Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Cortical Signatures of Dyslexia and Remediation: An Intrinsic Functional Connectivity Approach

  • Maki S. Koyama ,

    makisophiakoyama@gmail.com

    Affiliations Rutgers University Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Newark, New Jersey, United States of America, Phyllis Green and Randolph Cowen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience, New York University Child Study Center, NYU Langone Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America

  • Adriana Di Martino,

    Affiliation Phyllis Green and Randolph Cowen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience, New York University Child Study Center, NYU Langone Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America

  • Clare Kelly,

    Affiliation Phyllis Green and Randolph Cowen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience, New York University Child Study Center, NYU Langone Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America

  • Devika R. Jutagir,

    Affiliation Phyllis Green and Randolph Cowen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience, New York University Child Study Center, NYU Langone Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America

  • Jessica Sunshine,

    Affiliation Phyllis Green and Randolph Cowen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience, New York University Child Study Center, NYU Langone Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America

  • Susan J. Schwartz,

    Affiliation Center for the Developing Brain, Child Mind Institute, New York, New York, United States of America

  • Francisco X. Castellanos,

    Affiliations Phyllis Green and Randolph Cowen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience, New York University Child Study Center, NYU Langone Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America, Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg, New York, United States of America

  • Michael P. Milham

    Affiliations Center for the Developing Brain, Child Mind Institute, New York, New York, United States of America, Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg, New York, United States of America

Abstract

This observational, cross-sectional study investigates cortical signatures of developmental dyslexia, particularly from the perspective of behavioral remediation. We employed resting-state fMRI, and compared intrinsic functional connectivity (iFC) patterns of known reading regions (seeds) among three dyslexia groups characterized by (a) no remediation (current reading and spelling deficits), (b) partial remediation (only reading deficit remediated), and (c) full remediation (both reading and spelling deficits remediated), and a group of age- and IQ-matched typically developing children (TDC) (total N = 44, age range = 7–15 years). We observed significant group differences in iFC of two seeds located in the left posterior reading network – left intraparietal sulcus (L.IPS) and left fusiform gyrus (L.FFG). Specifically, iFC between L.IPS and left middle frontal gyrus was significantly weaker in all dyslexia groups, irrespective of remediation status/literacy competence, suggesting that persistent dysfunction in the fronto-parietal attention network characterizes dyslexia. Additionally, relative to both TDC and the no remediation group, the remediation groups exhibited stronger iFC between L.FFG and right middle occipital gyrus (R.MOG). The full remediation group also exhibited stronger negative iFC between the same L.FFG seed and right medial prefrontal cortex (R.MPFC), a core region of the default network These results suggest that behavioral remediation may be associated with compensatory changes anchored in L.FFG, which reflect atypically stronger coupling between posterior visual regions (L.FFG-R.MOG) and greater functional segregation between task-positive and task-negative regions (L.FFG-R.MPFC). These findings were bolstered by significant relationships between the strength of the identified functional connections and literacy scores. We conclude that examining iFC can reveal cortical signatures of dyslexia with particular promise for monitoring neural changes associated with behavioral remediation.

Introduction

Although the phonological deficit hypothesis (e.g., [1][2]) is the dominant explanatory theory of developmental dyslexia (a reading disorder), impaired phonological processing alone cannot explain the entirety of clinical symptomatology in the disorder. The majority of individuals with dyslexia suffer from multiple deficits in addition to or independent of phonological deficits [3][7]. Beyond abnormalities in phonological processing systems, emerging models of dyslexia highlight the potential contributions of dysfunction within systems supporting visual perceptual processing [8], auditory perceptual processing [9][11], rapid naming [12], and attention control [13][15]. In recent years, speculation has increased regarding cerebellar dysfunction in dyslexia [16][17]. Yet, with few exceptions [18][20], imaging studies of dyslexia have primarily focused on phonological deficits.

To help individuals with dyslexia improve literacy performance, substantial remediation efforts have been made, such as those targeting phonological deficits. These efforts have generally resulted in successful behavioral remediation (For review, see [21]). Recent task-based fMRI studies, using mainly phonological tasks, provide evidence of cortical activation changes associated with such behavioral changes (i.e., differences between before and after intervention) in individuals with dyslexia. Broadly, two cortical patterns appear to characterize behavioral remediation: 1) normalized (e.g., increased) activity detected in the known left hemisphere reading network (e.g., left temporoparietal and frontal regions) and 2) compensatory activity detected outside known reading networks (e.g., in right frontal regions) [22][26].

The goals of the present work are twofold. First, we aimed to conduct a broader examination of the cortical signatures of dyslexia by employing task-independent “resting-state” fMRI (R-fMRI). While task-based fMRI is of undoubted value, R-fMRI enables bypassing some of the limitations inherent to task-based developmental fMRI studies on reading and dyslexia (e.g., difficulties in developing reading tasks that can be equivocally performed by children in different age/grade groups). Second, we sought to obtain preliminary insights into the cortical signatures of behavioral remediation for reading and spelling deficits in individuals with dyslexia, using an observational, cross-sectional design. Specifically, we recruited individuals with documented evidence of a previous diagnosis of dyslexia, irrespective of the presence of prior remediation efforts or the current level of literacy competence. Consequently, participants varied substantially in literacy profiles, with some continuing to exhibit deficient reading and spelling, whereas others had apparently remediated reading difficulties and, in some cases, spelling deficiencies as well. To investigate the cortical signatures of this well-documented heterogeneity in dyslexia outcomes [27][28], we subdivided participants with a previous diagnosis of dyslexia into three groups: (a) children with current literacy deficits (no remediation), (b) children who remediated their reading deficits (partial remediation), and (c) children who remediated both reading and spelling deficits (full remediation).

Primary analyses focused on the examination of intrinsic functional connectivity (iFC) [29] in the aforementioned three dyslexia groups (i.e., no remediation, partial remediation, full remediation) and a matched typically developing comparison (TDC) group. Intrinsic functional connectivity is detected by examining inter-regional correlations in spontaneous low-frequency (<0.1 Hz) fluctuations in the R-fMRI signal [30]. This approach has delineated iFC patterns representing distinct functional networks [31][36] including reading networks [37][39]. Resting-state fMRI is increasingly being applied to studies of developmental disorders, including autism (e.g., [40][43]), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (e.g., [44][47]), and Tourette syndrome [48]. In dyslexia, only one R-fMRI study has been conducted, focusing on the inter-hemispheric functional connectivity of inferior frontal regions [49].

In our previous work, we employed seed-based correlation analyses to assess iFC of regions commonly implicated in word reading [50][51] and demonstrated the utility of this approach for mapping reading networks in children and adults [38][39]. These reading circuits encompass regions previously identified as being dysfunctional in dyslexia (For review, see [52]). Here, we aimed to detect differences in iFC patterns of known reading regions as a function of the presence or absence of history of dyslexia (i.e., dyslexia vs. typically developing), as well as remediation status (i.e., no remediation, partial remediation, full remediation, typically developing). Accordingly, we predicted that we would detect three types of outcomes: 1) atypical iFC associated with a history of dyslexia (e.g., iFC common to all dyslexia groups and distinct from that of the TDC group), 2) altered iFC reflecting cortical compensation associated with behavioral remediation (e.g., iFC distinguishing the remediation groups from both the TDC group and the no remediation group), and 3) altered iFC reflecting cortical normalization associated with behavioral remediation (e.g., iFC distinguishing the remediation groups only from the no remediation group but not from the TDC group). These hypothetical iFC profiles are illustrated in Figure 1.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Hypothetical profiles of intrinsic functional connectivity.

iFC = intrinsic Functional Connectivity, Dys-N = Dyslexia with No Remediation, Dys-R = Dyslexia with Reading Remediation, Dys-RS = Dyslexia with Reading and Spelling Remediation, TDC = Typically Developing Children.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.g001

Materials and Methods

Participant Characteristics

Participants were native English-speaking children with (n = 33) and without (n = 11) a history of dyslexia (i.e., documented prior diagnosis of dyslexia). We recruited children with a history of dyslexia through referrals from the clinical services at the New York University Child Study Center and the New York International Dyslexia Association. Inclusion in dyslexia groups was based upon parental report of prior diagnosis of reading disorder in accord with DSM-IV or ICD-10, accompanied by prior written documentation. We enrolled a total of 33 children with a history of dyslexia. Among them, 22 exhibited partial (n = 11) or full (n = 11) literacy remediation based on standardized subtests of the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test – Second Edition (WIAT) [53]. Specifically, children with a history of dyslexia were classified as remediated if their current standard scores were above 85, corresponding to one standard deviation below the norm, on the WIAT Word Reading subscale (partial remediation), or on both the WIAT Word Reading and the WIAT Spelling subscales (full remediation). The remaining 11 individuals with current standard scores lower than 85 on both the WIAT Word Reading and the WIAT Spelling subscales constituted the no remediation group. Accordingly, there were 3 dyslexia groups: (1) children with current deficits in both reading and spelling (“Dys-N”: Dyslexia with no remediation), (2) children with a previous diagnosis of dyslexia but exhibiting no current reading deficit (“Dys-R”: Dyslexia with reading remediation), and (3) children with a previous diagnosis of dyslexia but exhibiting no current deficits in either reading or spelling (“Dys-RS”: Dyslexia with reading and spelling remediation).

