Figures
Abstract
Effects of parental genotype or parent-offspring genetic interaction are well established in model organisms for a variety of traits. However, these transgenerational genetic models are rarely studied in humans. We have utilized an autism case-control study with 735 mother-child pairs to perform genome-wide screening for maternal genetic effects and maternal-offspring genetic interaction. We used simple models of single locus parent-child interaction and identified suggestive results (P<10−4) that cannot be explained by main effects, but no genome-wide significant signals. Some of these maternal and maternal-child associations were in or adjacent to autism candidate genes including: PCDH9, FOXP1, GABRB3, NRXN1, RELN, MACROD2, FHIT, RORA, CNTN4, CNTNAP2, FAM135B, LAMA1, NFIA, NLGN4X, RAPGEF4, and SDK1. We attempted validation of potential autism association under maternal-specific models using maternal-paternal comparison in family-based GWAS datasets. Our results suggest that further study of parental genetic effects and parent-child interaction in autism is warranted.
Citation: Tsang KM, Croen LA, Torres AR, Kharrazi M, Delorenze GN, Windham GC, et al. (2013) A Genome-Wide Survey of Transgenerational Genetic Effects in Autism. PLoS ONE 8(10): e76978. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978
Editor: Michael Edward Zwick, Emory University School Of Medicine, United States of America
Received: June 12, 2013; Accepted: August 28, 2013; Published: October 24, 2013
This is an open-access article, free of all copyright, and may be freely reproduced, distributed, transmitted, modified, built upon, or otherwise used by anyone for any lawful purpose. The work is made available under the Creative Commons CC0 public domain dedication.
Funding: This study was supported by the National Institute of environmental Health Sciences (http://www.niehs.nih.gov/ - R01-ES016669). The collection of data and biomaterials that participated in the National Institute of Mental Health Autism Genetics Initiative has been supported by National Institute of Health grants MH52708, MH39437, MH00219 and MH00980; National Health Medical Research Council grant 0034328; and by grants from the Scottish Rite, the Spunk Fund, Inc., the Rebecca and Solomon Baker Fund, the APEX Foundation, the National Alliance for Research in Schizophrenia and Affective Disorders, the endowment fund of the Nancy Pritzker Laboratory (Stanford), by gifts from the Autism Society of America, the Janet M. Grace Pervasive Developmental Disorders Fund, and families and friends of individuals with autism. The collection data and biomaterials also come from the Autism Genetic Resource Exchange (AGRE) collection supported by a National Institute of Health grant MH64547 and the Cure Autism Now Foundation. AGRE is a program of Autism Speaks and is supported, in part, by grant 1U24MH081810 from the National Institute of Mental Health to Clara M. Lajonchere (PI). Collection and submission of the data to dbGaP were supported by a grant from the Medical Research Council (G0601030) and the Wellcome Trust (075491/Z/04), Anthony P. Monaco, P.I., University of Oxford. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Autism is a heterogeneous neurodevelopmental disorder defined by deficits in language and social behavior as well as patterns of repetitive behaviors. Twin, sibling, and broader autism phenotype research has demonstrated the high heritability of autism spectrum disorders as well as autism-related traits [1], [2], [3], [4]. While family-based studies suggest that the inheritance of autism is complex, the occurrence of autism or autistic traits in individuals with diseases of known genetic etiology (such as Fragile X, Rett or Timothy syndrome) suggests a simpler genetic basis for autism in around 5% of autism cases [5]. The advent of more frequent and higher resolution clinical cytogenetic testing has also shown that genetic copy number variation or other aberrations can be found in 10–20% of individuals with autism, further implicating a highly penetrant genetic basis in some cases not associated with a known genetic disorder [5]. Several genome-wide association studies of common single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) have been performed, but the most significant results from these studies, much like other common complex diseases, show modest effect size [6], [7], [8]. Thus, the genetic etiology of the majority of autism cases remains elusive.
Genetic studies of autism to date have been focused on genetic risk carried by the affected proband. The possibility that parental genotypes influence risk for autism, separately or in conjunction with the genotype of the proband, has yet to be investigated. There are two main types of effects that we consider here: parental main effects, where the genes in one generation have an effect on the phenotype in the next generation independently of inheritance, and transgenerational epistasis, where a combination of genes in the parent and the offspring lead to a phenotype. These effects deviate from classical Mendelian models and have not been widely studied. However, there are several examples in model organisms that demonstrate their importance [9], [10]. These effects are not detectable by the analyses used in most genetic studies, although they would cause clustering in families and patterns of inheritance that may appear indistinguishable from complex heritable main proband effects.
There are several models through which transgenerational effects could contribute to autism risk. The first model is in relation to maternal main effects; specifically, this model postulates that genetic variation in the mother alters the fetal environment and impacts early neurodevelopment. One well-documented example of this type of maternal effect can be seen in neural tube defects (NTDs). It has been shown that low folate and vitamin B12 in the mother increase the risk of neural tube defects. Additionally, several groups have demonstrated that maternal genetic polymorphisms in MTHFR, which has a role in folate metabolism, and TCN2, which has a role in vitamin B12 transport, have an effect on the risk of NTDs [11], [12].
The second model is that of maternal-fetal genetic interaction. One example of this might be genetic incompatibility. It is long-recognized that maternal-fetal cellular exchange occurs during pregnancy and birth and can have consequences for the fetus/neonate or the mother. Further, research has shown that fetal cells exist and survive long-term in the maternal system in a phenomenon known as microchimerism [13]. Thus, if the fetus expresses an allele the mother does not, resulting in a foreign antigen, a damaging maternal immune response could be activated. Conversely, maternal cells can integrate into the fetus during gestation, and survive postnatally [14]. There is also evidence that microchimeric cells may cross the blood-brain barrier [15]. As a result, if the mother expresses an allele her offspring does not, the maternal cells could present an antigen foreign to the offspring. Finally, genetic similarity at specific compatibility loci could allow for integration of foreign cells which are ultimately disruptive [16]. Several approaches for analyzing maternal-fetal incompatibility and transgenerational epistasis have previously been described [17], [18].
A well-understood example of transgenerational epistasis leading to a damaging maternal immune response in humans is Rh disease. Rh disease occurs when the fetus inherits an Rh+ allele from the father and the mother is Rh negative, causing a maternal immune response against the foreign Rh antigen expressed by the fetal blood cells. Some evidence has pointed to Rh incompatibility as a risk factor for neuropsychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia [19]. In support of this model in autism, several studies have found maternal antibodies to fetal brain antigens in serum from mothers of autistic children [20], [21]. This mechanism has also been observed in animal models. Martin et al. found that rhesus macaques infused mid-gestation with serum from human mothers of children with autism went on to have offspring with autism-like phenotypes [22].
