Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Socially driven negative feedback regulates activity and energy use in ant colonies

  • Maurizio Porfiri ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    mporfiri@nyu.edu

    Affiliations Center for Urban Science and Progress, New York University Tandon School of Engineering, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America, Department of Biomedical Engineering, New York University Tandon School of Engineering, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America, Department of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering, New York University Tandon School of Engineering, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America

  • Nicole Abaid,

    Roles Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Mathematics, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, Virginia, United States of America

  • Simon Garnier

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Biological Sciences, New Jersey Institute of Technology, Newark, New Jersey, United States of America

?

This is an uncorrected proof.

Abstract

Despite almost a century of research on energetics in biological systems, we still cannot explain energy regulation in social groups, like ant colonies. How do individuals regulate their collective activity without a centralized control system? What is the role of social interactions in distributing the workload amongst group members? And how does the group save energy by avoiding being constantly active? We offer new insight into these questions by studying an intuitive compartmental model, calibrated with and compared to data on ant colonies. The model describes a previously unexplored balance between positive and negative social feedback driven by individual activity: when activity levels are low, the presence of active individuals stimulates inactive individuals to start working; when activity levels are high, however, active individuals inhibit each other, effectively capping the proportion of active individuals at any one time. Through the analysis of the system stability, we demonstrate that this balance results in energetic spending at the group level growing proportionally slower than the group size. Our finding is reminiscent of Kleiber’s law of metabolic scaling in unitary organisms and highlights the critical role of social interactions in driving the collective energetic efficiency of group-living organisms.

Author summary

Similarly to how larger organisms use less energy per unit mass than smaller ones, eusocial insects like ant colonies become more energy efficient as colony size increases. The mechanism underneath this efficiency remains a mystery. Here, we seek to uncover its origin in socially contagious “deactivation”, which runs counter to more conventional ideas of excitatory social interactions. Beyond providing insight into the collective behavior of highly integrated social groups, our findings on activity regulation open the door to the design of engineered multi-agent systems, like robotic swarms or active matter, which may achieve efficient performance in both function and energy use.

Introduction

As a complex social system—such as an ant colony or a human organization—increases in size, so does the need to better regulate the activities of its members [1]. Without regulation, individuals would randomly distribute their effort across time, wasting energy if too many individuals were active when the global workload does not require it, or wasting opportunities if too few individuals were to respond to increasing needs in the population. The question of activity—and hence energy—regulation is not unique to complex social systems; it is also increasingly studied in the context of artificial distributed systems such as fleets of robotic devices [2, 3] and Internet-of-Things networks [4], where redundancies are frequent and energy-wasting.

Both in natural and artificial distributed systems, the control of activities is often fully decentralized, making it impossible to consider global solutions for optimizing energy consumption. Studies on social insect colonies have shown that living systems can be surprisingly efficient in managing their collective activities [5]. In particular, their energy use per unit of mass appears to scale hypometrically with their colony size [6, 7]. In other words, larger colonies are more energy-efficient relative to their size than smaller ones, a phenomenon akin to Kleiber’s law [8] that states that the rate of energy use by a biological system scales hypometrically with its size. Understanding how ant colonies—a fully decentralized system—regulate their activities to achieve energy efficiency could, therefore, have important repercussions for the design of human organizations and artificial distributed systems.

In the literature, activity regulation in social insects is typically divided between activation mechanisms that boost the number of individuals engaged in work, and inactivation mechanisms that reduce it [9, 10]. On the activation side, two types of mechanisms are usually invoked. First, individuals may sense workload-associated stimuli and, if their intensity exceeds a certain value (called the “response threshold”), start performing work to reduce this stimulation [11]. Such response thresholds have been found in several social insect species, for instance, for triggering a defense reaction to threats [12] or a foraging response to the presence of nutrients [13, 14].

Second, individuals already engaged in work can stimulate others to work, for instance, when the workload has outgrown their capacity [15]. This is frequently observed in social insects in the context of foraging. For example, ant workers that have found a resource will use a combination of chemical and tactile signals to stimulate other workers to join them in exploiting it [16]. Likewise, honeybee scouts that have found a new food patch execute a stereotypical “waggle dance” back at their colony that stimulates other bees to leave the nest and encodes the direction and distance to the resource [17]. Such recruitment processes are akin to a form of “social contagion”, whereby the state of being active spreads in the group through local social interactions. As a result of this positive feedback loop, social organisms can mobilize a large portion of the available workforce quickly, facilitating rapid monopolization of resources [18] or overwhelming attackers by swiftly assembling defense forces around them [19].

While activation mechanisms are well studied and supported by documented examples, inactivation mechanisms are overlooked—especially those mediated by social interactions [20]. In existing models of activity regulation, inactivation is often treated as an intrinsic property of the individual rather than a socially driven one. For instance, inactivation has been modeled as a workload-associated threshold [21], a limit on the time an individual can be active [22, 23], or a constant probability per unit time [9, 11]. In each of these cases, the social environment does not influence the duration of the activity of an individual, or, at best, does it very indirectly (for example, through the impact other individuals have on the quantity of work remaining to be done). Yet, socially driven inactivation mechanisms have been shown to play an important role in counterbalancing activation mechanisms in social insects. For instance, honeybees use inhibitory signals to slow down the recruitment of foragers when food storing at the nest cannot match the influx of new nectar and pollen [24] and to delay the maturation of hive workers into foragers when the forager population is already high [25]. In ants, crowding along a foraging trail can inhibit the deposition of trail pheromone, reducing the risk of traffic jam [26]. Repellent pheromone can also be used to discourage foragers from visiting unrewarding routes [27], for instance because the resource they lead to is now depleted or overcrowded.