According to parental report (and supporting documentation for some cases), children in the remediation groups had a history of one or more targeted interventions prior to the current study (e.g., the Orton Gillingham approach; http://www.ortonacademy.org, Wilson Language Training; http://www.wilsonlanguage.com, or miscellaneous school intervention efforts). In contrast, none of the children with current literacy deficits (no remediation group) had a history of targeted dyslexia intervention training. This pattern suggests that behavioral remediation is related, at least in part, to targeted intervention training, although the specific forms of intervention varied and are not a focus of examination here due to limited sample size. Importantly, prior written documentation served to verify literacy impairment in the two remediation groups (standard scores lower than 85 on any type of standardized literacy test prior to remediation). Similarly, the written documentation provided evidence that the majority of these children had exhibited deficits in phonological skills (e.g., phonological awareness), consistent with the phonological deficit hypothesis (e.g., [54]).

For a control group, eleven typically developing children (TDC) were selected from a larger pool of children participating in ongoing studies at NYU Child Study Center. In the TDC group, all children’s standard scores were above 85 on both WIAT Word Reading and WIAT Spelling, confirming lack of impairments in either reading or spelling. They had no previous or current DSM-IV-TR diagnoses of psychiatric disorders based on the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children–Present and Lifetime Version [KSADS-PL: 55], which was administered to parents and child participants separately. The KSADS-PL was not administered to children with a history of dyslexia and/or their parents. Nevertheless, we ascertained whether these children had received a previous diagnosis of disorders other than dyslexia, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), through informal interviews with parents of dyslexic children and by reviewing documentation of prior clinical evaluations whenever available (Three out of 33 children with a history of dyslexia had been previously diagnosed with ADHD).

Children in each of the 4 groups were group-matched on age (overall mean age = 12.2±2.3 years: range = 7.7–15.7 years), sex, estimated full-scale IQ, and handedness. The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) [56] provided full-scale IQ estimates, and all participants had full-scale IQ estimates above 85. We also administered two subtests of the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE) [57]) – Sight Word Efficiency (SWE) and Phonemic Decoding Efficiency (PDE). Further literacy-related measures were administered to children in the dyslexia groups, including two subtests of the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP) [58] – Elision (a phonological awareness test) and Nonword Repetition (a phonological short-term memory test), as well as WIAT Reading Comprehension. Additionally, given the increased incidence of ADHD symptomatology in individuals with dyslexia (e.g., [59][60]), we asked all parents to complete the Conners Parent Rating Scales–Revised Long Version (CPRS-R-L: [61]) which addresses ADHD symptoms. Table 1 provides demographic and cognitive measures for each group, and Figure 2 illustrates their literacy profiles.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Group mean and standard deviation of literacy performance.

(A) WIAT = Wechsler Individual Achievement Test and (B) TOWRE = Test of Word Reading Efficiency, n = 11 for each group. SWE = Sight Word Reading Efficiency, PDE = Phonemic Decoding Efficiency, SD = Standard Deviation, Dys-N = Dyslexia with No Remediation, Dys-R = Dyslexia with Reading Remediation, Dys-RS = Dyslexia with Reading and Spelling Remediation, TDC = Typically Developing Children: -1SD represents a standard score of 85, one standard deviation below the norm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.g002

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic and cognitive profiles (means and standard deviation) for each group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.t001

To help anchor the results of the current study, we performed a secondary analysis that aimed to verify that iFC patterns derived from the TDC group represent normative/typical iFC profiles. We examined iFC in an age- and IQ-matched, independent group of TDC (n = 25, mean age = 11.7±1.6 years), as well as in a group of typical adults (TA; n = 25, mean age: 31.6±8.2), both of which were studied previously [39]. This enabled us to examine whether iFC strength for regions showing significant group effects was normative in the two additional typical groups. The current study was approved by the institutional review board of New York University (NYU) School of Medicine, and all parents and children provided written informed consent and assent.

MRI Data Acquisition

MRI data were collected on a Siemens Allegra 3.0 T scanner at the NYU Center for Brain Imaging. We collected resting-state fMRI data in an oblique plane using a customized multi-echo planar imaging (EPI): 180 whole-brain volumes; TR = 2000 ms; effective TE = 33 ms; flip angle = 90°; 33 contiguous 4 mm slices, matrix = 80×64; acquisition voxel size = 3×3×4 mm. This sequence uses the dead time that precedes the readout in EPI sequences with normal TEs (∼33 ms) to collect additional images at several echo times. However, the information from these additional echo times in not used in our reconstruction of the data. For spatial normalization and localization, we also acquired a high-resolution T1-weighted magnetization prepared gradient echo sequence (TR = 2530 ms; TE = 3.25 ms; TI = 1100 ms; flip angle = 7°; 128 slices; field of view = 256 mm).

Data Preprocessing

For each participant, image preprocessing was carried out using a combination of AFNI (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov) [62] and FSL (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/). Scripts containing data processing steps similar to those employed here have been released as part of the ‘1000 Functional Connectomes Project [63] (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/fcon_1000). Preprocessing comprised: 1) discarding the first 4 EPI volumes from each scan to allow for signal equilibration, 2) slice timing correction for interleaved acquisitions, 3) 3D motion correction with Fourier interpolation, 4) time series despiking (detection and reduction of extreme time series outliers), 5) spatial smoothing (6 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel), 6) mean-based intensity normalization of all volumes by the same factor, 7) temporal band-pass filtering (0.009–0.11 Hz), and 8) linear and quadratic detrending. Registration of each participant’s high-resolution anatomical image to standard 2×2×2 mm MNI space was accomplished in two steps [64]. First, a 12-degrees-of-freedom linear affine transformation was computed using FLIRT [65][66]. This transformation was then refined using FNIRT nonlinear registration [64]. Linear registration of each participant’s functional time series to the high-resolution structural image was performed using FLIRT. This functional-to-anatomical co-registration was improved by intermediate registration to a low-resolution image and b0 warping. To control for potential effects of physiological processes (e.g., fluctuations related to cardiac and respiratory cycles) [67][68], motion and large-scale neural signals present throughout the brain [69], each participant’s preprocessed data was regressed on nine nuisance covariates (signals from white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, the global signal, and six motion parameters). Each participant’s resultant 4D residual volume was spatially normalized by applying the previously computed transformation to 2 mm MNI space.

Seed Regions-of-interest (ROIs)

We examined functional connectivity associated with 12 regions of interest (ROIs), including 11 regions that had been derived from meta-analyses of reading in children [51] and adults [50] and validated in a previous R-fMRI study [39]. One additional region, the left inferior parietal lobule (L.IPL), was added based on the most recent meta-analysis of children with dyslexia [52]; the remainder of the regions in the meta-analysis were already included in our previous work [39]. The 12 ROIs in the current study were: 1) left inferior occipital gyrus (L.IOG), 2) left posterior fusiform gyrus (L.FFG), 3) left posterior superior temporal gyrus (L.STG), 4) left temporoparietal junction (L.TPJ), 6) left inferior parietal lobule (L.IPL), 7) left intraparietal sulcus (L.IPS), 8) left dorsal precentral gyrus (L.PCG), 9) left supplementary motor area (L.SMA), 8) left inferior frontal gyrus pars opercularis (L.IFGop), 10) left inferior frontal gyrus pars triangularis (L.IFGtr), 11) left middle frontal gyrus (L.MFG), and 12) left thalamus (L.THAL). Each ROI was a spherical seed (6 mm radius in 2 mm standard space) [39], centered on the MNI coordinates listed in Table 2. Figure 3 shows the spatial locations of the seeds.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Locations of seed ROIs.

