Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Psychological Processes Mediate the Impact of Familial Risk, Social Circumstances and Life Events on Mental Health

Abstract

Background

Despite widespread acceptance of the ‘biopsychosocial model’, the aetiology of mental health problems has provoked debate amongst researchers and practitioners for decades. The role of psychological factors in the development of mental health problems remains particularly contentious, and to date there has not been a large enough dataset to conduct the necessary multivariate analysis of whether psychological factors influence, or are influenced by, mental health. This study reports on the first empirical, multivariate, test of the relationships between the key elements of the biospychosocial model of mental ill-health.

Methods and Findings

Participants were 32,827 (age 18–85 years) self-selected respondents from the general population who completed an open-access online battery of questionnaires hosted by the BBC. An initial confirmatory factor analysis was performed to assess the adequacy of the proposed factor structure and the relationships between latent and measured variables. The predictive path model was then tested whereby the latent variables of psychological processes were positioned as mediating between the causal latent variables (biological, social and circumstantial) and the outcome latent variables of mental health problems and well-being. This revealed an excellent fit to the data, S-B χ2 (3199, N = 23,397) = 126654·8, p<·001; RCFI = ·97; RMSEA = ·04 (·038–·039). As hypothesised, a family history of mental health difficulties, social deprivation, and traumatic or abusive life-experiences all strongly predicted higher levels of anxiety and depression. However, these relationships were strongly mediated by psychological processes; specifically lack of adaptive coping, rumination and self-blame.

Conclusion

These results support a significant revision of the biopsychosocial model, as psychological processes determine the causal impact of biological, social, and circumstantial risk factors on mental health. This has clear implications for policy, education and clinical practice as psychological processes such as rumination and self-blame are amenable to evidence-based psychological therapies.

Introduction

Mental Health and Well-being

Mental health problems affect one person in every four, making them the leading cause of disability [1] and costing an estimated $2,500 billion worldwide in 2010 [2]. The origins and phenomenology of mental disorder have provoked debate amongst researchers and practitioners for decades [3]. This is despite widespread reference to the ‘biopsychosocial model’ [4], which assumes that biological, social (environmental), circumstantial (life events), and psychological factors are all important in the aetiology of mental health problems.

It is universally accepted that biology, the environment, and adverse life events collectively cause mental problems [4]. But the precise relationship between these variables is of theoretical importance and imperative for developing effective treatment, yet continues to remain a matter of pointed scientific and professional debate [3]. One critique of the biopsychosocial model is that it fails to clarify the nature of the interrelationships between each component in the model [5]. In particular, there is little agreement over how psychological processes (e.g. behaviours, thoughts, and emotions) are implicated.

From a biological perspective, mental health problems result from genetically transmitted physical abnormalities [6], along with the additive effects of negative life-events and environmental factors, which then subsequently affect psychological functioning [7]. Genetically transmitted biological factors act via complex epigenetic interactions between genes and environmental influences from conception into adulthood which include biological (e.g. maternal stress, nutritional deficiency) as well as social (e.g. abuse, neglect, social deprivation) factors [8]. These gene-environment interactions lead to observable biochemical, structural, and functional changes in the brain [9]. However, the precise identity, nature, and function of the genes involved have yet to be identified and the effects on the brain have never been reliably demonstrated. There is also unequivocal evidence that environmental factors (e.g. poverty, unemployment, social exclusion) and a range of life events (e.g. sexual, emotional, and physical abuse) have strong associations with mental health problems [10] although, again, the precise mechanisms by which their influence accrues is not clear.

Scholarly dispute is most evident in differing accounts of the role played by psychological factors [5]. There is a wealth of evidence that core processes such as reasoning ability, thinking styles, and behaviour are important in the development and maintenance of all mental health problems [5]. Thinking styles such as self-blame and rumination are two examples of psychological processes most commonly implicated across a wide range of mental health problems [11], [12]. However, biomedical approaches suggest that biological factors have a dominant position in the cause of mental health problems and thus they are the direct result of genes or gene-environment interactions. This implies that psychological factors are symptoms or consequences of these illnesses [7], [13].

The alternative to the strictly biological view is that biological factors, social factors and other environmental or life events lead to mental health problems through their conjoint effects on psychological processes, and these are the final common pathway to mental ill-health [5] (see figure 1). This has major implications for treatment, as it would place far greater importance on evidence based psychological interventions; whereas to date, such approaches are regarded as peripheral extras to pharmacology.

thumbnail
Figure 1. The hypothesized relationships between elements of the biopsychosocial model from Kinderman 2005.

