Figures
Abstract
Social insect colonies use negative as well as positive feedback signals to regulate foraging behaviour. In ants and bees individual foragers have been observed to use negative pheromones or mechano-auditory signals to indicate that forage sources are not ideal, for example being unrewarded, crowded, or dangerous. Here we propose an additional function for negative feedback signals during foraging, variance reduction. We show that while on average populations will converge to desired distributions over forage patches both with and without negative feedback signals, in small populations negative feedback reduces variation around the target distribution compared to the use of positive feedback alone. Our results are independent of the nature of the target distribution, providing it can be achieved by foragers collecting only local information. Since robustness is a key aim for biological systems, and deviation from target foraging distributions may be costly, we argue that this could be a further important and hitherto overlooked reason that negative feedback signals are used by foraging social insects.
Author Summary
Social insect colonies regulate the number of insects foraging at different food sources through a combination of positive and negative feedback signals. Through positive feedback signals—such as ants’ pheromone trails and bees’ waggle dances—insects recruit each other to increase the number of foragers committed to a food source that has been evaluated as profitable. Negative feedbacks are instead inhibitory signals that are delivered to reduce commitment to a food source where an unfavourable change has been detected, for example the arrival of a predator or a decrease in nutritional reward. Our mathematical analysis explains an additional function for negative feedback; inhibitory signals can also be useful in static conditions to reduce the variance in the number of insects allocated to each food source, thus better distributing insects among the available sources. Our results can help explain field observations that are not fully understood yet, such as the periodic delivery of a small number of inhibitory signals among honeybees visiting the same forage patch even in static conditions.
Citation: Reina A, Marshall JAR (2022) Negative feedback may suppress variation to improve collective foraging performance. PLoS Comput Biol 18(5): e1010090. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010090
Editor: Christian Hilbe, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Biology: Max-Planck-Institut fur Evolutionsbiologie, GERMANY
Received: January 12, 2022; Accepted: April 8, 2022; Published: May 18, 2022
Copyright: © 2022 Reina, Marshall. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: All data, simulation code, and a Mathematica notebook reproducing the analyses presented herein are available in the GitHub repository https://github.com/DiODeProject/VarianceSuppression. This is supplemented by a notebook for MuMoT, an open-source tool for multiscale modelling, which reproduces similar results to those presented herein. The notebook is available online at https://mybinder.org/v2/gh/DiODeProject/MuMoT/master?filepath=DemoNotebooks%2FVariance_suppression.ipynb and https://github.com/DiODeProject/MuMoT/blob/master/DemoNotebooks/Variance_suppression.ipynb.
Funding: A.R. acknowledges support from the Belgian F.R.S.-FNRS, of which he is a Chargé de Recherches (https://www.frs-fnrs.be). J.A.R.M. was funded by the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant agreement number 647704), https://erc.europa.eu/funding/. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Collectively-foraging social insects use feedback mechanisms in order to robustly and efficiently satisfy the nutritional requirements of the colony. Positive feedback signal usage by such foraging social insects is well known, such as mass-recruitment via pheromone in various ant species [1], and recruitment of small numbers of individuals such as via the honeybees’ waggle-dance [2], or rock ants’ tandem-running [3]. The use of negative feedback signals in these systems has, however received comparatively little attention. Negative feedback was predicted to be important for collectively foraging species [4, 5], and subsequently discovered in diverse systems such as Pharaoh’s ants [6, 7] and honeybees [8, 9]. Several studies have interpreted negative feedback as a mechanism to reduce recruitment to a resource based on some aspect of its quality, for example allowing unrewarded trails to be shut down [6, 7], allowing recruitment to a crowded source of forage to be reduced [10], or transferring information that a forage patch may have an increased predation risk [8, 11]. Subsequent studies have similarly focussed on the role of negative feedback in dealing with time-varying forage patches [12, 13], or with the amount of available comb storage space [14].