In a recent work [28], we investigated the impact of a socially driven inactivation mechanism on the scaling of energy use in eusocial organisms. We proposed an explanation for Kleiber’s law [8] in the context of colonies of harvester ants (Pogonomyrmex californicus) using data from Waters et al. [29]. Our explanation was based on scaling arguments adapted from urban science [30] and relied on a key biological phenomenon that we called “reverse social contagion”—a mechanism by which an individual engaged in a given behavior becomes more likely to interrupt this behavior as it interacts with more neighbors also engaged in the same behavior [31], see Fig 1a. Reverse social contagion can be observed, for instance, in the form of stop-signaling mechanisms [32] and blocking interactions [33], and has been proposed as a factor regulating collective decision-making [34, 35] and energy spending in social systems [28, 36].

thumbnail
Fig 1.

(a) Illustration of the concepts of social contagion (top) and reverse social contagion (bottom). (top) An inactive ant interacts with an ant engaged in a foraging task: through social contagion (for example, caused by active recruitment), it also begins foraging. (bottom) Two ants engaged in foraging interact: through reverse social contagion (for example, caused by steric exclusion), one of them ceases their activity to become inactive. Image courtesy of Isabella Muratore, reprinted from Porfiri et al. [28]. (b) State transition diagram for three classes of individuals (A, active, I, inactive, and R, refractory). The diagram describes two competing social feedback mechanisms (social contagion and reverse social contagion), along with a spontaneous transition from refractory to inactive state (that is, the completion of rest). Social contagion and completion of rest are well established in the literature since the seminal work by Goss and Deneubourg [9] (therein, inactive ants are called “activable inactives” and refractory ants “inactives” to specify that only the former ones can be activated). Reverse social contagion has been, instead, overlooked so far, thereby hindering our understanding of energy regulation in social groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1012623.g001

In that study, we focused on reverse social contagion in the context of movement, where an individual would cease to move (inactivate) in response to a social environment where many neighbors are also moving (being active). Each colony was composed of N individuals, of which A were active. The individuals in the colony interacted through a network with EN3/2 links, as estimated from the data. To explain activity regulation in the form of a reduction in the fraction of active individuals as the colony grows, we proposed a balance between reverse social contagion and spontaneous social activation. The former should scale with A2/N1/2, given that the number of interactions of active individuals with other active individuals is estimated as the product between the total count of interactions in the colony, E, and the probability that two individuals are simultaneously active, (A/N)2. The latter, instead, should scale with N, being an inherent property of the individuals. Balancing reverse social contagion and spontaneous social activation, one predicts a hypometric scaling of the colony’s activity with respect to its size, AN3/4, akin to Kleiber’s law [8].

In this article, we draw inspiration from this simple scaling argument to formulate a dynamic model for activity regulation in eusocial systems. The state-of-the-art modeling of the dynamics of eusocial systems has, so far, largely focused on socially driven positive feedback mechanisms to predict rhythmic activation patterns [22, 37, 38], ignoring the impact of social information on inactivation patterns. Here instead, we explicitly include socially driven negative feedback through reverse social contagion. We present a detailed study of the model, in the form of a system of coupled differential equations and stochastic Monte Carlo simulations.

Results

State transitions

We consider a colony of size N. Each individual in the colony can be in one of three possible states: active (A), inactive (I), or refractory (R). Individuals in inactive or refractory states are practically indistinguishable (that is, they do not display any activity); however, only the inactive ones can be activated after interacting with active individuals, whereas the refractory ones cannot.

We contemplate three coexisting phenomena, as shown in Fig 1b. First, an inactive individual can become active by interacting with an active individual (social contagion): , where the transition rate β0 > 0 is the probability per unit time that an inactive individual interacting with an active individual becomes active. Social contagion provides positive feedback, or autocatalysis, to the system [9]. Second, a refractory individual can spontaneously become inactive (hence, be ready for activation through social contagion): , where the transition rate δ > 0 is the probability per unit time that a refractory individual spontaneously becomes inactive. This phenomenon, in probabilistic terms, is similar to a variable rest period after activity. Third, we hypothesize that an active individual can become refractory upon interaction with another active individual (reverse social contagion): , where the transition rate γ0 > 0 is the probability per unit time that an active individual interacting with another active ant becomes refractory. This phenomenon creates negative feedback in the system, countering autocatalysis.

With respect to the autocatalytic ant colony model by Goss and Deneubourg [9] and the related mathematical model by da Silveira and Fontanari [38], we include reverse social contagion—an original contribution of this work—while making two simplifications. First, we neglect spontaneous activation and inactivation I → A and A → R, respectively. Second, we do not explicitly consider a fixed rest period in which individuals must persist once entering the refractory state, before being ready to be activated. These simplifications are intended to reduce the number of model parameters and focus on a minimalistic model that could highlight the role of reverse social contagion on activity regulation.

Compartmental model

We describe the colony dynamics through a compartmental model of the following form: (1a) (1b) (1c) Here, A(t), I(t), and R(t) are the number of active, inactive, and refractory individuals at time t, respectively, and 〈k〉 is the average degree of the network of interactions of the colony, encapsulating the number of interactions of each individual with other individuals at any time. Equation set (1) is similar to an epidemiological SIRS model, where recovery yields temporary immunity and recovered individuals return to the susceptible class at a given rate [39]. However, in contrast with epidemiological compartmental models where nonlinearities are typically restricted to mixed terms, we include a negative nonlinear quadratic term that captures reverse social contagion.