Seeds are presented as overlaid black circles. MNI coordinates of these seeds are provided in Table 2. L.IOC = Left Inferior Occipital Gyrus, L.FFG = Left Fusiform Gyrus, L.STG = Left Superior Temporal Gyrus, L.TPJ = Left Temporoparietal Junction, L.IPS = Left Inferior Parietal Lobule, L.IPS = Left Intraparietal Sulcus, L.PCG = Left Precentral Gyrus, L.SMA = Left Supplementary Motor Area, L.IFGop = Left Inferior Frontal Gyrus pars Opercularis, L.IFGtr = Left Inferior Frontal Gyrus pars Triangularis, L.MFG = Left Middle Frontal Gyrus, L.THAL = Left Thalamus.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.g003

Participant-level iFC Analyses

For each participant, the representative time series for each seed ROI was extracted from their 4D residuals volume in standard space by averaging the time series across all voxels within the ROI. This step ensured, to as great an extent as possible, that the mean time series were extracted from the same regions across participants. Calculation of the correlation between each seed ROI time series and that of every other brain voxel was performed in native space to minimize computational and storage demands. This procedure is consistent with our prior publications [70][72]. The resultant participant-level correlation maps were Fisher-r-to-z transformed, then transformed into MNI152 2 mm standard space for group-level analyses.

Group-level iFC Analyses

For each seed ROI, group-level analyses were carried out using a random-effects ordinary least squares model. Whole-brain correction for multiple comparisons was performed using Gaussian Random Field Theory (min Z >2.3; cluster significance: p<0.05, corrected). To test the effect of group on iFC, we performed a one-way ANOVA, treating group as a 4-level factor (Dyslexia-N, Dyslexia-R, Dyslexia-RS, and TDC). Subsequently, regions exhibiting significant main effects of group on iFC were interrogated by post hoc analysis using unpaired t-tests to determine significant pair-wise differences between groups.

To account for the effects of head motion [73][76], we calculated frame-by-frame changes in head position, referred to as framewise displacement (FD), for each participant, and included mean FD values as a covariate in the group-level model. As detailed elsewhere [73], FD is calculated from the derivatives of the rigid body realignment estimates produced by the motion correction algorithm during fMRI preprocessing. Statistically, mean FD values neither differed among the four groups, F (3, 40) = 1.06, p = 0.38, (Figure S1.A), nor correlated with age (R2 = 0.01, p = 0.49) (Figure S1.B).

In addition to the primary group analysis, which included both global signal regression (GSreg) at the individual level and mean FD regression (FDreg) at the group level (i.e., +GSreg+FDreg), we performed several additional confirmatory group analyses. These were carried out in response to recent debates regarding R-fMRI analytic methods, particularly concerning the effects of micro-movements (e.g., [73]) and global signal regression [77][78]. Four confirmatory group analyses were conducted: 1) with GSreg at the individual level but no mean FDreg at the group level (+GSreg-FDreg), 2) with GSreg and removal (“scrubbing”) of volumes with FD larger than 0.5 mm at the individual level, without FDreg at the group level (+GSreg+Scrub), 3) without GSreg at the individual level but with mean FDreg at the group level (-GSreg+FDreg), 4) without GSreg at the individual level or mean FDreg at the group level (-GSreg-FDreg).

Brain-behavior Relationships

We explored relationships between the strength of iFC among regions showing group effects and literacy performance (i.e., accuracy on WIAT Word Reading and WIAT Spelling subtests) as well as ADHD symptom scores (two CPRS DSM-IV Symptoms Subscales: Inattention and Hyperactivity-Impulsivity). Multiple regression analyses were performed to investigate whether individual differences in literacy performance (and ADHD symptoms) were associated with individual differences in the strength of iFC associated with each seed in each group, across dyslexia groups, and/or across all groups.

Secondary Analysis

n the current study, the sample size for each group was equal but relatively small (n = 11 for each group). In order to verify that iFC patterns derived from the TDC group are representative of normative/typical iFC profiles, we performed unpaired t-tests comparing the strength of iFC between regions exhibiting significant group effects between all pairs of the following three groups; 1) the current TDC group (n = 11), 2) an age- and IQ- matched, independent TDC group (n = 25), and 3) a typical adult (TA) group (n = 25). The latter two groups were described previously [39]. We expected that the two TDC groups would not differ from one another. Group differences between each of the two TDC groups and the TA group would only be expected if the networks exhibiting significant dyslexia-related differences were also developmentally sensitive.

Results

Dyslexia Sample Characterization

One-way ANOVA showed that the four groups (i.e., TDC, Dys-N, Dys-R, and Dys-RS) did not differ significantly with respect to age, sex, estimated full-scale IQ, or handedness, but differed significantly on the literacy measures (i.e., WIAT Reading, WIAT Spelling, TOWRE SWE, and TOWRE PDE), as expected (Table 1 and Figure 2). Post-hoc analysis using unpaired t-tests revealed significant differences on these measures between each pair of groups; as expected, the Dys-N group performed significantly worse on all these measures than the TDC group (Supplementary Material1).

On other literacy-related measures (i.e., WIAT Reading Comprehension, CTOPP Elision, and CTOPP Nonword Repetition) specifically administered to children with a history of dyslexia, the three dyslexia groups also differed significantly (Table 1). Significant post-hoc differences between each pair of groups were noted (Supplementary Material1). Of note, all dyslexic children’s performance (except for one child in the Dys-N group) fell within the normal range on WIAT Reading Comprehension. However, some dyslexic children, particularly those in the Dys-N group, showed impairments (i.e., standard scores <8) on the CTOPP phonological measures.

When reading and spelling errors were examined, some children with a history of dyslexia, even in the remediation groups, made errors such as substitution of visually similar words (e.g., ‘useless’ pronounced as ‘unless’), omission (e.g., ‘climb’ spelled as ‘clim’), transposition (e.g., ‘receive’ spelled as ‘recieve’), and sounding out silent letters (e.g., ‘b’ pronounced in ‘subtle’). These errors, typically made by individuals with dyslexia, are considered to reflect inadequate phonological and/or orthographic representations of words [79].

Among the 33 children with dyslexia, a previously established diagnosis of ADHD was reported for only three participants. However, as expected for individuals with dyslexia [80], elevated levels of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity, indexed as T scores higher than 60 on the Conners’ Inattentive and Hyperactive-Impulsive subscales, respectively, were noted in these children: One-way ANOVA showed a significant main effect of group for both inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity scores (Table 1), and post-hoc analysis revealed that both ADHD symptom domains were significantly elevated in the dyslexia groups relative to the TDC group (Material S1 and Figure S2.A). Consistent with recent evidence emphasizing the importance of attention in reading (For review, see [81]), inattention scores were negatively correlated with reading scores across all groups (R2 = 0.24, p<0.001), and across dyslexia groups (R2 = 0.12, p<0.05), particularly in Dys-R (R2 = 0.65, p<0.01) (Figure S2.B.1). A significant correlation between hyperactivity-impulsivity and reading scores was also observed across all groups (R2 = 0.13, p<0.05) (Figure S2.B.2). Accordingly, in addition to our primary analyses, we examined the association between each of these two measures of ADHD symptoms and iFC within the reading network (details below).

Group Effects

One-way ANOVA, with group as level (4 levels: no remediation, partial remediation, full remediation, and TDC), identified a significant main effect of group (Z >2.3; p<0.05, corrected) on iFC associated with two seeds in the posterior reading network – the left intraparietal sulcus (L.IPS) and left fusiform gyrus (L.FFG). Specifically, we observed a main effect of group on iFC between the L.IPS seed and left middle frontal gyrus (L.MFG, BA9; x = −34, y = 4, z = 40; see Figure 4), and on iFC between the L.FFG seed and the right middle occipital gyrus, extending towards the right intraparietal sulcus (R.MOG, BA19; x = 34, y = −70, z = 4; see Figure 5.A), as well as between the L.FFG seed and the right medial prefrontal cortex (R.MPFC, BA33; x = 14, y = 46, z = 10; see Figure 5.B), a region corresponding to the anterior region of the dorsal MPFC [82].

thumbnail
Figure 4. Atypical iFC between the left intraparietal sulcus (L.IPS) and left middle frontal gyrus (L.MFG) associated with historical diagnosis of dyslexia.

The box-and-whisker plot depicts L.IPS-L.MFG iFC for each group and group differences, whereas the two scatterplots represent the relationships between the strength of this connection and reading as well as spelling scores. iFC = intrinsic Functional Connectivity, Dys-N = Dyslexia with No Remediation, Dys-R = Dyslexia with Reading Remediation, Dys-RS = Dyslexia with Reading and Spelling Remediation, TDC = Typically Developing Children, Dys = Dyslexia, ss = standard scores: ***p<0.001, **p<0.01, *p<0.05 (corrected): L.IPS-L.MFG iFC was weaker in all dyslexia groups relative to TDC.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.g004

thumbnail
Figure 5. Compensatory changes in iFC of the left fusiform gyrus (L.FFG) in the remediation groups.

