Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Early Childhood Development and Schooling Attainment: Longitudinal Evidence from British, Finnish and Philippine Birth Cohorts

  • Evan D. Peet ,

    epeet@rand.org

    Affiliations Department of Global Health and Population, T. H. Chan School of Public Health, Harvard University, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America, RAND Corporation, 4570 Fifth Ave #600, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Dana C. McCoy ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Dana C. McCoy, Günther Fink

    Affiliation Graduate School of Education, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Goodarz Danaei ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Department of Global Health and Population, T. H. Chan School of Public Health, Harvard University, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Majid Ezzati ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation University of London Imperial College of Science, Technology & Medicine, Department Epidemiology & Biostatistics, MRC PHE Centre for Environment & Health, School of Public Health, London, United Kingdom

  • Wafaie Fawzi ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Department of Global Health and Population, T. H. Chan School of Public Health, Harvard University, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Marjo-Riitta Jarvelin ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliations University of London Imperial College of Science, Technology & Medicine, Department Epidemiology & Biostatistics, MRC PHE Centre for Environment & Health, School of Public Health, London, United Kingdom, University of Oulu, Institute of Health Sciences, Oulu, Finland, University of Oulu, Biocenter Oulu, Oulu, Finland, Oulu University Hospital, Unit of Primary Care, Oulu, Finland

  • Demetris Pillas,

    Affiliation University of London Imperial College of Science, Technology & Medicine, Department Epidemiology & Biostatistics, MRC PHE Centre for Environment & Health, School of Public Health, London, United Kingdom

  • Günther Fink

    Contributed equally to this work with: Dana C. McCoy, Günther Fink

    Affiliation Department of Global Health and Population, T. H. Chan School of Public Health, Harvard University, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

Abstract

Background

While recent literature has highlighted the importance of early childhood development for later life outcomes, comparatively little is known regarding the relative importance of early physical and cognitive development in predicting educational attainment cross-culturally.

Methods

We used prospective data from three birth cohorts: the Northern Finland Birth Cohort of 1986 (NFBC1986), the 1970 British Cohort Study (BCS1970), and the Cebu Longitudinal Health and Nutrition Survey of 1983 (CLHNS) to assess the association of height-for-age z-score (HAZ) and cognitive development measured prior to age 8 with schooling attainment. Multivariate linear regression models were used to estimate baseline and adjusted associations.

Results

Both physical and cognitive development were highly predictive of adult educational attainment conditional on parental characteristics. The largest positive associations between physical development and schooling were found in the CLHNS (β = 0.53, 95%-CI: [0.32, 0.74]) with substantially smaller associations in the BCS1970 (β = 0.10, 95% CI [0.04, 0.16]) and the NFBC1986 (β = 0.06, 95% CI [-0.05, 0.16]). Strong associations between cognitive development and educational attainment were found for all three cohorts (NFBC1986: β = 0.22, 95%-CI: [0.12, 0.31], BCS1970: β = 0.58, 95%-CI: [0.52, 0.64], CLHNS: β = 1.08, 95%-CI: [0.88, 1.27]). Models jointly estimating educational associations of physical and cognitive development demonstrated weaker associations for physical development and minimal changes for cognitive development.

Conclusion

The results indicate that although physical and cognitive early development are both important predictors of educational attainment, cognitive development appears to play a particularly important role. The large degree of heterogeneity in the observed effect sizes suggest that the importance of early life physical growth and cognitive development is highly dependent on socioeconomic and institutional contexts.

Introduction

Critical to labor market outcomes and quality of life, education is the single most important predictor of individual well-being and societal development [1,2]. From an individual or societal investment perspective, the returns to education are substantial—estimates for the labor market returns to each additional year of schooling typically vary between 6 and 12% [3,4]. Education also produces positive societal benefits through improvements in peer interactions [5], reductions in crime [6], and reductions in risky behaviors including drug use [7].

While a growing literature highlights the importance of early life conditions [8,9,10], understanding the relative contributions of multiple domains of early childhood development (ECD) to educational attainment is still limited. Early life nutritional, environmental, socio-economic, and other conditions are known to impact early physical development and predict educational attainment [10,11,12,13,14], but data measuring both early cognitive development and later-life educational attainment are scarce.

