Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Mate-Searching Behaviour of Common and Rare Wasps and the Implications for Pollen Movement of the Sexually Deceptive Orchids They Pollinate

  • Myles H. M. Menz ,

    myles.menz@bgpa.wa.gov.au

    Affiliations Kings Park and Botanic Garden, The Botanic Gardens and Parks Authority, Perth, Western Australia, Australia, School of Plant Biology, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia, Australia, Evolution, Ecology and Genetics, Research School of Biology, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Australia

  • Ryan D. Phillips,

    Affiliations Kings Park and Botanic Garden, The Botanic Gardens and Parks Authority, Perth, Western Australia, Australia, School of Plant Biology, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia, Australia, Evolution, Ecology and Genetics, Research School of Biology, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Australia

  • Kingsley W. Dixon,

    Affiliations Kings Park and Botanic Garden, The Botanic Gardens and Parks Authority, Perth, Western Australia, Australia, School of Plant Biology, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia, Australia

  • Rod Peakall,

    Affiliation Evolution, Ecology and Genetics, Research School of Biology, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Australia

  • Raphael K. Didham

    Affiliations School of Animal Biology, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia, Australia, Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation (CSIRO) Ecosystem Sciences, Centre for Environment and Life Sciences, Perth, Western Australia, Australia

Abstract

Pollinator behaviour directly affects patterns of pollen movement and outcrossing rates in plants. In orchids pollinated by sexual deception of insects, patterns of pollen movement are primarily determined by the mate-searching behaviour of the deceived males. Here, using a capture-mark-recapture study (CMR) and dietary analysis, we compare mate-searching behaviour in relation to local abundance of two pollinator species and explore the implications for pollen movement in sexually deceptive Drakaea (Orchidaceae). Drakaea are pollinated solely by the sexual deception of male thynnine wasps. The rare Drakaea elastica and widespread D. livida occur sympatrically and are pollinated by the rare but locally common Zaspilothynnus gilesi, and the widespread and abundant Z. nigripes, respectively. Local abundance was significantly different with Z. nigripes twice as abundant as Z. gilesi. For the 653 marked wasps, there was no significant difference in median movement distance between Z. gilesi and Z. nigripes. However, the maximum movement distance was twice as high for Z. gilesi (556 m) compared with Z. nigripes (267 m). This is up to three times greater than previously reported for thynnines in CMR studies. Recapture rates were six times higher in Z. gilesi (57%) compared to Z. nigripes (9%). Pollen loads and wasp longevity were similar, suggesting that this difference in recapture rate arises due to differences in the number of males moving at a scale >500 m rather than through diet or mortality. Differences in the frequency of longer movements may arise due to variation in the spatial distribution of the wingless females. We predict that pollen movement will largely be restricted to within populations of Drakaea (<500 m), with few movements between populations (>500 m).

Introduction

Pollinator behaviour directly influences patterns of pollen movement and the rate of outcrossing in plants, which are important for gene flow and seed fitness [1], [2]. Pollination by food-foraging animals predominantly leads to short distance pollen movement, as they tend to move between neighbouring flowers or inflorescences [3], [4]. On the other hand, pollen movement patterns may be different for pollinators displaying territorial and courtship behaviour compared with food foraging behaviour. Consequently, plants pollinated by sexual deception offer an interesting opportunity to test the consequences of mate-searching behaviour on pollen movement [5].

Pollination by sexual deception is one of the most specialised pollination systems. It involves the attraction of male insects by floral mimicry of the species-specific sex pheromones of female insects [6], [7]. Pollination is achieved when male insects are sexually attracted to the flower, often involving attempted copulation with the labellum, which brings them into contact with the pollinium and stigma [8], [9]. Sexual deception is primarily restricted to the Orchidaceae, with the exception of recently discovered cases from a South African daisy [10] and a European iris [11]. Sexual deception has been confirmed in orchids from Australia, Europe, Central and South America and southern Africa, utilising a wide taxonomic diversity of insect pollinators, within the Hymenoptera and Diptera [12]. Australia is recognised as one centre of diversity of sexually-deceptive pollination systems with at least 150 confirmed cases involving multiple orchid genera [12][15], the vast majority of which are pollinated by thynnine wasps [12], [15]. Thynnine wasps represent a diverse, predominantly Australian insect group, with an estimated 1500-2000 species, many of which are undescribed [13]. Male thynnines patrol in search of wingless females that they carry in copula to a food source, usually nectar or exudations from scale insects (Hemiptera: Psyllidae) [16][18].

