Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

A Prospective Cohort Study of the Effects of Adjuvant Breast Cancer Chemotherapy on Taste Function, Food Liking, Appetite and Associated Nutritional Outcomes

  • Anna Boltong ,

    anna.boltong@cancervic.org.au

    Affiliation Cancer Council Victoria, Melbourne, Australia

  • Sanchia Aranda,

    Affiliations Cancer Institute NSW, Eveleigh, NSW, Australia, Department of Cancer Experiences Research, Peter MacCallum Cancer Centre, East Melbourne, Victoria, Australia, Melbourne School of Health Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Carlton, Victoria, Australia

  • Russell Keast,

    Affiliation Centre for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research, Deakin University, Burwood, Victoria, Australia

  • Rochelle Wynne,

    Affiliation Melbourne School of Health Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Carlton, Victoria, Australia

  • Prudence A. Francis,

    Affiliation Breast Medical Oncology, Peter MacCallum Cancer Centre, East Melbourne, Victoria, Australia

  • Jacqueline Chirgwin,

    Affiliation Breast Medical Oncology, Eastern Health, Australia

  • Karla Gough

    Affiliation Department of Cancer Experiences Research, Peter MacCallum Cancer Centre, East Melbourne, Victoria, Australia

Abstract

Background

‘Taste’ changes are commonly reported during chemotherapy. It is unclear to what extent this relates to actual changes in taste function or to changes in appetite and food liking and how these changes affect dietary intake and nutritional status.

Patients and methods

This prospective, repeated measures cohort study recruited participants from three oncology clinics. Women (n = 52) prescribed adjuvant chemotherapy underwent standardised testing of taste perception, appetite and food liking at six time points to measure change from baseline. Associations between taste and hedonic changes and nutritional outcomes were examined.

Results

Taste function was significantly reduced early in chemotherapy cycles (p<0.05) but showed recovery by late in the cycle. Ability to correctly identify salty, sour and umami tastants was reduced. Liking of sweet food decreased early and mid-cycle (p<0.01) but not late cycle. Liking of savory food was not significantly affected. Appetite decreased early in the cycle (p<0.001). Reduced taste function was associated with lowest kilojoule intake (r = 0.31; p = 0.008) as was appetite loss with reduced kilojoule (r = 0.34; p = 0.002) and protein intake (r = 0.36; p = 0.001) early in the third chemotherapy cycle. Decreased appetite early in the third and final chemotherapy cycles was associated with a decline in BMI (p = <0.0005) over the study period. Resolution of taste function, food liking and appetite was observed 8 weeks after chemotherapy completion. There was no association between taste change and dry mouth, oral mucositis or nausea.

Conclusion

The results reveal, for the first time, the cyclical yet transient effects of adjuvant chemotherapy on taste function and the link between taste and hedonic changes, dietary intake and nutritional outcomes. The results should be used to inform reliable pre-chemotherapy education.

Introduction

Taste is one of the five senses and interacts with smell, touch and other physiological cues to affect the wider perception of flavor. Taste function is defined as the perception derived when chemical molecules stimulate taste receptor fields in areas of the tongue, soft palate and oropharyngeal region of the oral cavity to perceive the five basic taste qualities (sweet, sour, salty, bitter and umami) [1], measured via standardised processes [2]. Food hedonics, which also contributes to flavour perception, encompasses food liking: the immediate experience or anticipation of pleasure from the oro-sensory stimulation of eating a food [3], and appetite: a psychobiologically based sensation related to the maintenance of eating and a desire for specific foods [4].

Chemotherapy is known to affect other senses with ototoxicity and peripheral neuropathy recognized treatment-related toxicities, which in some cases may be permanent [5], [6]. ‘Taste’ changes are commonly reported by people receiving chemotherapy [7] even among those who do not report nausea. It is unclear to what extent this relates to altered taste function per se or to changes to the sense of smell or touch (including oral dryness) or to hedonic aspects such as food liking, or appetite, also described colloquially by patients and clinicians as ‘taste’ [8].