We hypothesize that the average degree scales with the network size as 〈k〉 = 2E0Nα−1 where E0 > 0 is some scaling coefficient and α ∈ [1, 2] is the scaling exponent for the number of interactions with the colony size. A value close to 1 represents a network with a constant degree that does not vary with the colony size, and a value approaching 2 captures all-to-all interactions of a complete graph. Our previous observations on harvester ants yielded a value of α ≃ 3/2 for physical proximity, as well as antennal contact networks [28]. Interestingly, the same value is also found in simulations of the classical Vicsek model for self-propelled particles when examining the dependence between the average degree in a particle cluster and its size [40]. We note that hypermetric scalings of the number of interactions with the group size have been documented for different social interaction types, across several other classes than insects, including mammals (non-primates, primates, and humans), ray-finned fishes, birds, and reptiles. The lowest values for α have been recorded for online social friendship (humans), and the largest ones for physical contact (primates, birds, and insects) and grooming (primates) [41].

By construction, the sum of the three populations (A(t), I(t), and R(t)) is equal to the colony size, N, for all times; in fact, summing Eqs (1a)–(1c), we recover that . Next, we can show that given well-defined initial conditions for the three variables (A0, I0, and R0 non-negative and summing to N), they all remain non-negative—see Materials and methods, “Positivity of the compartmental model.”

Solving the system of algebraic equations given by , we find two equilibria for the model, one trivial and one nontrivial—see equation set (4) from Materials and Methods, “Equilibria of the compartmental model.” Through the study of the Jacobian of equation set (1) in correspondence of the two equilibria, we show that the trivial equilibrium is unstable and the other equilibrium is (locally and marginally) stable—see Materials and methods, “Local stability analysis of the non-trivial equilibrium.” Just like endemic states in epidemic models, we can prove that the stability of the non-trivial equilibrium is global [42, 43]—see Materials and methods, “Global stability analysis of the non-trivial equilibrium.” As a result, the model does not admit limit cycles, and any dynamics will ultimately converge to the non-trivial equilibrium.

The stable equilibrium is energetically favorable, whereby any network of interactions for which the average degree increases with the colony size (α > 1) supports energetic regulation in the colony. In fact, the fractions of active and inactive individuals, A/N and I/N, become smaller and smaller as the colony increases. The majority of the colony will be in a refractory state, which is the least energy-costly state for ants [44] and other social insects [45, 46]. For example, experiments on freely moving Camponatus ants by Lipp et al. [44] determined that the metabolic rate of walking ants increases four- to seven-fold over resting rates.

From basic calculus, one can establish that the number of active individuals increases with δ and decreases with γ0, which indicates that slower rates of spontaneous refractory-to-inactive transition and faster rates of reverse social contagion facilitate energy savings by the colony. Likewise, increasing β0 reduces the fraction of individuals in the inactive state, and, to a lesser extent, increases the fraction of active individuals.

Scaling

For α = 1, the average degree is independent of the colony size so that the populations of the non-trivial equilibrium in equation set (4) from Materials and Methods, “Equilibria of the compartmental model,” scale isometrically with N, that is, A, I, RN. Interesting allometries that are reminiscent of Kleiber’s law emerge for α > 1, whereby taking the limit of equation set (4) from Materials and Methods, “Equilibria of the compartmental model,” for N ≫ 1 with 〈k〉 = 2E0Nα−1 yields (2a) (2b) (2c)

Fig 2 demonstrates how network connectivity impacts the scaling exponent above as network size varies.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Role of network connectivity on hypometric scaling of the collective activity.

(a,b) Sample networks with different number of nodes (N = 10, 20, and 50), for the same value of E0 = 0.0944 from Porfiri et al. [28] and different values of α = 1.4 (a) and α = 1.8 (b), respectively. (c) Scaling exponent (3 − α)/2 as a function of α, illustrating activity regulation with increased connectivity: for α = 1.4, increasing the network size contributes to a modestly sublinear growth of the number of active ants ((3 − α)/2 = 0.8), while for α = 1.8, increasing the network size yields dramatic sublinear growth of the number of active ants ((3 − α)/2 = 0.6).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1012623.g002

In the case α ≳ 3/2—corresponding to spatial proximity and physical contact in harvester ants and other insects [41]—the number of active ants scales hypometrically with N with an exponent ranging from approximately 1/2 to 3/4. Using the crude approximation that the energy cost for a colony is proportional to the number of active individuals, the observed scaling is in agreement with existing experimental observations of hypometric scaling of whole-colony metabolic rate with colony mass in ants [6, 29, 47, 48] and other group-living organisms [7, 49]. Since it is impossible to directly observe when an individual is in the refractory state, it is natural to ask whether this state is required for the model to show the scaling above. When the refractory state is excluded, hypometric scaling of the number of ants becomes unfeasible, demonstrating the need to force individuals to rest before being reactivated through a refractory state—see Materials and methods, “Compartmental model without refractory state.”

In the limit β0 → 0, there is no mechanism for individuals to be activated and the non-trivial equilibrium converges to the trivial equilibrium. In this case, activity regulation does not emerge, pointing to the need for a balance between positive and negative social feedback. Notably, should one replace reverse social contagion with a spontaneous transition from active to refractory, any nonlinear allometry would disappear—see Materials and methods, “Compartmental model without reverse social contagion.” The possibility of recovering Kleiber’s law of metabolic scaling in eusocial organisms relies on the balance between social contagion and reverse social contagion.