In species pollinated by sexual deception, patterns of flower visitation and pollen movement are predicted to reflect the optimal mate-searching behaviour of the pollinating male insect [5]. Intense male competition arises from the low number of sexually receptive females relative to males at any point in time [19][21] and is a characteristic of thynnine wasps. Consequently, male insects may search in such a way as to increase their chance of encountering unmated females [21], [22]. However, the mate-searching behaviour adopted may be partially determined by the spatial distribution of females. For example, if females are patchy in the landscape, then males may devote more time to searching in areas where females have previously emerged. By contrast, when females are more uniformly-distributed males may search more evenly in the landscape. To test if mate-searching behaviour of thynnines is affected by local abundance requires studying sympatric species of similar size with similar ecological requirements.

Here we compare pollinator mate-searching behaviour in relation to local pollinator abundance and explore the potential implications for pollen movement in sexually deceptive Drakaea (Orchidaceae). We supplement this with a dietary study of the pollinators to ensure that movement patterns and local abundance are not simply affected by differences in diet. Drakaea elastica Lindl. and Drakaea livida J.Drumm. occur sympatrically in southwestern Australia, but D. elastica is rare and threatened while D. livida is relatively common [23]. Drakaea elastica is pollinated by the rare but locally common Zaspilothynnus gilesi Turner (Hymenoptera: Thynnidae), while D. livida is pollinated by the widespread and abundant Z. nigripes Guerin [24]. The wasps themselves have no dependence or feeding association on Drakaea orchids, and are only attracted to orchids by sexual deception. We predict that the rare Z. gilesi will display more restricted movement patterns, whereas the common Z. nigripes will show a more even distribution of movement distances.

Methods

Study system

We used a capture-mark-recapture (CMR) study to compare the mate-searching behaviour of the rare Z. gilesi and the common Z. nigripes (Figure 1). Both species of Zaspilothynnus are within the same size range (∼20-30 mm in length). They frequently co-occur and have a similar flight phenology, allowing direct comparison of mate searching behaviour. The few thynnines for which reproduction has been studied have been found to be parasitoids on scarab beetle larvae (Coleoptera: Scarabaeidae) [16]-[18], [25][27]. The apterous females spend most of their lives underground, only emerging to attract a male [17], [26], [27]. Consequently, there is always an excess of males ready to mate relative to females. Nectar-feeding thynnines are most often observed feeding on open-flowered plants such as many Myrtaceae that are accessible to a broad array of pollinators [15], [28].

thumbnail
Figure 1. Male Zaspilothynnus wasps and the Drakaea they pollinate.

(A) The thynnine wasp Zaspilothynnus gilesi, the pollinator of the rare Drakaea elastica (Orchidaceae) (B), feeding on nectar of Astartea fascicularis (Myrtaceae); (C) Zaspilothynnus nigripes, the pollinator of the common Drakaea livida (Orchidaceae) (D), marked with green paint pen and an orange queen bee tag, feeding on Chamelaucium uncinatum (Myrtaceae). Photographs: (A), (B) and (D) MHM Menz, (C) J Hardwick.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059111.g001

Within continuous habitats, Drakaea typically form scattered sub-populations (<500 m), restricted to low-lying open sandy microhabitats, leading to a naturally patchy distribution [29]. The rare Drakaea elastica is endemic to the Swan Coastal Plain, Western Australia, and is listed as critically endangered under the Australian Federal Environmental Protection and Biodiversity Conservation Act (EPBC) [30], whereas D. livida is widespread and relatively common in the south-west of Western Australia [23] (Figure 1). Drakaea elastica is restricted to areas of grey sand within mixed Banksia spp. and Kunzea glabrescens Toelken woodland [30]. Natural vegetation in this region is highly fragmented due to land clearing for housing development and agriculture [31]. As a result, the majority of the remaining populations of this species are small (<50 plants) [32] and disjunct (>1 km apart). In many of these populations, D. elastica occurs sympatrically with D. livida.

Study site

The CMR study was carried out between 7 October and 3 November 2010 in a remnant Banksia spp. and Kunzea glabrescens woodland approximately 12 km south-west of Capel (33°38′36″S, 115°29′54″E), Western Australia. Willow peppermint trees, Agonis flexuosa (Willd.) Sweet, contributed to a large part of the overstorey, with cover values of up to 70% in the northern half of the study area. The 80×450 m study site was a portion of a longer, linear habitat remnant connected to a reserve otherwise surrounded by pastures. The study site was selected because (i) the rare Z. gilesi and common Z. nigripes were found to co-occur there in relatively large numbers, based on preliminary site-selection surveys, and (ii) there were no naturally-occurring Drakaea orchids at the site (thus avoiding potential interference from orchid-wasp interactions at orchids growing in situ). The remnant has an open understorey and prior to the CMR trial males of both wasp species were observed to patrol regularly in search of females throughout the study site.

