Figures
Abstract
A basic premise in behavioural ecology is the cost-benefit arithmetic, which determines both behavioural decisions and evolutionary processes. Aggressive interactions can be costly on an energetic level, demanding increased energy or causing injuries, and on a psychological level, in the form of increased anxiety and damaged relationships between opponents. Here we used urinary glucocorticoid (uGC) levels to assess the costs of aggression in wild chimpanzees of Budongo Forest, Uganda. We collected 169 urine samples from nine adult male chimpanzees following 14 aggressive interactions (test condition) and 10 resting events (control condition). Subjects showed significantly higher uGC levels after single aggressive interactions compared to control conditions, likely for aggressors as well as victims. Higher ranking males had greater increases of uGC levels after aggression than lower ranking males. In contrast, uGC levels showed no significant change in relation to aggression length or intensity, indicating that psychological factors might have played a larger role than mere energetic expenditure. We concluded that aggressive behaviour is costly for both aggressors and victims and that costs seem poorly explained by energetic demands of the interaction. Our findings are relevant for studies of post-conflict interactions, since we provide evidence that both aggressors and victims experience a stress response to conflict.
Citation: Wittig RM, Crockford C, Weltring A, Deschner T, Zuberbühler K (2015) Single Aggressive Interactions Increase Urinary Glucocorticoid Levels in Wild Male Chimpanzees. PLoS ONE 10(2): e0118695. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118695
Academic Editor: Allan Siegel, University of Medicine & Dentistry of NJ - New Jersey Medical School, UNITED STATES
Received: August 28, 2014; Accepted: January 23, 2015; Published: February 25, 2015
Copyright: © 2015 Wittig et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited
Data Availability: Table 1 shows the data set that provides the underlying numerical values behind any figures, tables, graphs, and statistical analyses that are necessary to replicate the findings in the study. For further questions contact Roman Wittig at wittig@eva.mpg.de.
Funding: Financial support was provided by British Academy [http://www.britac.ac.uk/] (CC), Leakey Foundation [http://www.leakeyfoundation.org/] (CC, RMW, TD), Leverhulm Trust [http://www.leverhulme.ac.uk/] (KZ), Max Planck Society [http://www.eva.mpg.de/] (AW, CC, RMW, TD) and the Royal Zoological Society of Scotland [http://www.rzss.org.uk/], in providing core funding for Budongo Conservation Field Station. This project has received additional funding from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Program for research, technological development and demonstration [http://ec.europa.eu/research/fp7/index_en.cfm] under grant agreement no 283871. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
A central principle in behavioural ecology is that the function and evolution of behaviour is analysed in terms of economic logic as costs and benefits, which determines an individual’s fitness [1, 2]. Cost-benefit analyses are regularly used to predict how animals should behave to maximize their net fitness gains, including during fights [3, 4]. Despite its importance, it has been surprisingly difficult to quantify the true costs and benefits of a behaviour. One approach is to look at physiological variables, such as hormones secreted in response to stressful events. Although the stress response is generally adaptive, high glucocorticoid (GC) levels are thought to be costly for animals, so measuring their prevalence in wild populations is of importance [5, 6]. GC release, whether it is part of the stress response to energetic stressors [7, 8] or to psychological stressors [9], is costly due to the subsequent release of energy reserves, otherwise needed to support reproduction and growth [10]. Additionally, a range of costly and negative health effects, including impaired immune and cognitive functions, have been linked to chronic release of GCs [11, 12]. Overall this suggests, that GC levels are a reliable proxy for costs, whether the release of GC is caused by energy demands [13–14] or by psychological effects, such as anxiety, uncertainty or relationship damage [15–19], (for review: [6]).
The relationship between high GC levels and aggressive behaviour has been established in a wide range of taxa (fish: Oncorhynchus mykiss [7]; reptiles: Urosaurus ornatus [20]; birds: Parus major [21]; mammals: Canis lupus [16], Cavia porcellus [22], Helogale parvula [16]; primates: Macaca assamensis [23], M. sylvanus [24], Pan troglodytes [14, 17], Homo sapiens [8, 25]). In most cases, these studies correlated aggression rates and GC levels over long sampling periods, making statements about causality difficult. Exceptions include several studies that have shown an increase of GC levels in plasma following prolonged aggressive interactions (fights over dominance in fish: [7]; Judo or wrestling matches in humans: [8, 25]). One way to determine causality between aggressive behaviour and excretion of GCs is to study the hormonal effect of single events of aggression under natural conditions.
