian V. Bilingualism and cognition. Biling Lang Cogn. 2015 Jan;18(1):3–24.
  46. 46. Costa A, Hernández M, Sebastián-Gallés N. Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition. 2008 Jan;106(1):59–86. pmid:17275801
  47. 47. Festman J, Münte TF. Cognitive Control in Russian–German Bilinguals. Front Psychol. 2012;3.
  48. 48. Singh N, Mishra RK. The modulatory role of second language proficiency on performance monitoring: evidence from a saccadic countermanding task in high and low proficient bilinguals. Front Psychol. 2015 Jan 5;5.
  49. 49. Schall JD, Stuphorn V, Brown JW. Monitoring and control of action by the frontal lobes. Neuron. 2002;36(2):309–322. pmid:12383784
  50. 50. Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Ryan J. Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2006;32(6):1341–54. pmid:17087588
  51. 51. Bialystok E, Viswanathan M. Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition. 2009 Sep;112(3):494–500. pmid:19615674
  52. 52. Prior A, Gollan TH. Good Language-Switchers are Good Task-Switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English Bilinguals. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011 Jun;17(4):682–91. pmid:22882810
  53. 53. Fedorenko E, Thompson-Schill SL. Reworking the language network. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014 Mar;18(3):120–6. pmid:24440115
  54. 54. Calabria M, Hernández M, Branzi FM, Costa A. Qualitative Differences between Bilingual Language Control and Executive Control: Evidence from Task-Switching. Front Psychol. 2012;2.
  55. 55. Muhle-Karbe PS, De Baene W, Brass M. Do tasks matter in task switching? Dissociating domain-general from context-specific brain activity. NeuroImage. 2014 Oct;99:332–41. pmid:24875143
  56. 56. Hallett PE. Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Res. 1978;18(10):1279–96. pmid:726270
  57. 57. Munoz DP, Everling S. Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004 Mar;5(3):218–28. pmid:14976521
  58. 58. Luna B. Developmental changes in cognitive control through adolescence. Adv Child Dev Behav. 2009;37:233–278. pmid:19673164
  59. 59. Harnishfeger KK. The development of cognitive inhibition: Theories, definitions, and research evidence. In: Dempster FN, Brainerd CJ, editors. Interference and inhibition in cognition. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1995. p. 175–204.
  60. 60. Nigg JT. On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychol Bull. 2000;126:220–246. pmid:10748641
  61. 61. Friedman NP, Miyake A. The Relations Among Inhibition and Interference Control Functions: A Latent-Variable Analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004;133(1):101–35. pmid:14979754
  62. 62. Collins T, Vergilino-Perez D, Delisle L, Dore-Mazars K. Visual Versus Motor Vector Inversions in the Antisaccade Task: A Behavioral Investigation With Saccadic Adaptation. J Neurophysiol. 2008 Feb 20;99(5):2708–18. pmid:18367698
  63. 63. Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict Monitoring and Cognitive Control. Psychol Rev. 2001;108(3):624–652. pmid:11488380
  64. 64. Nieuwenhuis S, Broerse A, Nielen MMA, Jong R de. A goal activation approach to the study of executive function: An application to antisaccade tasks. Brain Cogn. 2004 Nov;56(2):198–214. pmid:15518936
  65. 65. Watanabe M, Munoz DP. Neural correlates of conflict resolution between automatic and volitional actions by basal ganglia. Eur J Neurosci. 2009 Dec;30(11):2165–76. pmid:20128852
  66. 66. Koch I, Gade M, Schuch S, Philipp AM. The role of inhibition in task switching: A review. Psychon Bull Rev. 2010 Jan 15;17(1):1–14. pmid:20081154
  67. 67. Monsell S. Task switching. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003 Mar;7(3):134–40. pmid:12639695
  68. 68. Mueller SC, Swainson R, Jackson GM. ERP indices of persisting and current inhibitory control: A study of saccadic task switching. NeuroImage. 2009 Mar;45(1):191–7. pmid:19100841
  69. 69. Aron AR. From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(12):e55–e68. pmid:20932513
  70. 70. McDowell JE, Dyckman KA, Austin BP, Clementz BA. Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of reflexive and volitional saccades: Evidence from studies of humans. Brain Cogn. 2008 Dec;68(3):255–70. pmid:18835656
  71. 71. Paus T. Primate anterior cingulate cortex: where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2(6):417–424. pmid:11389475
  72. 72. Carter CS, van Veen V. Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: An update of theory and data. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7(4):367–79. pmid:18189010
  73. 73. van Veen V, Carter CS. The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies. Physiol Behav. 2002;77(4):477–482.