Here we propose an alternative function for negative feedback mechanisms in collective foraging, suppression of costly variation in the colony’s foraging performance. In the following, we present simple models of collective foraging with positive and negative feedback, and with positive feedback only. We show how both models are able to approach a desired target distribution over forage patches on average, when forager populations are assumed to be infinite. However, when finite forager populations are modelled, the two foraging systems differ in the robustness with which they achieve the target distribution; with positive feedback only, stochastic fluctuations can lead to the forager population being far from its target distribution at any point in time, however by adding negative feedback the forager distribution becomes more robust. We argue that this will increase colony-level foraging success [15, 16], and thus may represent a new functional explanation for the observation of negative feedback in foraging by social insect colonies.
Foraging theory is an active and complex research area, and our results do not rely on assumptions about the nature of the colony’s target distribution, other than it can be achieved by agents with access only to local information at both the forage source, and the colony. Thus, the target distribution may be akin to an Ideal Free Distribution, in which agents are distributed such that none can improve overall foraging efficiency by switching to a different forage patch [16, 17]. Alternatively, the target distribution may be based on the requirement of the colony for different micro- and macro-nutrients [18–21]. Or, the target distribution may be based on some other objective entirely, or on combinations of objectives such as those just discussed. In ignoring the nature of the distribution, therefore, our focus is purely on the dynamics of foraging, and how negative feedback can improve this.
For our analysis we adapt our model from a simple model of negative feedback for foraging in honeybee colonies [12], in itself inspired by models of negative feedback in house-hunting honeybee swarms [22–24]; however since other social insect species such as Pharaoh’s Ants also make use of negative feedback during foraging [6], we argue that the model is generally applicable.
Methods
We assume a target distribution of the individuals to the n patches in quantities proportional to the relative patch quality arbitrarily defined:
(1)
where qi is the quality of patch i. In our models an individual’s state can be either uncommitted (XU) or committed to patch i (Xi) with i ∈ {1, …, n}. Therefore, based on the number of patches n, the commitment of the population will be split among n + 1 subpopulations; we represent the subpopulation proportions as xU and xi, in the closed interval [0, 1]. Note that, in a finite population of S individuals, it will be impossible for the colony to achieve exactly the desired target distribution if xi S is not an integer number.
We analyse the population dynamics of the two systems parametrised to reach the same target distribution (with and without negative social feedback) using mean-field models of infinite and finite populations, using ordinary differential equations (ODEs) and stochastic simulation of the master equation respectively. Both types of analyses can be performed for models derived from chemical reaction equations, which specify how individuals in the system interact and change state (see Table 1).
The commitment states are ‘committed to foraging patch i’ (Xi) or ‘uncommitted’ (XU). Both models have the same positive and negative feedback for independent transitions: quality-dependent discovery and constant abandonment (leak a). The difference lies in the social feedback; one model (blue) has quality-insensitive recruitment (ri = ρ) but no negative social feedback (z = 0). The other model (red) has both quality-sensitive recruitment (ri = ρqi) and quality-insensitive self-inhibition (z > 0), as reported by field observations [30]. In these representative models, we set rates as constant and (linear) quality-sensitive functions of the quality according to the best function we obtain with numerical optimisation (see S1 Text).
The ODE model assumes an infinitely-large population size S and provides deterministic system dynamics in the absence of any noise from finite population effects. On the other hand, stochastic simulation of the master equation (Gillespie’s SSA [25]) gives a probabilistically correct simulation of dynamics of finite populations of size S.