Ethics statement

All necessary permits to conduct this research were obtained from the Western Australian Department of Environment and Conservation. No animal ethics approval was required for this research.

Capture, marking and recapture

Wasps were attracted using the baiting method developed by Stoutamire [33] and Peakall [8], where fresh orchid flowers are presented for short time intervals to lure male wasps. Orchids were picked and stored in a portable refrigerator. Depending on fresh flower availability, baiting for the two thynnine species Z. gilesi and Z. nigripes was undertaken simultaneously, with orchid baits placed at 108 and 88 random locations respectively, over the 27 day study period. Each location was only baited once for 10 minutes. When both orchid species were used simultaneously, they were placed approximately 30 cm apart. Baiting was restricted to warm (>18°C) and sunny days between 09:00 and 16:00 hrs, when thynnine wasps are most active [8]. Up to 10 locations were visited per day, with a minimum distance of 30 m between consecutive locations visited. Bait flowers were kept in a sealed container between baiting locations to avoid wasps following the flowers and to overcome habituation to the floral odour by the wasp.

When arrival rates of wasps at the orchid baits were relatively low (1-3 individuals in a 10-minute period), individuals were marked on the thorax with a queen bee tag (2.5 mm diameter), attached using the glue provided with the tags (sourced from: http://www.penders.net.au). When arrival rates at the orchid baits were higher (4-32 individuals in a 10-minute period) it was not feasible to tag individuals on first capture using queen bee tags. Instead, each individual was marked on the thorax using one or a combination of coloured paint pen (Uniball Posca) marks unique to each location. Upon first recapture, the paint pen mark was supplemented with a queen bee tag. There was no loss of information in this process as all single- and multi-coloured paint pen marks were clear at recapture allowing accurate identification of origin. After marking, wasps were released at the point of capture and observed to fly away from the area until lost from sight (>10 m). Marking of individuals continued for the duration of the study.

Sweep-netting for patrolling males was conducted for twelve 15-minute periods on a single day following the baiting. Sweep-netting sites were approximately 30 m apart along a sandy track running NW-SE through the study area, and extended into adjacent woodland for up to 250 m to the NW and SE beyond the boundary of the baiting area.

No baiting was conducted in adjacent pasture during the study, as the two species of thynnine wasp were not observed to mate-search in these areas. However, to confirm this for Z. gilesi at the study site, we conducted 48 baiting trials with D. elastica in the adjacent pasture up to 400 m from the study remnant in November 2011. This resulted in just a single male Z. gilesi being detected.

Movement distances and patrolling range

Locations of marking and recapture points were recorded using a hand-held GPS. A G-test was used to test for a difference in the distribution of recapture frequencies between Z. gilesi and Z. nigripes (individuals marked but not recaptured, or recaptured 1, 2, or 3-7 times). Mann-Whitney U-tests were used to test for a difference in median abundance of wasps per minute across baiting locations, and median number of days between marking and final recapture. All analyses were conducted in GenAlEx 6.5 [34], [35] unless otherwise specified.

Distances between mark and recapture site were calculated in ArcView v3.3 (ESRI Inc.). To test for differences in the frequency distributions of movement distances between Z. gilesi and Z. nigripes, we implemented a kernel density estimate comparison procedure using the kde.compare function [36] in R v2.14.0 [37] which was adapted from the sm.density.compare function from the R package ‘sm’ [38]. Kernel density estimation is a non-parametric procedure that produces a smoothed estimate of the frequency distribution of movement distances for each species. The kde.compare function compares the area between these two curves to the areas between pairs of curves resulting from a user-specified number of random permutations (here 5000) of the species labels in the data using the sm.density.compare function [38]. The kde.compare function expands on the sm.density.compare function by incorporating automatic bandwidth estimation for the kernel density estimates via the Sheather-Jones bandwidth estimation procedure [39], as implemented in the R function ‘dpik’ from the package ‘KernSmooth’ [40], and increasing the number of points evaluated to produce the accompanying plot thereby resulting in a more detailed figure. Significance is calculated as the percentage of the random permutations that yield an area between the curves greater than that between the curves estimated from the data as grouped by species. Mann-Whitney U-tests were used to compare the median distances moved between Z. gilesi and Z. nigripes for baiting-only recapture records, and baiting combined with sweep-netting records.