  74. 74. Donkers FCL, van Boxtel GJM. The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition. Brain Cogn. 2004;56(2):165–76. pmid:15518933
  75. 75. Huster RJ, Enriquez-Geppert S, Lavallee CF, Falkenstein M, Herrmann CS. Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: Functional networks and cognitive contributions. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013 Mar;87(3):217–33. pmid:22906815
  76. 76. Jonkman LM. The development of preparation, conflict monitoring and inhibition from early childhood to young adulthood; a Go/Nogo ERP study. Brain Res. 2006 Jun;1097(1):181–93. pmid:16729977
  77. 77. Folstein JR, Van Petten C. Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology. 2008;45:152–70. pmid:17850238
  78. 78. Chen A, Xu P, Wang Q, Luo Y, Yuan J, Yao D, et al. The timing of cognitive control in partially incongruent categorization. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29(9):1028–39. pmid:17894393
  79. 79. Jackson SR, Jackson GM, Roberts M. The selection and suppression of action: ERP correlates of executive control in humans. Neuroreport. 1999;10(4):861–5. pmid:10208561
  80. 80. Kousaie S, Phillips NA. Conflict monitoring and resolution: Are two languages better than one? Evidence from reaction time and event-related brain potentials. Brain Res. 2012 Mar;1446:71–90. pmid:22356886
  81. 81. Enriquez-Geppert S, Konrad C, Pantev C, Huster RJ. Conflict and inhibition differentially affect the N200/P300 complex in a combined go/nogo and stop-signal task. NeuroImage. 2010 Jun;51(2):877–87. pmid:20188191
  82. 82. Harper J, Malone SM, Bernat EM. Theta and delta band activity explain N2 and P3 ERP component activity in a go/no-go task. Clin Neurophysiol. 2014 Jan;125(1):124–32. pmid:23891195
  83. 83. Evdokimidis I, Liakopoulos D, Constantinidis TS, Papageorgiou C. Cortical potentials with antisaccades. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996;98:377–84. pmid:8647040
  84. 84. Everling S, Krappmann P, Flohr H. Cortical potentials preceding pro-and antisaccades in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1997;102(4):356–362. pmid:9146498
  85. 85. Cordones I, Gómez CM, Escudero M. Cortical Dynamics during the Preparation of Antisaccadic and Prosaccadic Eye Movements in Humans in a Gap Paradigm. Arias-Carrion O, editor. PLOS ONE. 2013 May 9;8(5):e63751. pmid:23671699
  86. 86. Engel AK, Fries P. Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010 Apr;20(2):156–65. pmid:20359884
  87. 87. Friston KJ. Causal modelling and brain connectivity in functional magnetic resonance imaging. PLOS Biol. 2009;7(2):220.
  88. 88. Stocco A, Yamasaki B, Natalenko R, Prat CS. Bilingual brain training: A neurobiological framework of how bilingual experience improves executive function. Int J Biling. 2014 Feb 1;18(1):67–92.
  89. 89. Crinion J, Turner R, Grogan A, Hanakawa T, Noppeney U, Devlin JT, et al. Language Control in the Bilingual Brain. Science. 2006 Jun 9;312(5779):1537–40. pmid:16763154
  90. 90. Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 2004;134(1):9–21. pmid:15102499
  91. 91. Greenhouse S, Geisser S. On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrica. 1959;24:95–112.
  92. 92. Keppel G. Design and analysis. 3rd ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1991.
