Peer Review History
Original SubmissionJuly 8, 2021 |
---|
PONE-D-21-22263The Impact of Hyperlinks, Skim Reading and Perceived Importance when Reading on the WebPLOS ONE Dear Dr. Fitzsimmons, Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. As you will see in the comments below, both reviewers believe that your manuscript will make a significant contribution to the empirical literature on reading. However, they raised a number of important concerns, particularly with regard to the statistical analyses (e.g., no inclusion of a power analysis) and clarity (e.g., parts of the introduction require some additional elaboration). Could you please address these concerns in a revised version of your manuscript by Nov 06 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file. Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:
If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter. If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols. We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript. Kind regards, Veronica Whitford Academic Editor PLOS ONE Journal requirements: When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements. 1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 2. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure: “GF was funded by an EPSRC grant for the Doctoral Training Centre in Web Science: EP/G036926/1. This work formed a part of a PhD completed in the Web Science DTC.” Please state what role the funders took in the study. If the funders had no role, please state: "The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript." If this statement is not correct you must amend it as needed. Please include this amended Role of Funder statement in your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf [Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.] Reviewers' comments: Reviewer's Responses to Questions Comments to the Author 1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: No ********** 4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 5. Review Comments to the Author Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters) Reviewer #1: Dear Authors, I really appreciate the opportunity to read and revise your manuscript. I find the topic very interesting, timely, and relevant. Your study offers the opportunity to increase our knowledge in the field of digital reading. I especially appreciate that your work sheds more light on how the presence of hyperlinks affects reading behavior. Your study is well designed, and I also value positively the use of LMM analyses. Moreover, I enjoyed reading your report. However, I think that there are some issues that should be addressed. I hope that my comments help you improve the manuscript and enhance the relevance of your work. ABSTRACT: - The use of the eye-tracking technique to measure reading times should be reported in the abstract, not only for the sake of clarity, but also to highlight one of the strengths of this work. - In my opinion, authors should specify that participants for Study 1 and 2 were not the same. As currently worded, the reader could interpret that it was the same sample. INTRODUCTION: - I wonder if the authors could use a more recent reference to support the idea that readers tend to skim when reading on the web, as the current one (Liu, 2005) appears to be somewhat old given the rapid evolution in the widespread use of the Internet. - I suggest to modify the heading ‘Reading on the Web and Importance’ -> ‘Reading on the Web and Text Importance’ - A brief introduction on the use of the eye-tracking technique as a measure in (digital) reading research could be included at the beginning of the section ‘Reading on the web and importance’. What are the more relevant measures, and what they are assumed to indicate (e.g., attention allocation, strategic reading…). This would be informative for those readers that are not familiar with this methodology. - p. 3, line 62: “of reading” -> on reading. - p. 3, line 62: I think that what a “static environment” is should be clarified at his point. - I miss further elaboration on the possible consequences of the interaction between hyperlinks, perceived importance, and skim reading on the quality of readers’ information processing when reading on the web, as this is the most important practical consequence of these reading practices. STUDY 1: Method: - I miss information on which degrees participants are enrolled in. - In a footnote, the authors explain that the imbalance between sub-samples’ size is due to the analyses performed. However, it still remains not clear to me. Why the analyses resulted in different group sizes? - Authors say that the sample sizes are typical of eye-tracking reading research. However, this is not the case for Study 1. The sample size in this study appears somewhat small to me. In my opinion, this should be mentioned as a limitation. - I think that the experimental setting (i.e., the use of 2 screens presenting the same text differently formatted) could appear somewhat strange to the reader. Although the authors explain in the ‘Procedure’ section how the participants interact with both devices, in my opinion this should be briefly clarified in the ‘Apparatus’ section, namely, that the participants read the sentence-by-sentence text on the laptop after they read the whole text on the desktop computer). - I miss information on the word length of the texts used. - When the authors refer to “target words” in ‘Stimuli and Design’ section, do they refer to linked words? As far as I understood, they are not “targets” in this study, so I think that the authors should refer to them, in this case, as hyperlinks instead of target words. - Why four sentences were inserted into each article? - If I am not wrong, the hyperlinks consisted of only one word in all cases (i.e., the target words in the previous Fitzsimmons et al.’s study). Whether this is the case or not, the number of words the hyperlinks consisted of should be explicitly mentioned. - Specify that the number of links were measured per sentence, and the position on the screen was also for each sentence. - If I understood well, the presence of links was counterbalanced across sentences and participants in the linked passages group. This allows to examine the effect of links regardless of the sentence content, as all sentences are linked taking the four versions altogether. For the sake of clarity, I think that this could be