From a practical point of view, for equation set (2) to accurately approximate equation set (4) from Materials and Methods, “Equilibria of the compartmental model,” one should have that . For colonies of finite size, this requires β0 to be sufficiently large compared to γ0, that is, positive feedback (social contagion) not to be dominated by negative feedback (reverse social contagion; a caveat to this argument would be the case of slow spontaneous transition from refractory to inactive, for which δ could be very small.

Model predictions for parameters calibrated on ant colonies

Model parameters are chosen as E0 = 0.0944, α = 1.47, γ0 = 1.21 s−1, and δ = 0.63 s−1, based on experimental results in Waters et al. [29], which we recently re-analyzed in the context of scaling [28]—see Materials and methods, “Calibration of model parameters on experimental data.” We explore three different values of β0 (namely, γ0/10, γ0 and 10γ0) to detail the interplay between the positive and negative feedback mechanisms of social and reverse social contagion, respectively.

We study the time-evolution of model in equation set (1) with initial conditions A0 = N − 1, I0 = 1, and R0 = 0 to visualize the effect of reverse social contagion in bringing the activity of the colony towards equilibrium. Numerical results for N = 500 in Fig 3 confirm the global stability of the non-trivial equilibrium in equation set (4) from Materials and Methods, “Equilibria of the compartmental model,” whereby the variables converge to the equilibrium even from initial conditions that are far away from the equilibrium. Changing the value of β0 with respect to the other parameters affects the time scale of the evolution, as well as the organization of the colony. Specifically, increasing the value of β0 accelerates convergence towards the steady state, due to stronger social contagion that facilitates the activation of individuals in the inactive status. Stronger positive feedback is also responsible for a reduced number of inactive individuals, as well as an increased number of active and refractory individuals, as one would predict from equation set (4) from Materials and Methods, “Equilibria of the compartmental model”.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Numerical predictions from compartmental model in equation set (1) for the time-evolution of the fraction of active (A, orange solid), inactive (I, green dashed), and refractory (R, black dotted) ants for a colony of N = 500 individuals and model parameters inspired by harvester ants plotted with time on a logarithmic scale.

(a) Reverse social contagion dominating social contagion (β0/γ0 = 1/10). (b) Reverse social contagion and social contagion at the same level (β0/γ0 = 1). (c) Social contagion dominating reverse social contagion (β0/γ0 = 10). Steady-state predictions in equation set (4) from Materials and Methods, “Equilibria of the compartmental model,” are marked as colored dots in each panel.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1012623.g003

Asymptotic results in Eqs (2a) and (2b) for the number of active and inactive individuals at the steady state are close to exact values for the largest value of β0, suggesting that scaling arguments could be safely used for order-of-magnitude calculations in the case of strong social contagion, see Fig 4. For smaller values of β0, the agreement between asymptotic predictions and exact values is qualitatively equivalent, albeit asymptotic values should only be used as an order of magnitude estimation. In fact, predictions of the scaling exponents for all salient variables, identified from observations over a range of colony sizes from 10 to 1,000, indicate close agreement between equation set (2) and exact values—see Text A and Fig A in the S1 File.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Comparison between asymptotic predictions in equation set (2) (dashed lines) and exact values (solid lines) in equation set (4) from Materials and Methods, “Equilibria of the compartmental model,” for the steady-state number of active (A, orange), inactive (I, green), and refractory (R, black) ants as a function of the colony size, N, for model parameters inspired by harvester ants and β0/γ0 = 10.

Experimental observations of harvester ants, presented by Porfiri et al. [28], are included for completeness (orange diamonds).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1012623.g004

Monte Carlo simulations

In agreement with evidence on classical epidemiological models [50], our compartmental model is a close representation of the macroscopic response of the colony for a homogeneous network of interactions. We demonstrate this point using Monte Carlo simulations, a typical modeling strategy to study collective behavior in ants [37].

The compartmental model in equation set (1) could be viewed as a mean-field approximation of an agent-based model, in which ants interact over a temporal network following the state transitions in Results, “State transitions.” More specifically, we simulate a network of N nodes. Each node can be in state A, I, or R. The system evolves in discrete time according to the state transitions described in Results, “State transitions,” with a time-step Δt = 0.0667 s. The network of interactions switches in time without memory, simulating complete mixing in the colony where any individual can in principle interact with any other ant. At each time step the network of interactions is drawn from an Erdős-Rényi network [51] with and link probability p = 〈k〉/N so that its average degree is 〈k〉. The model does not consider full motion trajectories, such as the work by Puckett et al. [52] and Ni et al. [53], and should be viewed as a limit case for full mixing. Fig 5 illustrates two consecutive time steps in the simulation.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Illustration of the Monte Carlo simulation, implemented on a switching Erdős-Rényi network.

