Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

An Assessment of the Impact of Hafting on Paleoindian Point Variability

  • Briggs Buchanan,

    Affiliations Department of Archaeology and Human Evolutionary Studies Program, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada, Department of Anthropology, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri, United States of America

  • Michael J. O'Brien,

    Affiliation Department of Anthropology, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri, United States of America

  • J. David Kilby,

    Affiliation Department of Anthropology and Applied Archaeology, Eastern New Mexico University, Portales, New Mexico, United States of America

  • Bruce B. Huckell,

    Affiliation Maxwell Museum of Anthropology and Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, New Mexico, United States of America

  • Mark Collard

    mcollard@sfu.ca

    Affiliations Department of Archaeology and Human Evolutionary Studies Program, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada, Department of Anthropology, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri, United States of America

Abstract

It has long been argued that the form of North American Paleoindian points was affected by hafting. According to this hypothesis, hafting constrained point bases such that they are less variable than point blades. The results of several studies have been claimed to be consistent with this hypothesis. However, there are reasons to be skeptical of these results. None of the studies employed statistical tests, and all of them focused on points recovered from kill and camp sites, which makes it difficult to be certain that the differences in variability are the result of hafting rather than a consequence of resharpening. Here, we report a study in which we tested the predictions of the hafting hypothesis by statistically comparing the variability of different parts of Clovis points. We controlled for the potentially confounding effects of resharpening by analyzing largely unused points from caches as well as points from kill and camp sites. The results of our analyses were not consistent with the predictions of the hypothesis. We found that several blade characters and point thickness were no more variable than the base characters. Our results indicate that the hafting hypothesis does not hold for Clovis points and indicate that there is a need to test its applicability in relation to post-Clovis Paleoindian points.

Introduction

Investigating the nature and causes of variation in point form is an important task for archaeologists interested in the Paleoindian period (ca. 13,600–11,450 calBP) of North America. There are two main reasons for this. One is that understanding variation in point size and shape is necessary for establishing the cultural-historical types that Paleoindian archaeologists rely on (e.g. [1][5]). The other is that variation in point size and shape may be informative regarding the behavior of Paleoindians, including their use of the landscape and their hunting practices (e.g. [6][12]).

One well-known hypothesis concerning variation in Paleoindian point form contends that it was affected by hafting. According to this hypothesis, hafting requirements constrained the size and shape of point bases but did not affect the size and shape of point blades [3], [4], [13]. An important implication of the hafting hypothesis is that the base is the most diagnostic portion of Paleoindian points [3], [4].

A key prediction of the hafting hypothesis is that base characters should be less variable than non-base characters. This prediction has been supported in several papers [3], [14][18], but there are reasons to be skeptical about the results of the relevant analyses. First, statistical tests were not used in the analyses, and thus it is unclear whether the differences in variability are any greater than would be expected on the basis of chance alone. Second, the analyses focused on points recovered from kill and camp sites. This is problematic because many points recovered from kill and camp sites were resharpened prior to being lost or discarded and therefore it is difficult to be sure that the differences in variability between the base and non-base portions of the points are the result of hafting constraints rather than a consequence of resharpening. Third, experimental studies using replica Clovis points suggest that both tip and base repairs would have been needed to maintain functionality [19], [20].

Given this uncertainty, we decided to re-test the hafting hypothesis. In our study, we focused on Clovis points, which are found throughout North America and are widely accepted to date to 13,600–13,000 calBP [21], [22]. We controlled for the potentially confounding effects of resharpening by analyzing points from caches as well as points from kill and camp sites. A cache is a tightly clustered deposit of artifacts that appear to have been deposited at the same time and are associated with little or no manufacturing and/or maintenance debris [23]. The majority of cached points were either not used or used only lightly before being deposited. Hence, including cached points decreases the potential for resharpening to confound tests of the hafting hypothesis. Additional differences from previous tests of the hafting hypothesis are that we used digitizing techniques to capture point form and employed a statistical test to compare the variability of the base and non-base characters.