Pollinator diet

Male wasps were swabbed for pollen to determine (i) which food plants the wasps were visiting and (ii) if both species were visiting the same food plant species. During the capture-mark-recapture experiment, 20 males of each species were swabbed for pollen using a gel containing fuchsin stain, as described by Wooller et al. [41]. Each wasp was swabbed on the top and underside of the thorax. Pollen carried by the wasps was identified using a compound microscope by comparing samples to a reference collection of pollen from plants flowering at the study site. Pollen load on each wasp was categorised by estimating the number of grains from each plant species, as 1, 1-10, 10-100, 100-1000, and ≥1000 grains. Only plant species represented by ≥10 pollen grains on an individual wasp were considered in the dietary analysis. Relative loads of each pollen species were compared between wasp species using G-tests.

Results

Marking and recapture

We captured and marked a total of 147 individuals of the rare Z. gilesi from 108 baiting trials (22% zero responses; range: 0-11 wasps per trial), and 506 individuals of the common Z. nigripes from 88 baiting trials (17% zero responses; range: 0-32 wasps per trial), spread over 14 individual days of sampling. In the 88 paired experiments there were no cases where a wasp alighted on an orchid other than the species it typically pollinates (N responses  =  653). Considering only locations where wasps were detected, median abundance of Z. gilesi (0.2 wasps per minute) was significantly lower than Z. nigripes (0.4 wasps per minute) (Z  =  3.385, P  =  0.001; Figure 2).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Mean number of male Zaspilothynnus wasps per minute attracted to orchid bait flowers.

(A) Zaspilothynnus gilesi and (B) Zaspilothynnus nigripes. Open circles represent baiting locations where no wasps were caught.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059111.g002

For Z. gilesi, we recaptured 83 of the 147 marked individuals (56%), with 46 (55%) of these recaptured once, 23 (28%) recaptured twice, eight (10%) recaptured three times, and six (7%) recaptured four to seven times (144 recapture events in total, from the baiting trials and sweep-netting combined). The median time between capture and final recapture for Z. gilesi was six days (range 0-23 days).

For Z. nigripes, we recaptured 48 of the 506 individuals (9%), with 36 (75%) of these recaptured once, eight (17%) recaptured twice, one (2%) recaptured three times, and three (6%) recaptured four to five times (68 recapture events in total, from the baiting trials and sweep-netting combined). The median time between capture and final recapture for Z. nigripes was nine days (range 0-25 days).

The distribution of recapture frequencies (G-test; individuals marked but not recaptured, or recaptured one, two, or three to seven times) differed significantly between the rare Z. gilesi and the common Z. nigripes (G  =  141.12, d.f.  =  3, P  =  0.001; Figure 3), with Z. gilesi recaptured more frequently. Interestingly, the proportion of same day recaptures was markedly higher for Z. gilesi (45%) compared to Z. nigripes (5%). Median number of days between marking and final recapture was significantly higher for Z. nigripes than Z gilesi (Z  =  3.46, P  =  0.001). This indicates that the lower recapture rate in Z. nigripes is unlikely to be due to higher mortality.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Comparison of the number of male Zaspilothynnus wasps marked and the number of times recaptured.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059111.g003

Movement distances

For Z. gilesi, recapture distances for the baiting trials ranged from 0-300 m (median  =  38 m, N  =  132), and 0-556 m (median  =  81.5 m, N  =  12) from the sweep-netting (Figures 4a and 5a). For Z. nigripes, recapture distances ranged from 0-150 m (median  =  46.5 m, N  =  58) for the baiting trials, and 0-267 m (median  =  49.5 m, N  =  10) from the sweep-netting (Figures 4b and 5b). The median recapture distances from both the baiting trials and the sweep-netting did not differ significantly between the rare Z. gilesi and the common Z. nigripes (Z  =  0.01, P  =  0.99; Z  =  0.59, P  =  0.55 respectively). Based on the kernel density comparison procedure [36], frequency distributions of recapture distances of Z. gilesi and Z. nigripes were not significantly different (Figure 6; baiting only, P  =  0.098; baiting and sweep-netting, P  =  0.127).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Movement vectors of male Zaspilothynnus wasps attracted to orchid bait flowers.