Active, inactive, and refractory nodes are colored orange, green, and black, respectively. Example of reverse social contagion: node 1 is active at time t and, through the interaction with another active node, it becomes refractory at t + δt. Example of spontaneous transition from refractory to inactive: at t, node 2 is refractory and it spontaneously become inactive at t + Δt. Example of social contagion: node 3 is inactive at t and, through the interaction with an active node, it becomes active at t + Δt.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1012623.g005

We simulate a colony of N = 500 individuals for T = 150 time steps starting from the same initial condition, consisting of all individuals being active except for one that is inactive, analogously to the compartmental model. Model parameters are the same as in the compartmental model (α = 1.47, γ0 = 1.21 s−1, and δ = 0.63 s−1). We choose the smallest value of β0 (β0 = γ0/10), corresponding to the slowest transient to best visualize the comparison between model predictions and agent-based simulations. We run 10 simulations and compute mean and standard deviations at each time step. As shown in Fig 6a, 6c and 6e, model predictions are in excellent agreement with the Monte Carlo simulations.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Comparison between the numerical results of the compartmental model in equation set (1) (dashed line) and simulations of an agent-based model (solid line, mean, and dashed regions, one standard deviation above and below the mean) for a colony of N = 500 individuals (a,c,e) and N = 10 individuals (b,d,f).

(a,b) Fraction of active ants (A, orange). (c,d) Fraction of inactive ants (I, green). (e,f) Fraction of refractory ants (R, black).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1012623.g006

The accuracy of the compartmental model is unaffected by reducing the colony size to N = 10, as shown in Fig 6b, 6d and 6f; therein, the number of realizations was raised to 100. Obviously, the variation among different realizations increases, but the mean is in excellent agreement with the compartmental model. Comparing results in the left and right columns of Fig 6, we confirm the hypometric scaling of the number of active individuals, whereby the fractions of active individuals increase more than twofold when lowering the size of the colony from 500 to 10.

Discussion

We showed that efficient activity regulation can emerge in highly integrated social groups from a balance between two socially driven feedback processes. First, an autocatalytic process of social contagion results in the activation of inactive individuals, after stimulation by already active individuals. This positive feedback is then counterbalanced by an “autoinhibitory” process that we call reverse social contagion where active individuals are increasingly likely to stop working as they interact with more active individuals. As a consequence, the number of active individuals in the group will be naturally capped at a proportion of the group size, whose value depends on the balance between the two aforementioned processes. This is compatible with observations of “lazy” individuals with very low levels of activity and commonly observed in large colonies of eusocial insects [54].

We also demonstrated that, when the number of interactions between the members of the group grows faster than the size of the group, the proportion of active individuals grows hypometrically with the group size. Hence, the group becomes energetically more efficient as its size increases. Both network and energy scalings are commonly observed in group-living organisms [6, 7, 41, 47], supporting the hypothesis that social interactions are critical in explaining the energetic properties of social groups (including human beings). Notably, this scaling does not occur when the refractory state is removed from the model, suggesting that the refractory state serving as a reservoir is critically important to recovering the relationship between activation and colony size observed in experimental data.

Finally, the reverse social contagion mechanism that we study here is fundamentally different from the inactivation mechanisms proposed in existing models of activity dynamics. Indeed, the latter are typically not socially driven, and they rely on constant duration of activity [22], constant rate of inactivation [9, 11], or the ability of an individual to estimate the amount of remaining work [21]. In the first two cases, activity regulation is entirely dependent on the activation process and the hypometric energy scaling disappears. In the last case, the inactivation process can depend on the social environment as in our model, albeit indirectly via stigmergic communication since the amount of remaining work is impacted by the actions of the active individuals. This scenario is already included in our model since it does not require specifying the mode of interaction (direct or indirect) between active individuals but simply its rate.

In conclusion, our study provides a generalizable explanation for activity regulation in social groups. Its predictions are in agreement with qualitative and quantitative observations of general activity patterns and energetic scaling found in highly integrated social species, such as ant colonies. Our next step is to incorporate multiple activity types and mobility in the model, towards a framework for the study of more complex and specific patterns of emergent division of labor and their impact on energy use in animal and human groups.

Methods and materials

Positivity of the compartmental model

To prove that given well-defined initial conditions, all the populations remain non-negative, we start by bounding from below the right-hand side of Eq (1c) and integrating over time to establish R(t)≥R0exp(−δt)>0 through the comparison principle [55]. Substituting the above bound for R(t) into Eq (1b) and applying the comparison principle again, we can bound the right-hand side and integrate over time to obtain . Eq (1a), can be written as , where , so that .

Equilibria of the compartmental model

Equation set (1) has two feasible equilibria (denoted with a superscript star), which are determined by setting to zero the left-hand side of two of the three equations in the set and imposing A + I + R = N. The first equilibrium is the trivial one A = R = 0 and I = N, where the entire population is inactive. The other equilibrium is the positive root of a quadratic equation, obtained by solving Eq (1a) for I and Eq (1c) for R (assuming the left-hand sides to be zero and setting A ≠ 0). Following these steps, we establish (3)

By solving for A and recalling the steps to obtain this equation from Eqs (1a) and (1c), we identify the equilibrium: (4a) (4b) (4c) where we introduce the function (5)

Note that, in principle, Eq (3) has two solutions, but the other is negative, since model parameters are all positive.

Local stability analysis of the non-trivial equilibrium

By carrying out the algebra, we determine the following expression for the Jacobian: (6)

By replacing the trivial equilibrium (A = R = 0 and I = N), we obtain a block-diagonal matrix, whose eigenvalues are 0, −δ, and β0k〉; the latter is positive, hence the instability of the equilibrium.

To assess the stability of the non-trivial equilibrium in equation set (4), we apply Routh-Hurwitz criterion [56] to the characteristic polynomial of the Jacobian. Specifically, the characteristic polynomial of the Jacobian in correspondence to the non-trivial equilibrium can be factored as (7) where (14) (8a) (8b)

Coefficients a1 and a2 are positive, like a0, as one might verify by comparing the squares of the two summands in each expression. Based on the Routh-Hurwitz stability criterion, the roots of the second-order polynomial in Eq (7) have strictly negative real parts, which yields marginal stability of the non-trivial equilibrium. The null eigenvalue is related to the original constraints that A(t) + I(t) + R(t) = N.