Materials and Methods

1. Sample

Our sample comprised 122 Clovis points. We focused on complete points and specimens missing at most an ear because it is difficult to implement the data-capture methods we employed with incomplete artifacts. Sixty-eight points are from kill/camp sites and 54 are from caches. We focused on Clovis points from western North America because the distribution of Clovis caches is limited to the west. Kill/camp points come from sites located in the Southwest (Lehner, Murray Springs, and Naco), the Southern Plains (Blackwater Draw, Domebo, Jake Bluff, and Miami), and the Northern Plains (Dent and Colby). Cached points come from sites located in the Northwest (East Wenatchee, Fenn, and Simon) and the Northern Plains (Anzick and Drake). It has been suggested that the Anzick points may be burial goods rather than part of a cache, because human skeletal remains have also been recovered at the site [24][26]. We do not find this argument convincing for two reasons. First, the artifacts and skeleton were recovered with a front-end loader, so there is no stratigraphic evidence that they are associated [27]. Second, radiocarbon dates derived from some of the artifacts recovered at the site do not overlap with radiocarbon dates derived from some of the human bones, which suggests that they are not contemporaneous [27], [28]. Locations of the sites and the number of points per site are shown in Figure 1 and Table 1, respectively.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Locations of archaeological sites in the western United States from which points used in the study were recovered.

Triangles = kill sites/camp sites. Circles = caches. (Figure is adapted from Buchanan et al. [71]).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036364.g001

Epoxy casts were used in lieu of some of the original points. Buchanan [59] compared casts of Clovis points from the Lehner site to the actual points and found that there was no statistical difference between the casts and the real artifacts. The paired t-tests he carried out gave p values ranging between 0.841 and 0.962. Consequently, the inclusion of epoxy casts in the sample is not expected to have affected the present study.

2. Data capture

The data-capture method we used was the same as the one employed by Buchanan [59], Buchanan and Collard [6], and Buchanan and Hamilton [7]. Briefly, digital images of the points were imported into the Thin Plate Spline Digitizing Program (Version 2.02) [60]. Thirty-two landmarks were used to define the edges and base of each point, and the coordinate data were used to compute ten interlandmark distances in Matlab 6.0. The characters are listed in Table 2 and illustrated in Figure 2. In addition to the ten characters derived from digitizing the points, base thickness (BT) and maximum thickness (MT) were taken directly from points using digital calipers or were taken from published sources. Base thickness was not available for four cached points (from East Wenatchee) and seven points from kill/camp sites (four from Jake Bluff and three from Blackwater Draw). The characters were selected to capture variability in the two major parts of the points, the base and the blade, as well as variability in overall length and thickness. The characters include traditional linear measurements as well as measurements that cannot be taken accurately with calipers. Five of the characters relate to the base (BT, BB, LB, BW, and LT), three to the blade (BL, MW, and TW), and four to overall point length (ML, OL, EL, and TB). The thirteenth character, MT, is maximum thickness.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Image of a Clovis point from Blackwater Draw, NM, showing approximate location of characters.

Character abbreviations follow Table 2. (Figure is adapted from Buchanan et al. [71]).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036364.g002

The precision of the digitized characters was estimated on a sample of points from Naco and Lehner. Measurement error–the percentage of the total variance attributable to within-individual variance resulting from imprecision of measurements–was calculated for each character using Model II ANOVA [61][63]. Points were chosen randomly and digitized in three non-consecutive sessions, and the variance components were calculated from the resulting dataset. Measurement error associated with the characters ranges from 0.002 to 0.031 percent, which compares favorably to measurement errors reported in biological morphological studies (e.g. [61], [63]). Furthermore, there is no relationship between percent measurement error and the coefficient of variation of a character (r = −0.072, p = 0.623), which suggests measurement error does not drive variation.

We estimated missing values for nearly complete points. This was accomplished with the expectation-maximization missing-data replacement method, which uses information about covariation among variables to predict missing values [64]. A recent simulation demonstrated that this form of missing-data replacement is more precise and reliable than principal-component estimation when using a moderate number of characters (6–12) and large sample sizes [64].