‘Taste’ changes in oncology populations have been linked to adverse effects on quality of life, morbidity and mortality due to an association with inadequate energy and nutrient intake, weight loss, malnutrition [11], reduced compliance with treatment regimens [9], reduced immunity [10], [11], altered food relationships [12], changed food rituals [13], emotional distress and interference with daily life [14]. The extent to which true taste problems play a role in these scenarios is unknown. The ability to perceive taste sensations guides food choice, which in itself is a determinant of health [15]. Because changes in taste function, liking of food and appetite all have the potential to underpin changes in dietary intake and nutritional status, understanding the extent of the contribution of each would help inform the development of effective interventions in future.

Study Objectives

The primary objective of this study was to measure the effect of adjuvant breast cancer chemotherapy on taste function and food hedonics across the treatment trajectory. The primary hypothesis was that taste function and food hedonics would be adversely affected by chemotherapy and that the greatest changes to taste function and food hedonics would occur early in a chemotherapy cycle. It was also hypothesised that changes in taste function and food hedonics would be associated with alterations in dietary intake and nutritional status. A secondary objective was to assess the relationship between changes in taste function and toxicities.

Methods

Study Design

This was a prospective, multi-centre cohort study that recruited patients planned for adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer at three hospital-based oncology clinics in Melbourne, Australia from April to December 2011. Potentially eligible patients were identified via medical oncology clinics and breast cancer multidisciplinary meetings.

Ethics statement

Institutional ethics approval was granted at Eastern Health and Peter MacCallum Cancer Centre and written informed consent was obtained from each patient before enrolment.

Participants

Patients aged 18 years or over scheduled to receive an anthracycline and/or taxane containing chemotherapy regimen for the adjuvant treatment of resected invasive breast cancer were eligible to participate. Patients were required to be able to read and converse in English. Exclusion criteria included chemotherapy already initiated, concurrent radiotherapy, previous radiotherapy to the head and neck region, or presence of cognitive impairment that might impact study outcomes.

Variables

Outcome measures were number of tastants identified correctly, food liking score, appetite rating, daily energy (kJ) and macronutrient (protein, fat and carbohydrate) intake, weight, body mass index (BMI) and nutritional status (Figure 1). Demographic and clinical data collected were obtained for age, years of education, smoking status, BMI, concurrent medications, presence of conditions implicated in taste function (liver or renal dysfunction, sinusitis, diabetes) and treatment related toxicities (nausea, dry mouth and oral mucositis).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Summary of outcome data collected at each study time point.

aDay 12–13 of the third chemotherapy cycle if receiving 14 day chemotherapy cycles. Cycle 3, Day 19–20 if receiving 21 day chemotherapy cycles.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103512.g001

Data sources and measurement

Figure 1 details study assessment time points and data collected. Time points were selected in order to avoid days in the chemotherapy cycle when patients may be taking corticosteroids. Intra-cycle time points were chosen to assess effects early, middle and late in a cycle. Stage of chemotherapy treatment was selected based on a qualitative study with chemotherapy recipients who reported symptoms being apparent by the third chemotherapy cycle and resolving by 6–8 weeks after completion of chemotherapy treatment [16].

Demographic and clinical data were obtained from the patients’ medical records and via direct questioning of the participant. Clinical assessment of relevant chemotherapy toxicities was performed at each study appointment in accordance with the US Department of Health and Human Services Common Terminology Criteria for Adverse Events (CTCAE) v.4.03, 2010.