Global stability analysis of the non-trivial equilibrium

To prove global stability of the non-trivial equilibrium, we replace I(t) with NA(t) − R(t) into Eqs (1a) and (1c) and study global asymptotic convergence of the A(t) and R(t) towards A and R in Eqs (4a) and (4c). We construct a Lyapunov function for this two-dimensional system that yields global stability [55]. We take inspiration from Goh-Lotka-Volterra Lyapunov functions to cope with the nonlinearity in the system [43]. Specifically, we propose the following function: (9)

Such a function is positive definite in [0, N]2, it is zero at (A, R), and all its level sets are bounded. For V to be a Lyapunov function, we must show that for any (A, R)∈[0, N]2, except of (A, R) where .

To prove this claim, we compute the time derivative of V(A, R) along the system trajectories, using equation sets (1) and (4) to obtain (10)

The first summand is a quadratic function in R that is negative in [0, N], except for R = R, where it is zero. Likewise, the second summand is a cubic function in A that is negative in [0, N], except for A = A where it is zero. As a result, for any (A, R)∈[0, N]2, except of (A, R) where .

Compartmental model without reverse social contagion

We study a variation of equation set (1) where reverse social contagion is replaced with a spontaneous transition from active to refractory at a constant rate μ > 0, (11a) (11b) (11c)

Searching for equilibria of the system, we set the left-hand side of each equation to zero and solve for A, I, and R. In addition to the trivial equilibrium presented in Results, “Compartmental model” (A = R = 0 and I = N), we determine the following equilibrium for μ < β0k〉: and . Hence, without reverse social contagion, the number of active ants would scale isometrically with colony size, whereby for α = 1 and N ≫ 1, and simply for α = 1.

Compartmental model without refractory state

We study a variation of equation set (1) where the refractory state is removed. Within this variation, active ants become inactive and inactive ants become active, both through social interaction with active individuals. The equation set becomes (12a) (12b)

Analogously to the study of the system without reserve social contagion in equation set (11), we can find the two equilibria for this system: the trivial equilibrium (A, I) = (0, N) and the endemic equilibrium . Hence, without refractory state, the number of active ants will scale isometrically with the colony size.

Calibration of model parameters on experimental data

The experimental dataset consists of 16 manually tracked videos of harvester ant colonies in 248 × 248 mm colony nest enclosures [6]. The length of each video was 30 s and the resolution was Δt = 66.7 ms, corresponding to 15 frames per second—note that manual tracking was performed on downsampled videos, retaining one every five frames. The size of the colony was different in each video, ranging from N = 40 to 360 workers (nominal values).

Network parameters E0 and α were estimated in our prior work [28] by constructing a spatial interaction network as follows. For each colony and each frame, we created an undirected interaction network, where two ants were connected by an edge if they were within a distance of 6 mm, corresponding to one body length. Then, we averaged the number of edges, E, in each video to compute the total number of edges for each colony. Finally, we fitted E against N in the logarithmic scale.

From our previous research [28], we could also estimate the ratio δ/γ0. Therein, for each pair of consecutive frames in a video, we identified active ants as those that would move at least one pixel. For each video, we computed the total number of active ants, A (also reported in Fig 4). Then, we fitted the overall extent of the reverse social contagion, 2A2E/N2, against N to determine a slope q = 0.519. By comparing against model predictions for A in Eq (2a), we estimate δ/γ0 = 0.519—such an estimate relies on the limit N ≫ 1.

To tease out δ from γ, we separately estimated γ0 through an additional analysis of the videos. Different from the previous analyses, this step required us to score the activity of each ant in time, rather than once for the entire video. In particular, at each frame, we considered an ant to be (instantaneously) active if it moved at least a pixel from the previous frame. Any ant that was not scored as active was deemed to be either inactive or in a refractory state, the two being indistinguishable from video data. Across videos, we counted: i) the total number of tracked frames in which two active ants interacted (9861), and ii) the total number of instances in which two active ants interacted in one of these frames and then one became not active in the next tracked frame (800). By dividing ii) by i), we estimated the probability of reverse social contagion (0.0811), which, upon dividing by Δt yielded our estimation for γ0 of 1.21 s−1. Based on the value of q, we then estimated δ as 0.63 s−1.

Estimating β0 without being able to discern inactive from refractory ants was not feasible. In fact, an argument equivalent to the one used to estimate γ0 could not be pursued for β0. Given that only inactive ants could cause social contagion, the probability of an event like A + I or R → 2 A should be interpreted as the probability of social contagion (β0Δt) times the probability that an ant that was not active was actually inactive (I/(I + R) from equation set (2)). Such a probability is, however, independent of β0. Alternative ways around this issue would require access to longer time series to study transient phenomena, or a larger set of experimental trials to retain the full dependence of the population variables on the colony size in equation set (4).

Supporting information

S1 File. Adequacy of the scaling formulas.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1012623.s001

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We are thankful to Ofek Lauber, Gian Carlo Maffettone, and Michael Napoli for verifying the derivations and to Eighdi Aung for helping with the video analysis.