3. Analyses

To test the prediction that base characters of Paleoindian points should be less variable than characters from other portions of points, we used the coefficient of variation (CV) and Fligner and Killeen's [65] distribution-free two-sample test (FK test). The CV, commonly used in archaeology (see refs in [66]), expresses the normalized amount of variation in a set of measurements, and is calculated by dividing the sample standard deviation by the sample mean and multiplying the quotient by 100. The FK test first ranks the CVs in the combined dataset from smallest to largest. Values that are tied are given sequential ranks. After the values are ranked, they are weighted by the sample size and then converted to the quantile of the standard normal distribution that corresponds to the weighted score. This value is then squared. Next, ties are resolved by averaging the weighted values associated with the tied values. These normalized scores are then summed to create the test statistic, T. Statistical significance is assessed using the large scale approximation z-score, which is calculated by dividing the difference between the T statistic and the expected T score by the variance. We chose the FK test to compare CVs because comparative analyses have shown that it is among the best tests for reducing type-I and type-II errors. For example, Donnelly and Kramer [67] used Monte Carlo methods and simulated data to evaluate 11 tests of relative variation measures, including a CV-based parametric bootstrap test, modifications of Levene's test, and the FK test. They found that the FK test performed best in terms of maintaining an acceptable type-I error rate when he samples were drawn from different underlying distributions, including situations where the samples had different underlying distributions. The FK test also consistently ranked as the most powerful or nearly the most powerful test in Donnelly and Kramer's [67] comparative analyses.

We carried out two analyses, one focused on kill/camp points and one on cached points. In both analyses, we used the FK test to compare the CV of each of the base characters to the CV of each of the three blade characters, the four length characters, and thickness. Because our dataset includes values for five base characters and eight non-base characters (three blade characters, four length characters, and thickness) we carried out a total of 40 FK tests in each analysis. The test prediction was that the CVs for the base characters should be significantly less than the CVs for the blade characters, the length characters, and for thickness. Both analyses were carried out in PAST version 2.00 [68]. Because we conducted multiple unplanned tests, we used Benjamini and Yekutieli's [69] method of significance-level correction. We employed this method rather than the commonly used Bonferroni correction because it has been shown to balance the reduction of type-I and type-II error rates better than Bonferroni correction [70].

Results

The CVs for the kill/camp points are presented in Table 3. To reiterate, the hafting hypothesis predicts that the base characters should have lower CVs than the blade characters, the length characters, and maximum thickness. This is not the case. Maximum thickness is less variable than all five of the base characters; blade character MW is less variable than base characters BW, BB, LT, and BT; and blade character TW is less variable than base character BT. Thus, the qualitative comparison of the CVs for the kill/camp points does not support the hafting hypothesis.

thumbnail
Table 3. Coefficients of variation for characters of kill/camp points, ranked from smallest to largest.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036364.t003

Table 4 summarizes the results of the FK tests that focused on kill/camp points. The tests indicate that the five base characters are significantly less variable than the four length characters. However, not all the base characters are less variable than the three blade characters or maximum thickness. Base characters BB and LT have CVs that are not statistically significantly different from the blade characters, and base characters LB and BT have CVs that are statistically indistinguishable from the CV for blade character MW. In addition, base character BT has a CV that is significantly greater than the CV for blade character TW, while base character BW has a CV that is not statistically different from the CVs for blade characters MW and TW. Lastly, none of the CVs for the base characters is statistically different from the CV for maximum thickness. Thus, the FK tests confirm that the kill/camp points do not support the predictions of the hafting hypothesis.

thumbnail
Table 4. Comparison of base characters (BT, BB, LB, BW, and LT) with characters describing the blade (BL, MW, and TW), different lengths (ML, OL, EL, and TB), and thickness (MT) of kill/camp points.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036364.t004

Table 5 presents the CVs for the cached points. As before, the hafting hypothesis' prediction is that the base characters should have lower CVs than the blade characters, the length characters, and maximum thickness. The ranking of the CVs is different from the ranking yielded by the kill/camp points, but the prediction is still not supported. Base character BT is the least variable character, but maximum thickness is less variable than base characters BB, LB, and BW, and blade character MW is less variable than blade character LT. Thus, the qualitative comparison of the CVs for the cached points also does not support the hafting hypothesis.

thumbnail
Table 5. Coefficients of variation for cached points, ranked from smallest to largest.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036364.t005

Results of the cache point-focused FK tests are summarized in Table 6. As in the qualitative comparison, the results differ from the results of the equivalent analysis of kill/camp points, but the prediction is still not supported. The CVs of all the base characters are statistically indistinguishable from the CV of maximum thickness, and the CVs of base characters BB, LB, BW, and LT are statistically indistinguishable from the CVs of at least two other non-base characters. Thus, the cached points-focused FK tests confirm that the cached points also do not support the predictions of the hafting hypothesis.