The box-and-whisker plots depict (A) L.FFG-R.MOG iFC and (B) L.FFG-R.MPFC iFC for each group and group differences, whereas the scatterplots represent the relationships between the strength of these connections and reading as well as spelling scores. iFC = intrinsic Functional Connectivity, R.MOG = Right Middle Occipital Gyrus, R.MPFC = Right Medial Prefrontal Cortex, Dys-N = Dyslexia with No Remediation, Dys-R = Dyslexia with Reading Remediation, Dys-RS = Dyslexia with Reading and Spelling Remediation, TDC = Typically Developing Children, Dys = Dyslexia, ss = standard scores: ***p<0.001, **p<0.01 (corrected): Both the partial and full remediation groups exhibited stronger L.FFG-R.MOG iFC, relative to both DTC and Dys-N, whereas the full remediation group exhibited stronger negative L.FFG-R.MPFC iFC, relative to TDC, Dys-N, and Dys-R.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.g005

Of note, our findings were generally robust to the particular micro-movement correction strategy employed (i.e., regression of mean FD, “FDreg”; scrubbing at FD >0.5 mm, “Scrub”; see Figure S3). In contrast, dependencies on global signal regression (GSreg) were noted for iFC of the L.FFG seed but not that of the L.IPS seed: In the absence of GSreg, the L.FFG findings failed to survive multiple comparison correction, but were significant only at uncorrected p<0.05. Figure S4 illustrates such region-specific effects of GSreg on the group differences in these intrinsic functional connections. These findings suggest that GSreg served to account for unexplained variation in these connections, although resolution of this issue is beyond the scope of the current study. Accordingly, the remainder of the results section focuses on findings revealed by the analysis conducted with GSreg.

Interrogation of Pairwise Group Differences

Statistical interrogation (i.e., pairwise comparisons via unpaired t-test) of regions exhibiting a main effect of group revealed two profiles – one associated with the historical diagnosis of dyslexia, regardless of remediation status (L.IPS-L.MFG), and one reflecting cortical compensation associated with behavioral remediation (L.FFG-R.MOG, L.FFG-R.MPFC).

Atypical Connectivity Associated with Historical Diagnosis of Dyslexia

Post-hoc analysis showed that the iFC between the L.IPS seed and L.MFG was weaker in all dyslexia groups relative to TDC (Figure 4): Dys-N (t = 8.44, p<0.001), Dys-R (t = 4.42, p<0.001), and Dys-RS (t = 3.75, p<0.01). Among dyslexia groups, iFC between L.IPS and L.MFG was significantly weaker in Dys-N relative to both Dys-R (t = 2.69, p<0.05) and Dys-RS (t = 4.06, p<0.001). That is, amongst dyslexia groups, iFC between L.IPS and L.MFG was weakest in the no remediation group. Notably, all children in TDC group exhibited positive L.IPS-L.MFG connectivity, as contrasted with negative connectivity exhibited by the majority of children in the no remediation group.

Examination of dimensional relationships between iFC and literacy competence (i.e., reading and spelling) further highlighted the contributions of L.IPS-L.MFG functional connectivity to literacy. Specifically, multiple regression analyses revealed that iFC between L.IPS and L.MFG was positively correlated with literacy performance across all groups (Reading, R2 = 0.46, n = 44, p<0.001; Spelling, R2 = 0.49, n = 44, p<0.001; scatter plots in Figure 4). That is, children who exhibited stronger positive iFC between L.IPS and L.MFG tended to have higher scores in literacy performance. Such brain-behavior relationships were also noted across all dyslexia groups (Reading, R2 = 0.27, n = 33, p<0.01; Spelling, R2 = 0.17, n = 33, p<0.05). When examining individual groups, only the relationship between L.IPS-L.MFG iFC and reading performance within the TDC group reached statistical significance (R2 = 0.52, n = 11, p<0.05).

Cortical Changes Associated with Behavioral Remediation

Post-hoc analysis revealed that the iFC patterns associated with the L.FFG seed differed significantly between the remediation groups and the remaining groups (i.e., the TDC group and the no remediation group) (Figure 5). First, the two remediation groups (the partial and full remediation groups) exhibited significantly stronger positive iFC between L.FFG and R.MOG, relative to TDC (Dys-R, t = 6.15, p<0.001; Dys-RS, t = 4.84, p<0.01), and relative to Dys-N (Dys-R, t = 5.30, p<0.001; Dys-RS, t = 3.39, p<0.01). No group difference was observed either between Dys-R and Dys-RS (t = 1.31) or between TDC and Dys-N (t = 0.85) (Figure 5.A). Second, Dys-RS (the full remediation group) exhibited stronger negative iFC between L.FFG and R.MPFC, relative to TDC (t = 4.61, p<0.001), Dys-N (t = 5.73, p<0.001), and Dys-R (t = 3.41, p<0.01). Among the latter three groups, no differences were observed (Figure 5.B). This pattern of results – the remediation groups(s) (Dys-R and Dys-RS for L.FFG-R.MOG iFC; Dys-RS for L.FFG-R.MPFC) being different from both TDC and the no remediation groups – is suggestive of compensatory cortical changes associated with remediation, rather than cortical normalization.

Examination of dimensional relationships between iFC and literacy competence further highlighted the contributions of these two L.FFG connections to literacy (scatter plots in Figure 5). First, iFC between L.FFG and R.MOG was positively correlated with literacy performance across dyslexia groups (Reading, R2 = 0.49, n = 33, p<0.001; Spelling R2 = 0.14, n = 33, p<0.05; Figure 5.A). When examining groups individually, iFC was positively correlated with reading scores in each remediation group (Dys-R, R2 = 0.55, n = 11, p<0.01; Dys-RS, R2 = 0.77, p<0.001), and with spelling scores within Dys-RS (R2 = 0.40, p<0.05). Such brain-behavior relationships were absent in the Dys-N and TDC groups. That is, children who exhibited stronger positive iFC between L.FFG and R.MOG tended to have higher scores in literacy performance across dyslexia groups, and this trend was particularly prominent in the remediation groups.

Second, iFC between L.FFG and R.MPFC was negatively correlated with literacy skills across dyslexia groups (Reading, R2 = 0.52, n = 33, p<0.001; Spelling, R2 = 0.33, n = 33, p<0.001; Figure 5.B). That is, children with dyslexia who exhibited more strongly negative iFC between L.FFG and R.MPFC tended to have higher literacy scores. When looking at groups individually, such brain-behavior relationships were present only within the full remediation group (Reading, R2 = 0.61, n = 11, p<0.01; Spelling, R2 = 0.50, n = 11, p<0.05), and absent in the TDC group and other dyslexia groups. This result with respect to the TDC group is consistent with our previous work, in which iFC between L.FFG and the medial prefrontal cortex negatively correlated with literacy performance in typical adults, but not in typically developing children [39].

Given that L.FFG is involved in both functional connections (L.FFG-R.MOG, L.FFG-R.MPFC) associated with compensatory changes, an obvious question regards the relationship between these two sets of L.FFG connections. We examined their relationship (Figure 6). The correlation between L.FFG iFC with R.MOG and with R.MPFC was significantly negative across all groups (R2 = 0.39, n = 44, p<0.001) as well as across dyslexia groups (R2 = 0.38, n = 33, p<0.001). When examining groups individually, this relationship was significant in the full remediation group (Dys-RS, R2 = 0.61, n = 11, p<0.01). That is, children with stronger positive L.FFG-R.MOG connectivity were characterized by stronger negative iFC for the L.FFG-R.MPFC circuit, and this was particularly true in children in the full remediation group.

thumbnail
Figure 6. Relationship between the two putatively compensatory functional connections of the left fusiform gyrus (L.FFG) seed.

