Figures
Abstract
Background
The proportion of energy from free sugars and saturated fat currently exceeds the UK-recommended intake across all age groups. Recognising the limits of reformulation programmes, the government in England has announced their intention to introduce legislation to restrict the promotion of foods high in free sugars, salt, and saturated fats in prominent store locations. Here, we evaluated a grocery store intervention to remove seasonal confectionery from prominent locations within a major UK supermarket.
Methods and findings
A nonrandomised controlled intervention study with interrupted time series (ITS) analysis was used. Data were analysed from 34 intervention stores located in 2 London boroughs and 151 matched control stores located elsewhere in the UK owned by the same retailer. Stores were matched based on store size and overall sales during the previous year. Between 15 February 2019 and 3 April 2019 (before Easter), stores removed free-standing promotional display units of seasonal confectionery from prominent areas, although these products were available for purchase elsewhere in the store.
Store-level weekly sales (units, weight (g), and value (£)) of seasonal chocolate confectionery products were used in primary analyses, with data from 1 January 2018 to 24 November 2019. Secondary outcomes included total energy, fat, saturated fat, and sugars from all in-store purchases. Multivariable hierarchical models were used to investigate pre/post differences in weekly sales of confectionery in intervention versus control stores. ITS analyses were used to evaluate differences in level and trends after intervention implementation.
Over a preintervention baseline period (15 February 2018 to 3 April 2018), there were no significant differences in sales (units, weight, and value) of all chocolate confectionery between intervention versus control stores. After intervention implementation, there was an attenuation in the seasonal increase of confectionery sales (units) in intervention stores compared to control (+5% versus +18%; P < 0.001), with similar effects on weight (g) (+12% versus +31%; P < 0.001) and value (£) (−3% versus +10%; P < 0.001). ITS analyses generally showed statistically significant differences in the level at the point of intervention (P ranges 0.010 to 0.067) but also in the trend afterwards (P ranges 0.024 to 0.053), indicating that the initial difference between intervention and control stores reduced over time. There was a significant difference in level change in total energy sold, adjusted for the total weight of food and drink (kcal/g, P = 0.002), and total fat (fat/g) (P = 0.023), but no significant changes in saturated fat or sugars from total sales in ITS models. There was no evidence that the main results varied across store deprivation index. The limitations of this study include the lack of randomisation, residual confounding from unmeasured variables, absolute differences in trends and sales between intervention versus control stores, and no independent measures of intervention fidelity.
Conclusions
Removal of chocolate confectionery from prominent locations was associated with reduced purchases of these products, of sufficient magnitude to observe a reduction in the energy content of total food purchases. These results from a “real-world” intervention provide promising evidence that the proposed legislation in England to restrict promotions of less healthy items in prominent locations may help reduce overconsumption.
Author summary
Why was the study done?
- The prevalence of obesity in the UK is continuing to increase especially in the most deprived areas.
- The proportion of energy from free sugars and saturated fat currently exceeds the UK-recommended intake across all age groups. Despite ambitious sugar reduction targets set by Public Health England, there has been little change in the confectionery category.
- Recognising the limits of reformulation programmes, the Government in England has announced their intention to introduce legislation to restrict the promotion of foods high in free sugars, salt, and saturated fats in prominent locations in grocery stores.
What did the researchers find?
- We partnered with a large UK food retailer to evaluate an intervention to remove seasonal chocolate confectionery from prominent areas of the store, specifically end-of-aisles and entrance areas, over 7 weeks before the Easter period.
- The intervention showed a significant attenuation of the seasonal increase in confectionery sales in intervention stores compared to control stores, with an absolute difference of approximately 127 units (approximately 21 kg) of confectionery per store per week.
- During the intervention period, there were significant reductions in total energy and fat from all food-related purchases in the stores implementing the intervention, but no significant changes in saturated fat or total sugars.
- There was no evidence that the main results varied according to the store deprivation index.
What do these findings mean?
- The Government in England has recently signalled its intention to bring forward legislation to restrict promotions of less healthy items in prominent locations in grocery stores, to help reduce overconsumption.
- This study provides new evidence that the government proposals to restrict foods high in fat, sugar, and salt from prominent locations in stores could lead to measurable reductions in total energy (calories) from purchases.