Most of the existing literature linking early life experiences to later life outcomes has relied on physical growth delays (stunting) to proxy for cognitive and other domains of ECD. Physical development measured by height predicts both concurrent and future welfare [15,16] through a direct effect and through associations with other domains of development. Physical and cognitive development are correlated and reflect the interaction between biology [17], and environmental investments [15,18,19,20] or insults [19,21]. While the direct impact of physical development on educational attainment has been assessed [22,23,24], the relationship between early physical and cognitive development remains unclear and the link between early cognitive development and schooling has not been quantified.

Evidence concerning the links between physical and cognitive development and schooling is scarce because of limited prospective data containing measures of ECD and years of schooling. In this paper, we used data from three of the most comprehensive studies containing measures of ECD and schooling in order to assess the relative importance of early physical and cognitive development for educational attainment.

Methods

Ethics Statement

The human subject data from the three cohorts of this study was analyzed anonymously.

Cohorts

We used data from the 1970 British Cohort Study (BCS1970), the 1983–84 Cebu Longitudinal Health and Nutrition Survey from the Philippines (CLHNS) and the 1985–86 Northern Finland Birth Cohort (NFBC1986). The BCS1970 is an ongoing longitudinal study of individuals born in England, Scotland or Wales between April 5th and April 11th, 1970 [25]. The NFBC1986 tracks mothers and their children living in Oulu and Lapland provinces who had expected dates of delivery between July 1st, 1985 and June 30th, 1986 [26]. The CLHNS randomly sampled 33 barangay, or neighborhoods, in Metro Cebu of the Philippines and women who gave birth between May 1, 1983, and April 30, 1984 composed the sample [27]. Table 1 presents additional information regarding each survey.

Physical development

Height, the most commonly used measure of physical development, was measured for all cohorts prior to age 5. In each cohort raw height measures were recorded in centimeters and have been converted to height-for-age z-score (HAZ) units by the most recent international reference curves produced by the World Health Organization (WHO) in 2007 and based on national cross-sectional anthropometric data from the US National Center for Health Statistics. For the NFBC1986 cohort, height was measured in 1988 and 1991 when the children were ages 2 and 5. Height was measured in 1972 on a subsample of children age 2 in the BCS1970 and in 1975 on the full sample of BCS1970 children age 5. In the CLHNS, height was measured in 1985–86 when the children were age 2. Previous evidence suggests that HAZ at age 2 is a preferable measure of early physical development because of the increasing variance in the reference group distribution by age [28,29]. Consequently, we used HAZ at age 2 when available (full samples of the NFBC1986 and CLHNS cohorts, and the 1972 subsample of the BCS1970 cohort) and supplemented with HAZ at age 5 when height is not observed at age 2 (1975 subsample of the BCS1970 cohort not observed at age 2).

Cognitive development

Cognitive development was measured differently in each cohort with measures for the present analysis selected for maximum comparability. The measures of cognition used in the analysis of each cohort capture similar dimensions of children’s non-verbal reasoning.

The measures of cognitive development in the NFBC1986 were produced by parental assessment of child’s spatial and temporal understanding obtained via questionnaire mailed in 1993 when children were 7–8 years old. Parents were asked to report their child’s understanding of spatial and temporal concepts at below, equal, or above average levels. This measure of cognition was chosen for comparability to other the measures of cognition available in the BCS1970 and CLHNS. Additionally, factor analysis of all reported measures indicated high loadings of reported spatial and temporal understanding on a general cognitive factor, which was distinct from factors represented by other measures.

For the BCS1970, tests of cognitive abilities were performed in 1975 on 5-year-old children. The Copying Designs test asked for two copies of eight designs [30]. The Human Figure Drawing test asked for ‘‘a picture of a man or lady.” When children finished, they were asked what the drawing was, what various parts of the drawing were and to label them. Subsequently, subjects drew another picture of the opposite sex as a measure of intellectual maturity [31]. The Profile test asked children to complete a profile of a human head and face. Both the Copying Design and Human Figure Drawing test were highly correlated. Factor analysis was performed to establish the presence of a general cognitive factor among the various measures. Amongst the three tests, the Copying Designs test loaded highest on general cognitive ability, followed by the Human Figure Drawing test and the Profile test. As a result of the correlations and factor analysis, our analysis used only the Copying Designs test. Furthermore, the Copying Designs test demonstrated the most conceptual similarity to the tests or reports of cognitive abilities available in the other cohorts.