In many non-human primates there is an inverse linear relationship between dominance rank and aggression received [26], resulting in more incidences of aggression towards lower ranking individuals. Consequently, studies have focused on the relation between GC levels and received aggression [23, 24]. However, other studies in humans [8, 25], rodents [22] and fish [7] have examined plasma cortisol increases in both winners and losers of aggressive interactions. In chimpanzees, however, aggressors do not always win [3], so GC levels might increase in both aggressors and victims (receivers) of aggression.
Giving or receiving of aggression affects the subsequent behaviour of opponents. Prior aggression sometimes attracts further aggression [27] and former opponents seem to avoid each other, if the aggression stays unreconciled [28]. These effects have been attributed to post-conflict anxiety and damaged relationships, both psychological stressors of aggressive interactions. Even though the stress response is likely caused by the resulting uncertainty, any release of GCs will still incur metabolic costs to the individual.
Here, we investigated whether single events of aggression in male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) caused increased urinary glucocorticoid (uGC) levels in victims and aggressors. We compared uGC levels related to aggression with uGC levels of samples related to pre-aggression periods. We also investigated the effect of duration and intensity of aggression on uGC levels, since fighting duration is correlated with energy use, and contact aggression can more easily lead to injuries than non-contact aggression. Finally we investigated the effects of dominance rank in relation to aggression. Given evidence of reproductive skew in male chimpanzees with dominant males siring more offspring [29, 30], high ranking males arguably have more to lose than lower ranking males.
Material and Methods
Study site and data collection
We observed nine adult male chimpanzees (P. t. schweinfurthii, Table 1) of the Sonso community living in the Budongo Forest (1°35’ - 1°55’ N, 31°08’ - 31°42’ E), Uganda, between February 2008 and July 2010. The Sonso community has been observed continuously since 1990 [31] and was comprised of 15 males (≥15 years: 10; 10–14 years: 5), 35 females (≥14 years: 27; 10–13 years: 8) and 28 juvenile and infants during the study period. Only one adult male was excluded from the study, due to not being sufficiently habituated to tolerate 6 hr focal follows. Budongo forest is a moist, semi-deciduous tropical rain forest with an average altitude of 1100m and a mean yearly rainfall of 1600mm [31].
With a team of up to six observers, we followed up to three parties of chimpanzees (independently moving subgroups) from approximately 7 a.m. to 5 p.m. through the forest, recording the party behaviour using all occurrence sampling [32] for aggression, grooming and affiliative social interactions. We waited for one of two target behaviours to occur: (1) aggressive interactions, in which one individual (aggressor) attacked another group member (victim) using either contact (hits, bites or tramples) or non-contact aggression (displays, charges or chases); (2) resting, in which one individual had no social interaction for a minimum of one hour and was sitting or lying for at least the first 30 minutes. Aggression events were measured in minutes. After observing a male engaging in one of the target behaviours, we switched to focal animal sampling [32] of that individual for the next 6 hours, collecting every possible urine sample and recording each change in behaviour. Urine was pipetted from plastic bags, when subjects were sitting > 10m high in the tree or from leaf matter when urination occurred on the ground after subjects had moved away. After collecting, urine samples were stored in a thermos flask containing ice and frozen upon arrival in camp, which was within 10 hours after collection. Urine collection did not commence when subjects had engaged in aggression or grooming within the hour prior to the target behaviour, and was aborted when subjects engaged in additional aggression or grooming within two hours after the target behaviour. We collected a total of 169 urine samples (aggression context N = 94, resting context N = 75) from nine adult male chimpanzees following 14 aggression events (9 contact and 5 non-contact) and 10 resting events, with a mean of 7.04 urine samples collected per chimpanzee per target behaviour.