Citation: Piernas C, Harmer G, Jebb SA (2022) Removing seasonal confectionery from prominent store locations and purchasing behaviour within a major UK supermarket: Evaluation of a nonrandomised controlled intervention study. PLoS Med 19(3): e1003951. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1003951
Academic Editor: Jean Adams, University of Cambridge, UNITED KINGDOM
Received: September 13, 2021; Accepted: February 23, 2022; Published: March 24, 2022
Copyright: © 2022 Piernas et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: This research was conducted according to a framework collaboration agreement between the University of Oxford and the food retailers. Access to the study dataset by external researchers is not permitted as this is defined as confidential information in the agreement. Access to the study data by external researchers will require the expressed written consent of the retailer. Please contact hw@theconsumergoodsforum.com. Access to the statistical code used in this analysis will be reviewed and granted upon request by the Nuffield Department of Primary Care PRimDISC committee (primdisc@phc.ox.ac.uk).
Funding: This study received funding from Guy’s and St Thomas’ Foundation (grant EIC181003). GH, CP and SJ are funded by the National Institute of Health Research (NIHR) Applied Research Collaborations Oxford. SJ is a NIHR Senior Investigator funded by the Oxford Biomedical Research Centre. The funders had no role in designing the study, data collection, analysis, interpretation of data, writing the report, or the decision to submit the report for publication.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Abbreviations: CGF, Consumer Goods Forum; HFSS, high in fat, sugar, or salt; IMD, Index of Multiple Deprivation; ITS, interrupted time series; SES, socioeconomic status; SSB, sugar-sweetened beverage; TIPPME, typology of interventions in proximal physical microenvironments
Introduction
Obesity is a global public health issue [1]. Currently in the UK, 26% of men, 29% of women, and 20% of children aged 10 to 11 years old have obesity, with significantly higher prevalence in the most deprived areas [2]. Despite years of health promotion to encourage and motivate individuals to choose a healthier diet, the proportion of energy coming from free sugars and saturated fat in the UK continues to exceed the recommended levels across all age groups with especially high intakes of free sugars among children [3]. A nutritionally poor diet increases the burden of major chronic diseases, including diabetes and cardiovascular disease, principally through increases in body weight as well as blood cholesterol, blood pressure, and insulin resistance [4–8].
Interventions to change food purchasing habits at the point of choice offer an upstream opportunity to change behaviour rather than relying on influencing consumption at the moment of eating. The World Health Organisation and other groups have advocated for the implementation of health-related taxes [9,10], but the acceptability of these interventions is relatively low [11,12]. Governments are increasingly interested in policy interventions to change supermarket environments because of the potential of these interventions to achieve population-level change in dietary habits and with higher public acceptability ratings [13–16].
Although sugar-sweetened beverages (SSBs) have been a major focus of policy actions [17], confectionery, together with cakes and biscuits, make a greater contribution to free sugars, saturated fat, and total energy intakes in the UK population [3]. Sales of chocolate and sugar confectionery in the UK have increased by 16.3% and 7.3%, respectively, in the last 5 years [18], with the highest purchases in the least affluent households [19]. Despite ambitious sugar reduction targets set by Public Health England there has been almost no change in the period 2015 to 2019, with sugar in sweet and chocolate confectionery reducing by just 0.1% and 0.4%, respectively, against the 20% reduction target [18]. This is perhaps because of the challenges of reformulation for this food category, especially compared to SSBs with their potential to use nonnutritive sweeteners. Instead, any reductions in calories, sugar, and saturated fat from confectionery are likely to depend upon reducing the volume of confectionery consumed. All food is rewarding, but the combination of high energy, fat, and sugar in confectionery is associated with strong and reinforcing biological signals [20]. There are also powerful social norms and cultural traditions that foster the notion of confectionery as a treat, and, thus, people tend to discount the long-term harms in favour of the short-term reward [21]. Few people consider confectionery to be a healthy food, and education alone is unlikely to be successful in reversing these powerful biological and societal drivers of consumption.
The 2020 obesity plan in England proposed new legislation to restrict volume- and location-based promotions on unhealthier products (i.e., those high in fat, salt, and sugar), as well as the placement of these products in prominent locations within supermarkets [16]. Placement and price promotions, together with availability, have been identified in previous systematic reviews of in-store interventions as potentially effective strategies to influence food purchasing behaviours [22–32]. According to the typology of interventions in proximal physical microenvironments (TIPPME) framework, availability and placement strategies work by increasing the range, variety, number, as well as visibility and accessibility of products, and this can stimulate purchases [33].
