Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Circadian rhythms are more resilient to pacemaker neuron disruption in female Drosophila

  • Aishwarya Ramakrishnan Iyer,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Biology, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America, Department of Neuroscience and Behavior, Barnard College, New York City, New York, United States of America

  • Eva Scholz-Carlson,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Biology, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America, Department of Neuroscience and Behavior, Barnard College, New York City, New York, United States of America

  • Evardra Bell,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization

    Affiliation Department of Neuroscience and Behavior, Barnard College, New York City, New York, United States of America

  • Grace Biondi,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Neuroscience and Behavior, Barnard College, New York City, New York, United States of America

  • Shlesha Richhariya,

    Roles Resources

    Affiliation HHMI, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Maria P. Fernandez

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    fernanm@iu.edu

    Affiliations Department of Biology, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, Indiana, United States of America, Department of Neuroscience and Behavior, Barnard College, New York City, New York, United States of America

Abstract

The circadian system regulates the timing of multiple molecular, physiological, metabolic, and behavioral phenomena. In Drosophila, as in other species, most of the research on how the timekeeping system in the brain controls the timing of behavioral outputs has been conducted in males, or sex has not been included as a biological variable. A critical set of circadian pacemaker neurons in Drosophila release the neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor (PDF), which functions as a key output factor in the network with complex effects on other clock neurons. Lack of Pdf or its receptor, PdfR, results in most flies displaying arrhythmicity in activity–rest cycles under constant conditions. However, our results show that female circadian rhythms are less affected by mutations in both Pdf and PdfR. CRISPR-Cas9-mediated mutagenesis of Pdf, specifically in ventral lateral neurons (LNvs), also has a greater effect on male rhythms. We tested the influence of M-cells on the circadian network and showed that speeding up the molecular clock specifically in M-cells led to sexually dimorphic phenotypes, with a more pronounced effect on male rhythmic behavior. Our results suggest that the female circadian system is more resilient to manipulations of M-cells and the PDF pathway, suggesting that circadian timekeeping is more distributed across the clock neuron network in females.

Introduction

Differences in neuronal circadian timekeeping between sexes remain relatively unexplored, despite the expanding body of research highlighting the influence of sex on the mechanisms underlying neuronal control of behavior [1]. In mammals, steroid hormones display daily, clock-driven changes in abundance, and while these sex hormones are not required to maintain rhythms, they differentially influence the amplitude of activity behavior between the sexes [1,2]. Furthermore, structural and functional sex differences have been observed in brain areas that receive direct input from the brain’s circadian timekeeping center [2,3]. Research in humans has also revealed significant sexual dimorphism: men tend to have lower-amplitude endogenous rhythms than women [4], are less resilient to nocturnal sleep disruptions, and spend less time asleep [5].

In mammals, the main circadian pacemaker resides in the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN), which in mice consist of a network of ~ 20,000 neurons (reviewed in [6]). The Drosophila circadian clock network has ~ 240 neurons and is the functional equivalent of the mammalian SCN [79] (reviewed in [10]). Each circadian clock neuron has an intracellular molecular timekeeping mechanism based on a transcriptional–translational feedback loop: the genes Clock (Clk) and cycle (cyc) promote rhythmic transcription of several key genes, including period (per) and timeless (tim), which build up daily and inhibit their own transcription [11]. Multiple kinases that act on components of these clock proteins and can affect the pace of the molecular clock have been identified. One such kinase is doubletime (DBT), which binds to and phosphorylates PER, regulating its nuclear accumulation [12,13].

The fly clock network consists of lateral neurons (LNs), which include ventrolateral (LNv), dorsolateral (LNd), and lateral posterior neurons (LPNs), as well as three groups of dorsal neurons (DN1, DN2, and DN3), some of which can be further subdivided [8,1417]. The ventral and dorsal LNs are sufficient to produce the normal endogenous bimodal rhythm of sleep and activity [18,19]. The small ventral lateral neurons (s-LNvs) are usually referred to as morning cells (M-cells) since they control the morning peak of activity under light–dark cycles (LDs). These cells are also essential for maintaining rhythmicity under free-running conditions [20,21]. The evening peak is controlled by the LNds and a Pdf-negative LNv, the 5th LNv (E-cells) [18,22,23]. Some dorsal neurons (DNs) also contribute to the timing and amount of sleep via the modulation of M and E cells [2427].

The release of the circadian neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor (PDF) by s-LNvs is essential for endogenous circadian timekeeping. A Pdf null mutation, Pdf01, results in a substantial fraction of arrhythmic flies [20], desynchronization of molecular oscillations [28,29], and phase changes in the electrical activity of some clock clusters, most notably the LNds [30]. Loss of PdfR also leads to loss of behavioral rhythms [3133]. Interestingly, PDF and PDFR also regulate behaviors that are sex-specific or sexually dimorphic. Rival-induced long mating durations require PDF expression in s-LNvs, PDFR expression in a subset of LNds, and NPF expression in LNds [34]. PDF controls rhythms in the sexually dimorphic pheromone profiles produced by oenocytes [35] and is involved in long-term mating suppression in males [36]. Both PDF and PDFR contribute to geotactic behaviors [31], and the phenotypes of Pdf01 mutants are sexually dimorphic, with males showing a more extreme negative geotaxis phenotype [37].

Sexual dimorphism in Drosophila sleep/wake cycles has been studied mostly under LD cycles. Males exhibit lower levels of activity and more sleep during the light phase [3840]. This increase in midday sleep is due to the activity of a subset of sleep-promoting DN1s, which are more active in males [38] and receive input from the male-specific P1 neurons that control male courtship [39]. Unlike studies on circadian rhythms, Drosophila sleep research often involves only females. Males also have an earlier and more pronounced morning peak and a larger phase angle between the morning peak and the evening peak [41]. Under conditions of constant darkness and temperature (DD), males of several wild-type strains have a small but significant reduction in the free-running period (FRP) relative to females of the same strain [41]. Moreover, males are more likely to retain a bimodal activity pattern in DD [41]. A recent transcriptomic analysis of fruitless (fru)-expressing neurons revealed clusters that are enriched for circadian clock genes [42]. A previous study reported that DN1s express the male-specific FruM protein [43] and that the number of cells in the DN1ps cluster is sexually dimorphic [44]. In addition, the E3 subset of LNds has been shown to be dimorphic in its expression of the neuropeptide NPF [45,46].

Given the sexually dimorphic roles of neuropeptides, including PDF, in other behaviors [47], we asked whether females were similarly affected by manipulations of the Pdf/PdfR pathway. We found that female circadian rhythms are less affected by null mutations in both Pdf and PdfR and that similar effects are observed via CRISPR-Cas9-mediated Pdf mutagenesis, specifically in the LNvs. Moreover, speeding up the molecular clock in the LNvs via expression of DBTs leads to an advance of the morning peak in males but not in females, and the pace of the FRP of activity is significantly shortened only in males. Taken together, our results show that the female circadian system is more resilient to manipulations in the PDF pathway and suggest that Pdf+ neurons play a more dominant role in the male than in the female circadian network.