CLHNS surveyors obtained only one measure of cognitive development at or prior to the age of 8: the Philippine Non-Verbal Intelligence test. The Philippine Non-Verbal Intelligence test assessed fluid ability (i.e., analytic or reasoning skills) and was adapted specifically for the CLHNS survey [32]. The test included 100 cards, each with five drawings of culturally appropriate objects including shapes, farm animals, and familiar activities, where one of the five objects differed in a meaningful way. Children were asked to identify the different object. No time limits were given and the difficulty increased as children advanced through the test.

For each cohort, the measures of cognitive development were standardized with the sample mean equal to 0 and the standard deviation equal to 1.

Educational Attainment

In each of the cohorts educational attainment was defined by the number of years of schooling to obtain the individual’s highest qualification. Different from the BCS1970 and CLHNS, educational qualification information in the NFBC1986 was obtained by linking a unique national identifier to the national education registry. For the 1985–86 NFBC1986 birth cohort, compulsory schooling began at age 7 and continued until age 16. The first qualification was potentially obtained at age 16 and subsequent qualifications resulted from three additional years of either vocational or upper secondary school attendance. Four years of tertiary education at either a university of applied science or a traditional university followed both vocational and upper secondary school. Post-graduate schooling and training for advanced degrees took place beginning at age 23, varying in length by degree. Given the educational system and individual information on the highest qualification, the years of completed education were derived.

For the BCS1970 cohort, qualifications were obtained through testing and completion of advanced degrees. At the time in the UK, primary school ran from ages 4 to 11, followed by secondary school until age 16. For the cohort born in 1970, education was compulsory until age 16 at which time the first qualification was potentially obtained: the Certificate of Secondary Education (CSE), or as it would later be known, the General Certificate of Secondary Education (GCSE), or the O-level. The Sixth Form level of education took place from ages 16 to 18 and prepared the children for the A-level exams. Between ages 18 and 22 the UK educational system divided into the vocational and collegiate tracks. Post-graduate schooling and training for advanced degrees took place beginning at age 23, varying in length by degree. Derivation of the years of education given the education system and available individual qualification information was determined through consultation with the Centre for Longitudinal Studies at the Institute of Education.

Children were asked their highest completed education during each wave of the CLHNS. In the 2002 and 2005 surveys the responses ranged from no school to 5 years of college. In subsequent tracking surveys, the responses included post-graduate years of education. Because of either temporary or permanent attrition, not every individual was observed in each survey between 2002 and 2009. Consequently, the years of education variable was derived from the maximum of the observed years of education between the years 2002 and 2009.

Potential Confounders

Covariates in the analysis were chosen and included for consistency across cohorts and to represent spatial, socioeconomic, and biological influences. Spatial indicators representing regions were only included for the BCS1970 and NFBC1986. While the CLHNS sampled from one metropolitan area, the BCS1970 and the NFBC1986 contained observations from multiple regions throughout each nation. 11 indicators for the BCS1970 represented London, Scotland, York and others, while 2 indicators for the NFBC1986 represented Lapland and Oulu.

Socioeconomic measures of each household at baseline included mothers’ and fathers’ highest educational grade attained, as well as social class indicators based on fathers’ occupation. Similar to the child’s years of education, parental years of education was derived from the highest reported qualification. Social class was divided into 6 categories and derived from reported main occupation in each of the three surveys. The 6 categories are: professional/manager, non-manual skilled laborer, manual skilled laborer, semi-skilled manual laborer, unskilled manual laborer, and other (which included the unemployed).

Biological factors in physical and cognitive development included individual and family level indicators. At the individual level, an indicator of child low birth weight (weight at birth less than 2500 grams) was included. At the family level, mother’s height (in centimeters), the number of previous pregnancies the mother has had, the mother’s age at childbirth, and the mother’s smoking behavior during pregnancy (binary indicator equal to 1 if the mother smoked at all during pregnancy) were the remaining biological covariates.

Statistical Analyses

The conceptual model in Fig 1 displays later life outcomes including educational attainment as both directly impacted by early life conditions and indirectly affected through the impact of early life conditions on the domains of ECD. The analysis first estimated the association between physical development (HAZ) and later life completed years of schooling. Second, the association between cognitive development (standardized measure of cognition specific to each cohort) and later life completed years of schooling was estimated. Third, joint associations between physical and cognitive development and educational attainment were estimated for each of the cohorts. For each analysis and cohort, three specifications—baseline, minimal adjustment, full adjustment—were employed in order to examine the sensitivity of the estimates. The baseline specification included a gender indicator and regional fixed effects. The minimally adjusted specification added socioeconomic indicators—parental education and social class indicators. The fully adjusted specification added biological measures—child low birth weight indicator, mother’s height, mother’s number of previous pregnancies, mother’s age at childbirth, and mother’s smoking behavior during pregnancy. Consequently the estimation draws on comparisons between children of similar biological characteristics.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Conceptual Model.