Taste identification testing was performed in accordance with the International Standards Organization (ISO), ISO 3972∶2011-Sensory Analysis-Methodology-Method of Investigating Sensitivity of Taste as part of a taste identification task. Tastants and their corresponding concentrations were: sucrose 300 mM, NaCl 200 mM, citric acid 5 mM, caffeine 10 mM, MSG 200 mM. These solutions were prepared in the Deakin University sensory laboratory from food grade chemicals and deionised water in the 7 day period prior to testing. At testing, five 2 ml solutions corresponding to the five basic taste qualities (sweet, salty, sour, bitter and umami) were each tasted in a single ‘sip and spit’ technique to determine the total number and individual tastants identified correctly at each time point. The mouth was rinsed with room temperature purified water three times before and after sampling and expectorating each solution. Perceived taste quality was identified by selecting one of seven choices. Correct responses were sweet for sucrose, salty for NaCl, sour for citric acid, bitter for caffeine, and savoury for MSG. Additional choices were none or metallic. Taste identification score was assigned as 0–5 correct choices.

Food liking was assessed using a 9-point hedonic scale [17] to measure liking of a standard sweet (chocolate) and umami (soup) food item from Like extremely (9) to Dislike extremely (1). Appetite was rated on a 10-point scale from Best appetite (10) to Worst appetite (1). Before all taste tests, participants were asked to refrain from smoking, chewing gum, using toothpastes or other oral care products, or eating or drinking anything other than water for a minimum of one hour.

Dietary intake data for the preceding 24 hour period was collected by a dietitian via telephone according to the United States Department of Agriculture (USDA) automated multiple-pass approach (AMPA) [18]. Dietary data were analysed using FoodWorks 2007 (Xyris software, Queensland, Australia) and daily nutrient intake was quantified as kilojoules and grams of protein per kg of body weight and carbohydrate and fat as a proportion of daily energy intake. Investigator assisted height and self-reported weight were used to calculate BMI (kg/m2). The Patient-Generated Subjective Global Assessment (PG-SGA) [19] is a validated method of assessing and classifying nutritional status in oncology populations and was used at baseline (T0) and 8 weeks after completion of chemotherapy (T6) [20].

Sample size

Sample size requirements for this study were determined based on estimates available for the hedonic scale and a difference between baseline and final follow-up (where attrition would be highest). In this case, sample size calculations were based on a paired-samples t-test with an alpha level of 0.05, 80% power, a difference of 0.8 points in food liking and a standard deviation of 2.1 [17] (a standardised difference of 0.42). Given these specifications, a total sample of 47 patients was required at final follow-up. Assuming attrition of up to 10%, a minimum of 52 patients were needed at baseline.

Statistical analysis

Recruitment bias was assessed by comparing the age, treatment centre and cancer stage of patients who consented to participate and those who declined participation using t-tests and chi-squared (or Fisher’s exact) tests as appropriate. Analysis of food liking and appetite was carried out by fitting a linear mixed model to each outcome separately; a reference cell model was used to generate estimates of baseline means and differences between baseline and follow-up assessments with 95% confidence intervals and tests of significance. An unstructured covariance type was used to model the covariance structure among repeated measures and all models were estimated by maximum likelihood. McNemar’s test was used to assess differences between proportions of participants correctly identifying all five tastants at follow-up assessments compared with baseline. Differences were also assessed for each tastant individually. An SPSS macro created by Garcia-Granero was used to perform this test. [21] Confidence intervals generated by this macro are based on methods developed by Newcombe [22]. Kendall’s Tau-b was used to examine associations of change scores for number of tastants correctly identified and liking of sweet and savoury test food items with change in BMI and PG-SGA score. Frequency statistics were used to summarise treatment toxicities at each time point and Kendall’s Tau-b was used to examine associations of change scores for number of tastants correctly identified with treatment toxicities at corresponding follow-up assessments. Correlation coefficients were interpreted as follows: 0.1, small association; 0.3, medium association; and 0.5, large association [23]. SPSS Windows Version 21 (Chicago, IL, USA) was used for all analyses. No adjustments were made for multiplicity.