thumbnail
Table 6. Comparison of base characters with characters describing the blade and point length, and thickness of cached points.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036364.t006

Discussion

The hafting hypothesis predicts that base characters of Paleoindian points should be less variable than their non-base counterparts. The results of our analysis of Clovis points from kill/camp sites were not consistent with this prediction. While the base characters were significantly less variable than the length characters, several base characters were indistinguishable in terms of variability from the blade characters and from maximum thickness. Our analysis of cached Clovis points also did not support the prediction that base characters of Paleoindian points should be less variable than their non-base counterparts. As with the analysis of kill/camp points, the base characters were not significantly less variable than the blade characters or maximum thickness. Thus, the results of our analyses do not support the hafting hypothesis.

One issue needs to be addressed before considering the implications of our results–our choice of base characters. Two of these characters, LT and BW, might be disputed with respect to their position relative to the haft. To reiterate, character LT is the average of the right and left distances from base landmarks to the position at one-third the total length along the opposite edge boundaries, and character BW is the width at one-third the total length above the base landmarks (Figure 1). It is conceivable that the distal terminus of character LT and both termini of character BW were above the haft and thus characters LT and BW may not in fact have been constrained by the haft. We think this is unlikely. However, even if it were the case, it would not affect our findings because the other three base characters–BB, LB, and BT–undeniably relate to the part of a point that would have been hafted and are statistically indistinguishable from several non-base characters. Thus, even if characters LT and BW were rejected as base characters, our analyses would still not support the predictions of the hafting hypothesis. It appears, then, that the hafting hypothesis does not hold for Clovis points.

There are several potential reasons why the hafting hypothesis does not hold for Clovis points. One is that Clovis points were hafted in such a way that the haft did not constrain the base characters. A second possibility is that constraints were placed on the base of Clovis points, but the base was not the only portion of Clovis points that was constrained. It could be, for example, that the haft covered more of the point than imagined by proponents of the hafting hypothesis and that consequently some non-base dimensions of the point were constrained by it. Alternatively, some of the non-base dimensions may have been constrained by the demands of flight or hide-penetration, or by cultural norms. Determining which of these hypotheses is correct will require a better understanding of how large / small the dimensions of a Clovis point can be without losing functionality when different methods of hafting are used (e.g. with/without a foreshaft, with/without mastic) and when different methods of spear-delivery are employed (e.g. thrusting, unassisted throwing, atlatl-assisted throwing). One way of shedding light on this is through the replication and experimental use of spears with different combinations of Clovis points, hafts, and delivery methods (e.g. [19], [20]).

An obvious implication of our results is that it would be sensible to re-assess whether the hafting hypothesis holds for post-Clovis points. Doing so should be fairly straightforward. Earlier we pointed out that there are two potential problems with previous tests of the hafting hypothesis. One is that they did not use statistical tests. We argued that this is problematic because it means we cannot be sure the differences in variability between the base and non-base characters identified in the analyses are consequential as opposed to being simply a result of chance. The other potential problem is that the analyses focused on points recovered from kill/camp sites. We suggested this is problematic because many such points were resharpened prior to being lost or discarded, and thus it is difficult to be sure that the differences in variability are the result of hafting rather than the consequence of resharpening. Given that our analysis of kill/camp Clovis points did not support the hafting hypothesis any better than our analysis of cached Clovis points, there is reason to believe that resharpening may not in fact have undermined the results of the previous tests of the hafting hypothesis and that the real problem is the failure to use a statistical method to control for the possibility that measures of variation may differ simply by chance alone. The corollary of this is that it should be possible to revisit the previous tests of the hafting hypothesis and subject the reported measures of variation to statistical analysis. This should provide a rapid indication of whether the hafting hypothesis applies to post-Clovis points.

Acknowledgments

For access to specimens we thank Eastern New Mexico University, University of Arizona, Arizona State Museum, Smithsonian Institution, Washington State Historical Society, Burke Museum of Natural History and Culture, Montana Historical Society, and Herrett Center for Arts and Sciences. We are also grateful to the reviewers and academic editor for their comments on our paper.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: BB MJO JDK BBH MC. Performed the experiments: BB. Analyzed the data: BB MJO JDK BBH MC. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: BB JDK. Wrote the paper: BB MJO JDK BBH MC.