Hormone analysis
Urinary GC levels were measured at the Lab for field-endocrinology at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology using high-performance liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS), applying a method that measures 23 endogenous steroids in small quantities of primate urine [33]. Samples with a recovery of the internal standard deviating by less than +/- 50% from the expected value were included in the analysis. In case of large deviation we re-measured the samples. If a large deviation persisted, we re-extracted and re-measured the samples. We excluded samples where the large deviation still persisted. Examination of LC-MS/MS data was carried out with MassLynx (version 4.1; QuanLynx-Software). Only a fraction of plasma cortisol can be found in chimpanzee urine [34], while metabolites of cortisol are found in higher quantities [33]. To quantify the urinary glucocorticoid excretion (uGC), we used the sum of urinary cortisol plus four of its metabolites (tetrahydrocortisol, tetrahydrocortisone, 5β-androstane and 11-oxoetiocholanolon). The sum of uGC comprised on average 9% cortisol, 37% tetrahydrocortisol, 35% tetrahydrocortison, 5% 5β-androstane and 14% 11- oxoetiocholanolon. We corrected the uGC levels with the creatinine levels of each sample to control for differences in water content of urine samples [35]. We excluded samples with a creatinine level of less than 0.05 mg creatinine / ml urine from the analysis.
Hormonal and behavioural data analysis
The adult males observed during our study had an average urination interval ± SD = 78 min ± 32. We defined GC clearance in urine of chimpanzees following the results of [34]. Using 3H-labeled cortisol, the peak recovery of 3H-labeled cortisol metabolites in chimpanzee urine was between ∼2 and 4.8 hours after administration [34]. Although some cortisol was most likely secreted into urine earlier, the highest level of radioactivity recovered was found in the second urine sample at 4.8 h after injection. Based on these finding and due to the variation in the urination interval, we set the window for peak GC clearance in chimpanzee urine to 135–270 min after onset of the target behaviour. Due to diurnal decline in uGC levels [36], absolute uGC levels were not comparable. Thus, we calculated a relative uGC level where we divided the uGC level of samples related to the target behaviour (peak-period samples) through the uGC levels of samples collected in the pre-target behaviour times window (pre-period samples). The pre-period urine samples were excreted between 0 min (t0) and 135 min (t1) after the start of the target behaviour, representing the peak uGC secretion of behaviour preceding the aggression or the resting. The peak-period urine samples were excreted between 135 min after the start (t1) and 270 min after the end of the target behaviour (t2 = 270+d min, with d = duration of target behaviour; Fig. 1), representing the peak uGC secretion of the aggression or resting event. We calculated the relative uGC level for each event, representing the percentage of how high the uGC level was during the peak-period in comparison to the uGC level during the pre-period. Therefore, we divided the mean hormone level of peak-period samples through the hormone level of the pre-period samples: relative uGC level = (mean uGC peak period / mean uGC pre period) x 100 for each subject for each series of urine samples (Fig. 1). However, since urine is stored in the bladder and excreted after varying latencies, some urine samples were expected to contain urine from the two adjacent time periods. To address this problem, we assigned these samples to the time period in which the greatest proportion of urine was likely to have been excreted (Fig. 1). Specifically, if the sample was collected within 30 min after the pre-period (t1) or the peak-period (t2), this was considered to be a potential ‘overlap zone’ (Fig. 1). ‘Overlap-zone’ samples could be assigned to periods on either side of the period changing point depending on the latency between the sample excreted in the ‘overlap zone’ and the previous urination. If Δt between the sample in the ‘overlap zone’ and t1 (or t2 respectively) was smaller than between the last sample before t1 (or t2) and t1 (or t2), we classified the sample in the ‘overlap zone’ as a sample from the period before t1 (or t2 respectively). In all other cases the sample was classified as belonging to the time period in which it was excreted (Fig. 1).
The time line is marked in relation to the start of the target behaviour. Point t1 refers to 135 min after the onset of the target behavior; point t2 refers to 270 min after the end of the target behaviour. The overlap zones correspond to 30 min after t1 and t2 respectively.
Statistical analysis
In order to examine the influence of various predictor variables on changes in urinary GC levels, we ran General Linear Mixed Models (GLMM: [37]) using R version 3.1.1 (R Core Team 2013) and the function lmer of the package lme4 [38], with Maximum Likelihood estimates and a Gaussian structure. Assumptions of normally distributed and homogenous residuals were fulfilled, shown by visual inspection of qq plots and residual plots against fitted values. We checked for model stability by excluding subjects one at a time from the data and state in the results when models were unstable. Variables did not exhibit problems of collinearity [39, 40] (Variance Inflation Factor < 2 in all cases, derived using R-package car [41], applied using a standard linear model excluding the random effect), suggesting that each predictor variable accounted for a portion of the variance.