In this study, we have used novel analyses to test specific models of maternal genetic effects in autism. We report the results of a case-control study using mother-child pairs to test novel models to investigate proband genetic main effects, maternal genetic main effects, and maternal-fetal transgenerational epistasis in autism.
Methods
Ethics Statement
The committee for the Protection of Human Subjects (CPHS), California Health and Human Services Agency, an institutional review board that operates in compliance with the Common Rule under the authority of a Federalwide Assurance, reviewed the project and granted a waiver of HIPAA authorization. A designated member of the Kaiser Permanente Northern California (KPNC) Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved this study, including waiving the requirement for informed consent and waiving the requirement that Privacy Rule authorization be obtained from study participants. The Utah State University IRB granted this project (Protocol #1548) an Exemption because the samples were pre-existing and no identifying information accompanied the samples handled at the USU laboratory. The UCSF Committee on Human Research considers this to be non-human subject research, meeting the criteria that 1) The coded private information and specimens were not collected specifically for the current research project and 2) UCSF PI and holder of the key have an agreement prohibiting the release of the key (EMA) or there are IRB-approved written policies for the repository or data management that prohibit the release of the key (replication data).
Primary study samples
Our samples are part of the Early Markers for Autism (EMA) study, which has been previously described [21], [23]. Briefly, EMA is a large, population-based, nested case-control study of autism that utilizes archived prenatal (maternal blood cell pellet) and newborn (neonatal blood spot) specimens (banked at −20°C) from mother-baby pairs. The study population derives from women in Orange, San Diego and Imperial Counties, California who were pregnant in 2000–2003 and who enrolled in the State's Prenatal Expanded Alphafetoprotein Screening Program. These participants were self-reported to be: 35% White, 42% Hispanic, 18% Asian, 3% African, 2% Other. They are well-matched between cases and controls, but to avoid population stratification we have employed empirical clustering based on genetic data (see Genetic Analysis below). Three groups of children born to these women were identified: children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), children with other developmental delay (DD) but not ASD, and general population controls (GP). Children with ASD or DD were ascertained from client records of the Regional Center of Orange County (RCOC), and San Diego Regional Center, two of the 21 Regional Centers operated by the California Department of Developmental Services (DDS) to coordinate services for persons with autism, developmental delay, and other developmental disabilities. Clients receiving DDS services for autistic disorder or for other DDS eligible conditions with suspected ASD were ascertained as possible ASD cases for this study. GP controls were randomly sampled from the birth certificate files after past or current DDS/RC clients had been excluded, and matched to ASD cases by sex, birth month and birth year.
ASD and DD diagnoses were verified by abstraction and expert review of Regional Center medical records following a protocol initially developed by the Metropolitan Atlanta Developmental Disabilities Surveillance Program [24]. Maternal mid-pregnancy (15–20 weeks gestation) venous blood specimens and newborn bloodspots were retrieved from the prenatal and newborn screening specimen archives maintained by the Genetic Disease Screening Program, California Department of Public Health. All study procedures were approved by the institutional review boards of the California Health and Human Services Agency, Kaiser Permanente Northern California, and UCSF.
DNA Extraction
Specimens to be tested were coded with a study identification number. For maternal samples, blood cell pellets collected in 4ml serum separator tubes were shipped on dry ice to the biomedical laboratory at Utah State University for processing. The cell pellets were broken up with pipette tips and about 400 ul from each sample was removed and placed into test tubes. The QIAGEN QIAamp 96 DNA Blood Kit was used following the manufacturer's protocol.
For newborn samples, one dried bloodspot (about 14 mm in diameter) collected on filter paper was entirely punched with a 3.2 mm paper punch. Since there is little DNA in a 3.2 mm paper punch, 15 to 18 punches were used for DNA extraction. Again, the QIAGEN QIAamp 96 DNA Blood Kit was used following the manufacturer's protocol. Three of the 3.2 mm punches were used per well.
The Invitrogen Quant-iT DNA Assay Kit was used to measure the DNA concentration and the spectrophotometer absorbance was measured at 260/280 and 260/230 on 3 ul of sample to determine DNA purity (Bio-Tek Synergy H instrument using the Take3 plate).
Genotyping
All maternal and neonatal samples were genotyped using the Affymetrix Axiom EUR array by the Genomics Core Facility (GCF) at UCSF, using standard protocols. The Axiom EUR array assays approximately 675,000 SNPs across the genome, and is optimized for genome-wide, gene-based, and candidate-SNP coverage [25]. Genotype calling was carried out using Affymetrix Power-Tools in accordance with the Axiom Genotyping Solution Data Analysis Guide provided by Affymetrix [26], [27].
Genotype calling and quality control were carried out separately for the neonatal bloodspot-derived DNA and maternal blood pellet DNA samples. For samples to be included in analysis, we required a genotype call rate >97%. We checked genetic relationships using PLINK's ‘–genome’ module to calculate pair-wise genetic relatedness between all individuals in our sample [28]. We used this information to correct any sample switches that were resolvable. Any additional pairs that were genetically unrelated were considered to be misidentified and excluded. We also calculated the F inbreeding coefficient for each sample and average pi-hat using PLINK [28]. Samples that were found to be outliers with F inbreeding coefficient significantly less than zero and high average pi-hat were considered contaminated.
We performed Axiom genotyping on DNA from 1,706 EMA samples. Among our neonatal bloodspot samples (N = 852), 3 (0.03%) were excluded due to failed genotyping, 36 (4.2%) were excluded due to low genotype call rate, 13 (1.5%) were excluded due to apparent contamination, 26 (3.1%) were excluded due to sample misidentification, and 10 (1.2%) were removed due to unresolved affection status resulting in 88 (10%) total excluded samples. Among our maternal blood pellet samples (N = 854), 14 (1.6%) were excluded due to low genotype call rate, 18 (2.1%) were excluded due to apparent contamination, 22 (2.6%) were excluded due to sample misidentification, and 10 (1.2%) were removed due to their child's unresolved diagnosis, resulting in 64 (7.5%) total excluded samples. The higher rate of samples excluded due to low call rate among the neonates (4.2%) compared to the mothers (1.5%) is consistent with the quality of DNA extracted from the bloodspot samples being on average lower than the quality available from the blood pellet samples. After quality control, we were left with 764 offspring samples (385 cases, 379 controls) and 790 maternal samples (390 cases, 400 controls). These samples comprise 735 complete maternal-offspring pairs (366 cases, 369 controls).