References

  1. 1. Buchanan B, Collard M (2010) A geometric morphometrics-based assessment of blade shape differences among Paleoindian projectile point types from western North America. J Archaeol Sci 37: 350–359.
  2. 2. Ellis C (2004) Understanding “Clovis” fluted point variability in the Northeast: a perspective from the Debert site, Nova Scotia. Can J Archaeol 28: 205–253.
  3. 3. Judge WJ (1973) Paleoindian Occupation of the Central Rio Grande Valley in New Mexico. University of New Mexico Press, Albuquerque.
  4. 4. Musil RR (1988) Functional efficiency and technological change: a hafting tradition model for prehistoric America. Willig A, Aikens CM, Fagan JL (eds) Early Human Occupation in Far Western North America: The Clovis-Archaic Interface. Nevada State Museum, Anthropological Papers Number 21. Carson City, Nevada. pp. 373–387.
  5. 5. Wormington HM (1957) Ancient Man in North America. Denver Museum of Natural History, Popular Series No. 4 p.
  6. 6. Buchanan B, Collard M (2007) Investigating the peopling of North America through cladistic analyses of early Paleoindian projectile points. J Anthropol Archaeol 26: 366–393.
  7. 7. Buchanan B, Hamilton MJ (2009) A formal test of the origin of variation in North American early Paleoindian projectile points. Am Antiq 74: 279–298.
  8. 8. Buchanan B, Collard M, Hamilton MJ, O'Brien MJ (2011) Points and prey: a quantitative test of the hypothesis that prey size influences early Paleoindian point form. J Archaeol Sci 38: 852–864.
  9. 9. Cheshier J, Kelly RL (2006) Projectile point shape and durability: the effect of thickness: length. Am Antiq 71: 353–363.
  10. 10. Hutchings WK (1997) The Paleoindian Fluted Point: Dart or Spear Armature? The Identification of Paleoindian Delivery Technology Through the Analysis of Lithic Fracture Velocity. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Archaeology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia.
  11. 11. Morrow JE, Morrow TA (1999) Geographic variation in fluted projectile points: a hemispheric perspective. Am Antiq 64: 215–231.
  12. 12. O'Brien MJ, Darwent J, Lyman RL (2001) Cladistics is useful for reconstructing archaeological phylogenies: Palaeoindian points from the southeastern United States. J Archaeol Sci 28: 1115–1136.
  13. 13. Keeley LH (1982) Hafting and retooling: effects on the archaeological record. Am Antiq 47: 798–809.
  14. 14. Bever MR, Meltzer DJ (2007) Exploring variation in Paleoindian life ways: the third revised edition of the Texas Clovis fluted point survey. Bull Texas Archeol Soc 78: 65–99.
  15. 15. Buchanan B (2002) Folsom lithic procurement, tool use, and replacement at the Lake Theo site, Texas. Plains Anthropol 47: 121–146.
  16. 16. Meltzer DJ (2006) Folsom: New Archaeological Investigations of a Classic Paleoindian Bison Kill. University of California Press, Berkeley.
  17. 17. Meltzer DJ, Bever MR (1995) Paleoindians in Texas: an update on the Texas Clovis fluted point survey. Bull Texas Archeol Soc 66: 17–51.
  18. 18. Tunnell C, Johnson L (2000) Comparing Dimensions for Folsom Points and Their By-products from the Adair-Steadman and Lindenmeier Sites and Other Localities. Texas Historical Commission Archeological Reports Series No. 1. Austin.
  19. 19. Frison GC (1989) Experimental use of Clovis weaponry and tools on African elephants. Am Antiq 54: 766–784.
  20. 20. Huckell BB (1982) The Denver elephant project: A report on experimentation with thrusting spears. Plains Anthropol 27: 217–224.
  21. 21. Haynes G (2002) The Early Settlement of North America. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
  22. 22. Haynes G, Anderson DG, Ferring CR, Fiedel SJ, Grayson DK, et al. (2007) Comment on “Redefining the age of Clovis: implications for the peopling of the Americas.” Science 317: 320b.
