Peer Review History
Original SubmissionFebruary 22, 2021 |
---|
PONE-D-21-05690 Individual pupil size changes as a robust indicator of cognitive familiarity differences PLOS ONE Dear Dr. Franzen, Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. Please submit your revised manuscript by Apr 29 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file. Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:
If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter. If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript. Kind regards, Manuel Spitschan Academic Editor PLOS ONE Additional Editor Comments: Please specifically address the reviewer's points on framing the research, limitations in the current phase scrambling techniques and the analytic strategy. Journal Requirements: When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements. 1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 2. We note that Figure 1 in your submission contain copyrighted images. All PLOS content is published under the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which means that the manuscript, images, and Supporting Information files will be freely available online, and any third party is permitted to access, download, copy, distribute, and use these materials in any way, even commercially, with proper attribution. For more information, see our copyright guidelines: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/licenses-and-copyright. We require you to either (1) present written permission from the copyright holder to publish these figures specifically under the CC BY 4.0 license, or (2) remove the figures from your submission: 2.1. You may seek permission from the original copyright holder of Figure 1 to publish the content specifically under the CC BY 4.0 license. We recommend that you contact the original copyright holder with the Content Permission Form (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=7c09/content-permission-form.pdf) and the following text: “I request permission for the open-access journal PLOS ONE to publish XXX under the Creative Commons Attribution License (CCAL) CC BY 4.0 (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Please be aware that this license allows unrestricted use and distribution, even commercially, by third parties. Please reply and provide explicit written permission to publish XXX under a CC BY license and complete the attached form.” Please upload the completed Content Permission Form or other proof of granted permissions as an "Other" file with your submission. In the figure caption of the copyrighted figure, please include the following text: “Reprinted from [ref] under a CC BY license, with permission from [name of publisher], original copyright [original copyright year].” 2.2. If you are unable to obtain permission from the original copyright holder to publish these figures under the CC BY 4.0 license or if the copyright holder’s requirements are incompatible with the CC BY 4.0 license, please either i) remove the figure or ii) supply a replacement figure that complies with the CC BY 4.0 license. Please check copyright information on all replacement figures and update the figure caption with source information. If applicable, please specify in the figure caption text when a figure is similar but not identical to the original image and is therefore for illustrative purposes only. 3. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information. [Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.] Reviewers' comments: Reviewer's Responses to Questions Comments to the Author 1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. Reviewer #1: Partly Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified. Reviewer #1: No Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 5. Review Comments to the Author Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters) Reviewer #1: The manuscript describes an experiment into the effects of brand-familiarity on pupil size. The experimental paradigm was designed with care to ensure that the observed pupillometric effects could be attributed to brand-familiarity rather than some other aspect of the stimuli. Brand-familiarity was initially assessed in an independent online study with a large representative sample size (n=763), precluding the requirement to obtain self-report measures from participants in the main pupillometry experiment (n=15). The study found that pupil size was greater on average when viewing familiar vs. unfamiliar brands, and additional analyses revealed how this difference is modulated by brand category and consumer behaviour. * Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? For the most part, the study is technically sound, but I note the following issues: 1) The sample size for the pupillometry experiment is low, especially in light of the small effect size and claims of a ‘robust indicator of brand familiarity’ (lines 492—495). Consider validating this claim by collecting more data or, alternatively, providing further statistical evidence (e.g., from simulations) that the effect is not down to chance. 2) The abstract indicates that the primary intention of the paper is to introduce a novel paradigm for exploring brand familiarity, which relegates the actual research to an example application of the paradigm. Note also that previous pupillometry studies have used a scrambled image mask approach (e.g., Nuske et al., 2014). 3) There is limited discussion of how the observed effects may relate to established theory regarding the mechanisms of cognitive pupil control. For example, the role of the locus coeruleus, and Adaptive Gain Theory (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005) or ‘Network Reset’ (Bouret and Sara, 2005). 4) Provide further explanation for why it is interesting to explore the pupil data with respect to brand category. What are the practical advantages of knowing this? 5) Note that pupil size can be affected by illusions of brightness. Laeng et al. (2012) first showed this, but there have been replications with real-world images (e.g., Castelotti et al., 2020). The image of Ovomaltine in Figure 1c is actually redolent of the stimuli used by Laeng et al. (2012), and it is possible that brightness illusions could be affecting the data for individual stimuli. This is unlikely to pose a major issue for the study, but it is worth mentioning. * Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? The analyses appear to be in good order, but I have the following recommendations: 1) In addition to Maris & Oostenwald (2007), cite Sassenhagen & Draschkow (2019) and follow their recommendations for the reporting of cluster-based permutation tests. It is my understanding that these tests indicate only whether an effect was, or was not, present in the data, and that they should not be used to infer the temporal locus of an effect. For example, lines 412-416 discuss the early vs. late significant clusters, but I would have thought it would be appropriate to report only the largest significant cluster. Also specify what software was used to perform the tests (e.g., custom implementation, MNE, Fieldtrip, etc.) 2) Clarify why only 2.5 s of data for the 3 s stimulus period were analysed and whether this affected the significance of the results. 