We filtered out markers with genotype call rate <97%, Fisher's Linear Discriminant (FLD) score <3.6, and Heterozygous cluster strength offset (HetSO) value <−0.1 as outlined in the Axiom Advanced Analysis Workflow. After these steps were carried out separately in the maternal and neonatal sample sets, the samples were combined and markers that were excluded in either set were subsequently excluded from transgenerational analysis. After combining the maternal and neonatal samples, we tabulated Mendelian errors of inheritance in all of our complete maternal-baby pairs and additionally excluded markers with >10 of these errors. We also excluded markers that violated Hardy Weinberg Equilibrium (HWE) in control mothers, as judged by a P-value <10−10 using the Hardy-Weinberg exact test statistic [29]. These quality control steps were carried out using PLINK [28]. Out of the 674,557 markers assayed by the Axiom EUR platform, 653,758 (97.0%) passed QC metrics in the neonatal samples and 659,993 (97.8%) passed QC metrics in the maternal samples. This is consistent with sample quality differences expected between neonatal blood spots and maternal blood pellets. 647,227 markers (96.0%) passing in both maternal and neonatal samples were included in our combined analysis.
Replication Datasets
Replication data are comprised of five datasets that have been used in previous and ongoing GWAS of autism. The first dataset is comprised of samples genotyped by the Autism Genome Project (AGP) and includes 4,074 samples; these samples were genotyped using the Illumina Human 1M-single Infinium BeadChip array [8]. The second dataset includes samples from the Autism Genetic Resource Exchange (AGRE) and the National Institute for Mental Health (NIMH, collections of DNA from multiplex families with ASD by the NIMH Autism Genetics Initiative); it includes 3,717 samples genotyped on the Affymetrix 500 k and Affymetrix 5.0 platforms [6]. The third dataset also consists of AGRE multiplex families and includes 4,327 samples. These samples were genotyped on the InfiniumII Illumina 550 k Bead Chip platform [7]. The fourth and fifth datasets contain samples collected by the Simons Simplex Collection (SSC) and include a total of 4,348 samples; 3,013 of these samples were genotyped on the Illumina 1M Duo platform, and 1,335 were genotyped on the Illumina 1 M platform [30], [31]. These two groups of samples were considered separately. Overall, we utilized 13,373 individuals (3,314 complete families) for the purposes of replicating the proband main effects, maternal main effects, and transgenerational epistatic effects we observed in the EMA discovery data (Methods S1, Table S1).
Imputation of Replication Datasets
In order to replicate our results using datasets genotyped on different array platforms, we imputed our replication samples using BEAGLE (Version 3.3.2– http://faculty.washington.edu/browning/beagle/beagle.html) with 1000genomes reference data (http://bochet.gcc.biostat.washington.edu/beagle/1000_Genomes.phase1_release_v3/) [32]. Quality control was applied to each dataset individually before and after imputation. Markers with lower than 90% imputed genotype calls were excluded from all analyses and considered to have failed imputation; markers that had 90% imputed genotype calls or greater in any subset of data were considered in analysis (Methods S2).
Genetic Analysis
In order to address population stratification in our sample, we employed the Cochran-Mantel-Haenszel (CMH) test for repeated tests of independence to compare the relative proportions of allele frequencies or binary classifiers between cases and controls. In order to stratify pairs for the purposes of this CMH test we clustered samples using PLINK's ‘–cluster’ module [28], and then classified each pair based on the maternal cluster solution for paired tests and based on the proband solution for the main effects case-control test (Methods S3). The only clustering restraint we imposed on the algorithm was cluster number (using the – K option). In order to set this tuning parameter in a non-arbitrary fashion, we chose the value of K which minimized the overall genomic inflation in the dataset, as measured by looking at the results of the CMH tests in both the neonates and the mothers. The number of clusters that turned out to minimize the genomic inflation in both tests was 9 (Figure S1).
We carried out case/control tests separately in the maternal and neonatal samples using PLINK's ‘– cmh’ module [28]. For the probands, this constitutes a case/control proband main effect GWAS for autism. For the mothers, this represents a novel genome-wide examination of maternal genetic main effects in autism, with the ‘affected’ phenotype defined by having a child with autism.
To test the hypotheses of transgenerational epistasis, we created three binary classifiers, as follows: 1) pairs where the offspring has an allele the mother does not vs. pairs where the mother possesses at least one copy of each allele present in the offspring (“Offspring Heterozygous” model), 2) pairs where the mother has an allele the offspring does not vs. pairs where the offspring possesses at least one copy of each allele present in the mother (“ Maternal Heterozygous” model), 3) pairs where the mother and offspring genotypes are identical vs. pairs where they are not identical (“Difference” model) as shown in Figure 1. A CMH test of the proportion of binary classifiers between the case and control pairs for each model was performed.
A) “Offspring Heterozygous” model where the offspring has an allele the mother does not vs. pairs where the mother possesses at least one copy of each allele present in the offspring. B) “Maternal Heterozygous” model where the offspring has an allele the mother does not vs. pairs where the mother possesses at least one copy of each allele present in the offspring. C) “Difference” model where the mother and offspring genotypes are identical vs. pairs where they are not identical.
Using the determined population strata, our genome wide CMH tests in the EMA discovery sample of proband main effects (λ = 1.002), maternal main effects (λ = 1.011), “Offspring-Heterozygous” (λ = 0.991), “Maternal-Heterozygous” (λ = 0.997), and “Difference” transgenerational epistatic effects (λ = 0.998) had no genomic inflation with accompanying q-q plots shown in Figure S2.
In order to demonstrate that maternal effects are not driven by proband main effects, and that transgenerational effects are not driven by a combination of maternal and proband main effects, we employed a multinomial maximum likelihood model (MMLM) as implemented in the EMIM software package (http://www.staff.ncl.ac.uk/richard.howey/emim) and described extensively [18], [33]. By fitting a model that allows for proband and maternal main effects and removing the maternal main effects using a likelihood ratio test (LRT), we can test whether results of the maternal main effects can be explained entirely by proband main effects. Similarly, by fitting a model that allows for maternal and proband main effects as well as transgenerational effects, then removing the transgenerational effects and comparing the model fit, we are able to test whether significant results obtained in the transgenerational epistatic models can be explained entirely by main effects.
To assess fit in our proposed transgenerational effect models, the EMIM package estimates probabilities for α to represent the baseline disease risk for a proband, R1 as the risk factor when the proband has one copy of the risk allele and R2 = R12 if two copies are present, S1 as the risk factor when the paired mother possesses a copy of the risk allele and S2 = S12 if two copies are present, and an interaction parameter to represent the transgenerational model of interest (e.g. jc for the Difference model). These parameters are estimated from the pedigree data assuming HWE and random mating. To assess fit in our proposed transgenerational effect models, we define the null model to account for neonatal and maternal genetic effects and our model of interest includes the additional interaction parameter. For example, in our Difference model where a mother is homozygous and the neonate is heterozygous, we would consider αR1S2 as the null model and contrast it to our model of interest αR1S2jc using a LRT:
Further methods and parameters are given in Methods S4, Table S2, and Table S3.