  93. 93. Fecteau JH, Munoz DP. Exploring the consequences of the previous trial. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003 Jun;4(6):435–43. pmid:12778116
  94. 94. Aichert DS, Williams SCR, Möller H-J, Kumari V, Ettinger U. Functional neural correlates of psychometric schizotypy: An fMRI study of antisaccades. Psychophysiology. 2012 Mar;49(3):345–56. pmid:22091533
  95. 95. Alahyane N, Brien DC, Coe BC, Stroman PW, Munoz DP. Developmental improvements in voluntary control of behavior: Effect of preparation in the fronto-parietal network? NeuroImage. 2014 Sep;98:103–17. pmid:24642280
  96. 96. Ford KA. Neural Processes Associated With Antisaccade Task Performance Investigated With Event-Related fMRI. J Neurophysiol. 2005 Feb 23;94(1):429–40. pmid:15728770
  97. 97. Marshall TR, O’Shea J, Jensen O, Bergmann TO. Frontal Eye Fields Control Attentional Modulation of Alpha and Gamma Oscillations in Contralateral Occipitoparietal Cortex. J Neurosci. 2015 Jan 28;35(4):1638–47. pmid:25632139
  98. 98. Garrido MI, Kilner JM, Kiebel SJ, Stephan KE, Friston KJ. Dynamic causal modelling of evoked potentials: A reproducibility study. NeuroImage. 2007 Jul;36(3):571–80. pmid:17478106
  99. 99. Litvak V, Mattout J, Kiebel S, Phillips C, Henson R, Kilner J, et al. EEG and MEG Data Analysis in SPM8. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011;2011:1–32.
  100. 100. Makeig S. Auditory Event-Related Dynamics of the EEG Spectrum and Effects of Exposure to Tones. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1993;86:283–93. pmid:7682932
  101. 101. Bocquillon P, Bourriez J-L, Palmero-Soler E, Molaee-Ardekani B, Derambure P, Dujardin K. The spatiotemporal dynamics of early attention processes: A high-resolution electroencephalographic study of N2 subcomponent sources. Neuroscience. 2014 Jun;271:9–22. pmid:24747215
  102. 102. Falkenstein M, Hoormann J, Hohnsbein J. ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1999;101:267–91.
  103. 103. Kossowska M, Czarnek G, Wronka E, Wyczesany M, Bukowski M. Individual differences in epistemic motivation and brain conflict monitoring activity. Neurosci Lett. 2014 Jun;570:38–41. pmid:24735782
  104. 104. Paap KR, Sawi OM, Dalibar C, Darrow J, Johnson HA. Beyond Panglossian Optimism: Larger N2 Amplitudes Probably Signal a Bilingual Disadvantage in Conflict Monitoring. AIMS Neurosci. 2015;2(1):1–6.
  105. 105. Nieuwenhuis S, De Rover M. Event-Related Potentials. In: Stolerman IP, Price LH, editors. Encyclopedia of Psychopharmacology. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2014. p. 1–6.
  106. 106. Kutas M, Delong KA. A sampler of event-related brain potential (ERP) analyses of language processing. In: Brain research in language. Springer; 2008. p. 153–186.
  107. 107. Stephan KE, Penny WD, Moran RJ, den Ouden HEM, Daunizeau J, Friston KJ. Ten simple rules for dynamic causal modeling. NeuroImage. 2010 Feb;49(4):3099–109. pmid:19914382
  108. 108. Jahfari S, Verbruggen F, Frank MJ, Waldorp LJ, Colzato L, Ridderinkhof KR, et al. How Preparation Changes the Need for Top-Down Control of the Basal Ganglia When Inhibiting Premature Actions. J Neurosci. 2012 Aug 8;32(32):10870–8. pmid:22875921
  109. 109. Abutalebi J, Rosa PAD, Castro Gonzaga AK, Keim R, Costa A, Perani D. The role of the left putamen in multilingual language production. Brain Lang. 2013 Jun;125(3):307–15. pmid:22538086
  110. 110. Hamm JP, Sabatinelli D, Clementz BA. Alpha oscillations and the control of voluntary saccadic behavior. Exp Brain Res. 2012 Aug;221(2):123–8. pmid:22782481
  111. 111. Buchholz VN, Jensen O, Medendorp WP. Different roles of alpha and beta band oscillations in anticipatory sensorimotor gating. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8.
  112. 112. Friedman NP, Miyake A, Corley RP, Young SE, DeFries JC, Hewitt JK. Not All Executive Functions Are Related to Intelligence. Psychol Sci. 2006 Feb 1;17(2):172–9. pmid:16466426