While previous research has documented that collective foraging is regulated by the actions and interactions that we included in our models, the relationship between their frequency (transition rates) and the estimated nest-site quality are still debated. Table 1 reports the best functions we obtained through numerical optimisation to approximate the target distribution. Including negative feedback inevitably requires a change also in the recruitment function, from constant to linearly proportional to the quality. The difference in the recruitment function between the two models has not been imposed by design but is the result of the numerical optimisation analysis that we show in detail in S1 Text. Here, we assume that social recruitment (positive feedback) is much more efficient than independent discovery, so ri ≫ qi, as has been documented in a large variety of social insect species [26–29]. For fair comparison, the average recruitment strength r is equalised between the two models so that quality-sensitive recruitment transitions—model with negative feedback—happens on average at the same rate of quality-insensitive recruitment—model without negative feedback (see S2 Text). The model with only positive feedback is easy to solve for the desired equilibrium distribution of foragers, with a simple parameterisation of individuals’ rates (see S3 Text). The model with negative feedback, however, requires a heuristic individual parameterisation based on site qualities, which we perform numerically. However, this heuristic has a simple functional form (see Fig A in S2 Text) so could easily be approximated by real foragers.
Results
The two top panels of Fig 1 show the time dynamics of the two models for representative values and n = 3 patches. Both models asymptotically approximate the target distribution of Eq (1).
The top panels show the dynamics of the ODEs for systems of infinite size S → ∞. The bottom panels show the trajectories of 30 representative runs of the stochastic simulation algorithm (SSA, [25]) for a system comprised of S = 200 individuals. The boxplots on the right of each bottom panel show the statistical aggregate at time 400 for 1000 runs of the SSA. (Other simulation parameters are: constant abandonment a = 10−3, average recruitment strength r = 100, and stop signal strength z ≃ 3.1.) While the infinite size dynamics predict convergence to the target distribution of Eq (1) (dashed lines) for both models, the stochastic trajectories show different results for the two models. The system without negative social feedback has smaller fluctuations over time but frequently stabilises at values far from the target distribution (bottom-left panel). The system with negative social feedback fluctuates more but always remains relatively close to the target distribution (bottom-right panel). The apparently quicker dynamics of the ODE model for the system without negative social feedback are due to the symmetric initial conditions. In the left inset, we show that a small perturbation of the initial population (i.e. x1, x2 = 0 and x3 = 0.05) delays the convergence by more than 5 orders of magnitude. Such a susceptibility to random fluctuations is made evident by the stochastic trajectories. The right inset shows a zoom of the larger plot.
Through numerical integration of the master equations, we investigate the effect of stochastic fluctuations on the system dynamics [25]. The fluctuation size is inversely proportional to the system size S, i.e. there are no fluctuations in very large groups (i.e. S → ∞) and large fluctuation in small groups. The effect of the system-size noise can be appreciated in the two bottom panels of Fig 1. They show 30 representative trajectories for a system of size S = 200. The higher variance can also be appreciated in the boxplots on the right of each bottom panel of Fig 1, in which the average of 1,000 simulations hits the target value in both models; however, the variance is reduced considerably with the introduction of negative social feedback. These results are not specific to the representative example of Fig 1, but are consistent throughout the wide parameter space (see analysis in S4 Text). Additionally, increasing abandonment, which is a form of independent, asocial negative feedback, is not sufficient to reduce variance (see S5 Text).
Large deviations from the target distribution could compromise the ability of the colony to intake the necessary nutrients for survival and reproduction, thus decreasing colony fitness. Fig 2 shows how the error in achieving the target distribution is significantly higher without negative social feedback. Similarly, the speed of adaptation to environmental changes is an important factor in the survival of the colony [31, 32]. The system without negative feedback can be incapable of adapting to changes in a timely manner because its temporal dynamics vary significantly depending on the initial commitment (see top-left inset of Fig 1). The system with negative feedback, instead, displays a constant convergence time regardless of the initial state of the system (see S6 Text). Fig 3 shows how the convergence speed and the deviation from the target distribution are influenced by the strength of the negative feedback; the strength of negative feedback can tune a speed-robustness trade-off, similarly to the tuning of speed-value and speed-coherence trade-offs in consensus decisions [23, 24, 33]. In agreement with field observations of honeybees, which increase stop signalling when a quick response is necessary [10], our analysis also predicts a speed-up of the group dynamics for higher levels of negative feedback. The dynamics of Fig 3 can also be discussed in light of the results of a recent empirical study which has found that sensitivity to inhibitory signalling changes as a function of colony size [34]. Our model, by linking the negative feedback rate with convergence speed and the deviation from the target distribution, offers predictions that can be tested on colonies of different sizes. For example, the time to adaptation to environmental changes is expected to be longer in smaller colonies, where bees show a lower sensitivity to inhibitory signalling [34], than in larger colonies.