Most reviews have generally highlighted the lack of high-quality evidence in real supermarkets, especially for interventions that disincentive purchases of less healthy options [23,24,26,32]. In collaboration with the Consumer Goods Forum (CGF), a global membership body of 400 major consumer goods retailers and manufacturers, and with agreements enabling access to sales data from a major UK supermarket, we conducted an independent evaluation of an intervention, designed and implemented by a national food retailer, to remove seasonal chocolate confectionery from prominent store locations before the Easter period.
Methods
This study is reported as per the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) guideline (S1 Checklist).
Study design and data source
Data from a major UK retailer (comprising 27.7% of the UK grocery market share in January 2019) were used in this project. The study was completely developed and implemented by the retail partner, so we followed the methods suggested for the monitoring and evaluation of natural experiments [34], and a nonrandomised controlled design was used. The study was implemented in 34 stores (hereafter referred to as intervention stores), with a matched sample of 151 unique control stores. Data on store-level weekly sales of seasonal chocolate confectionery (units, weight [g], and value [£] of each eligible product within the category) were obtained for both intervention and control stores, spanning dates from 1 January 2018 to 24 November 2019 (with 4 weeks missing from 26 November 2018 to 30 December 2018 from all stores), which comprised a total of 17,380 aggregated store-week data points (see flowchart of store data in Fig A in S1 Appendix). Data from nutrients in all food-related sales, including total energy, sugar, saturated fat, and total fat, were available from 1 January 2019 to 24 November 2019.
By using aggregated weekly sales data, this study was exempt from ethical review and approval. A preregistered protocol (https://osf.io/br96f/) was completed and fully available from 22 July 2020 before obtaining data for analysis.
Store selection and matching
Retail partner’s finance and data teams used proprietary analytics to select intervention and control stores for this study with no input from the research team. Based on each retailer’s operational considerations and with input from the CGF and the project partner, Impact on Urban Health, intervention stores were selected within London boroughs (Lambeth and Southwark, UK). The sample of intervention stores were located in neighbourhoods covering a range of socioeconomic deprivation strata based on the 2019 English Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) income domain, the official measure of relative deprivation in small areas (Lower-layer Super Output Areas) across England [35]. Selected intervention stores were all small supermarkets according to a retail food outlet categorisation system previously defined, which includes stores with 1 to 4 manned cash registers [36,37]. Control stores were selected across each retailer own stores, with store size and overall sales performance over the previous year used as the criteria for matching stores.
Intervention
The intervention aimed to reduce the extra availability of seasonal chocolate confectionery by removing free-standing promotional display units from prominent areas, for example, store entrance, as well as by substituting seasonal confectionery located in end of aisles with other products. A total of 178 uniquely barcoded products were removed from display units or end of aisles, but all these products were still available for purchase elsewhere in the store (although many of these products were seasonal and only available during a short period of time). The intervention was implemented in the run-up to the Easter period, for approximately 7 weeks (15 February 2019 to 3 April 2019) with a phased implementation: 17% of eligible products were removed from 15 February 2019; 53% more were removed from 13 March 2019; and the remaining 30% were removed from 18 March 2019.
Outcome measures
Primary outcome measures included store-level weekly sales data (units, weight, and value) for the whole category of seasonal chocolate confectionery. Secondary outcome measures included nutrient data (i.e., total energy, sugar, fibre, saturated fat, and total fat) from all food-related sales.
Store characteristics
Store characteristics relating to the customer population included the English IMD and ethnicity. The store postcode was matched to the IMD income domain, the official measure of relative deprivation in small areas (Lower-layer Super Output Areas) across England [38], which was used as a proxy for the socioeconomic status (SES) of the customer population. The store sample covered neighbourhoods from deciles 1 to 10; regrouped into IMD 1 to 3 (most deprived), 4 to 6 (mid), and 7 to 10 (least deprived). Ethnicity of the store customer population was classified by the retailer using internal proprietary systems and grouped as predominantly white versus other ethnicities.
Statistical analysis
Power analyses were not conducted, and the retailer chose the number of stores to roll out the interventions.
Descriptive analyses were used to investigate differences in store demographic characteristics between intervention and control stores using χ2 tests. We used data over the year prior to intervention (2018) to define preintervention baseline period (15 February 2018 to 3 April 2018), which matched as much as possible the intervention period. We tested differences in weekly sales of target products over the 2018 baseline periods between intervention and control stores using Student t tests.