Family-based Replication
In order to replicate the results of case/control tests in the probands, we carried out transmission disequilibrium tests (using PLINK's TDT module) in available family-based autism datasets [28]. We used the same family-based data to replicate the maternal effects, performing a chi-square test of allele frequency in PLINK with the mothers considered “cases” and the fathers considered “controls” [28].
We devised equivalent tests to our transgenerational epistasis models for replication in the available family-based autism datasets. To replicate the “Offspring Heterozygous” model, we considered all affected offspring trios with heterozygous offspring and compared the number of homozygous mothers to homozygous fathers using a chi-square test. To replicate the “Mother Heterozygous” model, we considered all affected offspring trios with homozygous offspring and compared the number of heterozygous mothers to heterozygous fathers. To replicate the “Difference” model, we considered all affected offspring trios and compared the number of mothers vs. fathers with the opposite genotype as the affected proband (Methods S5, Figure S3).
Several datasets genotyped on different platforms and collected by research consortia with disparate recruitment methods were used for replication. Therefore, we carried out random-effects meta-analysis in PLINK to combine the results into one meta-replication statistic specifying P – values, odds ratios and standard errors as input for all models in each dataset [28]. For each of the five main tests (two main effect tests and three transgenerational epistasis tests) the replication P-value is from the random-effects meta-analysis statistic. In cases where an allelic combination had zero observations in any single dataset, the replication datasets were considered jointly and the merged replication P-value is presented.
Results
Proband Main Effects
We carried out a CMH test of allele frequency over 653,758 high-quality markers for the 766 neonatal samples. Our strongest associations (P<10−5) were on chromosome 8p between CSMD1 and MCPH1 (rs28374251, P = 3.48×10−6), 13q in the intron of SPATA13 (rs7331042, P = 4.22×10−6), 8q in the intron of COL22A1 (rs12680005, P = 7.01×10−6) and14q between GCH1 and WDHD1 (rs76271340, P = 9.90×10−6) (Table 1). All Proband Main Effect results with CMH P<10−4 are listed in Table S4.
Maternal Main Effects
In order to assess main effects of maternal genotype, we performed a CMH test of allele frequency over 659,993 high-quality markers for the 800 maternal samples. To separate maternal main effects from proband main effects, we carried out a comparison between multinomial maximum likelihood models (MMLMs) including both maternal and proband main effects and only proband main effects using a likelihood ratio test (LRT). We found that in 93% (54/58) of our top results under the maternal main effect test, a model which included both maternal and neonatal main effects showed a significantly better fit than a model including only neonatal main effects (LRT P-value <0.05). Our strongest associations (P<10−5) that also showed evidence for maternal main effect in our MMLM (LRT P<0.05) were on chromosome 11q in the intron of MAML2 (rs1940153, P = 2.05×10−6, LRT P = 0.00122), 17p in the intron of GAS7 (rs9895531, P = 5.45×10−6, LRT P = 4.79×10−6), 1p between PRKACB and SAMD13 (rs528615, P = 6.19×10−6, LRT P = 9.63×10−5), 17q between TMEM97 and NLK (rs2006933, P = 3.08×10−6, LRT P = 9.1×10−4) and 13q between KLF5 and KLF12 (rs8001767, P = 9.95×10−6, LRT P = 8.24×10−4) (Table 1). All Maternal Main Effect results with CMH P<10−4 are listed in Table S5.
Transgenerational Epistatic Effects
We performed novel tests of transgenerational epistatic effects, including the LRT to assess the fit of these epistatic models. Under the “Offspring Heterozygous” model of transgenerational epistasis, 72% (29/36) of top association results showed that a model which included both offspring-heterozygous and main effects constituted a significantly better fit than a model including only main effects (LRT P-value <0.05). Our most suggestive associations (P<10−5) which also showed evidence for “Offspring Heterozygous” effect in our multinomial maximum likelihood model (LRT P<0.05) were on chromosome 7q in the 3′UTR of SEMA3C (rs1527470, P = 5.34×10−6, LRT P = 0.00373), 17q in the intron of ACCN1 (rs280039, P = 7.68×10−6, LRT P = 4.69×10−3) and 18q in the intron of SLC14A2 (rs959246, P = 9.42×10−6, LRT P = 1.41×10−4) (Table 1). All Offspring Heterozygous results with CMH P<10−4 are listed in Table S6.
We found that in 71% (20/28) of our top maternal-heterozygous effect SNPs, a model which included both maternal-heterozygous and main effects constituted a significantly better fit than a model including only main effects (LRT P-value <0.05). Our most suggestive associations (P<10−5) which also showed evidence for “Maternal Heterozygous” effect in our multinomial maximum likelihood model (LRT P<0.05) were on chromosome 9q in the intron of PALM2 (rs10816846, P = 1.44×10−6, LRT P = 1.39×10−4) and on 16p in the intron of KIAA0430 (rs2071330, P = 9.69×10−6, LRT P = 0.01) (Table 1). All Maternal Heterozygous results with CMH P<10−4 are listed in Table S7.
We also found that in 78% (38/49) of our top difference effect SNPs a model which included both difference and main effects constituted a significantly better fit than a model including only main effects (LRT P-value <0.05). For the “Difference” test, our lowest P-values (P<10−5) which also showed evidence for “Difference” effect in our multinomial maximum likelihood model (likelihood ratio test P<0.05) were on chromosomes 4q between C4orf37 and UNC5C (rs28539905, P = 1.99×10−6, LRT P = 0.0206; rs7691268, P = 4.12×10−6, LRT P = 0.0453), on 11p between INSC and SOX6 (rs939046, P = 2.03×10−6, LRT P = 2.46×10−6), on 14q in the intron of SLC7A8 (rs59358210, P = 7.86×10−6, LRT P = 0.00778) and on 15q in the intron of GABRB3 (rs7171512, P = 9.40×10−6, LRT P = 0.0326) (Table 1). All Difference results with CMH P<10−4 are listed in Table S8.
Overall, we observed that the MMLM LRT corresponding to the appropriate model provided the best fit for the majority of the top SNPs from each case control test, i.e. for the top offspring-heterozygous effect SNPs a LRT including maternal-heterozygous effects did not produce a greater proportion of significant results than the LRT including the appropriate offspring-heterozygous effect.