explicitly mentioned. - Authors could include an “Analyses” subsection including the first paragraph of the ‘Results’ section. Results: - Please revise APA standards. Statistical indexes (SD, p… must be italicized) - P. 11: Why the difference between Passage types may be due to lower rates of the unlinked sentences in the linked texts? Why the authors did not perform between-groups comparisons to examine this possibility? - I think that Figure 2 it is not fully self-explanatory. I understand that different boxes correspond to different positions on page, but this is not well clarified in the figure. The same with Figure 6 and length of sentence. - P. 13: I think there is something wrong when reporting descriptive data for the ratings of linked vs unlinked sentences. Mean and SD of the ratings for the whole linked texts in the previous section (‘Effect of Hyperlinks’) are the same than those for only the unlinked sentences in such texts. This is not possible, given that the ratings for the linked sentences are higher. - The scale for the independent continuous variables in the figures show negative values because the authors centered them. However, I suggest to change these scales in the figures by including not centered values. I think this would make it easy to visually interpret the results to the reader. - Please, revise the variable names in the figures, e.g., Position_on_Page -> Position on page. Discussion: - As I mentioned earlier, I do not fully understand which result the authors based on to conclude that the unlinked sentences were rated lower in the presence of other linked sentences than the same sentences in unlinked texts. The authors did not compare differences in the rating for unlinked sentences between passage-type groups, however they state in the discussion that: “When a sentence does not contain links but is in a hypertext environment with other sentences that do contain links, it is rated as being of lower importance”. - P 17. Authors conclude that “long sentences with many links were rated higher, no matter the position of the sentence on the page. This suggests that links do make a sentence more important, especially for long sentences”. This conclusion is based on the two-way interaction between sentence length and position. As far as I understood, the fact that long sentences were rated lower at the end of the page, but higher when they contain links, supports this conclusion. I infer that authors meant this when saying ‘when a sentence near the bottom of the page would be rated quite low, if it contains a link it gets a boost of importance”. But I think that the interaction should be explicitly mentioned here. - P. 18. Authors also conclude that “readers recognise the “inverted pyramid” structure of webpages, when providing offline ratings of importance”. I think that the authors did not controlled whether the most relevant content was actually located at the top of the texts used in the study, thus, in my opinion, it is not possible to conclude that the participants “recognised” the inverted pyramid structure. EXPERIMENT 2: Intro: - P.19. Authors wrote: “By using links, the reader can efficiently identify important information in the text and move through the text faster…”. However, it depends on whether the links are located in the sentences including important information, which is not always the case on the web. In my opinion, the fact that locating links in non-relevant sentences can mislead the reader and, therefore, hinder comprehension should be further discussed, especially in the ‘Discussion’ section. - P.21: I think that the direction of the association between the wrap-up effect and the other variables (information density, processing capacity, literacy development) should be clarified. Are they negatively or positively associated? Method: - Please, provide more information about the comprehension questions. Did they address text-based or inferential comprehension? Also, providing some examples of the questions would be illustrative. Results: - In my opinion, the experimental measures should be previously reported in the Method section. The same with the ‘Analyses’. - When reporting the measures, please specify what the authors considered as “first pass reading”. - Why the authors did not constrain their analyses to reading time per character, as this is the standard measure in eye-tracking reading research? Otherwise, reading times and sentence length are confounded. - P. 31. Please include spaces between statistical letters, symbols, and values when reporting results from ANOVA. - P. 31. The authors say that “…which suggests that the participants were to a degree engaged in a successful adaptive strategy because they had improved accuracy for comprehension questions relating to the most important information.” I would avoid the use of “successful” as it depends on whether the sentences identified as important are actually important based on the text content. Results from Study 1 show that sentence importance is rated based on features other than semantic content, thus an adaptive strategy would depend on, for example, whether links are located in semantically relevant sentences. Indeed, I would say that a reading strategy based on the presence of links seems to be less effective than relying on semantic content. As I mentioned before, links are often located in less relevant sentences on the web. Indeed, links often serve to provide additional, complementary information, hence secondary for the text content. Discussion: - I think that the authors did not sufficiently addressed the fact that, in this study, online measures showed that readers were not sensitive when skim reading to the signals they use as indicators of relevance when reading for comprehension. In this regard, this result is contrary to those in previous research discussed in the introduction of the manuscript (p. 4: “These findings suggested readers were prioritising the processing of visually salient words while skim reading webpage”). - In relation to the above, authors conclude on p. 33 that: “Given sentences with more links were rated as more important, and skim reading leads to increased focus on links [1], it seems readers use links as signals through the text to anchor attention, leading to increased comprehension of those sentences.” This conclusion is based on previous results that are in conflict with findings in the current study, as I noted above. - I