The boxplots show the distribution of the SSE for 103 numerical simulations for swarm size S = 200, average recruitment strength r = 100, and qualities q1 = 0.75 and q2 = 0.5.
Stronger stop signalling speeds up the convergence of the system (magenta curve) but also increases the predicted error from the target distribution (blue curve). These results are in agreement with field observations that documented an increase in stop signalling when a quick response to environmental changes was necessary [10]. The inset shows that for increasing recruitment strength ρ both convergence time and error decrease. Both error and convergence time are computed from the infinite population model (ODE). The error is computed as the sum for every foraging population of the squared distance R2 from the target at large time (convergence, computed analytically as the ODE’s stable fixed point in the unit-simplex). The convergence time (magenta curve) is computed as the time necessary to reach the (numerically computed) fixed point. As the system has an asymptotic convergence, the reported time corresponds to the R2 error becoming smaller than 10−4.
Discussion
Negative feedback has been considered in collective decisions, particularly as a means of symmetry breaking [22–24, 35], and in foraging, as a means of adapting to dynamically changing environments [7, 10, 12, 13]. Other than in entomology, negative feedback has been observed as a tool for noise reduction in gene networks [36–38] and in electronic systems [39, 40]. Here we have shown that negative feedback may play an important role in reducing variance in colony foraging performance. For example, considering the honeybee system that inspired our model, field observations have reported that levels of stop signalling increase in response to changes such as dangerous, overcrowded, or depleted food patches [10, 11, 13, 41]; however, it has not yet been fully understood why, even in static conditions, honeybees always deliver a small number of stop signals to foragers visiting the same forage patch [10, 13, 34]. This pattern is consistent with our model, and the analysis presented is an interpretation for such observed behaviour.
Our results suggest a further progression in the evolution of collective foraging behaviour; solitary foraging by members of social insect colonies evolved first, but was comparatively inefficient due to the need for foragers to repeatedly and independently discover forage sites [42, 43] (see S8 Text). Subsequently, positive social feedback evolved to improve foraging efficiency [44–46], but this came at the expense of robustness of the foraging outcome, through increased variance in foraging performance (see S9 Text). Finally, negative feedback evolved not only to respond better to changing environments, but also to reduce variance in foraging performance. The re-use of negative feedback signals, such as in the case of honeybee stop-signals which are used in both foraging [10, 34] and house-hunting [22] life history stages, would facilitate performance-enhancing innovations in signalling behaviours; however, it is not clear whether stop-signalling first arose in foraging or in house-hunting contexts (intuitively, we suggest the former, a more common life history event).
Some species have not evolved negative signalling mechanisms but rely on natural decay of feedback, such as pheromone evaporation. For instance, Lasius niger ants rely on the downregulation of positive feedback (i.e. pheromone deposition) in order to let pheromone decay take over [47, 48]. It is worth noting that this is not technically negative feedback; given the time taken from the first observations of the positive-feedback signals in colonies of honeybees and ants [3, 49, 50] to that of the corresponding negative feedback signals [6, 51], it may be worth further exploring social insects in which explicit negative feedback has not been observed, to search for expected negative feedback mechanisms, or explain why their life history means they would not be beneficial. As a motivating example, decreases in the number of waggle circuit repetitions in honeybee swarms were taken to be due to decay processes internal to scout bees [52], but the negative stop-signal was subsequently discovered to be significant in these swarms [22].