The following prespecified statistical models were used for the primary and secondary outcome analyses, using consistent methods for intervention evaluation [39]:
- Hierarchical models (negative binomial for unit sales; or linear mixed models for weight and value of purchases) were used with a fixed effect adjustment for store demographic characteristics and average weekly sales over the baseline preintervention period. This model was used to investigate differences in weekly sales of target products in intervention versus control stores over the time period while the intervention was active compared to the preintervention baseline period (2018) [40].
- Interrupted time series (ITS) analyses and corresponding plots with fitted linear trends were computed using all available data before and after the intervention for intervention and control stores [41]. To assess whether differences visible in the graphs were statistically significant between intervention and control stores and to account for any preintervention differences between groups in the outcome variable, we used a difference-in-difference approach, calculating the mean difference in weekly sales between intervention and control stores, and testing whether this time series of differences changed after versus before intervention using a linear regression model. We used a Chow-type test for level and trend changes after intervention implementation, and Newey–West standard errors with lag 4 to allow for autocorrelation in the time series. Since intervention implementation was phased, we conducted one model where intervention started on the week of 15 February 2019 when 17% of products were removed, and a second model where intervention started on the week of 13 March when 53% more products were removed.
Analyses were conducted using all intervention and control stores with all available data. A prespecified exploratory subgroup analysis on unit sales was performed by store IMD groups (IMD 1 to 3 high deprivation versus IMD 4 to 10 middle/low deprivation), and likelihood ratio tests were used to test the significance of interactions. Stata version 16 was used for all statistical tests with a 5% significance level.
Results
Differences in store characteristics
A total of 185 stores were analysed, with all the intervention stores located in areas of medium or high deprivation, which is representative of the population of Lambeth and Southwark (London, UK). The control group had 28% of stores in areas of low deprivation (Table 1). There were significant differences in IMD scores (P < 0.001) but not in ethnicity between intervention and control stores.
Primary analysis—Sales of confectionery
Over a preintervention baseline period (15 February 2018 to 3 April 2018), there were no significant differences in sales (units, weight, and value) of all chocolate confectionery between intervention versus control stores (Table 2).
After intervention implementation, there was an attenuation in the seasonal increase of confectionery sales (units) in intervention stores compared to control (+5% versus +18%; P < 0.001), with similar effects on weight (+12% versus +31%; P < 0.001) between 15 February 2019 to 3 April 2019. However, there was a decrease in value sales in intervention compared to control stores over the same time period (−3% versus +10%; P < 0.001) (Table 2). There were absolute differences in confectionery sales of approximately 127 units per store per week (+43.9 units in intervention stores versus +170.8 units in control stores over the intervention period compared to the baseline period) or 21 kg (11.5 kg in intervention stores versus 32.3 kg in control stores) (Table 2).
ITS analyses were conducted using 2 different time points for intervention implementation, firstly on 15 February 2019 when 17% of products were removed and secondly on 13 March 2019 when 53% more of products were removed. The trends before intervention implementation were generally consistent between intervention and control stores (Fig 1, Table A in S1 Appendix). After intervention implementation on 15 February 2019, there was a statistically significant difference in the level of weekly sales (Fig 1; Pdiff level = 0.026 in units/store/week and Pdiff level = 0.044 in £/store/week). There were stronger differences in level after intervention intensification on 13 March 2019 (Fig 1; Pdiff level = 0.010 in units/store/week, Pdiff level = 0.042 in g/store/week and Pdiff level = 0.026 in £/store/week). There were generally significant differences in the downward trends afterwards, indicating that the initial difference between intervention and control stores reduced over time.
*Solid dots (observed) and lines (modelled) represent intervention stores; white dots (observed) and dotted lines (modelled) represent control stores. ITS, interrupted time series.
Secondary analyses—Changes in energy and nutrients
Data on total energy (calories), total sugars, total fat, and saturated fat from all food-related sales for the 2019 year were used in ITS models to evaluate the impact of the intervention on the overall healthiness of grocery shopping (Fig 2). There was a significant level change in total energy sold, adjusted for the total weight of food and drink (kcal/g, Pdiff level = 0.002), and total fat (fat/g, Pdiff level = 0.023), but no significant changes in saturated fat or sugars from total sales in ITS models. There were no significant differences in the trends afterwards for any of the nutrients studied.
*Solid dots (observed) and lines (modelled) represent intervention stores; white dots (observed) and dotted lines (modelled) represent control stores. ITS, interrupted time series.