The model links early life conditions, development and later life outcomes. The analysis focuses on the bolded lines, the links between physical and cognitive development and later life educational attainment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137219.g001

Furthermore, in order to assess the shape of the relationship between physical and cognitive development and years of schooling, we fitted and graphed non-parametric local polynomial models separately for each cohort. All estimates and graphs were produced using STATA version 13 (College Station, TX, USA).

Results

In Table 2 we provide summary statistics of the dependent variable (years of education by highest grade attained), independent variables of interest (height-for-age z-score between ages 2–5 and standardized cognitive test score between ages 5–8), and each potential confounder. The distributions of HAZ among the NFBC1986 and BCS1970 cohorts were relatively similar while the distribution of HAZ in the CLHNS was skewed downward. Reported smoking during pregnancy was more prevalent among mothers in the BCS1970 and NFBC1986 cohorts than the CLHNS. The incidence of mothers and fathers obtaining more than secondary levels of education was much more common in the NFBC1986 than the BCS1970 and CLHNS. Similar to the cohort children, the distribution of mother’s height was skewed downwards in the CLHNS cohort. The number of prior pregnancies was much higher in the CLHNS than the NFBC1986 and BCS1970 and there were more mothers under 20 in the CLHNS.

In Table 3 the dependent variable is years of schooling defined by highest educational attainment and the independent variable of interest is the height-for-age z-score. In regards to the NFBC1986 cohort (top panel), a 1 standard deviation increase in HAZ between ages 2 and 5 was associated with an additional 0.12 years of schooling in unadjusted (baseline) model. Including socioeconomic confounders diminished the association marginally to 0.11. However, when biological confounders were included the association was reduced and a 1 standard deviation increase in HAZ related to an additional .06 years of school (the 95% confidence interval includes 0). The association was larger in the BCS1970 cohort (middle panel). In the baseline specification, a 1 standard deviation increase in HAZ was associated with .29 additional years of schooling, which was reduced to .18 additional years of schooling when socioeconomic confounders were included in the specification. Fully adjusted for biological confounders, the association was .10. Of the three cohorts, the largest association between physical development and educational attainment was observed in the CLHNS cohort (bottom panel). The baseline specification yielded an association of 1.027, while controlling for socioeconomic confounders reduced the association to .55. In the fully adjusted model, a 1 standard deviation increase in HAZ was associated with an additional .53 years of schooling.

thumbnail
Table 3. Physical Early Life Development and Educational Attainment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137219.t003

Table 4 presents the associations between cognitive development and educational attainment for each cohort. In the NFBC1986 cohort data (top panel), the baseline specification demonstrated that a 1 standard deviation increase in cognitive development score was associated with an additional .26 years of schooling. Including socioeconomic confounders reduced the association to .23, and including biological confounders further reduced the association to .22. The association was substantially larger in the BCS1970 cohort (middle panel); a 1 standard deviation increase in cognitive development score was associated with an additional .87 years of schooling in the baseline specification, .63 in the minimally adjusted specification, and .58 in the fully adjusted specification. However, the largest association was observed in the CLHNS cohort (bottom panel). In the CLHNS, a 1 standard deviation increase in cognitive development score was associated with an additional 1.58 years of schooling in the baseline specification, 1.10 in the minimally adjusted specification, and 1.08 in the fully adjusted specification.

thumbnail
Table 4. Cognitive Early Life Development and Educational Attainment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137219.t004

Table 5 displays the jointly estimated associations between physical and cognitive development and later life educational attainment for each cohort. Focusing on the fully adjusted specifications for each cohort, a 1 standard deviation increase in HAZ in the NFBC1986 cohort (top panel) was associated with .05 additional years of schooling and a 1 standard deviation increase in cognitive development score was associated with an additional .21 years of educational attainment. Neither of these jointly estimated associations were substantially different from separately estimated associations displayed in Tables 3 and 4 (HAZ: .06, cognitive development: .22). In the BCS1970 cohort (middle panel) only the HAZ-schooling association was marginally different when jointly estimated. Jointly estimated, a 1 standard deviation increase in HAZ was associated with an additional .08 years of schooling, in contrast to the separately estimated association of .11 (a 25% reduction). A 1 standard deviation increase in cognitive development score was associated with an additional .58 years of schooling, similar to the separately estimated association. The reduction in the HAZ-schooling association was similar (30%) in the CLHNS when jointly estimated. In the CLHNS (bottom panel), a 1 standard deviation increase in HAZ was associated with .37 additional years of schooling—reduced from .53 when separately estimated. However, as in the other cohorts, the separately estimated cognitive development-schooling association was similar to the separately estimated association in the CLHNS: a 1 standard deviation increase in cognitive development score was associated with an additional 1.02 years of schooling.