We ran several GLMMs. In the model including all the data, relative uGC level was the response variable and subject identity was the random factor. We ran additional models which including only the samples relating to aggressive events. This was in order to examine behavioural variation related to aggression (which obviously did not occur for resting events). Due to the small sample size, we were unable to fit all predictor variables into one model. Therefore we looked at the effects of each of the predictor variables on the relative uGC level separately. To establish the significance of the full model we used a likelihood ratio test, comparing the full model with the respective null model comprising only the random effect and the intercept. When models were insignificant, we considered the predictor variables to have no clear effect on the response variable.
Ethical statement
The ethics committee of the School of Psychology, University of St. Andrews, UK, approved this non-invasive behavioural and hormonal study with the Sonso chimpanzees located around Budongo Conservation Field Station (1°43’ N, 31°32’ E) in the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda. In accordance with ethical guidelines we kept 7m distance to the chimpanzees, never interacted with chimpanzee subjects and collected urine with plastic bags, when subjects were sitting > 10m high in the trees, or from leafs after subjects had voluntarily moved. Research was conducted under the permits by the Uganda Wildlife Authority (TDO/33/02) and Uganda National Council for Science and Technology (NS 181). Chimpanzees are an endangered species (IUCN red list) and under the protection of Uganda Wildlife Authority.
Results
Our data set comprised of 14 complete sets of urine samples, where we successfully collected both pre- and peak-samples, following aggression and 10 complete sets following resting (Table 1). Results of the GLMM which included all data showed that the target behaviour (aggression and resting) significantly influenced the relative uGC levels (Table 2A; likelihood ratio test: χ2 = 5.35, df = 1, p = 0.021). Males’ relative uGC levels were significantly higher after aggression (mean relative uGC ± SD = 112% ± 28) than after resting for >30 min (mean relative uGC ± SD = 85% ± 27; Fig. 2). Combining the target behaviour with the subject’s role during the aggression (aggression: aggressor, aggression: victim, and resting) showed that uGC levels tended to be higher (Table 2B; likelihood ratio test: χ2 = 5.48, df = 2, p = 0.065) in both aggressors (mean relative uGC ± SD = 107% ± 12) and victims (mean relative uGC ± SD = 113% ± 33; Fig. 3) than after resting. Estimates show that whilst differences in uGC levels were large for both aggressors and victims against resting, differences were small between aggressor and victim conditions (Table 2B).
The relative uGC level is the percentage of the uGC level during the peak-period in comparison to the pre-period.
The relative uGC level is the percentage of the uGC level during the peak-period in comparison to the pre-period.
Further, we investigated in three separate GLMMs whether the subject’s rank (rank 1–9), aggression duration (min.) or aggression intensity (contact or non-contact aggression) affected the relative uGC levels. Only subject’s dominance rank showed a significant effect (Table 3; likelihood ratio test: χ2 = 5.25, df = 1, p = 0.022) with higher ranking males showing greater elevation of relative uGC levels after aggression (Fig. 4). In contrast we did not find effects due to aggression intensity (contact or non-contact) or aggression duration (min.), although it should be noted that negative effects could be a result of small sample sizes (Likelihood ratio tests: aggression duration: χ2 = 0.45, df = 1, p = 0.50; aggression intensity: model unstable).
The relative uGC level is the percentage of the uGC level during the peak-period in comparison to the pre-period. Highest dominance rank = 1.
Discussion
Male chimpanzee aggressors and victims had larger increases in uGC levels after single aggressive interactions than after resting. In addition, higher ranking male opponents had greater increases in uGC levels than lower ranking males. Sample sizes were small but our results are comparable to previous studies showing raised plasma cortisol in both winners and losers in humans [25], rodents [42] and fish [7], suggesting that for male chimpanzees aggressive interactions are costly, regardless of the individual’s role in the conflict.
The proximate reasons for the increased uGC levels in our study remain unclear. In fish, plasma cortisol increased after fights in both winners and losers, but winners showed only increased plasma cortisol levels 5 min after the end of the fights, while losers still had increased plasma cortisol levels 24h after the fight [7]. If injury to losers could be ruled out, this study suggests different underlying mechanisms, which may involve energetic costs in winners in addition to a social stress reaction in losers [10]. In another study, chimpanzees showed increased uGC levels prior to hunting events and boundary patrols [43]. The author concluded that elevated uGC levels were in response to the anticipation of the cooperative and/or competitive events [43]. Following this idea, chimpanzees might anticipate the energetic needs of specific behaviours required in the near future. In our study, therefore, it might be possible that aggressors secreted cortisol to release sufficient energy for the attack, while victims primarily released cortisol in response to the psychological stressor of being attacked [26].