Differences by store deprivation
There was no evidence that the results varied across store IMD group for any of the metrics reported, with significant reductions in units, weight, and value in intervention stores located in high deprivation areas as well as those in mid or lower deprivation areas, compared to control stores (Table 3).
Discussion
This intervention to remove chocolate confectionery in prominent areas of the store showed a significant attenuation in the seasonal increase of confectionery sales (units) in intervention stores compared to control (+5% versus +18%; P < 0.001). Similar results were observed with weight (g) of confectionery (+12% versus +31%; P < 0.001) and value (£) (−3% versus +10%; P < 0.001), with an overall absolute difference between intervention and control stores of approximately 127 units (approximately 21 kg) of confectionery per store per week. In parallel, we observed significant reductions in total energy and fat from all food-related sales, but no significant differences in saturated fat or total sugars. There was no evidence that the intervention results varied according to the level of deprivation in the area in which the store was sited.
Systematic reviews of grocery store interventions have reported that positioning products in prominent locations, such as near checkouts or the end of an aisle, increases visibility of products and stimulates purchases [22–25]. A previous natural experiment across 12 stores in the US found that prominent positioning of sweet snacks at the end-of-aisles had a greater effect on sales of less healthier options compared to prominent positioning of the healthier ones and copositioning of both significantly increased sales of the unhealthier options only [42]. An observational study using sales from a UK grocery store estimated a 52% higher weekly volume sales of carbonated drinks when these were displayed in end of aisles [43]. But there is an important gap in the evidence with regard to interventions that can reduce the prominent positioning of unhealthy food, as most of the literature has focused on selling more healthy foods. A recent cluster RCT in Australian supermarkets tested a complex intervention to limit in-store promotional and marketing activities targeting high-fat/high-sugar products, including removal of price promotions, signage, and removal of products from prominent areas, and showed significant reductions in total sugars without affecting supermarket profit [44]. By just removing confectionery from prominent store locations, our study showed a significant attenuation in pre-Easter sales (units, weight, and value) of confectionery.
In the context of the increasing gap in dietary inequalities and long-term health outcomes, it is also important to understand if supermarket interventions help reduce, or at least do not exacerbate, dietary inequalities. It has been postulated that, compared to individual-level interventions, population-level approaches that trigger automatic (rather than conscious) behavioural responses [45,46] may be less likely to increase health inequalities. But evidence from systematic reviews is limited and most studies testing positioning interventions have not specifically looked at differential effects across sociodemographic groups [23,32]. Our results showed no evidence of differences in intervention results, and the intervention appeared to work equally well regardless of the area deprivation score of the store.
The 2020 obesity plan in England has laid out plans to introduce legislation to restrict the promotion of foods high in fat, sugar, or salt (HFSS), by restricting volume-based promotions such as “Buy One Get One Free” as well as restrictions to placement in prominent locations intended to encourage purchasing, both online and in physical stores [16]. Our results provide direct evidence on the reduced availability of chocolate confectionery in prominent locations, which will be of interest to policymakers and could help shape effective policies for confectionery and potentially other items.
This research was made possible through collaboration with food retailers, facilitated by an established industry programme led by the CGF to encourage healthier and more sustainable retail practices. This evaluation provides proof-of-concept that it is possible to establish these collaborations and has led to useful lessons for future collaborations, especially in relation to contractual agreements, and the design of larger and more definitive intervention studies. For example, the duration of intervention was limited here because of the seasonal nature of the products, but future studies of positioning interventions should aim to try implement the intervention for a longer time period. This is important since the trends from ITS models after implementation suggested that the effect of the intervention may be short-lived, though this may be related to the seasonal nature of the products targeted (i.e., a large proportion of the products targeted in the intervention are not available the rest of the year). Our analysis of the nutrient content of the total sales showed some evidence of no compensatory behaviours at least within the same retailer, although other research would need to investigate if customers are purchasing confectionery in different stores where no restrictions are imposed. Future research should also seek to analyse changes in purchases at a household rather than store level using data from customer loyalty cards rather than store-level sales to better study any potential impact of interventions on health inequalities [47,48].