These formed the basis for our covariance modeling.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076564.g001

Here we report on the first empirical test of the relationships between the key elements of the biospychosocial model of mental ill-health based on a representative population sample and using structural equation modeling (SEM).

Methods

Ethics Statement

This study complies with the guidelines of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical approval was obtained by the University of Liverpool’s School of Population, Community and Behavioral Science Research Ethics Committee May 2009.

Participants.

Participants were 32,827 (age 18–85 years) self-selected respondents to an open-access online battery of questionnaires (“The Stress Test”), approved by the University of Liverpool Committee on Research Ethics and conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. In order to determine if the sample was representative of the UK population, where comparable demographic data existed, UK respondents were compared to national data [14] for England and Wales to reveal that more respondents were white, had slightly higher earnings, and were better educated than the general population, although were comparable on other demographic features. The regional breakdown was also similar to other major health surveys [15]. Demographic details are summarised in table 1.

Procedure.

The Stress Test was promoted via multi-media formats (TV, radio and online) and launched on BBC Radio 4′s ‘All in the Mind; a flagship documentary focusing on issues of the human mind. The test’s URL [www.bbc.co.uk/labuk/experiments/stress/] was publicized on radio and TV broadcasts and made available via BBC web pages and social media. The test had 12 sections, which took approximately 20 minutes to complete in total. Questionnaire items were completed in a fixed order and answers selected from a drop-down menu. Some tasks were constrained within time limits. On completion, an overview of scores was displayed on a results home-page and URL links for comprehensive and tailored feedback based on test scores were presented. Once completed, participants were not permitted to re-take the test.

Measures.

Measures were selected on the basis of theoretical principles and empirical research to provide indicators of latent constructs representative of the components of the biopsychosocial model [4], [5]. The measurement battery was designed by authors PK and ST, and developed by all authors in collaboration with BBC Lab UK. Demographic data collected included: age; gender; ethnic group; occupation; gross annual or weekly household earnings; highest level of formal schooling; occupational status; parents’ income; relationship status; and number of children. Measured variables to represent the biological component of the theoretical model were a yes/no response to indicate participants’ reports of familial mental health diagnoses by a psychiatrist or GP [16], and performance on two cognitive tests to detect response to negative feedback and negative and positive stimuli. These were the ‘delayed match to sample’ and the ‘affective go no go’ tasks adapted from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery [17], [18], [19]. The social inclusion component of the model was represented by an 11-item questionnaire indicating social relationships with friends and family, and participation in social activities [20]. These were a combination of Likert scale and yes/no responses.

Indicators of the circumstantial component included recent life events measured using the List of Threatening Experiences Questionnaire [21]. and historical life events measured using a 5-point Likert scale of which participants indicated if they believed they had historically been physically, sexually, or emotionally abused, or bullied at school [22]. The first of the two key psychological processes, response style, was measured using an adapted Response Style Questionnaire [12], where participants indicated on a Likert scale their response to stressful situations from a list of coping strategies pertaining to rumination, problem solving/adaptive, or dangerous activities. The second, attributional style, was measured using a modified version of the Internal, Personal and Situational Attributions Questionnaire [23] to determine the degree to which individuals generate internal, personal, or situational causes for hypothetical negative events. Finally, mental health problems were assessed by the Goldberg Anxiety and Depression Scales [24] and the BBC Well-being Scale [25].

Results

Data Analysis

SEM relies on the identification and subsequent analysis of latent variables or factors [26], which represent underlying theoretical constructs that cannot directly be measured, to explore and test the simultaneous patterns of causal influence and response among multiple variables [27].

A two-step analytical approach was used, conducted using the EQS structural equation modeling (SEM) program [28]. First, missing data were deleted listwise, yielding complete data on 19,966 participants (retention of 60·8% of the original sample). However, the neurocognitive data accounted for a large proportion of missing data due to the invalid recording of data, likely due to technical error, With the exclusion of the neurocognitive data, listwise deletion of the remaining variables provided a sample of 27,397 (retention of 83% of the original sample). Analysis revealed no significant differences between those with and without missing data on demographic variables and a selection of measured variables.

Because of the multivariate kurtosis in the data, goodness of fit of models was evaluated with the adjusted robust comparative fit index (RCFI) based on the Satorra-Bentler χ2 statistic [29]. There is no absolute consensus on these matters, so, in accordance with more conservative recommendations [26], we used a ratio of χ2 to degrees of freedom of less than 2·0, a comparative fit index of greater than ·90 [30], and a Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) of less than ·05 [30]. As the χ2statistic is dependent on sample size, and likely to reject well-fitting models in large samples such as ours, we therefore concentrated on RCFI and the RMSEA indices to establish the validity of the model. An initial confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed to assess the adequacy of the proposed factor structure and the relationships between latent and measured variables [31]. Where the hypothesised factor structure yielded an inadequate fit of the data, model modifications were made consistent with theoretical and conceptual assumptions of the measured variables [32].