The scatterplot represents the relationships between the two L.FFG connections (one with R.MOG and another with R.MPFC). iFC = intrinsic Functional Connectivity, R.MOG = Right Middle Occipital Gyrus, R.MPFC = Right Medial Prefrontal Cortex, Dys-N = Dyslexia with No Remediation, Dys-R = Dyslexia with Reading Remediation, Dys-RS = Dyslexia with Reading and Spelling Remediation, TDC = Typically Developing Children, Dys = Dyslexia: The correlation between these two L.FFG connections was significantly negative across all groups, across dyslexia groups, and in the full remediation group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.g006

Consideration of ADHD Symptomology as a Confound

To investigate potential effects of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity on the identified iFC of reading regions, we plotted relationships between iFC strength and scores of the inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive scores (CPRS DSM-IV), respectively. The iFC between L.IPS and L.MFG, within the dorsal attention network (e.g., [83]), was negatively correlated with inattention scores across all groups (R2 = 0.18, n = 44, p<0.01) (Figure S5.A.1). This brain-behavior relationship remained significant even after hyperactivity-impulsivity scores were covaried (R2 = 0.11, n = 44, p<0.05). Similarly, iFC between these regions was negatively correlated with hyperactivity-impulsivity scores across all groups (R2 = 0.26, n = 44, p<0.001) and across dyslexia groups (R2 = 0.20, n = 33, p<0.01) (Figure S5.A.2). These brain-behavior relationships remained significant after inattention scores were covaried (across all groups; R2 = 0.20, n = 44, p<0.01: across dyslexia groups; R2 = 0.18, n = 33, p<0.05). In examining brain-behavior correlations for L.IPS-L.MFG iFC in each group individually, only one significant correlation was observed: between L.IPS-L.MFG iFC and inattention scores in the Dys-R group, both before (R2 = 0.37, n = 11, p<0.05) (Figure S5.A.1) and after (R2 = 0.45, n = 11, p<0.05) covarying hyperactivity-impulsivity. For the two L FFG-based connections that differed among groups, neither exhibited a significant correlation with ADHD symptoms across all groups, although iFC between L.FFG and R.MOG was significantly correlated with inattention scores in the Dys-RS group before (R2 = 0.51, n = 11, p<0.05) (Figure S5.B.1) and after (R2 = 0.56, n = 11, p<0.05) covarying hyperactivity-impulsivity.

Secondary Analysis

A secondary analysis aimed to verify that the iFC patterns observed in the current TDC group are representative of normative/typical iFC profiles. As expected, pair-wise comparisons using unpaired t-tests revealed that, for all identified connections (L.IPS-L.MFG, L.FFG-R.MOG, and L.FFG-R.MPFC), there were no significant differences between the two TDC groups (Figure 7). This allows us to assert that the group differences observed in the present study reflect atypical/altered iFC patterns associated with dyslexia. In addition, for all identified connections, there were no significant differences between the TDC groups and the TA group (Figure 7), indicating that either they are not developmentally sensitive, or they are mature prior to age 7 (but see [39] for developmental differences for brain-behavior relationships with respect to L.FFG-R.MPFC iFC).

thumbnail
Figure 7. The strength of the identified iFC in two typically developing child groups and a typical adult group.

iFC = Intrinsic Functional Connectivity, L.IPS = Left Intraparietal Sulcus, L.MFG = Left Middle Frontal Gyrus, L. FFG = Left Fusiform Gyrus, R.MOG = Right Middle Occipital Gyrus, R.MPFC = Right Medial Prefrontal Cortex, N.S. = Not Significant, TDC-C = Typically Developing Children in the Current work (n = 11), TDC-P = Typically Developing children in the Previous work (n = 25) [39], TA = Typical Adults (n = 25): Group differences were observed neither between the two TDC groups nor between TDC and TA groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.g007

Discussion

We identified group differences in the strength of intrinsic functional connectivity (iFC) of known reading regions among TDC and three dyslexia groups (i.e., no remediation, partial remediation, full remediation), with a particular focus on discerning potential effects of remediation. The findings are consistent with our first two predictions: 1) atypical iFC was associated with a history of dyslexia and 2) altered iFC consistent with cortical compensation associated with behavioral remediation. Specifically, the first finding suggests that dyslexia-related atypicality (i.e., reduced positive connectivity strength) in the L.IPS persists following remediation, even in fully remediated individuals. The second finding suggests that successful remediation of literacy deficits is associated with the emergence of compensatory changes in L.FFG iFC. Confidence in these results was bolstered by observing relationships between iFC strength and dimensional assessments of literary competence. However, we found no evidence supporting our third prediction, namely that we would find altered iFC reflecting cortical normalization. As discussed below, our findings, which complement those of task-based fMRI studies (e.g., [84][86]), should motivate the pursuit of longitudinal, multimodal (e.g., resting-state fMRI, task-based fMRI) studies with adequate sample size and systematic treatment of interventions.

Atypical Connectivity Associated with Historical Diagnosis of Dyslexia

Our findings highlight the presence of dyslexia-related differences in iFC within the dorsal attention network [31], [83], [87][88]. Specifically, our analyses revealed reduced iFC between the left intraparietal sulcus (L.IPS) and left middle frontal gyrus (L.MFG) for all dyslexia groups, irrespective of remediation status. Notably, all children in the TDC group exhibited positive L.IPS-L.MFG connectivity, whereas the majority of the Dys-N group exhibited negative L.IPS-L.MFG connectivity (and thus this group had the weakest L.IPS-L.MFG iFC). Considering that brain regions involved in similar functions (i.e., co-activated by similar tasks) typically exhibit positive iFC [31], this result suggests that dysfunctional iFC between frontal and parietal regions within the attention network characterizes a history of dyslexia.

Consistent with its hypothesized role in attentional function, weaker iFC in this circuit was also associated with higher parental ratings of inattention and of hyperactivity-impulsivity, both of which were negatively correlated with children’s reading competence (i.e., the more elevated the ADHD-related symptoms, the poorer the reading performance). Persistence of weaker L.IPS-L.MFG iFC, even among highly remediated individuals, suggests that interventions do not directly address alterations within the dorsal attention network. This is not surprising, as current interventions tend to exclusively focus on phonetics and word processing and ignore attentional function. Our findings support recent calls for an increased focus on attention as a potential target for intervention [81], [89][90].

Cortical Compensation Associated with Behavioral Remediation

A key question in the dyslexia literature is the extent to which intervention efforts “cure” the underlying deficit or invoke alternative, compensatory processing strategies capable of overcoming innate deficiencies [23], [91][94]. Our findings suggest the latter with respect to the iFC between the left fusiform gyrus (L.FFG) and regions in the right hemisphere outside the known reading network. L.FFG, part of the ventral visual pathway and known as the Visual Word Form Area [95][96], exhibits reduced activity (e.g., [86], [97][98]) and reduced functional connectivity with other language regions [85], [99][100] in individuals with dyslexia. Our findings indicate that L.FFG may exhibit compensatory changes associated with behavioral remediation (i.e., normalized literacy skills). Specifically, we observed two patterns of change that may reflect these compensatory effects. First, iFC between the L.FFG and the right middle occipital gyrus (R.MOG), a component of the dorsal visual pathway [101][102], was increased in both remediation groups. Second, the full remediation group (Dys-RS) exhibited stronger negative iFC between the L.FFG and the right medial prefrontal cortex (R.MPFC), a core region of the default network [103][105]. This pattern – the remediation groups (i.e., only in the Dys-RS group for L.FFG-R.MPFC iFC) being different from the TDC group and Dys-N group – is more likely to reflect cortical compensation than normalization. Of note, the strength of these two functional connections based in the L.FFG was strongly correlated within the full remediation group, further supporting the suggestion that L.FFG serves as a hub region for compensatory changes in the dyslexic brain.

Importantly, the two observed connections of L.FFG are relevant to literacy competence. First, stronger coupling between posterior visual regions (L.FFG-R.MOG) was associated with better literacy performance across dyslexic children, particularly those in the remediation groups. This atypically increased functional coupling within the visual network may suggest greater reliance on visual reading strategies to compensate for weak phonological processing skills (i.e., phonological weakness was documented in prior psychoeducational evaluations for children in the two remediation groups, although the majority of them showed no phonological deficits in the current assessment). Our interpretation is supported by reading errors made by children in the remediation groups, such as misreading of visually similar words (e.g., “useless” pronounced as “unless”). Of note, reading errors for visually similar words are reported to be common in dyslexic individuals [79], even in those who exhibit good reading comprehension [106].

The second observation with respect to L.FFG’s behavioral relevance is that stronger negative connectivity or functional segregation between task-positive and task-negative default networks/regions (L.FFG and R.MPFC) was associated with better literacy performance across dyslexic children, particularly those in the full remediation group. This brain-behavior relationship – the more negative the iFC, the better the literacy performance – is characteristic of typical adult readers [39]. Given that functional segregation from the default network is thought to support efficient cognitive processing in typical adults [107], stronger negative iFC between L.FFG-R.MPFC and its adult-like relationships with literacy competence in the full remediation group may reflect increased behavioral efficiency, as a compensatory mechanism, following effective remediation. This finding suggests the merits of expanding models of dyslexia to include interactions between task-positive or reading networks and the default network [39]. Although negative iFC has been reported by a number of studies (e.g., [108]), its underlying physiological mechanisms remain a topic of research. Thus, longitudinal studies are required to test our interpretations and to further clarify the roles of negative iFC/functional segregation between task-active and task-negative default regions/networks in clinical populations, particularly with respect to remediation effects.