Replication
We attempted replication of results that achieved a significance level of P<10−4 in our discovery samples using meta-analysis of five imputed family-based autism datasets (N = 3,314 complete affected trios). For the proband main effects, we found a nominal association by TDT on 2q between NXPH2 and LRP1B (rs4245867, discovery P = 1.01×10−5, replication P = 0.024) (Table S4). However, after Bonferroni correction for the number of top results taken into replication, this would not be considered significant.
By comparing maternal and paternal allele frequency (N = 7,019 mothers and fathers) to assess maternal-specific effects, we found a significant replication (meeting a Bonferroni threshold) on 6p in the intron of CD83 (rs11758033, discovery P = 4.48×105, replication P = 0.001) (Table S5).
Using maternal vs. paternal genotype mismatch equilibrium tests, we found that no nominal associations were replicated in the “Offspring Heterozygous” or in the “Maternal Heterozygous” model at P<0.05 and with an effect size in the same direction as in the discovery samples (Table 1). In the “Difference” model, we found a nominal association in the intron of TRIM2 (rs10517569, discovery P = 6.56×10−5, replication P = 0.031) (Table S8). This result is not significant considering correction for the number of tests performed in the replication dataset.
Discussion
Our analysis of proband genetic main effects constitutes a case/control GWAS of autism; previous autism studies employ primarily family-based designs [6], [8], [30], [31], with the exception of one study with both a family-based and case-control subset [7]. A case-control design well-matched for genetic ancestry has some possible advantages compared with family-based studies, including increased power to detect associations at markers with low minor allele frequency, of which we find several in our top results. Using this method we identified associations with SNPs at the P<10−4 level in or near several genes previously implicated in autism: NLGN4X, RAPGEF4, RORA, FAM135B and CNTNAP2 [34], [35], [36], [37], [38]. One SNP with nominal association in family-based samples is in a region previously reported in a rare deletion in a case with autism features – rs4245867 located between NXPH2 and LRP1B [39]. These findings are promising, but with no results approaching genome-wide significance (P<5×10−8), they are not definitive. We cannot rule out the possibility that maternal genetic risk factors make no contribution to ASD. Alternatively, our lack of conclusive results may be a direct result of our study size (385 cases and 379 controls); an analysis of power shows that we have 80% power to detect only common variants (>20% minor allele [main effects] or class [TE models] frequency) with genotype relative risk of at least 2 at genome-wide significance, or at least 2.5–3.5 for less common alleles. These effects are larger than those observed for SNP effects in most complex genetic disease, and even in our main effect proband case-control test we did not identify significant findings or replication in family-based data, despite strong evidence that common SNPs contribute to ASD risk [8]. Hence, for a true test of whether or not maternal risk factors contribute to the genetics of ASDs, a more powerful study will be required.
We conducted the first case/control GWAS of maternal genetic main effects which considers having an affected child to be the relevant phenotype. In a recent twin study of autism, Hallmayer et al. estimate that the shared environmental component of autism accounts for more of the variability in autism liability than previously appreciated [40]. Given these data, it is important to investigate whether a shared environmental component can be attributed to maternal genetic effects, which could determine the fetal environment. For our analysis of maternal main effects, we anticipated different categories of top results compared with genes represented by proband main effects, for example genes expressed in the placenta as opposed to genes expressed in the brain. The top maternal SNP was in the intron of the gene encoding mastermind-like 2, which is interesting as a candidate in this model because it is a positive regulator of notch signaling and is expressed placentally [41]. Another top signal is near KLF5, a transcription factor regulated by mastermind-like signaling and placentally expressed [42]. Surprisingly, we also identified associations with SNPs at the P<10−4 level near several genes previously implicated in autism, although not in our analysis of proband effects: PCDH9 and FOXP1 [43], [44].
Relevant to our models of maternal-fetal interaction, interestingly, one top signal is near a gene known to be immunogenic, KIAA0430 [45], and one of our significant results in the family-based data is near CD83, an immunomodulator also expressed neuronally [46]. The tests of transgenerational epistasis, particularly the “Difference” model, also identified many genes previously implicated in autism at the suggestive P<10−4 level, such as MACROD2, RORA, SDK1, GABRB3, PCDH9, NRXN1, RELN, FHIT, CNTN4 and LAMA1 [8], [36], [37], [43], [47], [48], [49], [50], [51], [52]. Given the number of reported autism candidate genes, this is not necessarily substantial, however, researchers studying the role of these genes in autism may want to consider examining parental genotype. In support of the transgenerational model, many of these top results were not driven by a proband or maternal main effect. However, no single result reached genome-wide significance or showed strong replication considering the number of tests performed. Many of the reported instances of transgenerational epistasis in animal models have involved a trans-acting effect between independent loci, in addition to a transgenerational effect [53]. In our study we lack sufficient power to search for two-way interactions genome-wide.
We have presented a novel study design and methodology for surveying transgenerational genetic effects on a genome-wide level. By using matched case and control pairs, we can potentially detect over- or under-representation of certain maternal-offspring genotype pairings in autism, and establish the directionality and magnitude of these effects. When we see a putative ‘maternal effect’, we first need to rule out that this result is simply driven by direct risk contributed by the alleles inherited by the proband, which we have done by use of a likelihood ratio test. The second possibility is that there is a main effect of maternal genotype, for example determining the prenatal developmental environment, which we have tested in our maternal main effect model. Finally, we could be observing maternal-fetal genetic interaction, which we have tested in several specific models. We believe that our methods are easy to interpret within this framework on a genome-wide scale and take full advantage of our case/control study design.
One challenge we encountered was the lack of a replication dataset that uses the same study design as our discovery samples. We were able to address this issue by implementing methods that would allow us to detect similar effects using family-based replication datasets. In order to do this, we make the assumption that maternal genetic effects will be distinct from paternal genetic effects, of which there are demonstrated examples [53]. This allows us to use the paternal genotypes and paternal-offspring genotype pairs in our family-based replication as “control” data, while the maternal counterparts are considered to be “case” data. However, this approach would fail to replicate general parental effects that act in both parents, which are also plausible [54].
While we do report nominal associations in our replication data, the number of results which were successfully replicated, as indicated by a P-value surviving Bonferroni correction and an effect size in the same direction as in the discovery data, was not more than expected. One factor in this could be the limitations of our imputed replication data. Because the design of the Axiom array is so different from the design of previous arrays, many of the SNPs it assays are poorly covered by the arrays used in our replication datasets. As a result, not all of the top discovery results had equivalent imputed genotype calls with high enough imputation quality to be included in our study, effectively reducing our ability to analyze all markers in our replication data. In our replication method for transgenerational tests this effect is even more exaggerated, since only trios where one parent is heterozygous and one is homozygous are informative. This, combined with data lost during our post-imputation QC, makes our effective replication sample size and marker coverage modest even though we used over 3,000 unique trios for replication.