Results

Mutations in PDF and PDFR lead to sexually dimorphic phenotypes

A null mutation in Pdf results in pronounced behavioral phenotypes in Drosophila males [20]. We assayed the locomotor activity rhythms of Pdf01 females under free-running conditions (DD) and found that a large proportion of the experimental females were still rhythmic (Fig 1A1C). The rhythmic power of experimental flies was significantly reduced in both sexes (Fig 1D), but the effect was less pronounced in females (Fig 1E), suggesting that Pdf01 females have more consolidated rhythms than Pdf01 males. We employed virgin females, as female rhythm strength has been shown to be significantly reduced after mating [48]. Mutant females that were rhythmic had a slightly, but significantly, shorter FRP than the controls (Table 1). This phenotype was not observed in experimental males (Table 1), consistent with a recent study [49]. Sleep cycles under DD also appeared to be more consolidated in females (Fig 1F). Pdf mutants have increased sleep, and this effect is mediated by PDF acting on the LNvs themselves [50]. We found that both sexes show an increase in total sleep in LD, but the effect was more pronounced in females (Fig 1G1I). While the increase in sleep in males was most prominent at midday, females exhibited increased sleep throughout most of the light phase (Fig 1G). We are employing a Pdf01 mutant in w1118 background, and neither the advanced evening peak nor the short FRP of the Pdf null mutant are consistently observed. The lack of a short FRP in rhythmic Pdf01 mutant flies in this genetic background is consistent with a previous study [49].

thumbnail
Table 1. Table representing the n, % rhythmicity, phase of the E peak, free-running period, and rhythmic power of w1118 and Pdf01 males and females.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.t001

thumbnail
Fig 1. Mutations in Pdf and PdfR lead to sexually dimorphic phenotypes.

(A) Representative double-plotted actograms of w1118 (left) and Pdf01 (right) male flies subjected to 6 days of LD followed by seven days of DD. (B) Representative actograms of w1118 (left) and Pdf01 (right) female flies subjected to six days of LD followed by seven days of DD. (C) Percentages of rhythmic flies are plotted for control (w1118) and Pdf01 males (n = 86 (w1118), n = 81 (Pdf01)) and females (right, n = 75 (w1118), n  = 101 (Pdf01)). The error bars represent the SEM values plotted across three replicate experiments. (D) Rhythmic power of control (w1118) and Pdf01 males and females were calculated using the Chi-squared Periodogram. (E) The differences in rhythmic power between experimental males and females and their respective controls are plotted. (F) Average sleep plots of flies over seven days in DD are plotted for male and female control (w1118) and experimental (Pdf01) flies. The controls are plotted in gray, and the experimental males and females are plotted in blue and magenta, respectively. (G) Average sleep plots under LD 12:12 for the control (w1118) and experimental (Pdf01) groups are plotted for males (left) and females (right). The plots are averaged over flies and days for a period of three days under LD 12:12. The controls are plotted in gray, and the experimental males and females are plotted in blue and magenta, respectively. (H) Total sleep values under LD conditions are plotted for male (left) and female (right) control (w1118) and experimental (Pdf01) flies. (I) The differences in total LD sleep values between experimental males and females and their respective controls are plotted. (J) Percentage of rhythmic flies are plotted for control (w1118) and han5304 males (n = 86 (w1118), n = 115 (han5304)) and females (n = 74 (w1118), n = 94 (han5304)) (K) Rhythmic power of the control (w1118) and han5304 males and females calculated using the Chi-squared periodogram are plotted. (L) The differences in rhythmic power between experimental males and females and their respective controls are plotted. (M) Average sleep plots under LD 12:12 for control (w1118) and experimental (han5304) flies are plotted for males (left) and females (right). The plots are averaged over flies and days for a period of three days under LD 12:12. The controls are plotted in gray, and the experimental males and females are plotted in blue and magenta, respectively. (N) Total sleep values under LD conditions are plotted for male (left) and female (right) control (w1118) and experimental (han5304) flies. (O) The differences in total LD sleep values between experimental males and females and their respective controls are plotted. Statistical comparisons were performed between the control and experimental flies of both sexes using a Mann–Whitney U test and percentage of rhythmic flies were compared using Fisher’s exact test. The box plots extend from the 25th to 75th percentile, with whiskers extending from the smallest to the largest value, and each point represents data from a single fly. Combined data from at least three replicate experiments are plotted. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.g001

To rule out the presence of remnant PDF expression in Pdf01 females, we stained the brains of control and experimental males and females with an anti-PDF antibody. We did not observe any traces of PDF in experimental flies of either sex, even with increased laser intensity (S1A Fig). PDF accumulates rhythmically in the dorsal termini of the s-LNv projections in a time-of-day-dependent manner both in LD and DD [20,21]. To determine if there were differences in the amplitude of PDF cycling between the sexes in a wild-type background (Canton-S), we dissected control males and females on the third day under DD at 6 time points over a 24-h cycle. Using a COSINOR-based curve fitting method [51], we found that both males and females have clear 24-h rhythms in PDF cycling in their dorsal projections, with no significant sex differences in amplitude (S1B and S1C Fig and Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Cosinor analysis parameters for Canton-S males and females.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.t002

Next, we asked whether the effects of a Pdf receptor mutation (PdfR) on activity and sleep were also sexually dimorphic. The expression of PDFR, a GPCR, can be detected in most clock neurons, with the exception of 3 LNds, half DN1ps, and some DN3s [52], which coincides with Cryptochrome expression in clock neurons [52]. The han5304 mutant is a PdfR hypomorph and exhibits Pdf01-like behavioral phenotypes under both LD 12:12 and DD [3133]. Under DD, both Han males and females showed a significant reduction in rhythmicity compared with the controls, but there was a greater proportion of rhythmic females (~65%) than males (~16%) (Fig 1J). The FRP of the experimental flies was significantly shorter for both sexes (Table 3), as reported previously for males. Rhythmic power was significantly lower than that of the controls for both han5304 males and females (Fig 1K), but the effect was more pronounced in males, suggesting that females have more consolidated rhythms (Fig 1L). Similar to the effect of the Pdf mutation, han5304 flies showed significantly higher levels of LD sleep than controls, and this effect was also more pronounced in females (Fig 1M1O). Taken together, these results suggest that female circadian rhythms are less affected by the loss of both Pdf and PdfR. However, the LD sleep phenotypes were more pronounced in females.

thumbnail
Table 3. Table representing the n, % rhythmicity, phase of the E-peak, free-running period, and rhythmic power of w1118 and PdfR01 males and females.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.t003

CRISPR-Cas9-mediated Pdf mutagenesis has more pronounced effects on male behavior

In the Pdf null mutant, background effects could contribute to the sexual dimorphism observed in behavioral rhythms. We therefore employed a tissue-specific CRISPR-Cas9-mediated knockout of Pdf in both males and females, as described in a recent study that focused on males [53]. To assess the efficiency of the manipulation, we stained for PDF in flies that constitutively expressed Pdf gRNA and Cas9 in Pdf+ neurons. This experiment was conducted at 28 °C, as this temperature was more effective at mimicking the behavioral phenotypes of the Pdf01 mutant males.