However, we suggest this is unlikely to be the only possible explanation, for the following reasons. First, a study in humans has shown that competitive martial arts matches release significantly more plasma cortisol than comparable non-competitive exercise [8], suggesting that uGC rises can be linked to a psychological component induced by competitive contexts. Second, we were unable to detect a significant relationship between the duration or intensity of aggression and the uGC elevation, although longer and more intense fights should have consumed more energy. This suggests that in chimpanzee conflicts, psychological rather than energetic stressors seem to have a greater influence on GC levels. This conclusion, however, needs to be treated with caution due to the small sample size.
With the above in mind, it is very unlikely that the energetic stress hypothesis [13] alone can explain why higher ranking male opponents had a greater increase in uGC levels than lower ranking ones. This is especially true, because our study shows greater increases in uGC levels in reaction to the aggression, rather than high absolute uGC levels. Another possible explanation comes from baboons, where dominant males showed higher fecal GC levels during times of social instability [44]. Social instability, the time of rank changes and male immigration, was also strongly correlated with drastically increased aggression. However, rates of aggression, given or received, did not correlate with fecal GC levels [44], again suggesting that GC level increases were not well explained by energetic costs. Rather, dominant male baboons had more to lose than subordinate ones when considering Chacma baboons’ strong reproductive skew. Reproductive skew is less pronounced in chimpanzees, but is nonetheless present [29, 30]. Thus, high ranking male chimpanzees likely have more to lose from defeat than low ranking males. As a result the GC reactivity due to psychological stressors might be stronger in high ranking than in low ranking males.
Social stress is often caused by a lack of control and predictability, such as the exclusion from food or other resources, or the threat of recurring aggression [6, 11]. This is particularly pertinent in chimpanzees, with their fission-fusion social structure that leads to constantly changing networks of social support, so that even dominant individuals can lose aggressive interactions [3, 45, 46]. Powerful aggressors cannot be certain of winning a conflict, and thus lack a degree of control and predictability. This may be a general pattern in animal groups with more egalitarian social hierarchies and may provide an explanation for increased uGC levels after aggression in aggressors as well as victims.
Our results further our understanding of conflict management in animals. Several studies on primates have shown an increase of self-directed behaviours in aggressors and victims [47–49], (but: [50]), suggesting that both suffered from anxiety after aggression [51, 52]. Traditionally, however, it was assumed that victims, not aggressors, suffer from post-conflict stress. For example, in a study of reconciliation in human boys [53], victims showed higher salivary cortisol levels following unreconciled aggression compared to reconciled aggression or following control conditions [53]. In chimpanzees, ‘consolation’ of victims has been shown to reduce self-directed behaviour [54]. However, these studies have not looked at hormonal responses of the aggressors, as it has not been recognized that aggressors might also suffer a stress reaction. The current study suggests that it is time to reconsider this assumption. Indeed, both victims and aggressors are known to engage in stress-reducing post-conflict coping strategies, such as reconciliation, ‘consolation’ or third-party mediated reconciliation [39, 48, 49, 55].
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Budongo Conservation Field Station, especially our field assistants Monday Gideon, Jackson Okuti, Sam Adue and Jacob Alio, and its director Dr. Fred Babweteera. We also thank Julie Duboscq and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: RMW CC TD KZ. Performed the experiments: RMW CC. Analyzed the data: RMW CC AW TD KZ. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: AW TD. Wrote the paper: RMW CC AW TD KZ.
References
- 1.
Maynard-Smith J. Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1982.
- 2. de Waal FBM. Primates—A natural heritage of conflict resolution. Science 2000;289: 586–590. pmid:10915614
- 3. Wittig RM, Boesch C. “Decision-making” in conflicts of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): an extension of the Relational Model. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2003;54: 491–504.
- 4. Smith BR, Blumstein DT. Fitness consequences of personality: a meta-analysis. Behav Ecol. 2008;19: 448–455.
- 5. Creel S. Social dominance and stress hormones. Trends Ecol Evol. 2001;16: 491–497.
- 6. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. Stress and coping mechanisms in female primates. Adv Stud Behav. 2009;19: 1–44.