We conclude by noting that our study highlights the importance of using multiscale modelling to understand collective behaviour [53–55]. In fact, through mean-field analysis we could not observe the dynamics that justify the use of the negative feedback. Instead, complementing the analysis with probabilistic models, we have been able to identify the system dynamics that favour the appearance of stop signalling as a mechanism for variance reduction. Multiscale modelling is a valuable framework which combines the use of a set of modelling techniques to analyse the system at various levels of complexity and noise. In this study, we only employed noise-free mean-field analysis and master equations with system-size dependent noise. However, further analysis could include the impact of spatial noise, and time-correlated information and/or interactions [53].
Supporting Information
S1 Text. Relationship between transition rates and food patch quality.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010090.s001
(PDF)
S4 Text. Variance reduction in a large parameter space.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010090.s004
(PDF)
S5 Text. Effect of asocial negative feedback (abandonment or leak α).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010090.s005
(PDF)
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Cristian Fernández-Oto, Gianni Di Caro, Ulrike Feudel, and David Wagg for useful discussions.
References
- 1.
Hölldobler B, Wilson EO. The Ants. Harvard University Press; 1990.
- 2.
Seeley TD. Honeybee Democracy. Princeton University Press; 2010.
- 3. Möglich M, Maschwitz U, Hölldobler B. Tandem Calling: A New Kind of Signal in Ant Communication. Science. 1974;186(4168):1046–7. pmid:4469698
- 4.
Stickland TR, Britton NF, Franks NR. Models of information flow in ant foraging: the benefits of both attractive and repulsive signals. In: Information Processing in Social Insects. Basel: Birkhäuser Basel; 1999. p. 83–100.
- 5. Detrain C, Deneubourg JL. Self-organized structures in a superorganism: do ants “behave” like molecules? Physics of Life Reviews. 2006;3(3):162–87.
- 6. Robinson EJH, Jackson DE, Holcombe M, Ratnieks FLW. ‘No entry’ signal in ant foraging. Nature. 2005;438(7067):442–2. pmid:16306981
- 7. Robinson EJH, Ratnieks FLW, Holcombe M. An agent-based model to investigate the roles of attractive and repellent pheromones in ant decision making during foraging. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 2008;255(2):250–8. pmid:18778716
- 8. Nieh JC. A Negative Feedback Signal That Is Triggered by Peril Curbs Honey Bee Recruitment. Current Biology. 2010;20(4):310–5. pmid:20153197
- 9. Kietzman PM, Visscher PK. The anti-waggle dance: use of the stop signal as negative feedback. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution. 2015;3(February):1–5.
- 10. Lau CW, Nieh JC. Honey bee stop-signal production: temporal distribution and effect of feeder crowding. Apidologie. 2010;41(1):87–95.
- 11. Tan K, Dong S, Li X, Liu X, Wang C, Li J, et al. Honey Bee Inhibitory Signaling Is Tuned to Threat Severity and Can Act as a Colony Alarm Signal. PLoS Biology. 2016;14(3):e1002423. pmid:27014876
- 12. Bidari S, Peleg O, Kilpatrick ZP. Social inhibition maintains adaptivity and consensus of honeybees foraging in dynamic environments. Royal Society Open Science. 2019;6(12):191681. pmid:31903216
- 13. Borofsky T, Barranca VJ, Zhou R, von Trentini D, Broadrup RL, Mayack C. Hive minded: like neurons, honey bees collectively integrate negative feedback to regulate decisions. Animal Behaviour. 2020;168:33–44.
- 14. Kietzman PM, Visscher PK. The influence of available comb storage space on the performance of honey bee communication signals that regulate foraging. Apidologie. 2021;52(1):133–40.
- 15. Shik JZ, Dussutour A. Nutritional Dimensions of Invasive Success. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2020;35(8):691–703. pmid:32668214
- 16.