  23. 23. Kilby JD (2008) An Investigation of Clovis Caches: Content, Function, and Technological Organization. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico. Albuquerque.
  24. 24. Jones JS (1996) The Anzick Site: Analysis of a Clovis Burial Assemblage. Master's thesis, Department of Anthropology, Oregon State University. Corvallis.
  25. 25. Jones JS, Bonnichsen R (1994) The Anzick Clovis burial. Curr Res Pleistocene 11: 42–43.
  26. 26. Lahren L, Bonnichsen R (1974) Bone foreshafts from a Clovis burial in southwestern Montana. Science 186: 147–150.
  27. 27. Owsley DW, Hunt DR (2001) Clovis and Early Archaic period crania from the Anzick site (24PA506), Park County, Montana. Plains Anthropol 46: 115–121.
  28. 28. Morrow JE, Fiedel SJ (2006) New radiocarbon dates for the Clovis component of the Anzick site, Park County, Montana. Morrow JE, Gnecco C (eds) Paleoindian Archaeology: A Hemispheric Perspective. University of Florida Press, Gainesville. pp. 123–138.
  29. 29. Taylor DC (1969) The Wilsall excavations: an exercise in frustration. Proc Montana Acad Sci 29: 147–150.
  30. 30. Wilke PJ, Flenniken JJ, Ozbun TL (1991) Clovis technology at the Anzick site, Montana. J Cal Great Basin Anthropol 13: 242–272.
  31. 31. Boldurian AT, Cotter JL (1999) Clovis Revisited: New Perspectives on Paleoindian Adaptations from Blackwater Draw, New Mexico. The University Museum, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.
  32. 32. Cotter JL (1937) The occurrence of flints and extinct animals in pluvial deposits near Clovis, New Mexico: part IV, report on excavation at the gravel pit, 1936. Proc Acad of Nat Sci Philadelphia 89: 1–16.
  33. 33. Cotter JL (1938) The occurrence of flints and extinct animals in pluvial deposits near Clovis, New Mexico: part VI, report on field season of 1937. Proc Acad Nat Sci Philadelphia 90: 113–117.
  34. 34. Hester JJ (1972) Blackwater Draw Locality No. 1: A Stratified Early Man Site in Eastern New Mexico. Fort Burgwin Research Center Publication No. 8. Ranchos de Taos, New Mexico.
  35. 35. Howard EB (1935) Occurrence of flints and extinct animals in pluvial deposits near Clovis, New Mexico, part I, introduction. Proc Nat Sci Philadelphia 87: 299–303.
  36. 36. Warnica JM (1966) New discoveries at the Clovis site. Am Antiq 31: 345–357.
  37. 37. Frison GC, Todd LC (1986) The Colby Mammoth Site: Taphonomy and Archaeology of a Clovis Kill in Northern Wyoming. University of New Mexico Press, Albuquerque.
  38. 38. Figgins JD (1933) A further contribution to the antiquity of man in America. Colo Mus Nat Hist Proc 12: 4–8.
  39. 39. Haynes CV , McFaul M, Brunswig RH, Hopkins KD (1998) Kersey–Kuner terrace investigations at the Dent and Bernhardt sites, Colorado. Geoarchaeology 13: 201–218.
  40. 40. Stanford DJ, Jodry MA (1988) The Drake Clovis cache. Curr Res Pleistocene 5: 21–22.
  41. 41. Leonhardy FC, editor. (1966) Domebo: A Paleo-Indian Mammoth Kill in the Prairie – Plains. Contributions of the Museum of the Great Plains No. 1, Lawton, Oklahoma.
  42. 42. Gramly RM (1993) The Richey Clovis Cache. Persimmon, New York.
  43. 43. Huckell BB, Bradley BA, Mehringer PJ (2006) Flaked stone artifacts from the East Wenatchee Clovis cache. Manuscript on file, Maxwell Museum of Anthropology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, New Mexico.
  44. 44. Lyman RL, O'Brien MJ, Hayes V (1998) A mechanical and functional study of bone rods from the Richey-Roberts Clovis cache, Washington, U.S.A. J Archaeol Sci 25: 887–906.
  45. 45. Mehringer PJ (1988) Weapons cache of ancient Americans. Natl Geogr 174: 500–503.
  46. 46. Frison GC (1991) The Clovis cultural complex: new data from caches of flaked stone and worked bone artifacts. Montet-White A, Holen S (eds) Raw Material Economies among Prehistoric Hunter-Gatherers. University of Kansas Publications in Anthropology 19. Lawrence, Kansas. pp. 321–333.