Limitations

The results of this study must be interpreted in light of its limitations. First, we employed an observational, cross-sectional design, rather than a longitudinal approach. This raises the question of whether there were differences in literacy performance between the dyslexia groups that preceded their intervention training, potentially explaining the patterns observed in the present study. Although we cannot rule out the presence of subtle pre-existing differences between the groups, the level of literacy performance prior to any intervention in children of the remediation groups was grossly similar to that of the current literacy performance in children of the no remediation group. That is, we obtained evidence that children in both remediation groups had standard scores lower than 85 on a standardized literacy test at the time of their prior assessment and diagnosis; children in the no remediation group (i.e., no experience of intervention training) currently had standard scores lower than 85 on both the WIAT Word Reading and the WIAT Spelling subscales when assessed as part of this study. Nevertheless, cortical changes associated with remediation would best be captured by longitudinal studies involving coordinated interventions for dyslexia, as remediation is not instantaneous, but instead occurs over time. Still, our findings should motivate continued examination of cortical signatures of different dyslexia profiles, assessed by multiple approaches, including R-fMRI, before, during, and after remediation efforts. The second limitation derives from the modest size of the individual dyslexia groups. Despite the modest sample size, we were able to stratify individuals with dyslexia on the basis of remediation status. This enhanced our statistical power to detect group differences [109]. Third, this study lacked one type of dyslexia group: children with a history of dyslexia diagnosis, who completed intervention training but remained deficient in reading skills [110]. Although a few children screened in the current study exhibited literacy deficits after having intervention training (confirmed by a previous clinical documentation and/or informal interview with parents), they were excluded due to their low IQ scores, which typically preclude diagnosis of dyslexia. It is worth noting that this widely applied diagnostic criterion based on the IQ-reading discrepancy has been criticized and remains a topic of investigation [111][112]. Future research is urgently needed to elucidate potential differences in reading-related iFC patterns between responders and non-responders (including those with low IQ) to intervention training. Finally, the present investigation was limited to R-fMRI data. Although this approach has been fruitful, the combination of task-based and task-independent modalities holds the greatest potential for the further characterization and interpretation of deficits, compensatory responses, and functional normalization in dyslexia.

Conclusions

Using task-independent R-fMRI, we delineated candidate cortical signatures of distinct dyslexia profiles. Reduced intrinsic functional connectivity within the dorsal attention network is associated with a history of dyslexia, calling for further focus on the role of attention in dyslexia and on targeted interventions, whereas the left fusiform gyrus in the ventral visual pathway appears to be a hub for compensatory mechanisms in dyslexia remediation. Intrinsic functional connectivity approaches appear to provide a firm basis for investigating the cortical correlates of dyslexia, for monitoring dynamic changes associated with behavioral remediation in individuals with dyslexia, and eventually for evaluating the effectiveness of dyslexia remediation.

Supporting Information

Figure S1.

Mean Framewise Displacement (FD). The box-and-whisker plot (A) depicts mean FC for each group and no group differences, whereas the scatter plot (B) depicts no relationship between age and mean FD. Dys-N = Dyslexia with No Remediation, Dys-R = Dyslexia with Reading Remediation, Dys-RS = Dyslexia with Reading and Spelling Remediation, TDC = Typically Developing Children.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.s001

(TIF)

Figure S2.

Conners’ Parent Rating Scales of DSM-IV Symptoms of Inattention and Hyperactivity-Impulsivity. The box-and-whisker plots depict (A.1) inattention and (A.2) hyperactivity-impulsivity scores for each group and group differences. The scatterplots represent relationships of WIAT Word Reading scores with (B.1) inattention and (B.2.) hyperactivity-impulsivity scores. Dys-N = Dyslexia with No Remediation, Dys-R = Dyslexia with Reading Remediation, Dys-RS = Dyslexia with Reading and Spelling Remediation, TDC = Typically Developing Children, ss = standard scores, **p<0.01, *p<0.05: The mean scores in Inattention and Hyperactivity-Impulsivity subscales were significantly lower in TDC than the dyslexia groups, and significant negative correlations between attention (i.e., both inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity) and reading scores were observed across all groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.s002

(TIF)

Figure S3.

Effects of five different statistical models with respects to global signal regression (GSreg), framewise displacement regression (FDreg), and data scrubbing (Scrub). +GSreg = analyzed with GSreg, -GSreg = analyzed without GSreg, +FDreg = analyzed with FDreg, -FDreg = analyzed without FDreg, +Scrub = frames larger than 0.5FD were Scrubbed from the dataset, iFC = Intrinsic Functional Connectivity, L.IPS = Left Intraparietal Sulcus, L.FFG = Left Fusiform Gyrus, R.MOG = Right Middle Occipital Gyrus, R.MPFC = Right Medial Prefrontal Cortex, Dys-N = Dyslexia with No Remediation, Dys-R = Dyslexia with Reading Remediation, Dys-RS = Dyslexia with Reading and Spelling Remediation, TDC = Typically Developing Children: The results from the primary analysis (+GSreg+FDreg) were replicated in other analyses, although those yielded from the model where the global signal was not regressed (i.e., -GSreg+FDreg and -GSreg-FDreg) were significant only at uncorrected p<0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.s003

(TIF)

Figure S4.

Region-specific effects of global signal regression on the group difference in intrinsic functional connectivity. L.IPS = left intraparietal suclus, L.FFG = left fusiform gyrus, R. MOG = right middle occipital gyrus, R.MPFC = right medial prefrontal cortex, +GSreg+FDreg = analyzed with both GSreg and FDreg, -GSreg+FDreg = analyzed without GSreg but with FDreg: Z >2.3, cluster significance, p<0.05, corrected; With no global signal regression, the L.FFG seed failed to show the group differences identified in the primary analysis where global signal was regressed (A: R.MOG and B: R.MPFC).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.s004

(TIF)

Figure S5.

Relationships between each iFC and ADHD-symptoms (Conners’ Parent Rating Scales; DSM-IV Symptoms of Inattention and Hyperactivity-Impulsivity). The upper scatterplots depict relationships between iFC and inattention scores (A.1, B.1, and C.1), whereas the lower ones depict relationships between iFC and hyperactivity-impulsivity scores (A.2, B.2, and C.2). iFC = intrinsic Functional Connectivity, L.IPS = Left Intraparietal Sulcus, L.FFG = Left Fusiform Gyrus, R.MOG = Right Middle Occipital Gyrus, R.MPFC = Right Medial Prefrontal Cortex, Dys-N = Dyslexia with No Remediation, Dys-R = Dyslexia with Reading Remediation, Dys-RS = Dyslexia with Reading and Spelling Remediation, TDC = Typically Developing Children, Dys = Dyslexia: The iFC between L.IPS and L.MFG was negatively correlated with inattention scores as well as hyperactivity-impulsivity scores across all groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055454.s005

(TIF)

Acknowledgments

We thank the New York branch of the International Dyslexia Association for helping us to recruit participants with dyslexia, as well as J.M. Fletcher for providing valuable insights into behavioral evaluations of dyslexia.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MSK MPM FXC CK. Performed the experiments: MSK DRJ JS. Analyzed the data: MSK. Wrote the paper: MSK MPM. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: CK AD SJS.