Giraldeau LA, Caraco T. Social foraging theory. Princeton University Press; 2000.
- 17. Lamb AE, Ollason JG. Foraging wood-ants Formica aquilonia yarrow (hymenoptera: formicidae) tend to adopt the ideal free distribution. Behavioural Processes. 1993;28(3):189–98. pmid:24897604
- 18. McNamara JM, Houston AI, Collins EJ. Optimality Models in Behavioral Biology. SIAM Review. 2001;43(3):413–66.
- 19. Olsson O, Brown JS, Helf KL. A guide to central place effects in foraging. Theoretical Population Biology. 2008;74(1):22–33. pmid:18550139
- 20. Dussutour A, Simpson SJ. Communal Nutrition in Ants. Current Biology. 2009;19(9):740–4. pmid:19345104
- 21. Houston AI, McNamara JM. Foraging currencies, metabolism and behavioural routines. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2014;83(1):30–40. pmid:23730810
- 22. Seeley TD, Visscher PK, Schlegel T, Hogan PM, Franks NR, Marshall JAR. Stop Signals Provide Cross Inhibition in Collective Decision-Making by Honeybee Swarms. Science. 2012;335(6064):108–11. pmid:22157081
- 23. Pais D, Hogan PM, Schlegel T, Franks NR, Leonard NE, Marshall JAR. A Mechanism for Value-Sensitive Decision-Making. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(9):e73216. pmid:24023835
- 24. Reina A, Marshall JAR, Trianni V, Bose T. Model of the best-of-N nest-site selection process in honeybees. Physical Review E. 2017;95(5):052411. pmid:28618584
- 25. Gillespie DT. A General Method for Numerically Simulating Stochastic Time Evolution of Coupled Chemical Reactions. Journal of Computational Physics. 1976;22:403–34.
- 26. Bonabeau E, Theraulaz G, Deneubourg JL. Group and Mass Recruitment in Ant Colonies: the Influence of Contact Rates. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 1998;195(2):157–66. pmid:9822561
- 27. Chadab R, Rettenmeyer C. Mass recruitment by army ants. Science. 1975;188(4193):1124–5. pmid:1215991
- 28. Dornhaus A, Klügl F, Oechslein C, Puppe F, Chittka L. Benefits of recruitment in honey bees: effects of ecology and colony size in an individual-based model. Behavioral Ecology. 2006;17(3):336–44.
- 29. l’Anson Price R, Grüter C, Hughes WOH, Evison SEF. Symmetry breaking in mass-recruiting ants: extent of foraging biases depends on resource quality. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2016;70(11):1813–20.
- 30. Jack-McCollough RT, Nieh JC. Honeybees tune excitatory and inhibitory recruitment signalling to resource value and predation risk. Animal Behaviour. 2015;110:9–17.
- 31. Grüter C, Schürch R, Czaczkes TJ, Taylor K, Durance T, Jones SM, et al. Negative Feedback Enables Fast and Flexible Collective Decision-Making in Ants. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(9):e44501. pmid:22984518
- 32. Gordon DM. The rewards of restraint in the collective regulation of foraging by harvester ant colonies. Nature. 2013;498(7452):91–3. pmid:23676676
- 33. Franks NR, Richardson TO, Stroeymeyt N, Kirby RW, Amos WMD, Hogan PM, et al. Speed–cohesion trade-offs in collective decision making in ants and the concept of precision in animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour. 2013;85(6):1233–44.