Results

Fifty-two participants were enrolled in the study. Figure 2 summarises numbers of participants screened, approached and recruited.

thumbnail
Figure 2. CONSORT diagram reporting numbers of individuals at each stage of the study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103512.g002

There were no differences between patients who consented to participate and those who declined participation in terms of age, treatment centre or stage of disease (all p>0.05). Compliance with data collection was high at ≥96% for clinical variables, ≥94% for demographic variables and ≥92% for all outcome measures. Demographic, clinical and social characteristics of the sample are shown in Table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic, clinical and social characteristics of study participants and non-participants at baseline.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103512.t001

Change in taste function

Before adjuvant chemotherapy, 33% of participants correctly identified all five tastants (Table 2). Taste function was reduced with significantly fewer participants correctly identifying all five tastants early cycle 3 (difference −18%, 95% CI −33, −3; p = 0.043) and early final cycle (difference −20%; 95% CI −35, −3; p = 0.039) compared with baseline. Fewer participants correctly identified all five tastants mid cycle 3 (difference −13%, 95% CI −28, 2; p = 0.15) but this difference was not significant and any effect had predominantly resolved by late cycle 3 (difference −4%, 95% CI −20, 12; p = 0.79). There was no difference between the proportions of participants who correctly identified all five tastants pre-chemotherapy and 2 months post-chemotherapy (difference 0%, 95% CI −15, 15; p = 0.77).

thumbnail
Table 2. Correct identification of standardised tastants: proportions of sample identifying all five correct at baseline and differences in proportion at subsequent time points (N = 52).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103512.t002

At baseline and at all subsequent time points, sucrose as sweet and caffeine as bitter were the most and least accurately identified tastants respectively (Table 2). Further, compared with baseline, there were no differences between the proportions of participants who correctly identified either of these tastants at any follow-up time point (all p>0.05). Conversely, there was a significant reduction in the proportions of participants who correctly identified MSG as savoury early cycle 3 (difference −16%, 95% CI −29, −2; p = 0.046) and NaCl as salty mid cycle 3 (difference −20%, 95% CI −35, −3; p = 0.039) and early final cycle (difference −23%, 95% CI −39, −6; p = 0.022). Compared with baseline, fewer participants correctly identified citric acid as sour at all subsequent time points (Table 2) but only the difference between early final cycle and baseline reached significance (difference −27%, 95% CI −41, −11; p = 0.004).

Change in food hedonics

Food liking and appetite (Table 3) exhibited similar patterns of differences as taste function indexed by total number of tastants correctly identified. Compared with baseline, appetite was significantly lower early cycle 3 (difference 2.1, 95% CI −2.9, −1.3; p<0.0005) and early final cycle (difference 2.1, 95% CI −3.0, −1.2; p<0.0005). Liking of sweet food was also significantly lower early cycle 3 (difference 0.9, 95% CI −1.5, −0.4; p = 0.002) and early final cycle (difference 1.1, 95% CI −1.7, −0.5; p = 0.001), as well as mid cycle 3 (difference 0.8, 95% CI −1.4, −0.3; p = 0.003), compared with before chemotherapy. Compared with baseline, none of the differences in liking of savoury food at subsequent time points were significant (all p>0.05).

thumbnail
Table 3. Food hedonics: estimates of food liking and appetite at baseline and differences at subsequent time points (N = 52).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103512.t003

Association between changes in taste function and appetite and nutritional outcomes

Changes in taste function.

There was a significant, medium-sized association between a change in taste function and kilojoule intake early cycle 3 (Tau-b = 0.31, p = 0.008). Participants whose ability to correctly identify all five tastants deteriorated consumed fewer kilojoules per kilogram (Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 4. Associations with taste and appetite changes from baseline to assessment points early in the third and final chemotherapy cycles.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103512.t004

Changes in appetite.

There were significant, medium-sized associations between reduced appetite early cycle 3 and reduced kilojoule and protein intake (Tau-b = 0.34, p = 0.002; and Tau-b = 0.36, p = 0.001, respectively) and decline in BMI early cycle 3 compared to baseline (Tau-b = 0.31, p = 0.004). There was also a significant, medium-sized association between changes in reduced appetite and decline in BMI early final cycle compared to baseline (Tau-b = 0.42, p<0.0005) (Table 4).