A major strength of this study is the use of a large dataset of objectively collected sales data, which is generalizable to all customers of the participating stores over the studied time period. Data were available over an extended time period and drawn from an intervention conducted in real supermarkets, which can provide important insights to inform population-level interventions to encourage healthier food purchasing. However, this “real-world” intervention study presents analytical challenges. Adjustment for confounding and other sources of heterogeneity was approached in several ways. Firstly, control stores were matched to intervention stores, with more than one control store per intervention store. Matching was done using store demographic factors and overall sales over the previous periods, which, in this case, resulted in nonsignificant differences in baseline sales between intervention and control stores. However, there were significant differences by IMD due to the fact that stores in less deprived areas were underrepresented in the intervention group, though we adjusted for deprivation in the models. The difference-in-difference approach used in ITS models also helps to remove the effect of any small absolute differences in sales between the intervention and control stores. Finally, with access to extended periods of time (2018 and 2019), we were able to use the 2018 period as a control in the models. Other limitations to note include the lack of randomisation, residual confounding from unmeasured variables, and absolute differences in trends and sales between intervention versus control stores. There could have been other interventions in stores running alongside the one evaluated here, which could have influenced the observed effects, but the use of control stores could potentially adjust for this. In addition, we have no independent measures of intervention fidelity and we had to rely on the retailer implementation plans, which means suboptimal implementation may have diminished the apparent effects of this intervention. The intervention was selected, developed, and implemented by the retailer, without the direct involvement of the research group. It is not possible to know the extent to which this was influenced by behavioural theory, prior commercial insights, or awareness of government thinking, though it is probable that all contributed to greater or lesser extent. Finally, there was limited data on store characteristics, and the retailer provided only restricted data on the ethnicity of the customer population. The very broad categorisation of ethnicity is unlikely to have removed all of the confounding related to ethnicity in our results, although there were no significant differences in the distribution of ethnicity between intervention and control stores. Similarly, the IMD used as a measure of store deprivation may also be a very crude proxy for the SES status of the customer population, particularly when people drive to larger out-of-town supermarkets or for smaller stores located in city centres with a large proportion of nonlocal customers.
There is limited evidence for effective interventions to discourage food options that contribute the most energy, saturated fat, and free sugars [3], particularly confectionery, biscuits, or cakes. These results showed that removal of seasonal confectionery in prominent locations is a promising strategy to reduce unhealthy food purchasing behaviours, with changes in just one subcategory of foods of sufficient magnitude to observe reductions in the energy content of total food purchases. These results provide promising evidence that the proposed legislation in England to restrict promotions of less healthy items in prominent locations may help reduce overconsumption.
Supporting information
S1 Appendix. Supplementary table and figure.
Fig A. Flowchart of store data. Table A. Model-based estimates of the difference-in-difference interrupted time series (β coefficients (95% CI)) of mean baseline trend, post-implementation level change, and post-implementation trend change.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1003951.s002
(DOCX)
Acknowledgments
We thank the Consumer Goods Forum and all the project partners for their contribution to this project. Tesco provided sales data for this analysis.
The views expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the National Health Service, the National Institute for Health Research, and the UK Department of Health and Social Care.
References
- 1.
World Health Organization. Obesity and Overweight 2018 [21/06/2021]. Available from: www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight.
- 2.
NHS Digital Official statistics National statistics. Statistics on Obesity, Physical Activity and Diet, England 2020 [21/06/2021]. Available from: https://digital.nhs.uk/data-and-information/publications/statistical/statistics-on-obesity-physical-activity-and-diet/england-2020.
- 3.
Public Health England. National Diet and Nutrition Survey: Results from years 7 and 8 (combined); 2018.
- 4. Te Morenga LA, Howatson AJ, Jones RM, Mann J. Dietary sugars and cardiometabolic risk: systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials of the effects on blood pressure and lipids. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;100(1):65–79. pmid:24808490
- 5.
National Institute of Health and Care Excellence (NICE). NICE Guidelines: Overweight and Obese Adults–lifestyle weight management; 2013. Available from: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ph53.
- 6.
National Institute of Health and Care Excellence (NICE). Prevention of Cardiovascular disease; 2010.
- 7. Mozaffarian D, Micha R, Wallace S. Effects on coronary heart disease of increasing polyunsaturated fat in place of saturated fat: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLoS Med. 2010;7(3):e1000252. pmid:20351774; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2843598
- 8. He FJ, Pombo-Rodrigues S, MacGregor GA. Salt reduction in England from 2003 to 2011: its relationship to blood pressure, stroke and ischaemic heart disease mortality. BMJ Open. 2014;4(4):e004549. pmid:24732242
- 9. Bloomberg M, Summers L, Ahmed M. Health taxes to save lives: Employing effective excise taxes on tobacco, alcohol, and sugary beverages: The Task Force on Fiscal Policy for Health. Bloomberg Philanthropies; 2019.