- 34. Bell HC, Hsiung K, Pasberg P, Broccard FD, Nieh JC. Responsiveness to inhibitory signals changes as a function of colony size in honeybees (Apis mellifera). Journal of The Royal Society Interface. 2021;18(184). pmid:34753311
- 35. Zabzina N, Dussutour A, Mann RP, Sumpter DJT, Nicolis SC. Symmetry Restoring Bifurcation in Collective Decision-Making. PLoS computational biology. 2014;10(12):e1003960. pmid:25521109
- 36. Lestas I, Vinnicombe G, Paulsson J. Fundamental limits on the suppression of molecular fluctuations. Nature. 2010;467(7312):174–8. pmid:20829788
- 37. Howell AS, Jin M, Wu CF, Zyla TR, Elston TC, Lew DJ. Negative Feedback Enhances Robustness in the Yeast Polarity Establishment Circuit. Cell. 2012;149(2):322–33. pmid:22500799
- 38. Laurenti L, Csikasz-Nagy A, Kwiatkowska M, Cardelli L. Molecular Filters for Noise Reduction. Biophysical Journal. 2018;114(12):3000–11. pmid:29925035
- 39.
Motchenbacher CD, Connelly JA. Low noise electronic system design. Wiley; 1993.
- 40. Pan D, Ruan X, Bao C, Li W, Wang X. Capacitor-Current-Feedback Active Damping With Reduced Computation Delay for Improving Robustness of LCL-Type Grid-Connected Inverter. IEEE Transactions on Power Electronics. 2014;29(7):3414–27.
- 41. Dong S, Tan K, Zhang Q, Nieh JC. Playbacks of Asian honey bee stop signals demonstrate referential inhibitory communication. Animal Behaviour. 2019;148:29–37.
- 42. Dechaume-Moncharmont FX, Dornhaus A, Houston AI, McNamara JM, Collins EJ, Franks NR. The hidden cost of information in collective foraging. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2005;272(1573):1689–95. pmid:16087424
- 43. Grüter C, Leadbeater E. Insights from insects about adaptive social information use. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2014;29(3):177–84. pmid:24560544
- 44. Biesmeijer JC, Seeley TD. The use of waggle dance information by honey bees throughout their foraging careers. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2005;59(1):133–42.
- 45. Grüter C, Leadbeater E, Ratnieks FLW. Social Learning: The Importance of Copying Others. Current Biology. 2010;20(16):R683–5. pmid:20728057
- 46. Donaldson-Matasci MC, Dornhaus A. How habitat affects the benefits of communication in collectively foraging honey bees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2012;66(4):583–92. pmid:26213439
- 47. Czaczkes TJ, Grüter C, Ratnieks FLW. Negative feedback in ants: crowding results in less trail pheromone deposition. Journal of The Royal Society Interface. 2013;10(81):20121009. pmid:23365196
- 48. Wendt S, Kleinhoelting N, Czaczkes TJ. Negative feedback: ants choose unoccupied over occupied food sources and lay more pheromone to them. Journal of The Royal Society Interface. 2020;17(163):20190661. pmid:32093538
- 49. Frisch Kv, Jander R. Über den Schwänzeltanz der Bienen. Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie. 1957;40(3):239–63.
- 50. Hölldobler B, Stanton RC, Markl H. Recruitment and Food-Retrieving Behavior in Novomessor (Formicidae, Hymenoptera): I. Chemical Signals. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1978;4:163–81.
- 51. Nieh JC. The stop signal of honey bees: reconsidering its message. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1993;33(1):51–6.
- 52. Seeley TD. Consensus building during nest-site selection in honey bee swarms: the expiration of dissent. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2003;53(6):417–24.
- 53. Marshall JAR, Reina A, Bose T. Multiscale Modelling Tool: Mathematical Modelling of Collective Behaviour without the Maths. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(9):e0222906. pmid:31568526
- 54. Biancalani T, Dyson L, McKane AJ. Noise-Induced Bistable States and Their Mean Switching Time in Foraging Colonies. Physical Review Letters. 2014;112(3):038101. pmid:24484166
- 55. Khaluf Y, Pinciroli C, Valentini G, Hamann H. The impact of agent density on scalability in collective systems: noise-induced versus majority-based bistability. Swarm Intelligence. 2017;11(2):155–79.