- 7. Overli O, Harris CA, Winberg S. Short-term effects of fights for social dominance and the establishment of dominant-subordinate relationships on brain monoamines and cortisol in Rainbow trout. Brain Behav Evol. 1999;54: 263–275. pmid:10640786
- 8. Suay F, Salvador A, Gonzalez-Bono E, Sanchis C, Martinez M, Martinez-Sanchis S, et al. Effects of competition and its outcome on serum testosterone, cortisol and prolactin. Psychoneuroendocrino. 1999;24: 551–566.
- 9. McEwan BS, Wingfield JC. The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Horm Behav. 2003;43: 2–15. pmid:12614627
- 10. Christiansen JS, Jorgensen EH, Jobling M. Oxygen consumption in relation to sustained exercise and social stress in Arctic Charr (Salvelinus alpinus L.). J Exp Zool. 1991;260: 149–156.
- 11. Sapolsky RM. Why stress is bad for your brain. Science 1996;273, 749–750. pmid:8701325
- 12. Goymann W, Wingfield JC. Allostatic load, social status and stress hormones: the costs of social status matter. Anim Behav. 2004;67: 591–602.
- 13. Muller MN, Wrangham RM. Dominance, cortisol and stress in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2004;55: 332–340.
- 14. Emery Thompson M, Muller MN, Kahlenberg SM, Wrangham RW. Dynamics of social and energetic stress in wild female chimpanzees. Horm Behav. 2010;58: 440–449. pmid:20546741
- 15. Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Whitten PL. The effect of new alpha males on female stress in free-ranging baboons. Anim Behav. 2005;69: 1211–1221.
- 16. Creel S. Dominance, aggression and glucocorticoid levels in social carnivores. J Mammal. 2005;86: 255–264.
- 17. Muller MN, Kahlenberg SM, Emery Thompson M, Wrangham RW. Male coercion and the costs of promiscuous mating for female chimpanzees. Proc R Soc B. 2007;274: 1009–1014. pmid:17264062
- 18. Crockford C, Wittig RM, Whitten PL, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Social stressors and coping mechanisms in wild female baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus). Horm Behav. 2008;53: 254–265. pmid:18076884
- 19. Wittig RM, Crockford C, Lehmann J, Whitten PL, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Focused grooming networks and stress allevation in wild female baboons. Horm Behav. 2008;54: 170–177. pmid:18396288
- 20. Knapp R, Moore MC. Hormonal responses to aggression vary in different types of agonistic encounters in male tree lizards, Urosaurus ornatus. Horm Behav. 1995;29: 85–105. pmid:7782065
- 21. van Duyse E, Pinxten R, Darras VM, Arckens L, Ens M. Opposite changes in plasma testosterone and corticosterone levels following a simulated territorial challenge in male great Tits. Behaviour 2004;141: 451–467.
- 22. Sachser N, Lick C. Social experience, behavior, and stress in Guinea pigs. Physiol Behav. 1991;50: 83–90. pmid:1946736
- 23. Ostner J, Heistermann M, Schülke O. Dominance, aggression and physiological stress in wild male Assamese macaques (Macaca assamensis). Horm Behav. 2008;54: 613–619. pmid:18625235
- 24. Wallner B, Möstl E, Dittami J, Prossinger H. Fecal glucocorticoids document stress in female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Gen Comp Endocr. 1999;113: 80–86. pmid:9882546
- 25. Elias M. Serum cortisol, testosterone, and testosterone-binding globulin responses to competitive fighting in human males. Aggressive Behav. 1981;7: 215–224.
- 26. Abbott DH, Keverne EB, Bercovitch FB, Shively CA, Mendoza SP, Saltzman W, et al. Are subordinates always stressed? A comparative analysis of rank differences in cortisol levels among primates. Horm Behav. 2003;43: 67–82. pmid:12614636
- 27. Aureli F, van Schaik CP. Post-conflict behaviour in longtailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis): II Coping with the uncertainty. Ethology. 1991;89: 101–114.
- 28. Wittig RM, Boesch C. How to repair relationships—reconciliation in wild chimpanzees. Ethology. 2005;111: 736–763.
- 29. Boesch C, Kohou G, Néné H, Vigilant L. Male competition and paternity in wild chimpanzees of the Taï forest. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2006;130: 103–115. pmid:16353223
- 30. Wroblewski EE, Murray CM, Keele BF, Schumacher-Stankey JC, Hahn BH, Pusey AE. Male dominance rank and reproductive success in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. Anim Behav. 2009;77: 873–885. pmid:19498952
- 31.
Reynolds V. The chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest, Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005.