- 10.
World Health Organization. Report of the commission on ending childhood obesity; 2016.
- 11. Diepeveen S, Ling T, Suhrcke M, Roland M, Marteau TM. Public acceptability of government intervention to change health-related behaviours: a systematic review and narrative synthesis. BMC Public Health. 2013;13(1):1–11. pmid:23947336
- 12. Reynolds J, Archer S, Ma P, Kenny M, Hollands GJ, Marteau T. Public acceptability of nudging and taxing to reduce consumption of alcohol, tobacco, and food: A population-based survey experiment. Soc Sci Med. 2019;236:112395. pmid:31326778
- 13.
Public Health England. Health matters: obesity and the food environment; 2017.
- 14.
Public Health England. Healthy Lives, Healthy People: Our strategy for public health in England; 2010.
- 15.
UK Government Department of Health and Social Care. Childhood obesity: a plan for action; 2017.
- 16.
UK Government Department for Health and Social Care. Tackling obesity: empowering adults and children to live healthier lives. 2020. 27 July 2020. Report No.
- 17.
Public Health England. Sugar reduction: the evidence for action; 2015.
- 18.
Public Health England. Sugar reduction. Report on progress between 2015 and 2019; 2020.
- 19. Pechey R, Jebb SA, Kelly MP, Almiron-Roig E, Conde S, Nakamura R, et al. Socioeconomic differences in purchases of more vs. less healthy foods and beverages: Analysis of over 25,000 British households in 2010. Soc Sci Med. 2013;92:22–6. pmid:23849275
- 20. Alonso-Alonso M, Woods SC, Pelchat M, Grigson PS, Stice E, Farooqi S, et al. Food reward system: current perspectives and future research needs. Nutr Rev. 2015;73(5):296–307. Epub 2015/04/09. pmid:26011903.
- 21. Story GW, Vlaev I, Seymour B, Darzi A, Dolan RJ. Does temporal discounting explain unhealthy behavior? A systematic review and reinforcement learning perspective. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:76–. pmid:24659960.
- 22. Bucher T, Collins C, Rollo ME, McCaffrey TA, De Vlieger N, Van der Bend D, et al. Nudging consumers towards healthier choices: a systematic review of positional influences on food choice. Br J Nutr. 2016;115(12):2252–63. Epub 2016/05/18. pmid:27185414.
- 23. Shaw SC, Ntani G, Baird J, Vogel CA. A systematic review of the influences of food store product placement on dietary-related outcomes. Nutr Rev. 2020. pmid:32483615
- 24. Cameron AJ, Charlton E, Ngan WW, Sacks G. A Systematic Review of the Effectiveness of Supermarket-Based Interventions Involving Product, Promotion, or Place on the Healthiness of Consumer Purchases. Curr Nutr Rep. 2016;5(3):129–38.
- 25. Karpyn A, McCallops K, Wolgast H, Glanz K. Improving Consumption and Purchases of Healthier Foods in Retail Environments: A Systematic Review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(20). Epub 2020/10/22. pmid:33081130; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7588922.
- 26. Adam A, Jensen JD. What is the effectiveness of obesity related interventions at retail grocery stores and supermarkets? -a systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2016;16(1):1247–. pmid:28031046.
- 27. Bianchi F, Garnett E, Dorsel C, Aveyard P, Jebb SA. Restructuring physical micro-environments to reduce the demand for meat: a systematic review and qualitative comparative analysis. Lancet Planet Health. 2018;2(9):e384–e97. pmid:30177007
- 28. Hollands GJ, Carter P, Anwer S, King SE, Jebb SA, Ogilvie D, et al. Altering the availability or proximity of food, alcohol, and tobacco products to change their selection and consumption. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2019;(9). CD012573. pmid:31482606
- 29. Bennett R, Zorbas C, Huse O, Peeters A, Cameron AJ, Sacks G, et al. Prevalence of healthy and unhealthy food and beverage price promotions and their potential influence on shopper purchasing behaviour: A systematic review of the literature. 2020;21(1):e12948.
- 30. Escaron AL, Meinen AM, Nitzke SA, Martinez-Donate AP. Supermarket and grocery store-based interventions to promote healthful food choices and eating practices: a systematic review. Prev Chronic Dis. 2013;10:E50. Epub 2013/04/13. pmid:23578398; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3625444.