Association between changes in taste, chemotherapy type and nutrition related toxicities

The majority of participants reported no symptoms of dry mouth, oral mucositis or nausea at all time points assessed. Nonetheless, there was a notable increase in the percentages of patients reporting treatment-related toxicities early and mid cycle 3 and early final cycle. None of the associations between changes in taste function at follow-up time points from before chemotherapy and self-reported toxicities were significant (all p>.05, Table 5).

thumbnail
Table 5. Presence of mouth dryness, oral mucositis and nausea at each time point and association between each toxicity and change in taste function.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103512.t005

Discussion

The results of this study investigating women receiving adjuvant chemotherapy support the hypothesis that taste function is adversely affected by chemotherapy and that chemotherapy-related effects are greatest early in a chemotherapy cycle. Further, changes in taste function are cyclical and transient, as are changes in food liking and appetite. The hypothesis that chemotherapy related taste and hedonic changes are associated with alterations in dietary intake and weight was also supported, although these effects are experienced variably. Associations between change in taste function and chemotherapy-related nausea, dry mouth and mucositis, were typically small or trivial in size. Thus this study contributes new knowledge in the area of chemotherapy-related changes in taste function, food hedonics and nutritional outcomes for women receiving adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer.

This is the first published study to have examined taste function more than once within a single chemotherapy cycle in a sample size greater than 10 participants and the first to assess perception of all five basic taste qualities in an adult chemotherapy population. Taste assessment in previous studies of chemotherapy populations was conducted on the day of chemotherapy administration (late cycle) when patients report symptoms are at their mildest. This flaw in methodology incorrectly suggested that taste function may be unaffected by chemotherapy [24]. Data from the current study supports a recommendation that future research incorporates early-mid cycle taste measurements in its design. It has been shown previously that chemotherapy related taste changes resolve some time between the end of treatment and 3 months after [25]. This study provides new evidence that for breast cancer patients, taste problems experienced in the first 4–6 days after chemotherapy administration will likely resolve over the course of a single chemotherapy cycle, repeat with each cycle and resolve completely in as little as two months after chemotherapy completion. These findings equip clinicians with accurate information to provide breast cancer patients regarding expected nature and duration of symptoms.

Given that patients undergoing chemotherapy commonly report a reversal in preference for sweet or savoury foods and aversions to items such as chocolate and coffee [26], the difference between change in liking of the prototypical sweet food and prototypical savoury food is noteworthy. It is not known whether a change in taste function per se is responsible for the changes in liking of food observed or whether this is driven more by changes in appetite or as a result of other factors such as chemotherapy induced nausea. It is postulated that the absence of a demonstrated decrease in ability to identify sweet and bitter tastants may account for this observation, suggesting these sweet and bitter taste qualities are disproportionally (and aversively) perceptible over others. It has been shown previously that hedonic scores for sucrose solutions decrease at high concentrations in patients with poorest appetites [27] suggesting that those with poorest appetites have greatest aversion to intensely sweet items.

This study adds to limited existing evidence for the link between taste and hedonic changes, dietary behaviour and nutritional outcomes. Although a reduction in taste function was associated with a lower kJ intake, appetite loss was more strongly related to dietary inadequacy and weight loss than was change in taste sensitivity. Reduced appetite had a medium-sized association with reduced BMI and a small-sized association with worsening nutritional status, however not all participants suffering taste or appetite deficits lost weight. The relationship between taste and food hedonics and alterations in dietary quality and nutritional outcomes was specific to early breast cancer populations in this study and should be tested in other clinical scenarios not least because of bidirectional weight change and its variable clinical implications.