thumbnail
Table 5. Physical and Cognitive Early Life Development and Educational Attainment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137219.t005

The shapes of the associations between physical and cognitive development and years of schooling in each cohort are displayed in Figs 24. Fig 2 shows the relationship between HAZ and cognitive development in each cohort. The relationships were linear in each cohort with similar slopes in the CLHNS and BCS1970 and a slope near to zero in the NFBC1986. The significant difference between the three cohorts was in level of HAZ; the highest level of HAZ was observed in the NFBC1986, next in the BCS1970, and the lowest in the CLHNS. Fig 3 shows the relationship between HAZ and years of schooling in each cohort. Generally, the relationships in each cohort were linear across the HAZ distribution. The NFBC1986 demonstrated a potentially non-linear relationship with a greater slope at lower levels of the HAZ distribution; however estimation of the relationship by inclusion of higher order polynomials in the multivariate regression did not demonstrate a statistically significant non-linear relationship. Across cohorts, the most significant difference appeared between the slope of the CLHNS and the NFBC1986/BCS1970. The slope of the relationship was much larger in the CLHNS than in both the NFBC1986 and the BCS1970. Fig 4 shows the relationship between cognitive development and years of schooling in each cohort. While slightly logarithmic in the CLHNS, the relationship was generally linear. Again, the most significant difference appeared between the slope of the CLHNS and the NFBC1986/BCS1970. For values of cognitive development below zero (i.e., below the sample mean), the slope of the relationship was much larger in the CLHNS than in both the NFBC1986 and the BCS1970.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Height for age z-score and early life cognitive development.

The shape of the relationship between height for age z-score and early life cognitive development in each of the 3 cohorts, including 95% confidence intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137219.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Height for age z-score and educational attainment.

The shape of the relationship between height for age z-score and educational attainment in each of the 3 cohorts, including 95% confidence intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137219.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Early life cognitive development and educational attainment.

The shape of the relationship between early life cognitive development and educational attainment in each of the 3 cohorts, including 95% confidence intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137219.g004

Discussion

The analyses presented in this paper have yielded four main results. First, both physical and cognitive development predicted later life educational attainment in each cohort, with the strongest associations for both factors in the CLHNS. Second, in each cohort and across all specifications, the associations between cognition and schooling were stronger than the associations between schooling and physical development. Third, jointly estimating the physical development-schooling and cognitive development-schooling associations did not alter the cognitive development-schooling association but did diminish the physical development-schooling association in two of the three cohorts. Last, the strength of the associations was heterogeneous across contexts, with the strongest associations observed in the CLHNS and weakest in the NFBC1986.

Overall, the results indicated that physical and cognitive development each separately contributes to educational attainment. Given the generally high correlation between the two domains of ECD previous studies focusing only on the link between physical development and schooling have likely overstated the importance of physical development for educational attainment.10-14 Furthermore, because we found substantial heterogeneity between cohorts, caution is required in generalizing this relationship cross-contextually. While cognitive development consistently demonstrated larger associations with educational attainment in each cohort, the difference between the cognitive development-schooling association and the physical development-schooling association varied widely by context.

Economic differences were likely drivers of observed contextual heterogeneity. The three cohorts represented different levels of economic development as described by per capita gross domestic product (GDP). Fig 5 displays the log of per capita GDP for the countries of each cohort between 1960 and 2013 using data obtained from the World Bank World Development Indicators. While the time periods of each study differ, they identify three distinct levels of economic development. The Philippines in 1983–84 represented the lowest level of economic development with a per capita GDP of $646 (in 2013 USD), the United Kingdom in 1970 the middle level with a per capita GDP of $2,242 (250% more than the Philippines in 1983), and Finland in 1985–86 the highest level of development with a per capita GDP of $14,705 (550% more than the UK in 1970). The observed magnitudes of the physical development-schooling association mirrored the national economic development: the largest associations—and, consequently, returns to investment—are observed in the least economically developed context, and the smallest associations in the most developed.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Log of GDP per capita of countries represented by each cohort study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137219.g005