In conclusion, we have presented a new model for genetic studies of autism that could also be applied to other diseases in order to search for parental genetic effects. Parental main effects of genotype and maternal-fetal incompatibility are important mechanisms to be considered in human disease, and we believe that our methods are a straightforward way to search for these effects on a genome-wide level in case-control or family-based datasets. Although we have identified some promising initial results, individual associations detected here will require replication and further study. Future study of maternal genetic effects could also consider the interaction of maternal genetics with the external environment, for example, maternal exposures during pregnancy.
Supporting Information
Figure S1.
Multidimensional-scaling plot of the EMA discovery cohort.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s001
(DOCX)
Figure S2.
Quantile-quantile plots for genome-wide application of our EMA discovery cohort tests.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s002
(DOCX)
Figure S3.
Quantile-quantile plots for genome-wide application of our transgenerational epistasis replication methods.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s003
(DOCX)
Methods S2.
Imputation of Replication Datasets.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s005
(DOCX)
Methods S3.
Stratification of the Discovery Sample.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s006
(DOCX)
Methods S4.
Multinomial Maximum Likelihood Modeling.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s007
(DOCX)
Table S2.
Parameterization of Multinomial Model.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s010
(DOCX)
Table S3.
Parameters Estimated for the Likelihood Ratio Test.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s011
(DOCX)
Table S4.
Top results (P<10−4) from the CMH test of allele frequencies in the proband samples.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s012
(DOCX)
Table S5.
Top results (P<10−4) from the CMH test of allele frequencies in the maternal samples.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s013
(DOCX)
Table S6.
Top results (P<10−4) using the “Offspring Heterozygous” model of transgenerational epistasis.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s014
(DOCX)
Table S7.
Top results (P<10−4) using the “Maternal Heterozygous” model of transgenerational epistasis.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s015
(DOCX)
Table S8.
Top results (P<10−4) using the “Difference” model of transgenerational epistasis.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076978.s016
(DOCX)
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank all of the families who have participated in and contributed to the public resources that we have used in these studies. We thank Neil Risch, Noah Zaitlen and Yambazi Banda at UCSF for helpful discussion.
The National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) collection Principal Investigators and Co-Investigators were: Neil Risch, Richard M. Myers, Donna Spiker, Linda J. Lotspeich, Joachim F. Hallmayer, Helena C. Kraemer, Roland D. Ciaranello, Luigi Luca Cavalli-Sforza (Stanford University, Stanford); William M. McMahon and P. Brent Petersen (University of Utah, Salt Lake City). The Stanford team is indebted to the parent groups and clinician colleagues who referred families and extends their gratitude to the families with individuals with autism who were partners in this research. The Autism Genetic Resource Exchange (AGRE) collection Principal Investigator is Daniel H. Geschwind (UCLA). The Co-Principal Investigators include Stanley F. Nelson and Rita M. Cantor (UCLA), Christa Lese Martin (Univ. Chicago), T. Conrad Gilliam (Columbia). Co-Investigators include Maricela Alarcon (UCLA), Kenneth Lange (UCLA), Sarah J. Spence (UCLA), David H. Ledbetter (Emory) and Hank Juo (Columbia). We gratefully acknowledge the resources provided by the AGRE Consortium and the participating AGRE families.
We are grateful to all of the families at the participating Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative (SFARI) Simplex Collection (SSC) sites, as well as the principal investigators (A. Beaudet, R. Bernier, J. Constantino, E. Cook, E. Fombonne, D. Geschwind, R. Goin-Kochel, E. Hanson, D. Grice, A. Klin, D. Ledbetter, C. Lord, C. Martin, D. Martin, R. Maxim, J. Miles, O. Ousley, K. Pelphrey, B. Peterson, J. Piggot, C. Saulnier, M. State, W. Stone, J. Sutcliffe, C. Walsh, Z. Warren, E. Wijsman). We appreciate obtaining access to phenotypic data on SFARI Base. Approved researchers can obtain the SSC dataset described in this study by applying to SFARI.
The AGP datasets used for the analysis described in this manuscript were obtained from dbGaP at http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/gap through dbGaP accession number, phs000267.v1.p1. Submission of the data, phs000267.v1.p1, to dbGaP was provided by Dr. Bernie Devlin on behalf of the Autism Genome Project (AGP).
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: LAW LAC GCW GND MK. Performed the experiments: KMT. Analyzed the data: CKY GCW GND LAW KMT. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: ART LAW GND MK. Wrote the paper: LAW KMT LAC GCW GND MK OZ.
References
- 1. Folstein S, Rutter M (1977) Infantile Autism: A Genetic Study of 21 Twin Pairs. Jour of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 18: 297–321.
- 2. Steffenburg S, Gillberg C, Hellgren L, Andersson L, Gillberg IC, et al. (1989) A Twin Study of Autism in Denmark, Finland, Iceland, Norway and Sweden. Jour of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 30: 405–416.
- 3. Ritvo ER, Freeman BJ, Mason-Brothers A, Mo A, Ritvo AM (1985) Concordance for the syndrome of autism in 40 pairs of afflicted twins. Am J Psychiatry 142: 74–7.
- 4. Mason-Brothers A, Ritvo ER, Pingree C, Petersen PB, Jenson WR, et al. (1990) The UCLA-University of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal factors. Pediatrics 86: 514–9.
- 5. Miles JH (2011) Autism spectrum disorders – a genetics review. Genet Med 13: 278–94.
- 6. Weiss LA, Arking DE (2009) Gene Discovery Project of Johns Hopkins & the Autism Consortium, Daly MJ, Chakravarti A (2009) A genome-wide linkage and association scan reveals novel loci for autism. Nature 461: 802–8.
- 7. Wang K, Zhang H, Ma D, Bucan M, Glessner JT, et al. (2009) Common genetic variants on 5p14.1 associate with autism spectrum disorders. Nature 459: 528–33.
- 8. Anney R, Klei L, Pinto D, Regan R, Conroy J, et al. (2010) A genome-wide scan for common alleles affecting risk for autism. Hum Mol Genet 19: 4072–82.
- 9. Nadeau JH (2009) Transgenerational genetic effects on phenotypic variation and disease risk. Hum Mol Genet 18: R202–10.
- 10. Parkhurst SM, Ish-Horowicz D (1991) Wimp, a dominant maternal-effect mutation, reduces transcription of a specific subset of segmentation genes in Drosophila. Genes Dev 5: 341–57.