3) It would be informative to see the pupil data for the scrambled image. Also, it would be helpful to comment on why the pupil is on average already 5% greater than baseline at the start of the stimulus period. Could this be effects of anticipation, with participants being able to accurately predict when the stimulus would appear? Note that a temporal jitter for the duration of the scrambled image may have ameliorated this affect by making it difficult for participants to predict the onset of the stimulus. 4) Pupil data with EyeLink systems are affected by optical distortion due to eye movements. It would therefore help to clarify how well participants were able to maintain central fixation, and to exclude trials with large eye movements. See Hayes & Petrov (2015) for more information on this. Minor points: 1) Line 227 – It is inaccurate to say that ‘participants’ eyes’ were calibrated’, as it is the eye tracker that gets calibrated. Say rather that a 9-point calibration was performed. 2) Line 268 – report degrees of visual angle rather than pixels * Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? Only the averages per participant are available on the OSF. Please make all of the underlying data available. * Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? Note the following minor issues: 1) Line 308: Clarify what is meant by ‘pupil elasticity’ 2) Lines 54-56: consider revising sentence 3) Lines 236-237: elaborate on the claim that the background color likely avoids discomfort 4) Line 466: ‘bootrapped’ 5) Lines 518-520: consider revising sentence 6) Lines 572-573: consider revising sentence Reviewer #2: Major comments This paper investigates whether familiarity with images of consumer products modulates pupil responses to the onset of these images. It is found that products or brands, that are unfamiliar to observers, evoke stronger pupil constrictions around 500-1000ms and/or relatively weaker dilation at later time points than familiar products. This finding is in line with previous findings but the design is novel because the authors tested the novel/old effect on pupil size in a more applied context (i.e., market research) and tried to circumvent distorting effects of image features. The paper is well written, the study design is clear and logic, and the results are relevant. However, some of the framing and claims need to be modified before I can recommend this paper for publication. First, I expect that readers would like to know how well familiar products can be dissociated from unfamiliar products by using the pupil as an objective measure. Second, the claims concerning the proposedly innovative character of the experimental design need to be toned down. Major point 1. Will market research truly benefit from pupillometry? I deem the applied context of this experiment as the most novel and remarkable aspect of the study, and, as such, wonder how well the pupil dissociates familiar from unfamiliar images on a trial level. I would appreciate it if the authors could provide e.g. area under the curve measures reflecting sensitivity and specificity of pupillometry as a signal detection method. My expectation is that it will be hard to decide per product whether it is truly familiar to an observer or consumer, making pupillometry a less applicable method for market research companies. Nonetheless, perhaps the authors can provide some guidelines on whether or not and how to apply pupillometry in practice. Major point 2. Currently the authors frame their paper mostly on the proposedly innovative aspect of their procedure, namely using a scrambled baseline image presentation before showing the target image. The idea is that pupil size already changes in response to the colors and luminance levels of the baseline image (see Gamlin et al., 1998, for pupil responses to colors and other features). When the target image appears thereafter, the pupil will only respond to the familiarity of the object, not to the change in feature content, which is here not of interest and adds unwanted variance to the pupil response and may potentially cover up the effect of familiarity. While I understand the argumentation, I see two problems with how this issue was addressed and “sold” to the readers. First, the scrambled images do, to some degree, but not fully control for luminance and other image features such as spatial frequency (Weiskrantz et al., 1998). For instance, brightness perception and its effects on pupil size highly depends on which regions of an image are gazed at (see Derksen et al., 2018, for subtle effects of brightness distributions across images). While the mean luminance (all pixels) of the scrambled baseline image may be equal to the luminance of the target image, the bright image borders are possibly less often fixated or covertly attended after the image is shown, still affecting the early pupil constriction and following dilation. Also, the currente scrambled images do not adapt the pupil to spatial frequency content and the presence of an initial pupil constriction around 500ms marks that some feature changes still evoke a pupil response (spatial frequency, contrast, foveal luminance, object contours, etc.). A phase-scrambled version of an object within the images would serve as better controls (e.g., phase-scrambled images are often used in face perception studies). Second, the use of baseline images is not innovative, but a standard procedure in perception and vision sciences. The authors should thus consider toning down their claims with respect to the innovative aspect of the procedure, crediting previous work on baseline control images, and discussing the limitations of their baseline implementation. Minor comments - 69-70 it will help the reader to clarify what is meant with implicit indicators. Do you mean measures from tasks like the implicit association task (IAT)? - Please double-check the references (e.g. Sirois & Brisson misses the title). References Derksen, M., van Alphen, J., Schaap, S., Mathot, S., & Naber, M. (2018). Pupil mimicry is the result of brightness perception of the iris and pupil. Journal of cognition, 1(1). Weiskrantz, L., Cowey, A., & Le Mare, C. (1998). Learning from the pupil: a spatial visual channel in the absence of V1 in monkey and human. Brain: a journal of neurology, 121(6), 1065-1072. Gamlin, P. D., Zhang, H., Harlow, A., & Barbur, J. L. (1998). Pupil responses to stimulus color, structure and light flux increments in the rhesus monkey. Vision research, 38(21), 3353-3358. Signed review Marnix Naber ********** 6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files. If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public. Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy. Reviewer #1: Yes: Dr Joel T. Martin Reviewer #2: Yes: Marnix Naber [NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.] While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step. |