PDF was reduced in the s-LNvs in both sexes (Fig 2A2C), although in most experimental brains, we noted faint staining in the dorsal projections of at least one s-LNv in at least one brain hemisphere (Fig 2A). We quantified PDF intensity in the cell bodies of the s-LNvs and found that the signal intensity and the number of cells were reduced in both sexes in a similar manner (Fig 2B2E). PDF expression within the large LNvs was less affected and could be detected in 2–3 l-LNv cell bodies in most brains (Fig 2A). In addition to behavioral phenotypes, Pdf01 mutation leads to pronounced misrouting of s-LNv projections in male flies [54]. We employed a transgene expressing a red fluorescent protein under the Pdf regulatory sequence [55] and observed faint projections occasionally defasciculating from the main bundle in one or both hemispheres in Pdf > Pdf-g; Cas9 flies of both sexes (Fig 2A, middle panels). To determine whether driver strength was similar between males and females, we analyzed nuclear signal levels in the s-LNvs of male and female Pdf > nls-mCherry flies and found no significant sex differences (S1D Fig). However, subtle sex differences in driver strength may be undetectable due to the constitutively high expression levels of the Gal4/UAS system.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Tissue-specific CRISPR-mediated Pdf manipulation leads to a reduction in PDF levels and the misrouting of s-LNv dorsal termini in both sexes.

(A) Representative confocal images of control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts) (top) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) (middle, males; bottom, females) flies stained with RFP and PDF antibodies. Experimental flies show a significant reduction in PDF levels in the s-LNvs (white arrows, PDF channel) and misrouting of the s-LNv dorsal termini (white arrows, RFP channel). (B) (left) Representative confocal images of the small and large LNvs of control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts) (males, top) and (females, bottom), (right) Representative confocal images of the small and large LNvs of experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) (males, top) and (females, bottom) brains stained with RFP and PDF antibodies. (C) Quantification of PDF levels from s-LNv cell bodies in control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) flies are plotted for males (left) and females (right). n > 12 brains for all genotypes. (D) Differences in the PDF intensity values of experimental males and females from their respective parental controls. (E) Number of PDF-positive s-LNvs in each brain are plotted for control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) males and females. n > 13 brains for all genotypes. Statistical comparisons were performed between the control and experimental flies of both sexes using the Mann–Whitney U test. The box plots extend from the 25th to 75th percentile, with whiskers extending from the smallest to the largest value, and each point represents data from a single fly. Combined data from at least two replicate experiments are plotted. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Scale bars = 50 μm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.g002

We analyzed activity–rest rhythms in Pdf > Pdf-g; Cas9 flies and found that while experimental males were largely arrhythmic, the percentage of rhythmic experimental females was not different from the controls (Fig 3A and 3B). The FRP of the experimental males was not significantly different from that of the controls, but a wide range of periods were observed. Compared with parental controls, Pdf > Pdf-g; Cas9 females had significantly shorter FRPs (Fig 3C). The rhythmic power was significantly lower in experimental flies of both sexes (Fig 3D), but the effect was less pronounced in females (Fig 3E). Sleep under LD was not affected (S2A and S2B Fig), whereas sleep under DD was similarly increased in males and females, both for several days under DD and for DD1 only (S2CS2F Fig). We also calculated the activity/waking minute for control and experimental flies and found that there are no significant differences in the waking activity for experimental flies of both sexes (S2G Fig).

thumbnail
Fig 3. CRISPR-Cas9-mediated Pdf mutagenesis has more pronounced effects on male behavior.

(A) Representative actograms of control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts) and (UAS Cas9; Pdf-g) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) males (left) and females (right) are plotted for six days of LD followed by nine days of DD. (B) Percentage of rhythmic flies are plotted for control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts) and (UAS Cas9; Pdf-g) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) males (n = 85 (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts), n = 89 (UAS Cas9; Pdf-g), n = 106 (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g)) and females (n = 79 (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts), n = 78 (UAS Cas9; Pdf-g), n = 85 (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g)). The error bars represent SEM values plotted across three replicate experiments. (C) Free-running periods of control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts and UAS Cas9; Pdf-g) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) males (left) and females (right) calculated via the chi-squared periodogram are plotted. (D) Rhythmic power of control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts and UAS Cas9; Pdf-g) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) males (left) and females (right) calculated via the chi-squared periodogram are plotted. (E) The differences in rhythmic power of experimental males and females from their respective controls are plotted. Flies were kept at 28 °C throughout development, and as adults, experiments were conducted at 28 °C. Statistical comparisons were performed between the control and experimental flies of both sexes using the Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons test for panels B–D and Mann–Whitney U test for Fig 3E. Percentage of rhythmic flies were compared using Fisher’s exact test. The box plots extend from the 25th to 75th percentile, with whiskers extending from the smallest to the largest value, and each point represents data from a single fly. Combined data from at least three replicate experiments are plotted. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.g003

We next restricted the CRISPR mutagenesis of Pdf to the small LNvs via a specific driver from the split-Gal4 collection generated by the Rubin Laboratory (SS00681-Gal4). Pdf knockdown in the s-LNv resulted in most males being arrhythmic (~30% rhythmicity), whereas the experimental females were ~57% rhythmic (S3A and S3E Fig and S1 Table). The percentage of rhythmic flies was significantly lower than that of both controls for both experimental males and females, but females were significantly more rhythmic (S3A Fig). The FRP of the experimental males and females was shorter than that of their respective control flies (S3B Fig). The rhythmic power of experimental males and females was lower than that of their respective controls (S3C Fig), but the effect was less pronounced in females (S3D Fig). This suggests that PDF from the s-LNv is important for the behavioral and sex-specific differences observed in Pdf01 mutants.

Speeding up the M-cell clock results in a more effective period shortening in males

Next, we sought to determine if the influence of the Pdf-releasing cells themselves was sexually dimorphic. While PDF is released from both large and small LNvs, only s-LNvs (Morning cells) play key roles in regulating free-running rhythm properties [18]. In males, manipulations that change the pace of the clock specifically in the LNvs result in changes in the phase of the morning peak of activity and in FRP [22,56]. We expressed the doubletime ‘short’ (DBTs) allele [57] under the Pdf-Gal4 driver (Fig 4A) and analyzed the effects on behavior in both sexes. We found that Pdf > DBTs males, but not females, have an advanced phase of the morning peak of activity (M-peak, Fig 4B and 4C). This suggests that under LD cycles, the M-oscillator is more effective at setting the phase of male than female behavior.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Speeding up the clock in M-cells leads to sexually dimorphic phenotypes.