While economic conditions seem related to the associations found across cohorts, both temporal and institutional influences are likely also key factors in explaining the observed differences. The heterogeneity across contexts indicates that the effects of physical growth and cognitive development on educational attainment are likely modified by economic conditions and educational systems. Confirming previous studies of health and nutritional investments in developing contexts, the patterns of associations across cohorts suggest greater returns on investments made to early physical development in the lowest income settings and that returns diminish as income and economic development increases [33,34]. It is also possible that improvements in physical growth are particularly important in settings like the Philippines, where a large fraction of children fall behind their expected age-specific trajectories.

Contextual patterns in the observed relationship between cognitive development and schooling are similarly associated with economic development and stem from nutritional, stimulatory, and other types of deprivation common in low income settings [35]. Consequently, benefits to investments such as early educational programs or home visits aimed at cognitive skill development may differ across contexts. While 1986 Finnish GDP per capita in 1986 exceeded 1970 British GDP per capita in by 550%, if, as has been postulated, both nations were sufficiently developed to surpass a developmental threshold where adequate nutrition and infection control exist [36], temporal and institutional differences may underlie observed differences at higher income levels. As seen in Fig 5, the per capita GDP of the UK and Finland are very similar in each year. While 1970 UK is less economically developed than 1986 Finland, 1970 UK is also less developed medically, scientifically, and in other ways that may impact the translation of early physical and cognitive development to educational attainment. Additionally, institutional differences factor in the differences between the BCS1970 and the NFBC1986. The UK educational system is merit based while the Finnish educational system downplays early development and provides a flexible learning environment for children to succeed at varying rates [37]. Perhaps as a result we observe less divergence in the schooling outcomes of children with high and low early cognitive development in the NFBC1986 than we do in the BCS1970.

Despite the strengths of this study, the conclusions both within and across cohorts are limited for a variety of reasons. First, in order to estimate identical models for each cohort, the set of confounders is limited and may result in confounding from unobserved or excluded information on parental investments, environmental conditions and other characteristics. To examine this possibility we performed additional estimations which included confounders such as parent’s marital status, number of antenatal visits to healthcare provider, gestational length, mother’s employment status, and delivery complications which are available in the BCS1970 and CLHNS. The inclusion of each of these confounders did not significantly alter the associations observed in the BCS1970 and CLHNS cohorts suggesting that the fully adjusted model may have adequately captured at least some unobserved heterogeneity associated with schooling.

The second limit to the study’s conclusions is that height and cognition are not measured at the same point in time for each of the cohorts. Height at age 2 is measured for the full NFBC1986 and CLHNS cohorts and a subsample of the BCS1970 cohort, while the full BCS1970 sample is observed at age 5. The same analysis has also been performed using HAZ at age 5, and average HAZ between ages 2 and 5 and the results do not substantially differ. Cognition is measured at age 5 in the BCS1970, and between ages 7 and 8 in both the NFBC1986 and CLHNS. It is possible that early preschool or school exposure may have affected these scores in the NFBC1986 and CLHNS cohorts. However, it is not clear whether early schooling increases or decreases the cognitive gap between children; if schooling allows less developed children to catch up, the later measures would underestimate early differentials; if schooling increases the gap by focusing on the most talented students, the opposite may be true.

Another limitation of the study is that the measures of cognition likely contain error and may not be completely comparable. Where available, alternative specifications of cognitive development have been assessed and demonstrate strong similarity to the results presented. However, error is particularly salient in the NFBC1986 measure of cognition derived from parental reports of their child’s understanding of spatial and temporal concepts. And despite our efforts to utilize the most comparable measures, cognition is measured differently in each cohort, making the levels incomparable across cohorts and prevented us from pooling the three cohorts in order to estimate an overall association between cognitive development and schooling. Fourth, the number and representativeness of the cohorts are limited. We are unaware of additional prospective cohorts containing the requisite measures; we generated only three points along the potentially non-linear ECD-schooling relationships implying that the results should not be generalized more broadly.