References

  1. 1. Rueffler C, Hermisson J, Wagner GP. Evolution of functional specialization and division of labor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109(6):E326–35. pmid:22308336
  2. 2. Liu W, Winfield AF, Sa J, Chen J, Dou L. Towards energy optimization: Emergent task allocation in a swarm of foraging robots. Adaptive Behavior. 2007;15(3):289–305.
  3. 3. Palmieri N, Yang XS, De Rango F, Marano S. Comparison of bio-inspired algorithms applied to the coordination of mobile robots considering the energy consumption. Neural Computing and Applications. 2019;31(1):263–286.
  4. 4. Mao W, Zhao Z, Chang Z, Min G, Gao W. Energy-efficient industrial internet of things: Overview and open issues. IEEE Transactions on Industrial Informatics. 2021;17(11):7225–7237.
  5. 5. Beshers SN, Fewell JH. Models of division of labor in social insects. Annual Review of Entomology. 2001;46:413–440. pmid:11112175
  6. 6. Waters JS, Holbrook CT, Fewell JH, Harrison JF. Allometric scaling of metabolism, growth, and activity in whole colonies of the seed-harvester ant Pogonomyrmex californicus. The American Naturalist. 2010;176(4):501–510. pmid:20735259
  7. 7. Fewell JH, Harrison JF. Scaling of work and energy use in social insect colonies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2016;70(7):1047–1061.
  8. 8. Kleiber M. Body size and metabolic rate. Physiological Reviews. 1947;27(4):511–541. pmid:20267758
  9. 9. Goss S, Deneubourg JL. Autocatalysis as a source of synchronised rhythmical activity in social insects. Insectes Sociaux. 1988;35(3):310–315.
  10. 10. Cole BJ, Trampus FI. Activity cycles in ant colonies: worker interactions and decentralized control. In: Detrain C, Deneubourg JL, Pasteels JM, editors. Information Processing in Social Insects. Basel: Birkhäuser Basel; 1999. p. 289–307.
  11. 11. Bonabeau E, Theraulaz G. Role and variability of response thresholds in the regulation of division of labor in insect societies. In: Detrain C, Deneubourg JL, Pasteels JM, editors. Information Processing in Social Insects. Basel: Birkhäuser Basel; 1999. p. 141–163.
  12. 12. Robinson GE. Modulation of alarm pheromone perception in the honey bee: evidence for division of labor based on hormonally regulated response thresholds. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1987;160(5):613–619.
  13. 13. Pankiw T, Waddington K, Page R. Modulation of sucrose response thresholds in honey bees (Apis mellifera L.): influence of genotype, feeding, and foraging experience. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology. 2001;187(4):293–301. pmid:11467502
  14. 14. Detrain C, Prieur J. Sensitivity and feeding efficiency of the black garden ant Lasius niger to sugar resources. Journal of Insect Physiology. 2014;64:74–80. pmid:24667145
  15. 15. Camazine S, Deneubourg JL, Franks NR, Sneyd J, Theraulaz G, Bonabeau E. Self-Organization in Biological Systems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 2001.
  16. 16. Traniello JF. Chapter 219—Recruitment Communication. In: Resh VH, Cardé RT, editors. Encyclopedia of Insects (Second Edition). San Diego: Academic Press; 2009. p. 869–877.
  17. 17. von Frisch K. The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press; 1967.
  18. 18. Planqué R, van den Berg JB, Franks NR. Recruitment strategies and colony size in ants. PLoS ONE. 2010;5(8):e11664. pmid:20694195
  19. 19. Ono M, Okada I, Sasaki M. Heat production by balling in the Japanese honeybee, Apis cerana japonica as a defensive behavior against the hornet, Vespa simillima xanthoptera (Hymenoptera: Vespidae). Experientia. 1987;43(9):1031–1034.
  20. 20. Anderson C, Fernandez-Nieves A. Active many-particle systems and the emergent behavior of dense ant collectives. Reports on Progress in Physics. 2024;87(6):066602. pmid:38804124
  21. 21. Feng T, Charbonneau D, Qiu Z, Kang Y. Dynamics of task allocation in social insect colonies: Scaling effects of colony size versus work activities. Journal of Mathematical Biology. 2021;82(5):42. pmid:33779857
  22. 22. Doering GN, Drawert B, Lee C, Pruitt JN, Petzold LR, Dalnoki-Veress K. Noise resistant synchronization and collective rhythm switching in a model of animal group locomotion. Royal Society Open Science. 2022;9(3):211908. pmid:35291326
  23. 23. Pagliara R, Gordon DM, Leonard NE. Regulation of harvester ant foraging as a closed-loop excitable system. PLOS Computational Biology. 2018;14(12):1–25. pmid:30513076
  24. 24. Nieh JC. The stop signal of honey bees: reconsidering its message. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1993;33(1):51–56.
  25. 25. Huang ZY, Robinson GE. Social control of division of labor in honey bee colonies. In: Information Processing in Social Insects. Basel: Birkhäuser Basel; 1999. p. 165–186.
  26. 26. Czaczkes TJ, Gruter C, Ratnieks FLW. Negative feedback in ants: crowding results in less trail pheromone deposition. Journal of the Royal Society Interface. 2013;10(81):20121009–20121009. pmid:23365196
  27. 27. Robinson EJH, Jackson DE, Holcombe M, Ratnieks FLW. Insect communication: ‘no entry’ signal in ant foraging. Nature. 2005;438(7067):442. pmid:16306981