- 11. Volcik KA, Blanton SH, Tyerman GH, Jong ST, Rott EJ, et al. (2000) Methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase and spina bifida: evaluation of level of defect and maternal genotypic risk in Hispanics. Am J Med Genet 95: 21–7.
- 12. Godbole K, Gayathri P, Ghule S, Sasirekha BV, Kanitkar-Damle A, et al. (2011) Maternal one-carbon metabolism, MTHFR and TCN2 genotypes and neural tube defects in India. Birth Defects Research Part A: Clinical and Molecular Teratology 91: 848–856.
- 13. Groër MW, Manion M, Szekeres C, El-Badri NS (2011) Fetal microchimerism and women's health: a new paradigm. Biol Res Nurs 13: 346–50.
- 14. Maloney S, Smith A, Furst DE, Myerson D, Rupert K, et al. (1999) Microchimerism of maternal origin persists into adult life. J Clin Invest 104: 41–7.
- 15. Tan XW, Liao H, Sun L, Okabe M, Xiao ZC, et al. (2005) Fetal microchimerism in the maternal mouse brain: a novel population of fetal progenitor or stem cells able to cross the blood-brain barrier? Stem Cells 23: 1443–52.
- 16. Nelson JL, Furst DE, Maloney S, Gooley T, Evans PC, et al. (1998) Microchimerism and HLA-compatible relationships of pregnancy in scleroderma. Lancet 351: 559–62.
- 17. Sinsheimer JS, Palmer CG, Woodward JA (2003) Detecting genotype combinations that increase risk for disease: maternal-fetal genotype incompatibility test. Genet Epidemiol 24: 1–13.
- 18. Ainsworth HF, Unwin J, Jamison DL, Cordell HJ (2011) Investigation of maternal effects, maternal-fetal interactions and parent-of-origin effects (imprinting), using mothers and their offspring. Genet Epidemiol 35: 19–45.
- 19. Palmer CG, Turunen JA, Sinsheimer JS, Minassian S, Paunio T, et al. (2002) RHD maternal-fetal genotype incompatibility increases schizophrenia susceptibility. Am J Hum Genet 71: 1312–9.
- 20. Braunschweig D, Ashwood P, Krakowiak P, Hertz-Picciotto I, Hansen R, et al. (2008) Autism: maternally derived antibodies specific for fetal brain proteins. Neurotoxicology 29: 226–31.
- 21. Croen LA, Braunschweig D, Haapanen L, Yoshida CK, Fireman B, et al. (2008) Maternal mid-pregnancy autoantibodies to fetal brain protein: the early markers for autism study. Biol Psychiatry 64: 583–8.
- 22. Martin LA, Ashwood P, Braunschweig D, Cabanlit M, Van de Water J, et al. (2008) Stereotypies and hyperactivity in rhesus monkeys exposed to IgG from mothers of children with autism. Brain Behav Immun 22: 806–16.
- 23. Croen LA, Goines P, Braunschweig D, Yolken R, Yoshida CK, et al. (2008) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and autism: maternal and infant peripheral blood levels in the Early Markers for Autism (EMA) Study. Autism Res 1: 130–7.
- 24. Yeargin-Allsopp M, Rice C, Karapurkar T, Doernberg N, Boyle C, et al. (2003) Prevalence of autism in a US metropolitan area. J of the Am Med Ass 289: 49–55.
- 25. Hoffmann TJ, Kvale MN, Hesselson SE, Zhan Y, Aquino C, et al. (2011) Next generation genome-wide association tool: design and coverage of a high-throughput European-optimized SNP array. Genomics 98: 79–89.
- 26.
Affymetrix Power Tools, Affymetrix Website. Available: http://www.affymetrix.com/partners_programs/programs/developer/tools/powertools.affx. Accessed 2012 June 7.
- 27.
Axiom Genotyping Solution Data Analysis Guide, Affymetrix Website. Available: http://media.affymetrix.com/support/downloads/manuals/axiom_genotyping_solution_analysis_guide.pdf. Accessed 2012 June 7.
- 28. Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MAR, et al. (2007) PLINK: a toolset for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analysis. Am J Hum Genet 8: 559–75.
- 29. Wigginton JE, Cutler DJ, Abecasis GR (2005) A Note on Exact Tests of Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium. Am J Hum Genet 76: 887–93.
- 30. Fischbach GD, Lord C (2010) The Simons Simplex Collection: a resource for identification of autism genetic risk factors. Neuron 68: 192–5.
- 31. Sanders SJ, Ercan-Sencicek AG, Hus V, Luo R, Murtha MT, et al. (2011) Multiple recurrent de novo CNVs, including duplications of the 7q11.23 Williams syndrome region, are strongly associated with autism. Neuron 70: 863–85.
- 32. Browning BL, Browning SR (2009) A unified approach to genotype imputation and haplotype phase inference for large data sets of trios and unrelated individuals. Am J Hum Genet 84: 210–223.
- 33. Howey R, Cordell HJ (2012) PREMIM and EMIM: tools for estimation of maternal, imprinting and interaction effects using multinomial modelling. BMC Bioinformatics 13: 149.
- 34. Jamain S, Quach H, Betancur C, Råstam M, Colineaux C, et al. (2003) Paris Autism Research International Sib Pair Study. Mutations of the X-linked genes encoding neuroligins NLGN3 and NLGN4 are associated with autism. Nat Genet 34: 27–9.
- 35. Bacchelli E, Blasi F, Biondolillo M, Lamb JA, Bonora E, et al. (2003) International Molecular Genetic Study of Autism Consortium (IMGSAC). Screening of nine candidate genes for autism on chromosome 2q reveals rare nonsynonymous variants in the cAMP-GEFII gene. Mol Psychiatry 8: 916–24.
- 36. Nguyen A, Rauch TA, Pfeifer GP, Hu VW (2010) Global methylation profiling of lymphoblastoid cell lines reveals epigenetic contributions to autism spectrum disorders and a novel autism candidate gene, RORA, whose protein product is reduced in autistic brain. FASEB J 24: 3036–51.
- 37. Connolly JJ, Glessner JT, Hakonarson H (2013) A Genome-Wide Association Study of Autism Incorporating Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, and Social Responsiveness Scale. Child Development 84: 17–33.
- 38. Arking DE, Cutler DJ, Brune CW, Teslovich TM, West K, et al. (2008) A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. Am J Hum Genet 82: 160–4.
- 39. Mulatinho MV, de Carvalho Serao CL, Hardekopf D, Pekova S, Mrasek K, et al. (2012) Severe intellectual disability, omphalocele, hypospadia and high blood pressure associated to a deletion at 2q22.1–q22.3: case report. Mol Cytogenet 5: 30.