(A) Depiction of the adult Drosophila brain hemisphere indicating the clock cell subsets (colored) having a faster running molecular clock. (B) Average activity plots of control (Pdf-Gal4) and (UAS-DBTs) and experimental (Pdf > DBTs) flies are plotted for males (left) and females (right). The plots are averaged over flies and days for a period of three days under LD 12:12. (C) The phase of the morning peak of activity under LD 12:12 is plotted for control (Pdf-Gal4), (UAS-DBTs) and experimental (Pdf > DBTs) males (left, n = 116 (Pdf-Gal4), n = 120 (UAS-DBTs), n = 88 (Pdf > DBTs)) and females (right, n = 85 (Pdf-Gal4), n = 109 (UAS-DBTs), n = 73 (Pdf > DBTs)). (D) Percentages of rhythmic flies are plotted for controls (Pdf-Gal4), (UAS-DBTs), and experimental (Pdf > DBTs) males (n = 90 (Pdf-Gal4), n = 108 (UAS-DBTs), n = 93 (Pdf >DBTs) and females (n = 66 (Pdf-Gal4), n = 83 (UAS-DBTs), n = 70 (Pdf> DBTs). The error bars represent the SEM values plotted across three replicate experiments. (E) Representative actograms of controls (Pdf-Gal4 and UAS-DBTs) and experimental (Pdf > DBTs) females (top) and males (bottom) plotted for five days of LD followed by 10 days of DD. (F) Free-running periods of control (Pdf-Gal4), (UAS-DBTs) and experimental (Pdf> DBTs) males (left) and females (right) calculated via the chi-squared periodogram are plotted. (G) Percentage of flies having a short (18–21.5 h) and normal (23–25 h) free-running periods are plotted for experimental (Pdf > dBTs) males and females (H) Rhythmic power of control (Pdf-Gal4), (UAS-DBTs) and experimental (Pdf> DBTs) males (left) and females (right) calculated using the Chi-squared periodogram are plotted. (I) The differences in rhythmic power between experimental males and females and their respective controls are plotted. Statistical comparisons were performed between the control and experimental flies of both sexes using the Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons test for panels C, F, and H and Mann–Whitney U test for Fig 4I. Percentage of rhythmic flies were compared using Fisher’s exact test. The box plots extend from the 25th to 75th percentile, with whiskers extending from the smallest to the largest value, and each point represents data from a single fly. Combined data from at least three replicate experiments are plotted. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.g004

Under free-running conditions, both Pdf > DBTs male and female flies showed a significantly lower percentage of rhythmic flies than their controls (Fig 4D), but there were fewer rhythmic females (~40%) than males (~65%) (Fig 4D and 4E). The FRP of most Pdf > DBTs males was ~ 18.5 h (Fig 4F), whereas the Pdf > DBTs females showed a large proportion of individuals with a period of ~ 24 h, reflecting the pace of the molecular clock in the rest of the clock network (Figs 4F, 4G, and S4H). Some Pdf > DBTs males (~23%) and a smaller proportion of females (~6%) also presented complex rhythms (Table 4). The average period value of the second-period component (which has a lower power value) was ~ 24.07 ± 0.4 for the experimental males and 19.7 ± 0.4 for the experimental females. Neither male nor female control flies exhibited complex rhythms (Table 4). Among rhythmic flies, both Pdf > DBTs males and females had lower rhythmic power than the controls (Fig 4H), with no difference between the sexes (Fig 4I). As a control, we expressed DBTs via Clk856-Gal4 which is expressed in most clock neurons (S4A Fig). Speeding up the molecular clock in most clock neurons significantly advanced both the morning and evening activity peaks of both males and females (S4BS4D Fig and S2 Table). There were no significant differences between experimental males and females in the percentage of rhythmic flies, rhythmic power, or shortening of FRPs following DBT expression in all clock neurons (S4ES4G Fig). We also compared per01 mutant males and females, and our results show that both are nearly completely arrhythmic (S4I Fig).

thumbnail
Table 4. Table representing the % rhythmicity, % complex rhythms, free-running period, and rhythmic power of control and Pdf > DBTs males and females.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.t004

These results support the notion that M-cells are more dominant in the male than in the female circadian network. Previous studies have shown that blocking synaptic neurotransmission by expressing the tetanus toxin light chain (TeTxLC) in small and large LNvs affects male activity rhythms, likely in a Pdf-independent manner [5860]. We analyzed male and female Pdf > TeTxLC flies and found that neither sex significantly changed the ability to maintain rhythmicity under free-running conditions (S5A and S5B Fig, and S3 Table). Both sexes significantly lengthened the FRP, but the effect was more pronounced in males (S5C and S5D Fig). Rhythmic power was not affected in experimental flies of either sex (S5E Fig). These results are consistent with previous studies which show that TeTxLC expression in Pdf+ neurons lead to behavioral phenotypes that are different from those of loss of Pdf, and suggest that manipulations of neuronal activity of the Pdf-expressing neurons have a more pronounced effect in male FRP.

We next asked whether changing the pace of the clock in LNds (E-cells) via DBTS expression also had sexually dimorphic effects on behavior. These cells can be subdivided into at least three different clusters on the basis of their anatomy [8], physiology [56], transcriptomic profiles [15], and connectivity patterns [17]. The PDFR-expressing E1 and E2 clusters have been shown to regulate evening activity under LD [61,62] and to be able to maintain free-running activity rhythms in the absence of a functional clock in M cells [63,64], whereas the behavioral role of the E3 cluster remains unknown. We used the MB122-B split-gal4 driver to target the E1 and E2 subsets (Fig 5A) and found that while expressing DBTS in this group of evening cells significantly advanced the phase of the E-peak in experimental flies of both sexes (Fig 5B and 5C), the effect was more pronounced in females (Fig 5D). Speeding up of clocks in the E1 + E2 LNds did not significantly alter the FRP or rhythmic power of experimental flies of either sex (Fig 5E and 5F). M-cells have been shown to be the dominant oscillators in DD and to regulate rhythm properties such as persistence and the FRP of endogenous locomotor rhythms to a large extent [18,22,23,56], although manipulations of other clock cells can affect rhythm properties to some extent [56,65]. Thus, speeding up the clock in the PDFR+ E1 and E2 clusters leads to similar behavioral phenotypes in males and females under free-running conditions. Taken together, these results indicate that the relative influence of the M and E subsets of clock neurons are sexually dimorphic.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Speeding up the clock in an E-cell subset leads to a more advanced evening peak in females.