An additional limitation to the study’s conclusions is that while the baseline samples are representative of most of the UK, the two northernmost Finnish provinces, and one Philippine metropolitan area, each cohort contains a significant amount of attrition which may be related to early physical and cognitive development. While unassociated with observed baseline characteristics in the NFBC1986 [37], attrition in the BCS1970 was selective on socioeconomic status (specifically father’s social class by occupation) [38] and attrition in the CLHNS was selective on socioeconomic status (both parental education and father’s social class by occupation) [39]. However, these differences are relatively minor; for instance, the proportion of mothers with primary or less education in the original (full) CLHNS sample differs by less than 4 percentage points from the proportion observed in the sample of children followed up; for mother’s with secondary education the difference is 1 percentage point. While attrition does not appear to play a key role in biasing the sample based on observed characteristics, the degree to which the baseline and analytic samples differed on unobserved characteristics remains unknown.

In spite of these limitations, the results of this study provide insight into the relationships between physical development, cognitive development, and educational attainment across multiple contexts spanning different institutions and levels of economic development. Overall, the results indicate that both physical and cognitive development are separately and jointly important for children’s subsequent educational attainment, with cognitive development playing a particularly strong role. Previous studies likely overstate the importance of physical development due to correlation with cognitive development, which is consistently a stronger determinant of educational attainment. The heterogeneity in the associations across contexts reflects both economic and institutional conditions.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the advice and suggestions of the remainder of our research team under the Saving Brains project. Also, we acknowledge the contribution of the team of researchers who collected and worked on the Northern Finland Birth Cohort study and their collaboration with this study. Specifically, we thank Paula Rantakallio, Outi Tornwall, Minttu Jussila, and acknowledge the contribution of the late Leena Peltonen.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: EDP DCM GD ME WF GF. Analyzed the data: EDP DCM. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MRJ DP. Wrote the paper: EDP DCM GF.