(A) Zaspilothynnus gilesi and (B) Zaspilothynnus nigripes. Open circles represent baiting locations where no wasps were caught.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059111.g004

thumbnail
Figure 5. Movement distances of male Zaspilothynnus wasps attracted to orchid bait flowers.

(A) Zaspilothynnus gilesi and (B) Zaspilothynnus nigripes. Open bars represent movements recorded from baiting trials and shaded bars from sweep-netting.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059111.g005

thumbnail
Figure 6. Probability density plots of movement distances of male Zaspilothynnus wasps lured to orchid baits.

The shaded area represents a null model of no difference between the two frequency distribution curves. Significance values are based on 5000 random permutations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059111.g006

Pollinator diet

Only three plant species were represented by ≥10 pollen grains on any one individual. These were Agonis flexuosa, Eucaluptus marginata Sm. and an unidentified herbaceous species of Asteraceae. Of the 40 wasps sampled, 38 carried mixed pollen loads with ≥10 pollen grains from multiple plant species. Of the 20 Z. gilesi sampled, 90% were carrying E. marginata pollen, 100% were carrying A. flexuosa pollen, and 5% were carrying Asteraceae pollen. Of the 20 Z. nigripes sampled, 100% were carrying E. marginata pollen, 100% were carrying A. flexuosa pollen, and 5% were carrying Asteraceae pollen. Pollen load frequencies for both E. marginata and A. flexuosa were not significantly different between the two wasp species (G  =  0.633, d.f.  =  1, P  =  0.89; G  =  0.038, d.f.  =  1, P  =  0.99, respectively).

Discussion

Using a capture-mark-recapture (CMR) framework, we tested whether two sympatric orchid pollinators exhibit different patterns of local abundance, mate-searching behaviour and nectar resource use. For both species the majority of movements were concentrated in localized areas, indicating high fidelity to patrolling areas during mate searching. This confirms previous observations of male thynnines repeatedly patrolling set paths [42]. The median movement distances of 38 m and 46.5 m (Z. nigripes and Z. gilesi, respectively; length  =  20 - 30 mm) are greater than that previously documented for smaller bodied (<20 mm) thynnine pollinators of sexually deceptive orchids (14.8-24 m) [5], [8], [43]. Further, the maximum distance moved (556 m, Z. gilesi) was approximately three times greater than previously reported for thynnines in a CMR study (161 m, Neozeleboria cryptoides, length  =  9 mm) [43]. In contrast, the bee Colletes cunicularius (length  =  10 mm), the pollinator of four species of European Ophrys orchids [44], moved on average only 5.2 m in search of mates in the vicinity of a large nesting aggregation [45]. These comparisons demonstrate that mate-searching behaviour of pollinators and subsequent pollen movement will vary between sexually deceptive systems [45].

Despite similar movement distributions, there were also significant differences in recapture rate and the number of same-day recaptures between the two species. The abundant Z. nigripes had a significantly lower recapture rate (9%) compared to the less abundant Z. gilesi (57%). Given that there was no evidence for differences in mortality, this lower recapture rate may be due to male Z. nigripes searching for females at a scale of >500 m (representing the extent of the study area). This may be due to variation in distributions of females and resources in the landscape between the two species. Male insects are predicted to display an optimal mate-search strategy that increases their chance of encountering unmated females [5], [21], [22]. If females are rare or patchy in the landscape, then males may devote more time to searching in areas where females have previously emerged, with occasional longer distance movements in search of females elsewhere. Conversely, in the case of common species such as Z. nigripes, females may be more uniformly encountered in the landscape, meaning that mate-searching/patrolling of males is less restricted to a particular local area.

The observation of markedly higher same-day recaptures for Z. gilesi compared to Z. nigripes, despite similar movement distributions, could be due to male Z. nigripes learning to avoid the orchid mimic. The thynnine wasp N. cryptoides is known to exhibit short-term (≤24 hours) patch avoidance following attraction to a synthetic pheromone bait [46]. In periods of low female availability, such as may characterise Z. gilesi, males may more readily respond to the orchid, regardless of the accuracy of the pheromone or morphological mimicry. This could be experimentally tested by presenting captive male wasps with orchid flowers, in the presence or absence of females. Additionally, response to male competition could be tested by presenting orchid flowers to males at varying orchid to male ratios. Direct comparison of multiple thynnine species under a controlled environment may elucidate interspecific differences in male learning and response to orchid flowers.