(A) Depiction of the adult Drosophila brain hemisphere indicating the clock cell subsets (colored) having a faster running molecular clock. (B) Average activity plots of control (MB122B-Gal4) and (UAS-DBTs) and experimental (MB122B > DBTs) flies are plotted for males (left) and females (right). The plots are averaged over flies and days for a period of three days under LD 12:12. (C) Phase of the evening peak of activity under LD 12:12 for controls (MB122B-Gal4) and (UAS-DBTs) and experimental (MB122B > DBTs) males (left, n = 92 (MB122B-Gal4), n = 93 (UAS-DBTs), n = 93 (MB122B > DBTs)) and females (right, n = 85 (MB122B-Gal4), n = 87 (UAS-DBTs), n = 89 (MB122B > DBTs)). (D) The differences in the phase of the E peak between experimental males and females and their respective controls are plotted. (E) Free-running period of control (MB122B-Gal4 and UAS-DBTs) and experimental (MB122B > DBTs) males (left) and females (right) calculated via the chi-squared periodogram are plotted. (F) Rhythmic power of control (MB122B-Gal4 and UAS-DBTs) and experimental (MB122B> DBTs) males (left) and females (right) calculated via the chi-squared periodogram are plotted. Statistical comparisons were performed between the control and experimental flies of both sexes using the Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons test for panels 5C, 5E, and 5F and Mann–Whitney U test for Fig 5D. The box plots extend from the 25th to 75th percentile, with whiskers extending from the smallest to the largest value, and each point represents data from a single fly. Combined data from at least three replicate experiments are plotted. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.g005

Discussion

The critical importance of considering sex as a biological variable has gained increasing recognition in biomedical research [66,67]. Bias toward male subjects is particularly prevalent in neuroscience, with single-sex studies using male animals outnumbering those using female animals at a ratio of 5.5:1 [68]. This disparity extends to chronobiology, resulting in a limited understanding of how sex affects circadian organization in the nervous system. However, work from several laboratories has revealed sexual dimorphism within the SCN and in its input and output pathways [1,69]. Sex differences in SCN morphology have been described in both animal models and humans, and sex differences in SCN electrical activity and steroid hormone receptors have also been reported (reviewed in [2]). Notably, sex differences in the number of SCN neurons that express the neuropeptide vasoactive intestinal polypeptide (VIP) and in Vip mRNA expression have been reported (reviewed in [2]). The roles of the mammalian VIP and the Drosophila PDF in circadian physiology are highly similar, although neither these peptides nor their receptors are sequence orthologs [70].

Several studies have shown the importance of PDF in generating coherent rhythms of ~ 24-h periodicity. Here, we report that females lacking Pdf or its receptor PdfR are more likely to maintain consolidated activity–rest behavior than males. This could be because of sex differences in PDF signaling mechanisms, PDFR expression, or the influence of other clock neurons within the network. In males, other neuropeptides are known to act in concert with PDF to maintain consolidated rhythms in the network, although none of them have as profound an effect as PDF in regulating activity-rest rhythms in DD [49,71]. Single mutants of DH31 and CCHamide1 do not affect activity rhythms by themselves, but the double mutants of these neuropeptides along with Pdf01 (DH3101/Pdf01 and Pdf01/CCHaSK8) are almost completely arrhythmic, suggesting that these neuropeptides act hierarchically in the network, with PDF being at the top of that hierarchy [49,71]. The importance of PDF relative to other peptides released by clock neurons may also be sexually dimorphic.

Although CRISPR manipulation was only partially effective at eliminating PDF expression, it nevertheless produced phenotypes reminiscent of those produced by the Pdf01 mutation. We observed faint staining in the dorsal projections of at least one s-LNv in at least one hemisphere in most brains, and PDF staining in a single s-LNv projection reaching the dorsal brain has been shown to be sufficient for behavioral rhythms [72]. Experimental flies in which Pdf was knocked out starting at the onset of promoter expression early in development showed extensive misrouting of their dorsal termini, similar to what has been reported for Pdf01 males [54]. Instances of s-LNv misrouting have also been observed in other core clock mutants, such as per01 and tim01 [73] and cyc01 [74]. No correlation between misrouting and behavioral phenotypes was found by others for Pdf01 males [54]. Importantly, Pdf > Pdfg;Cas9 manipulation recapitulates the sexually dimorphic circadian phenotypes of Pdf01 mutants: a larger fraction of females are rhythmic, and females exhibit greater rhythm power.

In males, changing the speed of the M cell clock leads to phase changes in the morning peak under LD [61]. To determine whether M-cell manipulations also have sexually dimorphic effects on behavior, we sped up the molecular clock by expressing the doubletime short allele (DBTs). Surprisingly, our results revealed that speeding up the clock of M-cells advances the phase of the morning peak in males, whereas the female morning peak phase is not affected. These results support previous studies conducted in males on the role of M-cells in regulating the morning peak of activity [18,23,61] and suggest that M-cells are unable to regulate the phase of morning activity in the same way in females. In DD, males had largely coherent short-term rhythms, and the majority (65%) were rhythmic. In contrast, only 40% of the females were rhythmic, and their period showed a bimodal distribution. These findings further support the notion that M-cells are less dominant than the circadian clock network in females. A possible explanation for this is that other clock neurons are able to “resist” their influence, and the conflict between the fast-paced M cell clock and the ~ 24 clock other clock neurons is what leads to greater arrhythmicity in females.

The expression of a TeTxLC construct in flies blocks neurotransmission by binding and cleaving the synaptic protein Synaptobrevin [75]. Expressing tetanus toxin in LNvs did not result in a reduction in rhythmic power, but it lengthened the male FRP, as reported in previous studies [59,60,76]. The behavioral phenotypes resulting from the blockade of synaptic transmission differ from those resulting from the loss of PDF [20] or the ablation of Pdf-expressing neurons [23], possibly because tetanus toxin affects classical transmission and not the dense core vesicle-mediated release of neuropeptides such as PDF [77]. Abrogating the dorsal termini of the small LNvs, where most of the output synapses are found [17,77], does not result in behavioral phenotypes similar to those of Pdf01 under either LD or DD [24]. Our results show that blocking synaptobrevin-dependent synaptic transmission in M-cells does not affect rhythmicity but rather lengthens the FRP. The period lengthening is more pronounced in males, supporting the notion that M-cells have a greater influence on the circadian network in males.

Cryptochrome and PdfR-expressing clusters of evening cells—the sNPF-expressing E1 cluster and the ITP-expressing E2 cluster [78]—have roles in setting the phase of the E-peak under LD and sustaining behavioral rhythms in the absence of a functional molecular clock in M-cells [63,64]. To test whether these cells have a differential influence on the network in males and females, we expressed DBTS under a driver that is expressed specifically in the E1 and E2 subsets of LNds. Our results showed that speeding up the clocks in the E1 + E2 clusters resulted in a phase advance in the evening peak of activity in both sexes, but the effect was more pronounced in females. A possible reason for the behavioral differences observed between the sexes could be redundancy in females, such that the network is not as dependent on PDF or M-cells for timekeeping. This finding suggests that the female network could have a more distributed mode of timekeeping throughout the circadian clock network.