- 40. Hallmayer J, Cleveland S, Torres A, Phillips J, Cohen B, et al. (2011) Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68: 1095–102.
- 41. Lin SE, Oyama T, Nagase T, Harigaya K, Kitagawa M (2002) Identification of new human mastermind proteins defines a family that consists of positive regulators for notch signaling. J Biol Chem 277: 50612–20.
- 42. Narayanan G, Poonepalli A, Chen J, Sankaran S, Hariharan S, et al. (2012) Single-cell mRNA profiling identifies progenitor subclasses in neurospheres. Stem Cells Dev 21: 3351–62.
- 43. Marshall CR, Noor A, Vincent JB, Lionel AC, Feuk L, et al. (2008) Structural variation of chromosomes in autism spectrum disorder. Am J Hum Genet 82: 477–88.
- 44. O'Roak BJ, Deriziotis P, Lee C, Vives L, Schwartz JJ, et al. (2011) Exome sequencing in sporadic autism spectrum disorders identifies severe de novo mutations. Nat Genet 43: 585–9.
- 45. Dunster K, Lai FP, Sentry JW (2005) Limkain b1, a novel human autoantigen localized to a subset of ABCD3 and PXF marked peroxisomes. Clin Exp Immunol 140: 556–63.
- 46. Glezer I, Bittencourt JC, Rivest S (2009) Neuronal expression of Cd36, Cd44, and Cd38 antigen transcripts maps to distinct and specific murine brain circuits. J Comp Neurol 517: 906–24.
- 47. Delahanty RJ, Kang JQ, Brune CW, Kistner EO, Courchesne E, et al. (2011) Maternal transmission of a rare GABRB3 signal peptide variant is associated with autism. Mol Psychiatry 16: 86–96.
- 48. Feng J, Schroer R, Yan J, Song W, Yang C, et al. (2006) High frequency of neurexin 1beta signal peptide structural variants in patients with autism. Neurosci Letters 409: 10–3.
- 49. Persico AM, D'Agruma L, Maiorano N, Totaro A, Militerni R, et al. (2001) Collaborative Linkage Study of Autism. Reelin gene alleles and haplotypes as a factor predisposing to autistic disorder. Mol Psychiatry 6: 150–9.
- 50. Sebat J, Lakshmi B, Malhotra D, Troge J, Lese-Martin C, et al. (2007) Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism. Science 316: 445–9.
- 51. Roohi J, Montagna C, Tegay DH, Palmer LE, DeVincent C, et al. (2008) Disruption of contactin 4 in three subjects with autism spectrum disorder. J Med Genet 46: 176–82.
- 52. Anney R, Klei L, Pinto D, Almeida J, Bacchelli E, et al. (2012) Individual common variants exert weak effects on the risk for autism spectrum disorderspi. Hum Mol Genet 21: 4781–92.
- 53. Nadeau JH (2009) Transgenerational genetic effects on phenotypic variation and disease risk. Review Hum Mol Genet 18: R202–10.
- 54. Rogenhofer N, Engels L, Bogdanova N, Tüttelmann F, Markoff A, et al. (2012) Paternal and maternal carriage of the annexin A5 M2 haplotype are equal risk factors for recurrent pregnancy loss: a pilot study. Fertil Steril 98: 383–8.
- 55. Kraus DM, Elliott GS, Chute H, Horan T, Pfenninger KH, et al. (2006) CSMD1 is a novel multiple domain complement-regulatory protein highly expressed in the central nervous system and epithelial tissues. J Immunol 176: 4419–30.
- 56. Ozgen HM, van Daalen E, Bolton PF, Maloney VK, Huang S, et al. (2009) Copy number changes of the microcephalin 1 gene (MCPH1) in patients with autism spectrum disorders. Clin Genet 76: 348–56.
- 57.
Passemard S, Kaindl AM, Titomanlio L, Gerard B, Gressens P, et al.. (2009) Primary Autosomal Recessive Microcephaly. Gene Reviews PMID: 20301772.
- 58. Bristow JM, Sellers MH, Majumdar D, Anderson B, Hu L, et al. (2009) The Rho-family GEF Asef2 activates Rac to modulate adhesion and actin dynamics and thereby regulate cell migration. J Cell Sci 122: 4535–46.
- 59. Becker JA, Befort K, Blad C, Filliol D, Ghate A, et al. (2008) Transcriptome analysis identifies genes with enriched expression in the mouse central extended amygdala. Neuroscience 156: 950–65.
- 60. Koch M, Schulze J, Hansen U, Ashwodt T, Keene DR, et al. (2004) A novel marker of tissue junctions, collagen XXII. J Biol Chem 279: 22514–21.
- 61. Ichinose H, Nomura T, Sumi-Ichinose C (2008) Metabolism of tetrahydrobiopterin: its relevance in monoaminergicneurons and neurological disorders. Chem Rec 8: 378–85.
- 62. Hsieh CL, Lin CL, Liu H, Chang YJ, Shih CJ, et al. (2011) WDHD1 modulates the post-transcriptional step of the centromeric silencing pathway. Nucleic Acids Res 39: 4048–62.
- 63. Chao CC, Chang PY, Lu HH (2005) Human Gas7 isoforms homologous to mouse transcripts differentially induce neurite outgrowth. J Neurosci Res 81: 153–62.
- 64. Kohannim O, Hibar DP, Stein JL, Jahanshad N, Hua X, et al. (2012) Discovery and Replication of Gene Influences on Brain Structure Using LASSO Regression Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Front Neurosci 6: 115.
- 65. Wilcox CB, Feddes GO, Willett-Brozick JE, Hsu LC, DeLoia JA, et al. (2007) Coordinate up-regulation of TMEM97 and cholesterol biosynthesis genes in normal ovarian surface epithelial cells treated with progesterone: implications for pathogenesis of ovarian cancer. BMC Cancer 7: 223.
- 66. Kutzleb C, Petrasch-Parwez E, Kilimann MW (2006) Paralemmin, a Prenyl-Palmitoyl–anchored Phosphoprotein Abundant in Neurons and Implicated in Plasma Membrane Dynamics and Cell Process Formation Histochem. Cell Biol 127: 13–30.
- 67. Izaki T, Kamakura S, Kohjima M, Sumimoto H (2006) Two forms of human Inscuteable-related protein that links Par3 to the Pins homologues LGN and AGS3. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 341: 1001–6.
- 68. Azim E, Jabaudon D, Fame RM, Macklis JD (2009) SOX6 controls dorsal progenitor identity and interneuron diversity during neocortical development. Nat Neurosci 12: 1238–47.