References

  1. 1. Bradley L, Bryant PE (1978) Difficulties in auditory organisation as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature 271: 746–747.
  2. 2. Stanovich KE (1988) Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of learning disabilities 21: 590–604.
  3. 3. Ramus F, Rosen S, Dakin SC, Day BL, Castellote JM, et al. (2003) Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain : a journal of neurology 126: 841–865.
  4. 4. Pennington BF, Santerre-Lemmon L, Rosenberg J, MacDonald B, Boada R, et al. (2012) Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. Journal of abnormal psychology 121: 212–224.
  5. 5. Georgiou GK, Papadopoulos TC, Zarouna E, Parrila R (2012) Are auditory and visual processing deficits related to developmental dyslexia? Dyslexia 18: 110–129.
  6. 6. Valdois S, Bidet-Ildei C, Lassus-Sangosse D, Reilhac C, N’Guyen-Morel MA, et al. (2011) A visual processing but no phonological disorder in a child with mixed dyslexia. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior 47: 1197–1218.
  7. 7. Peyrin C, Lallier M, Demonet JF, Pernet C, Baciu M, et al. (2012) Neural dissociation of phonological and visual attention span disorders in developmental dyslexia: FMRI evidence from two case reports. Brain and Language 120: 381–394.
  8. 8. Stein J (2001) The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 7: 12–36.
  9. 9. Goswami U, Thomson J, Richardson U, Stainthorp R, Hughes D, et al. (2002) Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 99: 10911–10916.
  10. 10. Tallal P (1980) Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language 9: 182–198.
  11. 11. Hämäläinen JA, Salminen HK, Leppänen PHT (2012) Basic Auditory Processing Deficits in Dyslexia: Systematic Review of the Behavioral and Event-Related Potential/Field Evidence. Journal of Learning Disabilities.
  12. 12. Wolf M, Bowers PG, Biddle K (2000) Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: a conceptual review. Journal of learning disabilities 33: 387–407.
  13. 13. Bosse ML, Tainturier MJ, Valdois S (2007) Developmental dyslexia: the visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition 104: 198–230.
  14. 14. Vidyasagar TR, Pammer K (2010) Dyslexia: a deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14: 57–63.
  15. 15. Facoetti A, Trussardi AN, Ruffino M, Lorusso ML, Cattaneo C, et al. (2010) Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 22: 1011–1025.
  16. 16. Stoodley CJ, Stein JF (2011) The cerebellum and dyslexia. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior 47: 101–116.
  17. 17. Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ (2005) Developmental dyslexia, learning and the cerebellum. Journal of neural transmission Supplementum: 19–36.
  18. 18. Demb JB, Boynton GM, Heeger DJ (1998) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of early visual pathways in dyslexia. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 18: 6939–6951.
  19. 19. Eden GF, VanMeter JW, Rumsey JM, Zeffiro TA (1996) The visual deficit theory of developmental dyslexia. NeuroImage 4: S108–117.
  20. 20. Peyrin C, Demonet JF, N’Guyen-Morel MA, Le Bas JF, Valdois S (2011) Superior parietal lobule dysfunction in a homogeneous group of dyslexic children with a visual attention span disorder. Brain and Language 118: 128–138.
  21. 21. Duff FJ, Clarke PJ (2011) Practitioner Review: Reading disorders: what are the effective interventions and how should they be implemented and evaluated? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 52: 3–12.
  22. 22. Sarkari S, Simos PG, Fletcher JM, Castillo EM, Breier JI, et al. (2002) Contributions of magnetic source imaging to the understanding of dyslexia. Semin Pediatr Neurol 9: 229–238.
  23. 23. Simos PG, Fletcher JM, Sarkari S, Billingsley RL, Denton C, et al. (2007) Altering the brain circuits for reading through intervention: a magnetic source imaging study. Neuropsychology 21: 485–496.
  24. 24. Gaab N, Gabrieli JD, Deutsch GK, Tallal P, Temple E (2007) Neural correlates of rapid auditory processing are disrupted in children with developmental dyslexia and ameliorated with training: an fMRI study. Restor Neurol Neurosci 25: 295–310.
  25. 25. Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, Blachman BA, Pugh KR, Fulbright RK, et al. (2004) Development of left occipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically- based intervention. Biol Psychiatry 55: 926–933.
  26. 26. Eden GF, Jones KM, Cappell K, Gareau L, Wood FB, et al. (2004) Neural Changes following Remediation in Adult Developmental Dyslexia. Neuron 44: 411–422.
  27. 27. Davis N, Barquero L, Compton DL, Fuchs LS, Fuchs D, et al. (2011) Functional correlates of children’s responsiveness to intervention. Developmental neuropsychology 36: 288–301.
  28. 28. Torgesen JK, Alexander AW, Wagner RK, Rashotte CA, Voeller KK, et al.. (2001) Intensive remedial instruction for children with severe reading disabilities: immediate and long-term outcomes from two instructional approaches. Journal of learning disabilities 34: 33–58, 78.
  29. 29. Kelly C, Biswal BB, Craddock RC, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2012) Characterizing variation in the functional connectome: promise and pitfalls. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16: 181–188.
  30. 30. Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS (1995) Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magnetic resonance in medicine : official journal of the Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine/Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine 34: 537–541.
  31. 31. Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME (2006) Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 103: 10046–10051.
  32. 32. Cordes D, Haughton VM, Arfanakis K, Wendt GJ, Turski PA, et al. (2000) Mapping Functionally Related Regions of Brain with Functional Connectivity MR Imaging. American Journal of Neuroradiology 21: 1636–1644.
  33. 33. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V (2003) Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100: 253–258.
  34. 34. Margulies DS, Kelly AMC, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, et al. (2007) Mapping the functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex. NeuroImage 37: 579–588.
  35. 35. Di Martino A, Scheres A, Margulies DS, Kelly AMC, Uddin LQ, et al. (2008) Functional Connectivity of Human Striatum: A Resting State fMRI Study. Cerebral Cortex 18: 2735–2747.
  36. 36. Krienen FM, Buckner RL (2009) Segregated Fronto-Cerebellar Circuits Revealed by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Cerebral Cortex 19: 2485–2497.
  37. 37. Hampson M, Tokoglu F, Sun Z, Schafer RJ, Skudlarski P, et al. (2006) Connectivity‚Äìbehavior analysis reveals that functional connectivity between left BA39 and Broca’s area varies with reading ability. NeuroImage 31: 513–519.
  38. 38. Koyama MS, Kelly C, Shehzad Z, Penesetti D, Castellanos FX, et al. (2010) Reading Networks at Rest. Cerebral Cortex 20: 2549–2559.
  39. 39. Koyama MS, Di Martino A, Zuo X-N, Kelly C, Mennes M, et al. (2011) Resting-State Functional Connectivity Indexes Reading Competence in Children and Adults. The Journal of Neuroscience 31: 8617–8624.
  40. 40. Di Martino A, Kelly C, Grzadzinski R, Zuo X-N, Mennes M, et al. (2011) Aberrant Striatal Functional Connectivity in Children with Autism. Biological psychiatry 69: 847–856.
  41. 41. Cherkassky VL, Kana RK, Keller TA, Just MA (2006) Functional connectivity in a baseline resting-state network in autism. NeuroReport 17: 1687–1690.
  42. 42. Kennedy DP, Courchesne E (2008) The intrinsic functional organization of the brain is altered in autism. NeuroImage 39: 1877–1885.
  43. 43. von dem Hagen EA, Stoyanova RS, Baron-Cohen S, Calder AJ (2012) Reduced functional connectivity within and between ‘social’ resting state networks in Autism Spectrum Conditions. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
  44. 44. Cao X, Cao Q, Long X, Sun L, Sui M, et al. (2009) Abnormal resting-state functional connectivity patterns of the putamen in medication-na√Øve children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Brain Research 1303: 195–206.
  45. 45. Fair DA, Posner J, Nagel BJ, Bathula D, Dias TGC, et al. (2010) Atypical Default Network Connectivity in Youth with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Biological psychiatry 68: 1084–1091.
  46. 46. Castellanos FX, Margulies DS, Kelly C, Uddin LQ, Ghaffari M, et al. (2008) Cingulate-Precuneus Interactions: A New Locus of Dysfunction in Adult Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Biological psychiatry 63: 332–337.
  47. 47. Chabernaud C, Mennes M, Kelly C, Nooner K, Di Martino A, et al. (2012) Dimensional brain-behavior relationships in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological psychiatry 71: 434–442.
  48. 48. Church JA, Fair DA, Dosenbach NU, Cohen AL, Miezin FM, et al. (2009) Control networks in paediatric Tourette syndrome show immature and anomalous patterns of functional connectivity. Brain : a journal of neurology 132: 225–238.
  49. 49. Farris EA, Odegard TN, Miller HL, Ring J, Allen G, et al.. (2011) Functional connectivity between the left and right inferior frontal lobes in a small sample of children with and without reading difficulties. Neurocase: 1–15.
  50. 50. Bolger DJ, Perfetti CA, Schneider W (2005) Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: universal structures plus writing system variation. Human Brain Mapping 25: 92–104.
  51. 51. Houde O, Rossi S, Lubin A, Joliot M (2010) Mapping numerical processing, reading, and executive functions in the developing brain: an fMRI meta-analysis of 52 studies including 842 children. Developmental Science 13: 876–885.
  52. 52. Richlan F, Kronbichler M, Wimmer H (2011) Meta-analyzing brain dysfunctions in dyslexic children and adults. NeuroImage 56: 1735–1742.
  53. 53. Wechsler D (2001) Wechsler Individual Achievement Test–Second Edition (WIAT-II) San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
  54. 54. Snowling M (1998) Dyslexia as a Phonological Deficit: Evidence and Implications. Child and Adolescent Mental Health 3: 4–11.