Varying use of anti-nausea medications may be a confounding factor in this study as nausea has previously been associated with learned food aversions during chemotherapy treatment [28]. It is postulated that degree of nausea control is likely to influence self-rated appetite and food liking. Future studies should consider incorporating a standardised anti-emetic regimen. Previous studies of taste and food hedonics in chemotherapy populations suffered from methodological issues of heterogeneity in cancer type, stage and prescribed chemotherapy. This was not a factor in the current study. However, time lapse between assessments did vary between individuals within the sample due to differing number of chemotherapy cycles administered, chemotherapy cycle length, and treatment delays. The post-chemotherapy time point (T5) represented a period of 5–8 months from the baseline assessment. This variability has implications for nutritional outcome measures as weight gain has previously been shown to vary with duration of treatment in studies of women receiving adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer [29].

Conclusions

Despite the stated limitations, this study characterised, for the first time, changes in taste perception and food hedonics over repeated chemotherapy cycles in women with early breast cancer and provided evidence that taste per se, as opposed to other elements of flavour is adversely affected at key points during chemotherapy. Findings will inform the design of future studies seeking to understand the mechanisms of changes in taste perception during chemotherapy, and ultimately, the design of interventions aimed at reducing the negative nutritional consequences of chemotherapy treatment. Understanding more about sensory risk factors for weight gain in breast cancer groups should be prioritised in future clinical research, given the link between obesity and poorer outcomes in this population.

Implementation of findings into practice

Patients do not systematically receive specific information regarding the possible nature, timing of onset and duration of taste problems by health professionals, nor are possible consequences of changes or management strategies routinely discussed. This research has generated new evidence to guide assessment and predictors of chemotherapy induced sensory and hedonic changes. Findings of the study will shape tailored patient information provision in preparation for chemotherapy.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: AB SA RK RW PF JC KG. Performed the experiments: AB. Analyzed the data: KG AB. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: RK. Contributed to the writing of the manuscript: AB SA KG. Data acquisition: AB. Quality control of data and algorithms: AB SA. Manuscript editing and review: AB SA RW RK PF JC KG.