Patterns of local abundance were significantly different between Z. gilesi and Z. nigripes. Utilisation of different food sources could potentially lead to variation in the local abundance of pollinators. However, dietary analysis in this study showed that both wasps were visiting the same plant species as nectar sources. This suggests that different nectar preferences are unlikely to be responsible for the observed differences in local abundance between Z. gilesi and Z. nigripes. The visitation of A. flexuosa and E. marginata supports the observation that larger-bodied thynnines regularly feed on nectar from open-flowered Myrtaceae [15]. While frequent visitation of Agonis flexuosa may be partially due to its high abundance at the study site, E. marginata was rare at the study site. Given the similarity in plant species visited, differences in larval host availability may be more important in regulating local abundance of these parasitic wasps than nectar availability.

Consequences for pollinator ecology and pollen movement in rare orchids

We found no difference in nectar plant visitation between the two species, suggesting that another part of the lifecycle is limiting the abundance of Z. gilesi. There have been very few studies on host choice in thynnines [18], [26], [27], and no studies of the host associations of Zaspilothynnus wasps. Given the similarity in other aspects of their ecology, availability of suitable larval hosts could be contributing to the difference in overall abundance between Z. gilesi and Z. nigripes. For example, Z. nigripes may be less host-specific than Z. gilesi, or might utilise a far more common and widespread host. Thus, host specificity and abundance may play a major role in limiting populations of Z. gilesi and other rare thynnine species and requires further investigation.

Investigation of the behaviour and ecology of thynnine wasps yields insights that are relevant for the conservation of Drakaea orchids. Our study of the movement patterns of Z. gilesi revealed that the majority of D. elastica pollen movement will be within 100 m, with the possibility for rare longer distance movements in continuous habitats. Where remnant populations of D. elastica are small and highly fragmented, we predict that there will be little pollen movement between these remnant populations (>1 km apart), given the inability of the agricultural matrix to support Z. gilesi. Direct investigation of orchid pollen movement will be required to validate these predictions and understand population connectivity via pollen flow in this species. Further, the long-distance movements of these wasps may provide the opportunity for long-distance pollen movement of the food plants. It would be interesting to combine observation of food-foraging and mate-searching to elucidate their relative roles in pollen movement for both orchids and food plants.

Knowledge of the behavioural ecology of these wasps is needed to understand how movements interact with habitat and nectar resource use at the landscape scale. Capture-mark-recapture studies of pollinators are usually focused on the local-scale and by themselves do not necessarily provide information of landscape-scale movements. Resolving patterns of landscape use could be achieved through the use of radio-telemetry, where recent advances leading to miniaturisation of transmitters has allowed long distance movements of individual pollinators to be tracked [47]-[49]. However, commercially available radio-tags are not yet small enough for application on many pollinator species.

The potential for habitat requirements to vary across multiple spatial scales will have implications for population dynamics of these wasps and ultimately orchid pollen movement and fruit set. For example, at the local scale, populations may be dependent on the presence of nectar plants, whereas at the landscape scale, occupancy may be affected by the proportion of remnant vegetation. Further insight into landscape use and foraging movements would require the study of copulating pairs and individuals at food sources. If large-bodied thynnines move relatively long distances to forage on nectar, this may offer the opportunity to connect fragmented populations of both plants and pollinators through the provision of habitat corridors containing nectar-producing plants [28], [50], [51].

Acknowledgments

Thanks go to Ben Fitzpatrick for providing the kde.compare code and statistical advice. Etienne Laliberte and Michael Renton provided advice on statistical methods. Kerry Menz and Belinda Newman provided field assistance, and Michael Whitehead provided valuable discussion regarding the manuscript. Lynne Milne assisted with pollen identification. This study was carried out under permits from the Western Australian Department of Environment and Conservation. No animal ethics approval was required for this research.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MHMM RDP KWD RP RKD. Performed the experiments: MHMM. Analyzed the data: MHMM. Wrote the paper: MHMM RDP KWD RP RKD.