think that the fact that readers had too broad reading goals (i.e., reading for comprehension or reading for skimming) could influence their effort in detecting important parts of the texts when skim reading. It is possible that a more specific reading goal would have encouraged them to identify important parts also when skimming and this could have been reflected in their reading behavior. I would mention this as a limitation of the study. GENERAL DISCUSSION - In p. 35 it is concluded that “it could be suggested that the multi-faceted signal functions of hyperlinks cause them to be considered as a more important typographical cue than others”. In my opinion, a conclusion like that needs to be based on an experimental comparison between the influence of hyperlinks and, for example, the influence of mere highlighting. I suggest to mention this in the manucript. - My main concern about the General Discussion is that I think that the authors should elaborate on the fact that readers rely on physical cues regardless of semantic content which could impact negatively reading comprehension when reading on the web. As I noted above, the presence of links is not a guarantee of content relevance, but sometimes they are located in sentences that are secondary for the text main content to provide additional info. Other times, links are even located in random words to link to commercial websites. Thus, the findings of this study have important educational implications that must be considered when training students to gather information from the web. - In relation to the above, I think that it could be suggested that further research is needed to examine to what extent readers base their use of reading strategies on cues other than semantic relevance, which must ultimately be the cues on which a good (digital) reader must base their decisions. - I also miss further discussion on why readers consider longer sentences as more important only at the top of the page and, conversely, shorter sentences are considered more important at the bottom. If I may suggest, a possible explanation is that readers’ attention or allocation of cognitive effort decreases as they move down the webs and they then focus on shorter sentences. As they focus more on these sentences and, consequently, they comprehend them better, this could be an explanation for an increased perceived importance. Reviewer #2: Dear Prof. Whitford, The manuscript entitled " The Impact of Hyperlinks, Skim Reading and Perceived Importance when Reading on the Web." is an empirical paper investigating the effect of highlighting and text position on the intelligibility of a text. The authors implemented two studies, one rating study investigating subjective importance ratings of sentences and a second study, based on the ratings from study one, that investigates specific online reading behaviors with eye-tracking methodology. The relatively complex analysis required careful implementation and design. The study resulted in several highly interesting findings, such as the influence of highlighting, text position, and sentence length. Besides, it showed that the latter effects were significantly reduced when participants implemented fast skimming compared to regular reading. The study is a highly valuable piece of evidence, but several issues prevent me from recommending the manuscript for publication. Please find my specific comments below. Besides, I would recommend making data and code related to the paper openly available. Best wishes, Benjamin Gagl #Comments ##Statistical power. Please provide argumentation for the sample size based on statistical power. If no a-priory power estimation was implemented, please give a post-hoc assessment for a range of potentially interesting effect sizes. Such an analysis would be highly informative for future replication efforts. Please note, recent developments now provide convenient tools for power estimations based on LMM models (e.g., see Green, P., & MacLeod, C. J. (2016). SIMR: an R package for power analysis of generalized linear mixed models by simulation. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 7(4), 493-498. or Kumle, L., Vo, M. L. H., & Draschkow, D. (2021). Estimating power in (generalized) linear mixed models: An open introduction and tutorial in R. Behavior Research Methods, 1-16.). ##More details on the stimuli. Considerable detail is missing in the description of the stimuli. Starting with the N of trials/sentences/paragraphs, or potential informativeness measures the sentences (e.g., number of content words). Also, characteristics of hyperlinked words (lengths, status, frequency etc.). Besides, it is currently not clear to me how one can end up with a sentence length or position on a page that is negative (e.g., see Figure 2/3). Would you mind providing more information here. ##Presentation of results in general. From a presentation standpoint, it makes sense to start with higher-order interactions first. This is as only resolving them first would make the statistical results fully accessible. Similar to your procedure for a parsimonious random effect structure. Also, one could, at times, remove figures that depict the lower-order interactions to simplify the presentation (e.g., Fig. 1/7) ##p. 12 270-272: Please revisit this sentence. It is currently unclear which terms were excluded based on this sentence. Would you please elaborate on the procedure in more detail. ##Elaborate on the differences between skim and normal reading. E.g., the role of para/extra-foveal processing of text (e.g., as investigated in Gagl, 2016). ##Details of the eye-movement measurement procedure. E.g., calibration procedure, fixation detection algorithm, etc. The current description is missing detail here. For guidance, see the parameter list in the BEP-20 extension proposal of the BIDS data standard (https://docs.google.com/document/d/1eggzTCzSHG3AEKhtnEDbcdk-2avXN6I94X8aUPEBVsw/edit#heading=h.1yd8ejg9cdbt). Also, comments are welcome! #Minors ##Please harmonize the description of random effects, e.g., cp. Description in Table 1 and 3. ##Figure 8/9. Why is the figure style different from, e.g., Fig.10 (i.e., one figure with different line coloring). Using the same style would help to see the differences in the interactions better. ********** 6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files. If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public. Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #2: Yes: Benjamin Gagl [NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.] While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step. |