Across species, sex differences in the circadian timing system are largely related to the regulation of reproduction-related behaviors. In mammals, the SCN determines the timing of the release of reproductive hormones and influences the timing of mating (reviewed in [69]) and aggression [79]. In Drosophila, the circadian clock controls the timing of sex-specific and sexually dimorphic behaviors, such as male courtship [80] and female sexual receptivity [81] and egg laying [82]. This regulation of rhythmic behaviors requires connectivity between clock neurons and downstream sex-specific circuits. For example, the DN1p cluster, which has been shown to be more active in males [38], is functionally connected to the male-specific fru-expressing P1 neurons that regulate male courtship [39]. In females, Allatostatin C-producing DN1ps have been shown to connect to downstream targets to control rhythms in oogenesis [83], and the Janelia female hemibrain connectome revealed that the LNds form connections with the doublesex-expressing PC1 cluster [17]. Our data suggest that the relative hierarchy of circadian oscillators is sexually dimorphic, with a less dominant M oscillator in females. Interestingly, when males are exposed to constant light (LL), the prevalence of the morning oscillator decreases, while the evening oscillator becomes more dominant [8486]. One proposed mechanism for this shift in the hierarchy of circadian oscillators involves GW182, which influences the circadian neural network’s response to light and modulates the level of PDFR signaling [87]. If the evening oscillator is more dominant in the female circadian network, it is possible that males and females respond differently to constant light, at least under specific light intensities. The existence of sex differences in the hierarchy of circadian oscillators may serve an adaptive purpose, ensuring the precise timing of essential female-specific behaviors crucial for reproductive fitness, such as sexual receptivity and egg laying.

Materials and methods

Fly lines and rearing

All the genotypes were reared on corn syrup soy media (Archon Scientific; Durham, NC) under LD 12:12 cycles at 25 °C unless specified otherwise (see figure legends for details). The fly lines used in this study were Canton-S, w1118, Pdf01, PdfR01, Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts, UAS-Cas9; UAS-Pdfg, Pdf-Gal4, UAS-DBTs, UAS TeTxLC, Clk856Gal4, s-LNvGal4, and MB122B-Gal4. See the fly lines and reagents table below for more details. All experiments were conducted with virgin females, as mating affects female rhythm strength [48]. We employed a Pdf01 mutant line outcrossed in the w1118 background. See Table 5 for details about fly lines.

Activity recording and analysis

Individual male and virgin female flies (3–5 days old) were housed in glass locomotor tubes containing 2% agar–4% sucrose food on one end and yarn on the other end. Locomotor activity was recorded using Drosophila activity monitors (DAM, Trikinetics, Waltham, United States of America). The experiments were conducted in Tritech or Percival incubators under controlled light and temperature conditions. Flies were entrained to 12:12 LD cycles for at least 5 days and then transferred to constant darkness (DD) for at least 7 days at a constant temperature of 25 °C unless otherwise specified (see figure legends for details). The raw data obtained from the DAM system were scanned and binned into activity counts of 15-min intervals via the DAM File scan. The data were analyzed via the CLOCKLAB software (Actimetrics, Wilmette, IL).

The values of period and rhythmic power were calculated for a period of 7 days via a chi-squared periodogram with a cutoff of p = 0.01. The rhythmic power for each designated rhythmic fly was determined by subtracting the chi-squared significance value from the power of the periodogram. Flies that did not exhibit a periodicity peak above the significance threshold were categorized as “arrhythmic,” and their period and rhythmic power were not included in the analysis. The values of the morning and evening peaks were calculated via PHASE software [88]. The total LD sleep values for all the genotypes were calculated for a period of 3 days (LD days 2–4) via the PHASE software. Representative actograms were generated via ClockLab, and activity plots were generated via PHASE. The period, rhythmic power, total sleep, and phase values of all the flies for a particular experimental genotype were compared against the background or parental controls via either the Mann–Whitney test or the Kruskal–Wallis ANOVA followed by the Dunn’s multiple comparisons test. The details of the statistical comparisons and the number of flies used in a given experiment are indicated in their respective figure legends. The number of rhythmic flies of the experimental genotype was compared against their respective background or parental controls via Fisher’s exact test. All the statistical analyses were performed via GraphPad Prism 9.0.

If both the experimental males and females were significantly different from their respective control flies of the same sex, the extent of sex differences were calculated by subtracting the average values of the control from each individual experimental value. These differences were then directly compared between males and females using the appropriate statistical analysis (the test used in each case is mentioned in the figure legends for the respective figures). In case of experiments with two parental controls, the average value to calculate the difference would be the average of the Gal4 and UAS control genotypes.

Immunohistochemistry

The brains of adult male or female flies were dissected in ice-cold Schneider’s insect media (S2) and fixed immediately after dissection in 2% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in S2 media for 30 min at room temperature. The fixed brains were washed (three washes of 10 min each) with 0.3% phosphate-buffered saline-Triton X 100 (PBS-TX) and then treated with blocking solution (5% normal goat serum made in 0.3% PBS-TX) for 1 h at room temperature. The brains were then incubated with primary antibodies at 4 °C for 24 h. The primary antibodies used were anti-PDF (mouse, 1:3000, C7, DSHB) and anti-RFP (rabbit, 1:2,000, Rockland Immunochemicals). After incubation, the brains were subjected to six washes with 0.3% PBS-TX and incubated with Alexa Fluor-conjugated secondary antibodies overnight at 4 °C. The following secondary antibodies were used: goat anti-mouse 488 (1:3,000, Invitrogen) and goat anti-rabbit 568 (1:3,000, Invitrogen). After incubation, the brain samples were washed six times with 0.3% PBS-TX, cleaned and mounted on a clean glass slide using Vectashield mounting media.

Image acquisition and analysis

The brains were imaged via a confocal microscope (Olympus FV3000) with an Olympus UPLanXApo 20× or 40× objective. Image analysis was performed via Fiji software [89]. In the samples, small and large LNvs were classified on the basis of their anatomical locations and expression of the PDF. PDF intensities in these cells were measured by selecting the slice of the Z-stack that showed the maximum intensity, drawing a region of interest (ROI) around the cells, and measuring their intensities. Three to four separate background values were also measured around each cell, and the final intensity was taken as the difference between the cell intensity and the average background.

For quantification of the PDF in the dorsal projections, a rectangular box was drawn as the ROI starting from the point where the PDF projection turns into the dorsal brain, and the intensity is measured. Three to four background values were also measured around the projection. The intensity values obtained from both hemispheres for each cell type for each brain were averaged and used for statistical analysis. PDF intensity from the s-LNv was compared between the experimental and control genotypes via a Mann–Whitney test. To estimate different aspects of rhythmicity in PDF oscillations in the dorsal termini of s-LNv in males and females, we used a COSINOR-based curve-fitting method [Cornelissen, 2014]. COSINOR analysis was implemented via the CAT Cosinor function from the CATkit package written for R [90].

Supporting information

S1 Fig. PDF rhythmic accumulation is similar in males and females (related to Fig 1).