Revision 1 |
PONE-D-21-05690R1 Individual pupil size changes as a robust indicator of cognitive familiarity differences PLOS ONE Dear Dr. Franzen, Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. Please submit your revised manuscript by Jul 12 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file. Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:
If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter. If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols. We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript. Kind regards, Manuel Spitschan Academic Editor PLOS ONE Journal Requirements: Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice. Additional Editor Comments (if provided): I would kindly ask you to make the minor changes suggested by Reviewer 2. [Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.] Reviewers' comments: Reviewer's Responses to Questions Comments to the Author 1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation. Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed ********** 2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions? The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English? PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here. Reviewer #1: Yes Reviewer #2: Yes ********** 6. Review Comments to the Author Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters) Reviewer #1: All comments and concerns were addressed. The manuscript is now in my opinion technically sound, with data that support the conclusions and rigorous statistical analyses. Underlying data have been made availalble and the manuscript is in standard english. Reviewer #2: The authors have addressed all my comments. However, my main concerns regarding phase-scrambling as a better baseline and the question how feasible pupillometry is as an application in market reserach remain. Also, I don't think that the plotting of fixation points helps to check for a potential confound of gaze angle (larger gaze angles lead to smaller pupil sizes) and thus whether the pupil differences between the familiar and unfamiliar conditions are confounded by the radius of gaze angle (see point 4 of reviewer 1). First, I do not agree with the authors argumentation why phase-scrambling is problematic. Oliva & Torralba's results refer to a computer vision algorithm trained to categorize natural scene images converted to the phase domain. This paradigm and task is totally different from humans categorizing phase-scrambled images, which is really really hard, if not impossible with 0% phase coherence. Zero percent phase coherence also gets rid of any clues about contour. That phase-scrambling is a relatively advanced technique, is also not a good reason not to choose it. I understand it is out of the scope of the study to redo the experiment with better baseline images. I only expected that the authors at least consider phase-scrambling as an improved baseline paradigm to be described in the discussion. Because the authors have removed any reference to their baseline method as novel and innovative, the current, rather simple baseline implementation isn't a major issue anymore. Second, please correct me if I am wrong, but I understand from the new discriminant analysis that familiar versus unfamiliar images can be classified based on pupil size merely above chance performance (0.52>0.50). This low score basically suggests that pupillometry is not a feasible technique to determine a person's image familiarity and that it is much more efficient to just ask them how familiar observers are with the images. This should be discussed. Although this finding reduces the impact and novelty of the findings, I still appreciate the effort the authors have put in this experiment and paper, and because the paper generalizes previous findings to an applied setting, I think the paper remains relevant to the field of pupillometry and market research. Third, I would like to respond to the reply to a point made by the other reviewer about how optical distortion due to the difference between gaze and eye-tracker angle on pupil size. I can imagine why the authors do not want to invest a lot of time in implementing a model and controlling for a potential confound, although personally I would try to at least get more insights into the potential presence of this confound, if not only to satisfy the reviewer and show appreciation for their feedback. I suggest that the authors calculate the mean gaze angle deviation from the center of the screen (i.e., average fixation radius; mean(sqrt((x_gaze-width_screen/2)^2 + y_gaze-height_screen/2)^2)) per familiarity condition and see whether this differs. As a second control analysis, the authors should plot pupil size as a function of horizontal and vertical gaze position. They can bin gaze positions across the screen locations and plot the average pupil size per bin. If any differences in pupil size are observed (between familiar and unfamiliar, and across fixated screen locations), I suggest to incorporate gaze radius, and/or horizontal and vertical gaze positions as nonlinear, covariates in the discriminant analysis to see how much it explains away the difference in pupil size between the conditions. ********** 7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files. If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public. Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy. Reviewer #1: Yes: Joel T. Martin Reviewer #2: Yes: Marnix Naber [NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.] While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step. |
Revision 2 |
Individual pupil size changes as a robust indicator of cognitive familiarity differences PONE-D-21-05690R2 Dear Dr. Franzen, We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements. Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication. An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org. Kind regards, Manuel Spitschan Academic Editor PLOS ONE Additional Editor Comments (optional): Reviewers' comments: |
Formally Accepted |
PONE-D-21-05690R2 Individual pupil size changes as a robust indicator of cognitive familiarity differences Dear Dr. Franzen: I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org. If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org. Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access. Kind regards, PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff on behalf of Dr. Manuel Spitschan Academic Editor PLOS ONE |
Open letter on the publication of peer review reports
PLOS recognizes the benefits of transparency in the peer review process. Therefore, we enable the publication of all of the content of peer review and author responses alongside final, published articles. Reviewers remain anonymous, unless they choose to reveal their names.
We encourage other journals to join us in this initiative. We hope that our action inspires the community, including researchers, research funders, and research institutions, to recognize the benefits of published peer review reports for all parts of the research system.
Learn more at ASAPbio .