Revision 1 |
PONE-D-21-22263R1 The Impact of Hyperlinks, Skim Reading and Perceived Importance when Reading on the WebPLOS ONE Dear Dr. Fitzsimmons, Thank you once for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. As you will see below, both Reviewers 1 and 2 agree that your manuscript was considerably strengthened by the last round of revisions and that you have satisfactorily addressed their major concerns. However, they have noted some relatively minor issues that would need to be addressed before the manuscript is accepted for publication. Could you please submit the revised version by March 11, 2022? If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file. Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:
If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols. We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript. Kind regards, Veronica Whitford, Ph.D. Academic Editor PLOS ONE Reviewers' comments: Reviewer's Responses to Questions Comments to the Author 1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation. Reviewer #1: (No Response) Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed ********** 2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 6. Review Comments to the Author Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters) Reviewer #1: I think that you satisfactorily addressed my comments and your decisions on not following some of them were satisfactorily justified. I only have some additional minor comments. Once again, thank you for giving me the opportunity to help you improve your manuscript: - I think that total number of words of each text should be reported. - Authors say that all links were a single word, but I can see in the provided stimuli example, there were links consisting of more than one word. - Spaces between “F” and the “d.f.” parenthesis must be removed in the statistical results from ANOVAs. - The variable “Position on page” is still written as “Position_on_page” in Figure 1. - This is only a suggestion. When discussing on the fact that links are not always relevant on the web, I would also mention those cases in which links bring the reader to some information that is related to the text, but it is not relevant to its main ideas (as it is the case of Wikipedia). If I am not wrong, authors currently mention only those links inserted for commercial purposes. - I am really sorry, but I couldn’t find the following, maybe due to the fact that page and line numbers provided in your responses don’t match those in the pdf that I downloaded. See my comment and your response: In p. 35 it is concluded that “it could be suggested that the multi-faceted signal functions of hyperlinks cause them to be considered as a more important typographical cue than others”. In my opinion, a conclusion like that needs to be based on an experimental comparison between the influence of hyperlinks and, for example, the influence of mere highlighting. I suggest to mention this in the manuscript. We now mention this point and reference the experimental work that exists exploring this particular comparison for support (lines 1086, page 37) Reviewer #2: Dear Prof. Whitford, The revised version of the manuscript entitled " The Impact of Hyperlinks, Skim Reading and Perceived Importance when Reading on the Web " considered all of my concerns. The quality of the manuscript increased considerably, but a few minor issues prevent me from recommending the manuscript for publication yet. Please find my specific comments below. Best wishes, Benjamin Gagl #Minor Issues ##Statistical power. The authors now provide a post-power analysis, but an essential parameter for the interpretation of the power analysis is missing, i.e., the effect size. Several effect sizes are presented in the results section, so it would be interesting which effect sizes have been used. Also, if multiple effect sizes have been used, please provide a range for the resulting power values. ##High vs. low frequency comparison (p11; L 245). Would you mind not estimating statistics regarding differences in stimulus characteristics? This procedure has been identified as problematic, and the problem is described in detail here: Sassenhagen, J., & Alday, P. M. (2016). A common misapplication of statistical inference: Nuisance control with null-hypothesis significance tests. Brain and language, 162, 42-45. Instead, a value range including how much overlap was present would be more informative. ##L12 p. 1: Line sweep missing. ********** 7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files. If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public. Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #2: Yes: Benjamin Gagl [NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.] While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step. |
Revision 2 |
The Impact of Hyperlinks, Skim Reading and Perceived Importance when Reading on the Web PONE-D-21-22263R2 Dear Dr. Fitzsimmons, We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements. Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication. An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org. Kind regards, Veronica Whitford, Ph.D. Academic Editor PLOS ONE |
Formally Accepted |
PONE-D-21-22263R2 The Impact of Hyperlinks, Skim Reading and Perceived Importance when Reading on the Web Dear Dr. Fitzsimmons: I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org. If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org. Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access. Kind regards, PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff on behalf of Dr. Veronica Whitford Academic Editor PLOS ONE |
Open letter on the publication of peer review reports
PLOS recognizes the benefits of transparency in the peer review process. Therefore, we enable the publication of all of the content of peer review and author responses alongside final, published articles. Reviewers remain anonymous, unless they choose to reveal their names.
We encourage other journals to join us in this initiative. We hope that our action inspires the community, including researchers, research funders, and research institutions, to recognize the benefits of published peer review reports for all parts of the research system.
Learn more at ASAPbio .