(A) Representative confocal images of control (w1118) (top) and experimental (Pdf01) (bottom) male and female flies stained with the PDF antibody. (B) Scatterplots of PDF staining intensities of the s-LNv dorsal projections of both male and female flies plotted at different time points over a 24-h cycle on DD day 3. Each dot represents the mean PDF intensity value averaged over both hemispheres of one brain. The cyan and pink lines are the best-fit cosine curves from the parameters that were extracted via COSINOR analysis. See Table 2 for more details. (C) Amplitude values of PDF oscillation obtained from COSINOR curve fits are plotted for male and female flies. The error bars are 95% CI values calculated from the standard error obtained from COSINOR analysis. The overlapping error bars indicate that the amplitude values of males and females are not significantly different. n > 7 brain samples/time point. (D) Nuclear RFP intensity values are plotted for the sLNvs of Pdf > nls-mCherry males (n = 14 brains) and females (n = 10 brains).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.s001

(EPS)

S2 Fig. CRISPR-Cas9-mediated Pdf mutagenesis has similar effects on male and female sleep (Related to Fig 3).

(A) Average sleep plots under LD 12:12 of control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts and UAS Cas9; Pdf-g) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) males (left) and females (right) are plotted. The plots are averaged over flies and days for a period of 3 days under LD 12:12. The Gal4 and UAS controls are light and dark gray traces, and the experimental males and females are blue and magenta traces respectively. (B) Total sleep values under LD conditions are plotted for control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts and UAS Cas9; Pdf-g) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) males (left) and females (right). (C) Average sleep plots of flies over eight days in DD are plotted for male and female control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts and UAS Cas9; Pdf-g) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) flies. The Gal4 and UAS controls are light and dark gray traces, and the experimental males and females are blue and magenta traces respectively. (D) Total sleep values under DD 1–8 are plotted for control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts and UAS Cas9; Pdf-g) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) males (left) and females (right). (E) The differences in the total sleep values of experimental males and females from their respective controls are plotted for DD 1–8. (F) Total sleep values for DD day1 are plotted for control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts and UAS Cas9; Pdf-g) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) males (left) and females (right). (G) Activity/waking minute for DD day1 are plotted for control (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts and UAS Cas9; Pdf-g) and experimental (Pdf-RFP, Pdf-Gal4; tub-Gal80ts > Cas9; Pdf-g) males (left) and females (right). Statistical comparisons were performed between the control and experimental flies of both sexes using the Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons test for panels S2B and S2D and Mann–Whitney U test for Panel S2E. The box plots extend from the 25th to 75th percentile, with whiskers extending from the smallest to the largest value, and each point represents data from a single fly. Combined data from at least three replicate experiments are plotted. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.s002

(EPS)

S3 Fig. CRISPR-Cas9-mediated Pdf mutagenesis in small ventral lateral neurons has more pronounced effects on male circadian behavior.

(A) Percentage of rhythmic flies are plotted for control (s-LNv-Gal4) and (UAS Cas9; Pdf--g) and experimental (s-LNv > Cas9; Pdf--g) males (n = 72 (s-LNv-Gal4), n = 62 (UAS Cas9; Pdfg), n = 73 (s-LNv > Cas9; Pdfg)) and females (right, n = 61 (s-LNv-Gal4), n = 56 (UAS Cas9; Pdfg), n = 73 (s-LNv > Cas9; Pdfg)) (B) Free-running period of control (s-LNv-Gal4 and UAS Cas9; Pdf-g) and experimental (s-LNv> Cas9; Pdf-g) males and females calculated via the Chi-squared periodogram are plotted. (C) Rhythmic power of control (s-LNv-Gal4 and UAS Cas9; Pdf-g) and experimental (s-LNv > Cas9; Pdf-g) males (left) and females (right) calculated via the chi-squared periodogram are plotted. (D) The differences in rhythmic power between experimental males and females and their respective controls are plotted. (E) Representative actograms of control (s-LNv-Gal4) and (UAS Cas9; Pdfg) and experimental (s-LNv > Cas9; Pdfg) males (left) and females (right) are plotted for 5 days of LD followed by 10 days of DD. Statistical comparisons were performed between the control and experimental flies of both sexes using the Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons test. Percentage of rhythmic flies were compared using Fisher’s exact test. The box plots extend from the 25th to 75th percentile, with whiskers extending from the smallest to the largest value, and each point represents data from a single fly. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.s003

(EPS)

S4 Fig. Speeding up the clock in all clock cells leads to a phase advance and shortening of the free-running period of activity rhythms (related to Figs 4 and 5).

(A) Diagram of an adult Drosophila brain hemisphere indicating the clock cell subsets (blue) expressing DBTs. (B) Average activity plots of control (Clk856-Gal4) and (UAS-DBTs) and experimental (Clk856 > DBTs) flies are plotted for males (left) and females (right). The plots are averaged over flies and days for a period of three days under LD 12:12. (C) Phases of the morning peak of activity under LD 12:12 for controls (Clk856-Gal4) and (UAS-DBTs) and experimental (Clk856 > DBTs) males (left, n = 62 (Clk856-Gal4), n = 57 (UAS--DBTs), n = 60 (Clk856 > DBTs)) and females (right, n = 59 (Clk856-Gal4), n = 59 (UAS--DBTs), n = 50 (Clk856 > DBTs)) are plotted. (D) Phase of the evening peak of activity under LD 12:12 for controls (Clk856--Gal4) and (UAS-DBTs) and experimental (Clk856 > DBTs) males (left) and females (right) are plotted. (E) Percentage of rhythmic flies are plotted for control (Clk856--Gal4) and (UAS-DBTs) and experimental (Clk856 > DBTs) males (left) and females (right). The error bars represent the SEM values plotted across two replicate experiments. (F) Free-running periods of rhythmic flies calculated via the chi-squared period are plotted for controls (Clk856--Gal4) and (UAS-DBTs) and experimental (Clk856 > DBTs) males (left) and females (right). (G) Rhythmic power of flies calculated via the Chi-squared periodogram is plotted for control (Clk856--Gal4) and (UAS-DBTs) and experimental (Clk856 > DBTs) males (left) and females (right). (H) Representative actograms of Pdf > dBTs females showing free-running period values close to 24 h. (I) Percentage of rhythmic flies are plotted for 10 days in DD for control (Canton-S) and Per01 males and females. Statistical comparisons were performed between the control and experimental flies of both sexes via Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons test. Percentage of rhythmic flies were compared using Fisher’s exact test. The box plots extend from the 25th to 75th percentile, with whiskers extending from the smallest to the largest value, and each point represents data from a single fly. Combined data from at least two replicate experiments are plotted. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.s004

(EPS)

S5 Fig. Blocking neurotransmission in Pdf-expressing cells leads to more pronounced lengthening of the free-running period in males (Related to Fig 4).