In the development of the model path, elimination was monitored via successive improvement of the χ2, RCFI, and RMSEA statistics. This ‘measurement model’ phase of analysis will be reported in detail elsewhere (“Establishing the construct validity and factor structure of latent psychosocial variables in psychiatric research”, Pontin et al., submitted). Once the factor structure was established, the predictive path model was tested whereby the latent variables of psychological processes were positioned as a mediating variable between the causal latent variables (biological, social and circumstantial) and the outcome latent variables of mental health problems and well-being.

Direct and Mediated Paths to Well-being and Mental Health Problems

The initial CFA established that we had a robust measurement model with latent factors comprising all of the key components of the hypothesis under test, S-B χ2 (3,199, N = 27,397) = 126,654·8, p<·001; RCFI = ·97; RMSEA = ·04 (·038–·039). These latent factors are listed in tables 2, 3, 4 and 5 together with their standardised factor loadings, and can also be seen in figure 2.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Psychological processes mediate the impact of familial risk, social circumstances and life events on mental health.

Results of a structural equation model testing the mediating effects of the psychological processes of response style and self-blame on the contribution of familial mental health history, relationship status, income and education, social inclusion and life events on mental health problems and well-being, with S-B χ2 (3,199, N = 27,397) = 126,654·8, p<·001; RCFI = ·97; RMSEA = ·04 (·038–·039). The path diagram shows completely standardised robust parameter estimates which represent the relative contribution of each latent factor to the model. All coefficients are statistically significant, p<·001. Latent factors are represented by ovals. The double headed arrow between mental health problems and well-being represents the correlations between these latent constructs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076564.g002

thumbnail
Table 2. Measured variables and latent factors (causal factors).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076564.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Measured variables and latent factors (mediating psychological factors).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076564.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Measured variables and latent factors (mental health problems).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076564.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Measured variables and latent factors (well-being).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076564.t005

Structural Model: Direct and Mediated Paths to Well-being and Mental Health Problems

The second step in the analysis tested how the latent factors revealed in the CFA to represent key elements of the biopsychosocial model [4] were related to mental health problems and well-being, and to test the hypothesised mediating role of psychological processes [5].

Initially, we tested a default model, exploring the relationships between putative causal factors (familial mental health history, relationship status, income and education, social inclusion and life events) with well-being and mental health problems, without the mediating role of psychological processes. This revealed a poor fit to the data, χ2 (3,205, N = 27,397) = 168355·3, p<·001; RCFI = ·78; RMSEA = ·04 (·043–044).

Next, we used SEM to test a model with the same latent factor predictors, but including the hypothesised mediating role of psychological processes (see figure 2), and conducted on the 23,397 participants with complete datasets for these variables. This revealed an excellent fit to the data, anxiety, S-B χ2 (3199, N = 23,397) = 126654·8, p<·001; RCFI = ·97; RMSEA = ·04 (·038–·039). All parameter estimates are shown in figure 2.

Structural equation models can be used to infer causality more robustly than conventional correlational analyses, as they account for interactions between factors [27]. In our results, there was a significantly improved model fit following the insertion of the psychological processes factor as a mediator of the relationship between the known causal factors and mental health and well being. Exploration of the direct and mediated paths also strongly supports the significant mediating role of psychological processes in the causation of mental health problems (see table 6) and poorer well-being (see table 7), illustrated by the strength of the mediator expressed in the path parameters.

thumbnail
Table 6. Direct and mediated predictors of mental health problems.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076564.t006

These results show that life events (childhood abuse and bullying, and stressful life events in adulthood) were the strongest direct predictors of mental health problems (depression and anxiety). A familial history of mental health problems and social status (income and education) were the next most significant direct predictors of mental health problems - and here it should be remembered that genetic or biological factors are not the only vectors for the familial transmission of mental health problems [3]. Social inclusion and relationship status were also significant direct predictors of mental health problems.

As hypothesised, however, the key psychological processes of response style and self-blame were significant mediators of all these paths. The overall fit of the model - its ability to explain the data reported in this population – was significantly improved by the inclusion of psychological processes as mediators in the hypothesised relationship between biological factors, life events, and environmental challenges, and mental health and well being. Moreover, life events and familial mental health history were the most significant direct predictors of mental health problems. However, the causal pathways involving the mediation of response style and self-blame were stronger predictors than direct paths. This was also true for the (smaller) effect of relationship status. The direct effects of social status (income and education) and social inclusion on mental health problems remained more significant than the mediated routes, but in each case there was a significant mediation effect.