References

  1. 1. Ghazoul J (2005) Pollen and seed dispersal among dispersed plants. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 80: 413–443.
  2. 2. Groom MJ (1998) Allee effects limit population viability of an annual plant. Am Nat 151: 487–496.
  3. 3. Pyke GH (1978) Optimal foraging: movement patterns of bumblebees between scapes. Theor Popul Biol 13: 72–98.
  4. 4. Pyke GH (1981) Optimal foraging in hummingbirds: rule of movement between inflorescences. Anim Behav 29: 889–896.
  5. 5. Peakall R, Beattie AJ (1996) Ecological and genetic consequences of pollination by sexual deception in the orchid Caladenia tentaculata. Evolution 50: 2207–2220.
  6. 6. Schiestl FP, Ayasse M, Paulus HF, Lofstedt C, Hansson BS, et al. (1999) Orchid pollination by sexual swindle. Nature 399: 421–422.
  7. 7. Schiestl FP, Peakall R, Mant JG, Ibarra F, Schulz C, et al. (2003) The chemistry of sexual deception in an orchid-wasp pollination system. Science 302: 437–438.
  8. 8. Peakall R (1990) Responses of male Zaspilothynnus trilobatus Turner wasps to females and the sexually deceptive orchid it pollinates. Funct Ecol 4: 159–167.
  9. 9. Stoutamire WP (1975) Pseudocopulation in Australian terrestrial orchids. Am Orchid Soc Bull 44: 226–233.
  10. 10. Ellis AG, Johnson SD (2010) Floral mimicry enhances pollen export: the evolution of pollination by sexual deceit outside of the Orchidaceae. Am Nat 176: E143–E151.
  11. 11. Vereecken NJ, Wilson CA, Hötling S, Schulz S, Banketov SA, et al. (2012) Pre-adaptations and the evolution of pollination by sexual deception: Cope’s rule of specialization revisited. Proc Biol Sci 279: 4786–4794.
  12. 12. Gaskett AC (2011) Orchid pollination by sexual deception: pollinator perspectives. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 86: 33–75.
  13. 13. Griffiths KE, Trueman JWH, Brown GR, Peakall R (2011) Molecular genetic analysis and ecological evidence reveals multiple cryptic species among thynnine wasp pollinators of sexually deceptive orchids. Mol Phylogenet Evol 59: 195–205.
  14. 14. Phillips RD, Brown AP, Hopper SD, Dixon KW (2011) Orchid biogeography and factors associated with rarity in a biodiversity hotspot, the Southwest Australian Floristic Region. J Biogeogr 38: 487–501.
  15. 15. Phillips RD, Faast R, Bower CC, Brown GR, Peakall R (2009) Implications of pollination by food and sexual deception for pollinator specificity, fruit set, population genetics and conservation of Caladenia (Orchidaceae). Aust J Bot 57: 287–306.
  16. 16. Burrell RW (1935) Notes on the habits of certain Australian Thynnidae. J NY Entomol Soc 43: 19–29.
  17. 17. Given BB (1953) Evolutionary trends in the Thynninae (Hymenoptera: Tiphiidae) with special reference to feeding habits of Australian species. Trans R Entomol Soc Lond 105: 1–17.
  18. 18. Ridsdill Smith TJ (1970) The biology of Hemithynnus hyalinatus (Hymenoptera: Tiphiidae), a parasite on scarabeid larvae. J Aust Entomol Soc 9: 183–195.
  19. 19. Alcock J (2000) Interactions between the sexually deceptive orchid Spiculaea ciliata and its wasp pollinator Thynnoturneria sp. (Hymenoptera: Thynninae). J Nat Hist 34: 629–636.
  20. 20. Alcock J, Kemp DJ (2005) The scramble competition mating system of the sphecid wasp Palmodes praestans (Kohl). J Nat Hist 39: 2809–2814.
  21. 21. Goh MZ, Morse DH (2010) Male mate search for female emergence sites by a parasitic wasp. Anim Behav 80: 391–398.
  22. 22. Alcock J, Barrows EM, Gordh G, Hubbard LJ, Kirkendall L, et al. (1978) The ecology and evolution of male reproductive behaviour in the bees and wasps. Zool J Linn Soc 64: 293–326.
  23. 23. Hopper SD, Brown AP (2007) A revision of Australia's hammer orchids (Drakaea: Orchidaceae), with some field data on species-specific sexually deceived wasp pollinators. Aust Syst Bot 20: 252–285.
  24. 24. Phillips RD, Xu T, Hutchinson M, Dixon KW, Peakall R (2013) Convergent specialisation - the sharing of pollinators by sympatric genera of sexually deceptive orchid. J Ecol: In press.
  25. 25. Janvier H (1933) Études biologiques de quelques Hymenoptères du Chili. Ann Sci Nat Zool 16: 209–356.
  26. 26. Ridsdill Smith TJ (1970) The behaviour of Hemithynnus hyalinatus (Hymenoptera: Tiphiidae), with notes on some other Thynninae. J Aust Entomol Soc 9: 196–208.