- 32. Altmann J. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour. 1974;49: 227–267. pmid:4597405
- 33. Hauser B, Deschner T, Boesch C. Development of a liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry method for the determination of 23 endogenous steroids in small quantities of primate urine. J Chromatogr B. 2008;862: 100–112. pmid:18054529
- 34. Bahr NI, Palme R, Möhle U, Hodges JK, Heistermann M. Comparative aspects of the metabolism and excretion of cortisol in three individual nonhuman primates. Gen Comp Endocr. 2000;117: 427–438. pmid:10764553
- 35. Erb RE, Tillson SA, Hodgen GD, Plotka ED. Urinary creatinine as an index compound for estimating rate of excretion of steroids in the domestic sow. J Anim Sci. 1970;30: 79–85. pmid:5436444
- 36. Muller MN, Lipson SF. Diurnal patterns of urinary steroid excretion in wild chimpanzees. Am J Primatol. 2003;60: 161–166. pmid:12910467
- 37.
Baayen RH. Analyzing Linguistic Data. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2008.
- 38.
Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1–7; 2014. Available: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.
- 39. Wittig RM, Boesch C. Receiving post-conflict affiliation from the enemy’s friend reconciles former opponents. PLoS One. 2010;5: e13995. pmid:21085592
- 40. Wittig RM, Crockford C, Deschner T, Langergraber KE, Ziegler TE, Zuberbühler K. Food sharing is linked to urinary oxytocin levels and bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proc R Soc B. 2014;281: 20133096. pmid:24430853
- 41.
Fox J, Weisberg S. An R companion to applied regression. 2nd edn. Thousand Oaks (CA): Sage Publications; 2011. Available: http://socserv.socsci.mcmaster.ca/jfox/Books/Companion.
- 42. Dijkstra H, Tilders FJH, Hiehle MA, Smelik PG. Hormonal reactions to fighting in rat colonies: prolactin rises during defence, not during offence. Physiol Behav. 1992;51: 961–968. pmid:1319590
- 43.
Sobolewski ME. The hormonal correlates of male chimpanzee social behavior. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Michigan. 2012. Available: http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/handle/2027.42/96063/mesobole_1.pdf?sequence=1.
- 44. Bergman TJ, Beehner JC, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Whitten PL. Correlates of stress in free-ranging male chacma baboons, Papio hamadryas ursinus. Anim Behav. 2005;70: 703–713.
- 45. Mitani J. Male chimpanzee form enduring and equitable social bonds. Anim Behav. 2009;77: 633–640.
- 46. Wittig RM, Crockford C, Langergraber KE, Zuberbühler K. Triadic social interactions operate across time: a field experiment with wild chimpanzees. Proc R Soc B. 2014;281: 20133155. pmid:24500174
- 47. Cooper MA, Aureli F, Singh M. Sex Differences in Reconciliation and Post‐Conflict Anxiety in Bonnet Macaques. Ethology. 2007;113: 26–38.
- 48. Schino G, Rosati L, Geminiani S, Aureli F. Post‐Conflict Anxiety in Japanese Macaques (Macaca fuscata): Aggressor’s and Victim’s Perspectives. Ethology. 2007;113: 1081–1088.
- 49. Romero T, Colmenares F, Aureli F. Testing the function of reconciliation and third‐party affiliation for aggressors in hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas). Am J Primatol. 2009;71: 60–69. pmid:18837043
- 50. Koski SE, Sterck EH. Triadic postconflict affiliation in captive chimpanzees: does consolation console? Anim Behav. 2007;73: 133–142.
- 51. Castles DL, Whiten A, Aureli F. Social anxiety, relationships and self-directed behaviour among wild female olive baboons. Anim Behav. 1999;58: 1207–1215. pmid:10600141
- 52. Troisi A. Displacement activities as a behavioural measure of stress in nonhuman primates and human subjects. Stress. 2002;5: 47–54. pmid:12171766
- 53. Butovskaya ML. Reconciliation, dominance and cortisol levels in children and adolescents (7–15-year old boys). Behaviour. 2008;145: 1557–1576.
- 54. Fraser ON, Stahl D, Aureli F. Stress reduction through consolation in chimpanzees. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. 2008;105: 8557–8562. pmid:18559863
- 55. Fraser ON, Koski SE, Wittig RM, Aureli F. Why are bystanders friendly to recipients of aggression? Commun Integra Biol. 2009;2: 1–7