  55. 55. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Ryan N (1996) The schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Medical Center.
  56. 56. Wechsler D (1999) Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporatio.
  57. 57. Torgesen J, Wagner R, Rashotte C (1999) Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE). Austin, TX: Pro-ED Inc.
  58. 58. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA (1999) CTOPP, Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing. Austin, Tex.: PRO-ED. 1 case. p.
  59. 59. Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK, DeFries JC (2007) Understanding comorbidity: a twin study of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. American journal of medical genetics Part B, Neuropsychiatric genetics : the official publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics 144B: 709–714.
  60. 60. Willcutt EG, Pennington BF (2000) Comorbidity of Reading Disability and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of learning disabilities 33: 179–191.
  61. 61. Conners CK (1997) Conners' Rating Scales-Revised: Technical manual. North Tonawanda, NY:: Multi-Health Systems.
  62. 62. Cox RW (1996) AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Computers and biomedical research, an international journal 29: 162–173.
  63. 63. Biswal BB, Mennes M, Zuo XN, Gohel S, Kelly C, et al. (2010) Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107: 4734–4739.
  64. 64. Andersson J, Jenkinson M, Smith S (2007) Non-linear registration, aka spatial normalisation. Oxford, UK: FMRIB.
  65. 65. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S (2002) Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage 17: 825–841.
  66. 66. Jenkinson M, Smith S (2001) A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med Image Anal 5: 143–156.
  67. 67. Bianciardi M, Fukunaga M, van Gelderen P, Horovitz SG, de Zwart JA, et al. (2009) Sources of functional magnetic resonance imaging signal fluctuations in the human brain at rest: a 7 T study. Magn Reson Imaging 27: 1019–1029.
  68. 68. Shmueli K, van Gelderen P, de Zwart JA, Horovitz SG, Fukunaga M, et al. (2007) Low-frequency fluctuations in the cardiac rate as a source of variance in the resting-state fMRI BOLD signal. NeuroImage 38: 306–320.
  69. 69. Schölvinck ML, Maier A, Ye FQ, Duyn JH, Leopold DA (2010) Neural basis of global resting-state fMRI activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107: 10238–10243.
  70. 70. Margulies DS, Kelly AM, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, et al. (2007) Mapping the functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex. NeuroImage 37: 579–588.
  71. 71. Castellanos FX, Margulies DS, Kelly C, Uddin LQ, Ghaffari M, et al. (2008) Cingulate-precuneus interactions: a new locus of dysfunction in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 63: 332–337.
  72. 72. Di Martino A, Shehzad Z, Kelly C, Roy AK, Gee DG, et al. (2009) Relationship between cingulo-insular functional connectivity and autistic traits in neurotypical adults. Am J Psychiatry 166: 891–899.
  73. 73. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2012) Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage 59: 2142–2154.
  74. 74. Van Dijk KR, Sabuncu MR, Buckner RL (2012) The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. NeuroImage 59: 431–438.
  75. 75. Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Loughead J, Ruparel K, Elliott MA, et al. (2012) Impact of in-scanner head motion on multiple measures of functional connectivity: relevance for studies of neurodevelopment in youth. NeuroImage 60: 623–632.
  76. 76. Carp J (2011) Optimizing the order of operations for movement scrubbing: Comment on Power et al. NeuroImage.
  77. 77. Murphy K, Birn RM, Handwerker DA, Jones TB, Bandettini PA (2009) The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? NeuroImage 44: 893–905.
  78. 78. Saad ZS, Gotts SJ, Murphy K, Chen G, Jo HJ, et al. (2012) Trouble at rest: how correlation patterns and group differences become distorted after global signal regression. Brain connectivity 2: 25–32.
  79. 79. Moats LC (1996) Phonological spelling errors in the writing of dyslexic adolescents. Reading and Writing 8: 105–119.
  80. 80. Willcutt EG, Pennington BF (2000) Comorbidity of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: differences by gender and subtype. J Learn Disabil 33: 179–191.
  81. 81. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA (2008) Paying attention to reading: the neurobiology of reading and dyslexia. Development and psychopathology 20: 1329–1349.
  82. 82. Amodio DM, Frith CD (2006) Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 7: 268–277.
  83. 83. Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature reviews Neuroscience 3: 201–215.
  84. 84. Hoeft F, Hernandez A, McMillon G, Taylor-Hill H, Martindale JL, et al. (2006) Neural Basis of Dyslexia: A Comparison between Dyslexic and Nondyslexic Children Equated for Reading Ability. The Journal of Neuroscience 26: 10700–10708.
  85. 85. Pugh KR, Mencl WE, Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, Fulbright RK, et al. (2000) The angular gyrus in developmental dyslexia: task-specific differences in functional connectivity within posterior cortex. Psychological science 11: 51–56.
  86. 86. Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, Pugh KR, Mencl WE, Fulbright RK, et al. (2002) Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biological psychiatry 52: 101–110.
  87. 87. Corbetta M (1998) Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping neural‚Äâsystems? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 95: 831–838.
  88. 88. Pessoa L, Rossi A, Japee S, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG (2009) Attentional control during the transient updating of cue information. Brain Research 1247: 149–158.
  89. 89. Zorzi M, Barbiero C, Facoetti A, Lonciari I, Carrozzi M, et al.. (2012) Extra-large letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
  90. 90. Franceschini S, Gori S, Ruffino M, Pedrolli K, Facoetti A (2012) A Causal Link between Visual Spatial Attention and Reading Acquisition. Current biology : CB 22: 814–819.
  91. 91. Gaab N, Gabrieli JD, Deutsch GK, Tallal P, Temple E (2007) Neural correlates of rapid auditory processing are disrupted in children with developmental dyslexia and ameliorated with training: an fMRI study. Restorative neurology and neuroscience 25: 295–310.
  92. 92. Eden GF, Jones KM, Cappell K, Gareau L, Wood FB, et al. (2004) Neural changes following remediation in adult developmental dyslexia. Neuron 44: 411–422.
  93. 93. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Fulbright RK, Skudlarski P, Mencl WE, et al. (2003) Neural systems for compensation and persistence: young adult outcome of childhood reading disability. Biological psychiatry 54: 25–33.
  94. 94. Temple E, Deutsch GK, Poldrack RA, Miller SL, Tallal P, et al. (2003) Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100: 2860–2865.
  95. 95. Dehaene S, Le Clec’H G, Poline J-B, Le Bihan D, Cohen L (2002) The visual word form area: a prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus. NeuroReport 13: 321–325.
  96. 96. Dehaene S, Cohen L (2011) The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15: 254–262.
  97. 97. Blau V, Reithler J, van Atteveldt N, Seitz J, Gerretsen P, et al. (2010) Deviant processing of letters and speech sounds as proximate cause of reading failure: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of dyslexic children. Brain : a journal of neurology 133: 868–879.
  98. 98. van der Mark S, Bucher K, Maurer U, Schulz E, Brem S, et al. (2009) Children with dyslexia lack multiple specializations along the visual word-form (VWF) system. NeuroImage 47: 1940–1949.
  99. 99. van der Mark S, Klaver P, Bucher K, Maurer U, Schulz E, et al. (2011) The left occipitotemporal system in reading: disruption of focal fMRI connectivity to left inferior frontal and inferior parietal language areas in children with dyslexia. NeuroImage 54: 2426–2436.
  100. 100. Horwitz B, Rumsey JM, Donohue BC (1998) Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95: 8939–8944.
  101. 101. Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO, Brewer AA (2007) Visual field maps in human cortex. Neuron 56: 366–383.
  102. 102. Renier LA, Anurova I, De Volder AG, Carlson S, VanMeter J, et al. (2010) Preserved functional specialization for spatial processing in the middle occipital gyrus of the early blind. Neuron 68: 138–148.
  103. 103. Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R, Buckner RL (2010) Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain’s Default Network. Neuron 65: 550–562.
  104. 104. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL (2008) The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1124: 1–38.
  105. 105. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, et al. (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98: 676–682.
  106. 106. McCloskey M, Rapp B (2000) Attention-referenced visual representations: evidence from impaired visual localization. Journal of experimental psychology Human perception and performance 26: 917–933.
  107. 107. Kelly AM, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2008) Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability. NeuroImage 39: 527–537.
  108. 108. Fox MD, Zhang D, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME (2009) The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. Journal of Neurophysiology 101: 3270–3283.
  109. 109. Kraemer HC (1991) To increase power in randomized clinical trials without increasing sample size. Psychopharmacology bulletin 27: 217–224.
  110. 110. Odegard T, Ring J, Smith S, Biggan J, Black J (2008) Differentiating the neural response to intervention in children with developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia 58: 1–14.
  111. 111. Ferrer E, Shaywitz BA, Holahan JM, Marchione K, Shaywitz SE (2010) Uncoupling of reading and IQ over time: empirical evidence for a definition of dyslexia. Psychol Sci 21: 93–101.
  112. 112. Tanaka H, Black JM, Hulme C, Stanley LM, Kesler SR, et al. (2011) The brain basis of the phonological deficit in dyslexia is independent of IQ. Psychol Sci 22: 1442–1451.