  28. 28. Porfiri M, De Lellis P, Aung E, Meneses S, Abaid N, Waters JS, et al. Reverse social contagion as a mechanism for regulating mass behaviors in highly integrated social systems. PNAS Nexus. 2024;3(7):pgae246. pmid:38962249
  29. 29. Waters JS, Ochs A, Fewell JH, Harrison JF. Differentiating causality and correlation in allometric scaling: Ant colony size drives metabolic hypometry. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2017;284(1849):20162582. pmid:28228514
  30. 30. Bettencourt LM. The origins of scaling in cities. Science. 2013;340(6139):1438–1441. pmid:23788793
  31. 31. Reina A, Marshall JA. Negative feedback may suppress variation to improve collective foraging performance. PLoS Computational Biology. 2022;18(5):e1010090. pmid:35584189
  32. 32. Goldberg TS, Bloch G. Inhibitory signaling in collective social insect networks, is it indeed uncommon? Current Opinion in Insect Science. 2023;59:101107. pmid:37634618
  33. 33. Ratnieks FLW, Grüter C, Czaczkes TJ. Rapid up- and down-regulation of pheromone signalling due to trail crowding in the ant Lasius niger. Behaviour. 2014;151(5):669–682.
  34. 34. Seeley TD, Visscher PK, Schlegel T, Hogan PM, Franks NR, Marshall JAR. Stop signals provide cross inhibition in collective decision-making by honeybee swarms. Science (New York, NY). 2012;335(6064):108–111. pmid:22157081
  35. 35. Bidari S, Peleg O, Kilpatrick ZP. Social inhibition maintains adaptivity and consensus of honeybees foraging in dynamic environments. Royal Society Open Science. 2019;6(12):191681. pmid:31903216
  36. 36. Pais D, Hogan PM, Schlegel T, Franks NR, Leonard NE, Marshall JAR. A mechanism for value-sensitive decision-making. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(9):e73216. pmid:24023835
  37. 37. Detrain C, Deneubourg JL. Self-organized structures in a superorganism: do ants “behave” like molecules? Physics of Life Reviews. 2006;3(3):162–187.
  38. 38. da Silveira PM, Fontanari JF. A soluble model for synchronized rhythmic activity in ant colonies. Mathematical Biosciences. 2024;375:109245. pmid:38969059
  39. 39. Hethcote HW. Qualitative analyses of communicable disease models. Mathematical Biosciences. 1976;28(3-4):335–356.
  40. 40. Baglietto G, Albano EV, Candia J. Complex network structure of flocks in the Standard Vicsek Model. Journal of Statistical Physics. 2013;153:270–288.
  41. 41. Rocha LE, Ryckebusch J, Schoors K, Smith M. The scaling of social interactions across animal species. Scientific Reports. 2021;11(1):12584. pmid:34131247
  42. 42. Korobeinikov A, Wake GC. Lyapunov functions and global stability for SIR, SIRS, and SIS epidemiological models. Applied Mathematics Letters. 2002;15(8):955–960.
  43. 43. Cangiotti N, Capolli M, Sensi M, Sottile S. A survey on Lyapunov functions for epidemic compartmental models. Bollettino dell’Unione Matematica Italiana. 2023;17:1–17. pmid:37360758
  44. 44. Lipp A, Wolf H, Lehmann FO. Walking on inclines: energetics of locomotion in the ant Camponotus. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2005;208(4):707–719. pmid:15695763
  45. 45. Stabentheiner A, Vollmann J, Kovac H, Crailsheim K. Oxygen consumption and body temperature of active and resting honeybees. Journal of Insect Physiology. 2003;49(9):881–889. pmid:16256690
  46. 46. Käfer H, Kovac H, Oswald B, Stabentheiner A. Respiration and metabolism of the resting European paper wasp (Polistes dominulus). Journal of Comparative Physiology B, Biochemical, Systemic, and Environmental Physiology. 2015;185(6):647–658. pmid:26135799
  47. 47. Hou C, Kaspari M, Vander Zanden HB, Gillooly JF. Energetic basis of colonial living in social insects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107(8):3634–3638. pmid:20133582
  48. 48. White CR, Kearney MR, Matthews PG, Kooijman SA, Marshall DJ. A manipulative test of competing theories for metabolic scaling. The American Naturalist. 2011;178(6):746–754. pmid:22089869
  49. 49. Hartikainen H, Humphries S, Okamura B. Form and metabolic scaling in colonial animals. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2014;217(5):779–786. pmid:24265433
  50. 50. Bansal S, Grenfell BT, Meyers LA. When individual behaviour matters: homogeneous and network models in epidemiology. Journal of the Royal Society Interface. 2007;4(16):879–891. pmid:17640863
  51. 51. Estrada E. The Structure of Complex Networks: Theory and Applications. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012.
  52. 52. Puckett JG, Ni R, Ouellette NT. Time-frequency analysis reveals pairwise interactions in insect swarms. Physical Review Letters. 2015;114(25):258103. pmid:26197145
  53. 53. Ni R, Puckett JG, Dufresne ER, Ouellette NT. Intrinsic fluctuations and driven response of insect swarms. Physical Review Letters. 2015;115(11):118104. pmid:26406859
  54. 54. Charbonneau D, Hillis N, Dornhaus A. ‘Lazy’ in nature: ant colony time budgets show high ‘inactivity’ in the field as well as in the lab. Insectes Sociaux. 2015;62(1):31–35.
  55. 55. Khalil HK. Control of Nonlinear Systems. Prentice Hall, New York, NY; 2002.
  56. 56. Ogata K. System Dynamics. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson; 2004.