References

  1. 1. Banerjee AV, Benabou R, Mookherjee D. Understanding Poverty. Oxford University Press. 2006.
  2. 2. Stein AD, Behrman JR, DiGirolamo A, Martorell R, Quisumbing A, Ramakrishnan U. Schooling, educational achievement, and cognitive functioning among young Guatemalan adults. Food and Nutrition Bulletin. 2005;26:46–54.
  3. 3. Card D. Estimating the Return to Schooling: Progress on Some Persistent Econometric Problems. Econometrica, 2001;69:1127–1160.
  4. 4. Psacharopoulos G, Patrinos HA. Returns to Investment in Education: A Further Update. Education Economics, 2002;12:111–134.
  5. 5. Moretti E. Estimating the Social Return to Higher Education: Evidence From Longitudinal and Repeated Cross-Sectional Data. Journal of Econometrics, 2004;121:175–212.
  6. 6. Lochner L, Moretti E. The Effect of Education on Crime: Evidence from Prison Inmates, Arrests, and Self-Reports. American Economic Review, 2003;94:155–189.
  7. 7. Heckman J, Stixrud J, Urzua S. The Effects Of Cognitive and Noncognitive Abilities On Labor Market Outcomes and Social Behavior. Journal of Labor Economics, 2006;24: 411–482.
  8. 8. Cunha F, Heckman J, Schennach SM. Estimating the Technology of Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Skill Formation. Econometrica, 2010;78:883–931. pmid:20563300
  9. 9. Hertzman C, Boyce T. How experience gets under the skin to create gradients in developmental health. Annual Review of Public Health, 2010;31:329–347. pmid:20070189
  10. 10. Grantham-McGregor SM, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Richter L, Strupp B, et al. Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet, 2007;369:60–70. pmid:17208643
  11. 11. Leung JTY, Shek DTL. Poverty and adolescent developmental outcomes: a critical review. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 2011;23:109–114. pmid:21870672
  12. 12. Nores M, Barnett WS. Benefits of early childhood interventions across the world: (Under) Investing in the very young. Economics of Education Review, 2010;29:271–282.
  13. 13. Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, Grantham-McGregor SM. Effects of early childhood psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on cognition and education in growth-stunted Jamaican children: prospective cohort study. Lancet, 2005;366:1804–1807. pmid:16298218
  14. 14. Anderson LM, Shinn C, Fullilove MT, Scrimshaw SC, Fielding JE, Normand J, et al. The Effectiveness of Early Childhood Development Programs: A Systematic Review. American Journal of Preventative Medicine, 2003;24:32–46.
  15. 15. Case A, Paxson C. Stature and Status: Height, Ability, and Labor Market Outcomes. Journal of Political Economy, 2006;116:499–532.
  16. 16. Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, Simonoff E, Grantham-McGregor SM. Early Childhood Stunting Is Associated with Poor Psychological Functioning in Late Adolescence and Effects Are Reduced by Psychosocial Stimulation. The Journal of Nutrition, 2007;137:2464–2469. pmid:17951486
  17. 17. Hertzman C. The biological embedding of early experience and its effects on health in adulthood. Annals of New York Academy of Science, 1999;896:85–95.
  18. 18. Grantham-McGregor SM. Linear Growth Retardation and Cognition. Lancet, 2002;359:542. pmid:11867104
  19. 19. Crimmins EM, Finch CE. Infection, Inflammation, Height and Longevity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2006;103:498–503.
  20. 20. Lynn R. A Nutrition Theory of the Secular Increases in Intelligence, Positive Correlation between Height, Head Size and IQ. British Journal of Education Psychology, 1989;59:372–377.
  21. 21. Ekdahl CT, Claasen JH, Bonde S, Kokaia Z, Lindvall O. Inflammation Is Detrimental for Neurogenesis in Adult Brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2003;100:13632–13637.
  22. 22. Daniels MC, Adair LS. Growth in Young Filipino Children Predicts School Trajectories through High School. The Journal of Nutrition, 2004;134:1439–1446. pmid:15173409
  23. 23. Glewwe P, Jacoby HG, King EM. Early childhood nutrition and academic achievement: a longitudinal analysis. Journal of Public Economics, 2001;81:345–368.
  24. 24. Moock PR, Leslie J. Childhood Malnutrition and Schooling in the Terai Region of Nepal. Journal of Development Economics, 1986;20:33–52.
  25. 25. Centre for Longitudinal Studies (CLS). 1970 British Cohort Study. 2014. Accessed 02 February 2015. Available: http://www.cls.ioe.ac.uk/page.aspx?&sitesectionid=795&sitesectiontitle=Welcome+to+the+1970+British+Cohort+Study+(BCS197070)
  26. 26. Jarvelin M, Elliot P, Kleinschmidt I, Martuzzi M, Grundy C, Hartikainen AL, et al. Ecological and individual predictors of birthweight in a northern Finland birth cohort 1986. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 1997;11:298–317. pmid:9246691
  27. 27. Feranil A, Gultiano S, Adair LS. The Cebu Longitudinal Health and Nutrition Survey: Two Decades Later. Asia-Pacific Population Journal, 2008;23(3):39–54.
  28. 28. Leroy JL, Ruel M, Habicht JP. Critical windows for nutritional interventions against stunting: Response. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 2013;98:854–858. pmid:23964056
  29. 29. Lundeen EA, Stein AD, Adair LS, Behrman JR, Bhargava SK, Dearden KA, et al. Height-for-age z scores increase despite increasing height deficits among children in 5 developing countries. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 2014;100:821–825. pmid:25008854
  30. 30. Davie R, Butler NR, Goldstein H. From Birth to Seven. London, Longmans, 1972.
  31. 31. Harris D. Children’s drawings as measures of intellectual maturity. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1963.
  32. 32. Guthrie GM, Tayag A, Jimenez JP. The Philippine Non-Verbal Intelligence Test. Journal of Social Psychology, 1977;102:3–11.
  33. 33. Strauss J, Thomas D. Health, Nutrition and Economic Development. Journal of Economic Literature, 1998;36:766–817.
  34. 34. Komlos J. Stature, Living Standards, and Economic Development: Essays in Anthropometric History. University of Chicago Press, 1994.
  35. 35. Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK. Economic Deprivation and Early Childhood Development. Child Development, 1994;65:296–318. pmid:7516849
  36. 36. Vulliamy G, Nikki ML. The Comparative Context for Educational Reform in England and Finland. Symposium on a Comparative Analysis of Curriculum Change in English and Finnish Primary Schools: the York-Finnish Project, 1997.
  37. 37. Pirkola J, Pouta A, Bloigu A, Hartikainen AL, Laitinen J, Jarvelin MR, et al. Risks of Overweight and Abdominal Obesity at Age 16 Years Associated With Prenatal Exposures to Maternal Pre-pregnancy Overweight and Gestational Diabetes Mellitus. Diabetes Care, 2010;33:1115–1121. pmid:20427685
  38. 38. Mostafa T, Wiggins RD. Handling attrition and non-response in the 1970 British Cohort Study. Centre for Longitudinal Studies Working Paper, 2014.
  39. 39. Adair LS, Popkin BM, Akin JS, Guilkey DK, Gultiano S, Borja J, et al. Cohort Profile: The Cebu Longitudinal Health and Nutrition Survey. International Journal of Epidemiology, 2010;20:619–625.