- 31. Gittelsohn J, Trude ACB, Kim H. Pricing Strategies to Encourage Availability, Purchase, and Consumption of Healthy Foods and Beverages: A Systematic Review. Prev Chronic Dis. 2017;14:E107. Epub 2017/11/05. pmid:29101767; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5672888.
- 32. Hartmann-Boyce J, Bianchi F, Piernas C, Payne Riches S, Frie K, Nourse R, et al. Grocery store interventions to change food purchasing behaviors: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Am J Clin Nutr. 2018;107(6):1004–16. pmid:29868912.
- 33. Hollands GJ, Bignardi G, Johnston M, Kelly MP, Ogilvie D, Petticrew M, et al. The TIPPME intervention typology for changing environments to change behaviour. Nat Hum Behav. 2017;1(8):1–9.
- 34. Taillie LS, Grummon AH, Fleischhacker S, Grigsby-Toussaint DS, Leone L, Caspi CE. Best practices for using natural experiments to evaluate retail food and beverage policies and interventions. Nutr Rev. 2017;75(12):971–89. pmid:29190370
- 35.
Department for Communities and Local Government. The English Indices of Deprivation 2015. London, UK; 2015.
- 36. Black C, Ntani G, Inskip H, Cooper C, Cummins S, Moon G, et al. Measuring the healthfulness of food retail stores: variations by store type and neighbourhood deprivation. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2014;11:69. Epub 2014/06/03. pmid:24884529; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4132210.
- 37. Lake AA, Burgoine T, Greenhalgh F, Stamp E, Tyrrell R. The foodscape: classification and field validation of secondary data sources. Health Place. 2010;16(4):666–73. pmid:20207577
- 38.
McLennan D, Noble S, Noble M, Plunkett E, Wright G, Gutacker N. The English Indices of Deprivation 2019. Ministry of Housing, Communities and Local Government; 2019.
- 39. Piernas C, Cook B, Stevens R, Stewart C, Hollowell J, Scarborough P, et al. Estimating the effect of moving meat-free products to the meat aisle on sales of meat and meat-free products: A non-randomised controlled intervention study in a large UK supermarket chain. PLoS Med. 2021;18(7):e1003715. Epub 2021/07/16. pmid:34264943; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC8321099.
- 40. Zheng H, Kimber A, Goodwin VA, Pickering RM. A comparison of different ways of including baseline counts in negative binomial models for data from falls prevention trials. Biom J. 2018;60(1):66–78. pmid:29067697
- 41. Bernal JL, Cummins S, Gasparrini A. Interrupted time series regression for the evaluation of public health interventions: a tutorial. Int J Epidemiol. 2016;46(1):348–55. pmid:27283160
- 42. Liu PJ, Dallas SK, Harding M, Fitzsimons GJ. The Sales Impact of Featuring Healthy Foods, Indulgent Foods, or Both: Findings from a Large-Scale Retail Field Study. J Assoc Consum Res. 2018;3(3):346–63.
- 43. Nakamura R, Pechey R, Suhrcke M, Jebb SA, Marteau TM. Sales impact of displaying alcoholic and non-alcoholic beverages in end-of-aisle locations: An observational study. Soc Sci Med. 2014;108:68–73. pmid:24632050
- 44. Brimblecombe J, McMahon E, Ferguson M, De Silva K, Peeters A, Miles E, et al. Effect of restricted retail merchandising of discretionary food and beverages on population diet: a pragmatic randomised controlled trial. Lancet Planet Health. 2020;4(10):e463–e73. Epub 2020/10/11. pmid:33038320.
- 45. Adams J, Mytton O, White M, Monsivais P. Why Are Some Population Interventions for Diet and Obesity More Equitable and Effective Than Others? The Role of Individual Agency. PLoS Med. 2016;13(4):e1001990. pmid:27046234
- 46. Marteau TM, Ogilvie D, Roland M, Suhrcke M, Kelly MP. Judging nudging: can nudging improve population health? BMJ. 2011;342. pmid:21266441
- 47. Felgate M, Fearne A, DiFalco S, Martinez MG. Using supermarket loyalty card data to analyse the impact of promotions. Int J Mark Res. 2012;54(2):221–40.
- 48. Green MA, Watson AW, Brunstrom JM, Corfe BM, Johnstone AM, Williams EA, et al. Comparing supermarket loyalty card data with traditional diet survey data for understanding how protein is purchased and consumed in older adults for the UK, 2014–16. Nutr J. 2020;19(1):1–10. pmid:31901246