A broadly similar pattern was observed in the prediction of well-being. Again, life events were the strongest predictors of well-being, followed by a familial history of mental health problems and social inclusion. Again, psychological processes were very significant mediating factors. This mediation effect was most significant in the path involving life-circumstances and social status (income and education).

Discussion

Social Determinants of Mental Health Problems, and Psychological Therapies

Our results demonstrate that psychological processes of response style (specifically a greater tendency to ruminate) and self-blame (or an internal attributional style for negative events) powerfully determine the impact of familial histories of mental health problems, life events and traumas, and social deprivation in the aetiology of depression and anxiety and in the maintenance of well-being. This study is the first multivariate empirical test of specific and previously published hypotheses [5] about the role of psychological processes as mediators in a revision of the ubiquitous bio-psycho-social model [4]. Our access to this unprecedented and large data set has allowed clear dissection of the inter-connections between factors, and in particular, has permitted analysis of the specific mediating effect of psychological factors. Our results clearly support the contention that biological, social, and circumstantial causal agents affect our mental health and well-being through their impact on how we process information and perceive the world. In this study, life events constituted the most significant direct causal factor, and two key processes – self-blame and response style – significantly mediated all causal pathways.

Our results did not support a fully mediated model (that is, with no residual direct effects), but this is entirely unsurprising. Mental health and well-being can be safely assumed to be the result of a huge number of causal factors with a large number of mediating psychological processes. In this study, we examined only two of the very many psychological processes hypothesised to be important in mental health. Nevertheless, we are confident both that these findings are themselves robust and that other psychological processes would also act as mediators in causal paths similar to those revealed here.

The present study was designed as an empirical test of a hypothesised set of relationships derived from previously published theoretical research [5]. Because of this, and for practical reasons, we reduced the huge complexity of mental health to a testable model of linear relationships between a limited number of variables. Mental health problems, like all other clinical conditions, can be understood on many simultaneous levels, incorporating genetic, metabolic, cellular, systemic bodily, personal, social anthropological and spiritual dimensions. We did not, for example, address issues concerning individuals’ understanding of their own mental health issues [33], nor did we dissect the complex relationships between genetics, heritability and family history [34]. Further research is clearly required to explore how the detailed pathway from genetics through neurocognitive processes on the one hand, and interpersonal and interpretative frameworks on the other, link to mental health outcomes. Anxiety and depression are recognised as two major dimensions underlying common mental health problems, but there are clearly very many more recognised psychological difficulties. Further research could also explore whether different psychological mechanisms mediate the pathways from either specific or generic causal and risk factors to different mental health problems.

There was also a potential element of self-selection in the present study, given the recruitment strategy and the on-line methodology. However, although more of our participants were white, had slightly higher earnings, and were better educated than the England and Wales average [14], there was broad comparability with other national demographic data, with a similar regional breakdown to other major health surveys [15].

These results support a significant revision of the biopsychosocial model. Instead of regarding these three causal agents as co-equal partners in the aetiology of mental health problems, these results demonstrate that the impact of physical and social causes on mental health and well-being outcomes is mediated by psychological processes. In other words, psychological processes determine the causal impact of biological, social, and circumstantial risk factors.

These findings and this interpretation have significant implications. Reductionist biological accounts of mental health have been robustly criticised on scientific, ethical, and practical grounds [3]. An alternative, scientifically valid, model may have implications for policy, education and clinical practice [5], [33]. Psychological processes such as rumination and self-blame are amenable to evidence-based psychological therapy [35]. Significant gains in mental health are achieved when people experiencing mental health problems are supported in achieving greater control over their own psychological processes [36]. A clear understanding of the role of psychological processes in the aetiology of mental health problems and the maintenance of well-being is an important step in that process. Further research in this area should include further validation of this model (particularly through prospective studies), careful consideration of the interactions between causal factors (particularly biological factors) and the mediating role of psychological mechanisms.

Acknowledgments

We thank staff of BBC LabUK for their support for the development and conduct of the study, and J. Rees for critical discussions and reading of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: PK EP ST. Performed the experiments: PK EP. Analyzed the data: PK MS EP. Wrote the paper: PK MS EP ST.