References

  1. 1. Breslin PA, Spector AC (2008) Mammalian taste perception. Curr Biol 18(4): R148–55. Epub 2008/02/28.
  2. 2. International Organization for Standardization (2011) ISO 3972: 2011 - Sensory Analysis – Methodology – Method of investigating sensitivity of taste. Geneva: ISO.
  3. 3. Mela DJ (2006) Eating for pleasure or just wanting to eat? (2006) Reconsidering sensory hedonic responses as a driver of obesity. Appetite 47(1): 10–7. Epub 2006/05/02.
  4. 4. Blundell JE (1979) Hunger, appetite and satiety-constructs in search of identities. In: Turner M, editor. Nutrition and Lifestyles. London: Applied Science Publishers. pp. 21–42.
  5. 5. Gilmer-Knight KR, Kraemer DF, Neuwelt EA (2005) Ototoxicity in Children Receiving Platinum Chemotherapy: Underestimating a Commonly Occurring Toxicity That May Influence Academic and Social Development. J Clin Oncol 23(34): 8588–96.
  6. 6. Quasthoff S, Hartung HP (2002) Chemotherapy-induced peripheral neuropathy. J Neurol. 249(1): 9–17.
  7. 7. Zabernigg A, Gamper EM, Giesinger JM, Rumpold G, Kemmler G, et al. (2010) Taste alterations in cancer patients receiving chemotherapy: a neglected side effect? Oncologist 15(8): 913–20. Epub 2010/07/30.
  8. 8. Boltong A, Keast R, Aranda S (2011) Talking About Taste: How do Oncology Clinicians Discuss and Document Taste Problems? Cancer Forum 35(2): 81–7.
  9. 9. Doty RL, Shah M, Bromley SM (2008) Drug-induced taste disorders. Drug Saf 31(3): 199–215. Epub 2008/02/28.
  10. 10. Schiffman SS, Sattely-Miller EA, Taylor EL, Graham BG, Landerman LR, et al. (2007) Combination of flavor enhancement and chemosensory education improves nutritional status in older cancer patients. J Nutr Health Aging 11(5): 439–54.
  11. 11. Schiffman SS (2007) Critical illness and changes in sensory perception (2007) Proc Nutr Soc. 66(3): 331–45. Epub 2007/07/20.
  12. 12. Schiffman SS, Graham BG (2000) Taste and smell perception affect appetite and immunity in the elderly. European journal of clinical nutrition (Suppl 3): S54–S63.
  13. 13. Bernhardson B (2003) 5 themes described the experiences of patients with chemotherapy induced oral mucositis. Evid Based Nurs 6(2): 62–.
  14. 14. Bernhardson BM, Tishelman C, Rutqvist LE (2007) Chemosensory changes experienced by patients undergoing cancer chemotherapy: a qualitative interview study. J Pain Symptom Manage 34(4): 403–12.
  15. 15. McQuestion M, Fitch M, Howell D (2011) The changed meaning of food: Physical, social and emotional loss for patients having received radiation treatment for head and neck cancer. Eur J Oncol Nurs 15(2): 145–51. Epub 2010/09/25.
  16. 16. Boltong A, Keast R, Aranda S (2012) Experiences and consequences of altered taste, flavour and food hedonics during chemotherapy treatment Support Care Cancer. 20(11): 2765–74.
  17. 17. Peryam DR, Pilgrim FJ (1957) Hedonic scale method of measuring food preferences. Food Technology 11 Suppl: 9–14
  18. 18. Raper N, Perloff B, Ingwersen L, Steinfeldt L, Anand J (2004) An overview of USDA’s Dietary Intake Data System. J Food Comp Anal 17(3–4): 545–55.
  19. 19. Ottery F (2000) Patient-Generated Subjective Global Assessment. In: McCallum P & Polisena C, editor (2000) The Clinical Guide to Oncology Nutrition Chicago: American Dietetic Association. pp. 11–23.
  20. 20. Bauer J, Capra S, Ferguson M (2002) Use of the scored Patient-Generated Subjective Global Assessment (PG-SGA) as a nutrition assessment tool in patients with cancer. Eur J Clin Nutr 56: 779–85.
  21. 21. Garcia-Granero M (2011) Two proportions test (related) - SPSS. Available: http://www.how2stats.net/2011/09/two-proportions-test-related-spss.html. Accessed 2014 Jul 06.
  22. 22. Newcombe R (1998) Interval estimation for the difference between independent proportions: comparison of eleven methods. Stat Med 17(8): 873–90.
  23. 23. Cohen J (1998) Set Correlation and Contingency Tables. Appl Psych Meas 12(4): 425–34.
  24. 24. Boltong A, Keast R (2012) The influence of chemotherapy on taste perception and food hedonics: A systematic review. Cancer Treat Rev 38(2): 152–63.
  25. 25. Steinbach S, Hummel T, Bohner C, Berktold S, Hundt W, et al. (2009) Qualitative and quantitative assessment of taste and smell changes in patients undergoing chemotherapy for breast cancer or gynecologic malignancies. J Clin Oncol 27(11): 1899–905.
  26. 26. Boakes RA, Tarrier N, Barnes BW, Tattersall MH (1993) Prevalence of anticipatory nausea and other side-effects in cancer patients receiving chemotherapy. Eur J Cancer. 29A(6): 866–70. Epub 1993/01/01.
  27. 27. Trant AS, Serin J, Douglass HO (1982) Is taste related to anorexia in cancer patients? Am J Clin Nutr 36(1): 45–58. Epub 1982/07/01.
  28. 28. Schwartz MD, Jacobsen PB, Bovbjerg DH (1996) Role of nausea in the development of aversions to a beverage paired with chemotherapy treatment in cancer patients. Physiol Behav 59(4–5): 659–63. Epub 1996/04/01.
  29. 29. Heasman K, Sutherland H, Campbell J, Elhakim T, Boyd N (1985) Weight gain during adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer. Breast Cancer Res Tr 5(2): 195–200.