  27. 27. Ridsdill Smith TJ (1971) Field notes on the occurrence of Hemithynnus hyalinatus (Hymenoptera: Tiphiidae) as a parasite of scarabeids on the New England Tablelands. J Aust Entomol Soc 10: 265–270.
  28. 28. Menz MHM, Phillips RD, Winfree R, Kremen C, Aizen MA, et al. (2011) Reconnecting plants and pollinators: challenges in the restoration of pollination mutualisms. Trends Plant Sci 16: 4–12.
  29. 29. Phillips RD, Barrett MD, Dixon KW, Hopper SD (2011) Do mycorrhizal symbioses cause rarity in orchids? J Ecol 99: 858–869.
  30. 30. Brown AP, Thomson-Dans C, Marchant N, editors (1998) Western Australia's Threatened Flora. Perth: Department of Conservation and Land Management. 220 p.
  31. 31. Government of Western Australia (2000) Bush forever. Vol. 1: Policies, Principles and Processes. Perth: Western Australian Planning Commission.193 p.
  32. 32. Department of Environment and Conservation (2008) Glossy-leafed Hammer Orchid (Drakaea elastica) Recovery Plan. Western Australia: Department of Environment and Conservation. 30 p.
  33. 33. Stoutamire WP (1983) Wasp-pollinated Species of Caladenia (Orchidaceae) in South-western Australia. Aust J Bot 31: 383–394.
  34. 34. Peakall R, Smouse PE (2006) GENALEX 6: genetic analysis in Excel Population genetic software for teaching and research. Mol Ecol Notes 6: 288–295.
  35. 35. Peakall R, Smouse PE (2012) GenAlEx 6.5: Genetic analysis in Excel. Population genetic software for teaching and research - an update. Bioinformatics 28: 2537–2539.
  36. 36. Langlois TJ, Fitzpatrick BR, Fariclough DV, Wakefield CB, Hesp SA, et al. (2012) Similarities between line fishing and baited stereo-video estimations of length-frequency: novel application of kernel density estimates. PLoS One 7: e45973.
  37. 37. R Development Core Team (2011) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available: http://cran.r-project.org/. Accessed: 21 November 2011.
  38. 38. Bowman AW, Azzalini A (2010) R package 'sm': nonparametric smoothing methods. R package version 2.2–4. Available: http://www.stats.gla.ac.uk/~adrian/sm. Accessed: 15 February 2012.
  39. 39. Sheather SJ, Jones MC (1991) A reliable data-based bandwidth selection method for kernel density estimation. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol 53: 683–690.
  40. 40. Wand M (2011) KernSmooth: Functions for kernel smoothing for Wand & Jones (1995). R package version 2.23–7. Available: http://cran.r-project.org/. Accessed: 15 February 2012
  41. 41. Wooller RD, Russell EM, Renfree MB (1983) A technique for sampling pollen carried by vertebrates. Aust Wildlife Res 10: 433–434.
  42. 42. Alcock J (1981) Notes on the reproductive behavior of some Australian thynnine wasps (Hymenoptera: Tiphiidae). J Kansas Entomol Soc 54: 681–693.
  43. 43. Whitehead MR, Peakall R (2012) Microdot technology for individual marking of small arthropods. Agric For Entomol 14: 171–175.
  44. 44. Paulus HF, Gack C (1990) Pollinators as prepollinating isolation factors: evolution and speciation in Ophrys (Orchidaceae). Israel J Bot 39: 43–79.
  45. 45. Peakall R, Schiestl FP (2004) A mark-recapture study of male Colletes cunicularius bees: implications for pollination by sexual deception. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 56: 579–584.
  46. 46. Whitehead MR, Peakall R (2012) Short term but not long term patch avoidance in an orchid-pollinating solitary wasp. Behav Ecol 24: 162–168.
  47. 47. Hagen M, Wikelski M, Kissling WD (2011) Space use of bumblebees (Bombus spp.) revealed by radio-tracking. PLoS One 6: e19997.
  48. 48. Pasquet RS, Peltier A, Hufford MB, Oudin E, Saulnier J, et al. (2008) Long-distance pollen flow assessment through evaluation of pollinator foraging range suggests transgene escape distances. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105: 13456–13461.
  49. 49. Wikelski M, Moxley J, Eaton-Mordas A, Lopez-Uribe MM, Holland R, et al. (2010) Large-range movements of neotropical orchid bees observed via radio telemetry. PLoS One 5: e10738.
  50. 50. Dixon KW (2009) Pollination and Restoration. Science 325: 571–573.
  51. 51. Townsend PA, Levey DJ (2005) An experimental test of whether habitat corridors affect pollen transfer. Ecology 86: 466–475.