  47. 47. Frison GC, Bradley BA (1999) The Fenn Cache: Clovis Weapons and Tools. One Horse Land and Cattle Company, Santa Fe, New Mexico.
  48. 48. Bement LC, Carter BJ (2003) Clovis bison hunting at the Jake Bluff site, NW Oklahoma. Curr Res Pleistocene 20: 5–7.
  49. 49. Bement LC, Carter BJ (2010) Jake Bluff: Clovis bison hunting on the southern Plains of North America. Am Antiq 75: 907–933.
  50. 50. Haury EW, Sayles EB, Wasley WW (1959) The Lehner mammoth site, southeastern Arizona. Am Antiq 25: 2–30.
  51. 51. Holliday VT, Haynes CV , Hofman JL, Meltzer DJ (1994) Geoarchaeology and geochronology of the Miami (Clovis) site, Southern High Plains of Texas. Quaternary Res 41: 234–244.
  52. 52. Sellards EH (1938) Artifacts associated with fossil elephant. B Geol Soc Am 49: 999–1010.
  53. 53. Haynes CV , Huckell BB, editors. (2007) Murray Springs: A Clovis Site with Multiple Activity Areas in the San Pedro Valley, Arizona. Anthropological Papers of the University of Arizona Number 71. Tucson.
  54. 54. Hemmings ET (1970) Early Man in the San Pedro Valley, Arizona. PhD dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of Arizona. Tucson.
  55. 55. Haury EW, Antevs E, Lance JF (1953) Artifacts with mammoth remains, Naco, Arizona. Am Antiq 19: 1–24.
  56. 56. Butler BR (1963) An early man site at Big Camas Prairie, south-central Idaho. Tebiwa 6: 22–33.
  57. 57. Butler BR, Fitzwater RJ (1965) A further note on the Clovis site at Big Camas Prairie, south-central Idaho. Tebiwa 8: 38–39.
  58. 58. Woods JC, Titmus GL (1985) A review of the Simon Clovis collection. Idaho Archaeol 8: 3–8.
  59. 59. Buchanan B (2005) Cultural Transmission and Stone Tools: A Study of Early Paleoindian Technology in North America. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico. Albuquerque.
  60. 60. Rohlf FJ (2004) TPS shareware series. Department of Ecology and Evolution, State University of New York, Stony Brook, New York. http://life.bio.sunysb.edu/morph.
  61. 61. Bailey RC, Byrnes J (1990) A new, old method for assessing measurement error in both univariate and multivariate morphometric studies. Sys Zool 39: 124–130.
  62. 62. Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1995) Biometry: The Principles and Practice of Statistics in Biological Research. 3rd ed. Freeman. New York.
  63. 63. Yezerinac SM, Lougheed SC, Handford P (1992) Measurement error and morphometric studies: statistical power and observer experience. Sys Biol 41: 471–482.
  64. 64. Strauss RE, Atanassov MN, de Oliveira JA (2003) Evaluation of the principal-component and expectation-maximization methods for estimating missing data in morphometric studies. J Vert Paleontol 23: 284–296.
  65. 65. Fligner MA, Killeen TJ (1976) Distribution-free two-sample tests for scale. J Am Stat Assoc 71: 210–213.
  66. 66. Eerkens JW, Bettinger RL (2001) Techniques for assessing standardization in artifact assemblages: can we scale material variability? Am Antiq 66: 493–504.
  67. 67. Donnelly SM, Kramer A (1999) Testing for multiple species in fossil samples: an evaluation and comparison of tests for equal relative variation. Am J Phys Anthropol 108: 507–529.
  68. 68. Hammer Ø, Harper DAT, Ryan PD (2001) PAST: Palaeontological statistics software package for education and data analysis. Palaeontol Electronica 4: 9.
  69. 69. Benjamini Y, Yekutieli D (2001) The control of false discovery rate under dependency. Ann Stat 29: 1165–1188.
  70. 70. Narum SR (2006) Beyond Bonferroni: less conservative analyses for conservation genetics. Conserv Genet 7: 783–787.
  71. 71. Buchanan BB, Kilby JD, Huckell BB, O'Brien MJ, Collard MC (2012) A morphometric assessment of the intended function of cached Clovis points. PLoS One 7: 1–12.