References

  1. 1. World Health Organization (2011) Global Status Report on Non-Communicable Diseases 2010. Geneva: World Health Organization.
  2. 2. Bloom DE (2011) The Global Economic Burden of Noncommunicable Diseases. Geneva: World Economic Forum.
  3. 3. Bentall RP (2003) Madness Explained: Psychosis and Human Nature. London: Allen Lane.
  4. 4. Engel G (1977) The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science 196: 129–136.
  5. 5. Kinderman P (2005) A psychological model of mental disorder. Harvard Rev Psychiat 13: 206–217.
  6. 6. Sullivan PF, Neale MC, Kendler KS (2000) Genetic epidemiology of major depression: review and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 157: 1552–1562.
  7. 7. Guze SB (1989) Biological psychiatry: is there any other kind? Psychol Med 19: 315–23.
  8. 8. Van Os J, Kenis G, Rutten BPF (2010) The environment and schizophrenia. Nature 468: 203–212.
  9. 9. Maguire EA, Gadian DG, Johnsrude IS, Good CD, Ashburner J, et al. (2000) Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Pro Natl Acad Sci USA 97: 4398–4403.
  10. 10. Marmot MG, Wilkinson RG(2006) Social Determinants of Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  11. 11. Buchanan GM, Seligman MEP (1995) Explanatory Style. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  12. 12. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, Lyubomirsky S (2008) Rethinking rumination. Perspec Psychol Sci 3: 400–424.
  13. 13. Pilgrim D (2002) The biopsychosocial model in Anglo-American psychiatry: past, present and future? JMH 1: 585–594.
  14. 14. Office for National Statistics (2003) Labour Force Survey: Household Datasets, Spring Quarter. London: Office for National Statistics.
  15. 15. Health Survey of England (2011) Health, social care and lifestyles. London: Office for National Statistics.
  16. 16. Milne BJ (2008) The validity of the family history screen for assessing family history of mental disorders. Am J Med Genet B 50: 41–9.
  17. 17. Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, Herrod JJ, Robbins TW, Paykel ES (1997) Abnormal response to negative feedback in unipolar depression: Evidence for a diagnosis specific impairment. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 63: 7482.
  18. 18. Murphy FC (1999) Emotional bias and inhibitory control processes in mania and depression. Psychol Med 29: 1307–1321.
  19. 19. Chamberlain SR, Sahakian BJ (2006) The neuropsychology of mood disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep 8: 458–463.
  20. 20. National Development Team for Social Inclusion. Reviewing Measures of Social Exclusion. Available socialinclusion.org.uk/publications/AGuidetoInclusion Measuresdisseminationversion.doc. Accessed 4 May 4 2012.
  21. 21. Brugha TS, Cragg D (1990) The list of threatening experiences: The reliability and validity of a brief life events questionnaire. Acta Psychiat Scand 82: 77–81.
  22. 22. Bernstein DP, Fink L (1994) Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. Am J Psych 151: 1132–6.
  23. 23. Kinderman P, Bentall RP (1996) A new measure of causal locus: The internal, personal and situational attributions questionnaire. Pers Ind Differ 20: 261–4.
  24. 24. Goldberg D, Bridges K, Duncan P, Grayson D (1998) Detecting anxiety and depression in general medical settings. BMA 297: 897–9.
  25. 25. Kinderman P, Schwannauer M, Pontin E, Tai S (2011) The development and validation of a general measure of well-being: The BBC well-being scale. Qual Life Res 20: 1035–42.
  26. 26. Ullman JB. Structural Equation Modeling (2006) In: Tabachnick BG, Fidell S, editors. Using Multivariate Statistics. 5th ed. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. 653–771.
  27. 27. Grace JB (2006) Structural Equation Modeling and Natural Systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  28. 28. Bentler, PM (2006) EQS Structural Equations Program Manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software.
  29. 29. Satorra A, Bentler PM (2001) A scaled difference chi-squared test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika 66: 507–514.
  30. 30. Bentler PM (1990) Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull 107: 238–246.
  31. 31. Jackson DL, Gillaspy JA, Purc-Stephenson R (2009) Reporting practices in confirmatory factor analysis research: An overview and some recommendations. Psychol Meth 14: 6–23.
  32. 32. MacCallum RC, Austin JT (2000) Applications of structural equation modeling in psychological research. Ann Rev Psychol 51: 201–226.
  33. 33. Kleinman A, Einsenberg L, Good B (1978) Culture, illness, and care: Clinical lessons from anthropologic and cross-cultural research. Ann Int Med 88: 251–258.
  34. 34. Craddock N, Owen MJ (2010) The Kraepelinian dichotomy - going, going… but still not gone. Br J Psychiatry 196(2): 92–95.
  35. 35. Bracken P (2012) Psychiatry beyond the current paradigm. BJP 201: 430–4.
  36. 36. Beck AT (1967) Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. New York: Harper & Row.