(A) Representative actograms of control (Pdf-Gal4) and (UAS TeTxLC) and experimental (Pdf > TeTxLC) males (left) and females (right) are plotted for five days of LD followed by eight days of DD. (B) Percentage of rhythmic flies are plotted for control (Pdf-Gal4), and (UAS TeTxLC), and experimental (Pdf > TeTxLC) males (left, n = 56 (Pdf-Gal4), n = 51 (UAS TeTxLC), n = 52 (Pdf > TeTxLC)) and females (right, n = 53 (Pdf-Gal4), n = 52 (UAS TeTxLC), n = 52 (Pdf > TeTxLC)). Error bars are SEM values plotted across two replicate experiments. (C) Free-running period of control (Pdf-Gal4 and UAS TeTxLC) and experimental (Pdf > TeTxLC) males (left) and females (right) calculated using the Chi-squared Periodogram are plotted. (D) The difference in free-running period of experimental males and females from their respective controls are plotted. (E) Rhythmic power of control (Pdf-Gal4 and UAS TeTxLC) and experimental (Pdf > TeTxLC) males (left) and females (right) calculated using the Chi-squared periodogram are plotted. Statistical comparisons were performed between the control and experimental flies for both sexes using the Kruskal–Wallis test followed by the Dunn’s multiple comparisons test for all panels except S6D where comparisons were made using the Mann–Whitney U test. Percentage of rhythmic flies were compared using Fisher’s exact test. The box plots extend from the 25th to 75th percentile, with whiskers extending from the smallest to the largest value, and each point representing data from a single fly. Combined data from at least two replicate experiments are plotted.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.s005

(EPS)

S1 Table. Table representing the n, % rhythmicity, free-running period, and rhythmic power of control and s-LNv > Cas9; Pdfg males and females.

* indicates that the experimental genotypes are significantly different from their respective control flies of the same sex. # indicates that experimental males and females are significantly different from each other. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.s006

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Table representing the n, % rhythmicity, free-running period, and rhythmic power of control and Clk856 > dBTs males and females.

* indicates that the experimental genotypes are significantly different from their respective control flies of the same sex. # indicates that experimental males and females are significantly different from each other. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.s007

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Table representing the n, % rhythmicity, free-running period, and rhythmic power of control and Pdf > TeTxLC males and females.

* indicates that the experimental genotypes are significantly different from their respective control flies of the same sex. # indicates that experimental males and females are significantly different from each other. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003146.s008

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Charlotte Helfrich-Forster, Abhilash Lakshman, Cahir O’Kane, Orie Shafer, Jeff Price, and Paul Taghert for helpful discussions and Orie Shafer and members of the Fernandez Lab for helpful comments on the manuscript. We also thank Justin Blau, Aljoscha Nern, Gerry Rubin, Michael Rosbash, and Paul Taghert for sharing fly lines. The mouse anti-PDF antibody was obtained from the Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, created by the NICHD of the NIH and maintained at The University of Iowa, Department of Biology, Iowa City, IA 52242. Stocks obtained from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (NIH P40OD018537) were used in this study. We used FlyBase (release FB2025_01) to find information on phenotypes, stocks, and gene expression.

References

  1. 1. Anderson ST, FitzGerald GA. Sexual dimorphism in body clocks. Science. 2020;369(6508):1164–5. pmid:32883849
  2. 2. Bailey M, Silver R. Sex differences in circadian timing systems: implications for disease. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2014;35(1):111–39. pmid:24287074
  3. 3. Vida B, Hrabovszky E, Kalamatianos T, Coen CW, Liposits Z, Kalló I. Oestrogen receptor alpha and beta immunoreactive cells in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of mice: distribution, sex differences and regulation by gonadal hormones. J Neuroendocrinol. 2008;20(11):1270–7. pmid:18752649
  4. 4. Lyall LM, Wyse CA, Graham N, Ferguson A, Lyall DM, Cullen B, et al. Association of disrupted circadian rhythmicity with mood disorders, subjective wellbeing, and cognitive function: a cross-sectional study of 91 105 participants from the UK Biobank. Lancet Psychiatry. 2018;5(6):507–14. pmid:29776774
  5. 5. Bixler EO, Papaliaga MN, Vgontzas AN, Lin H-M, Pejovic S, Karataraki M, et al. Women sleep objectively better than men and the sleep of young women is more resilient to external stressors: effects of age and menopause. J Sleep Res. 2009;18(2):221–8. pmid:19302341
  6. 6. Hastings MH, Maywood ES, Brancaccio M. The mammalian circadian timing system and the suprachiasmatic nucleus as its pacemaker. Biology (Basel). 2019;8(1):13. pmid:30862123
  7. 7. Helfrich-Förster C. The neuroarchitecture of the circadian clock in the brain of Drosophila melanogaster. Microsc Res Tech. 2003;62(2):94–102. pmid:12966496
  8. 8. Schubert FK, Hagedorn N, Yoshii T, Helfrich-Förster C, Rieger D. Neuroanatomical details of the lateral neurons of Drosophila melanogaster support their functional role in the circadian system. J Comp Neurol. 2018;526(7):1209–31. pmid:29424420
  9. 9. Reinhard N, Fukuda A, Manoli G, Derksen E, Saito A, Möller G, et al. Synaptic connectome of the Drosophila circadian clock. Nat Commun. 2024;15(1):10392. pmid:39638801
  10. 10. Helfrich-Förster C, Reinhard N. Mutual coupling of neurons in the circadian master clock: what we can learn from fruit flies. Neurobiol Sleep Circadian Rhythms. 2025;18:100112. pmid:39906412
  11. 11. Yu W, Hardin PE. Circadian oscillators of Drosophila and mammals. J Cell Sci. 2006;119(Pt 23):4793–5. pmid:17130292
  12. 12. Kloss B, Price JL, Saez L, Blau J, Rothenfluh A, Wesley CS, et al. The Drosophila clock gene double-time encodes a protein closely related to human casein kinase Iepsilon. Cell. 1998;94(1):97–107. pmid:9674431
  13. 13. Price JL, Blau J, Rothenfluh A, Abodeely M, Kloss B, Young MW. double-time is a novel Drosophila clock gene that regulates PERIOD protein accumulation. Cell. 1998;94(1):83–95. pmid:9674430
  14. 14. Helfrich-Förster C. Organization of endogenous clocks in insects. Biochem Soc Trans. 2005;33(Pt 5):957–61. pmid:16246020
  15. 15. Ma D, Przybylski D, Abruzzi KC, Schlichting M, Li Q, Long X, et al. A transcriptomic taxonomy of Drosophila circadian neurons around the clock. Elife. 2021;10:e63056. pmid:33438579
  16. 16. Shafer OT, Helfrich-Förster C, Renn SCP, Taghert PH. Reevaluation of Drosophila melanogaster’s neuronal circadian pacemakers reveals new neuronal classes. J Comp Neurol. 2006;498(2):180–93. pmid:16856134
  17. 17. Shafer OT, Gutierrez GJ, Li K, Mildenhall A, Spira D, Marty J, et al. Connectomic analysis of the Drosophila lateral neuron clock cells reveals the synaptic basis of functional pacemaker classes. Elife. 2022;11:e79139. pmid:35766361
  18. 18. Grima B, Chélot E, Xia R, Rouyer F. Morning and evening peaks of activity rely on different clock neurons of the Drosophila brain. Nature. 2004;431(7010):869–73. pmid:15483616
  19. 19. Rieger D, Shafer OT, Tomioka K, Helfrich-Förster C. Functional analysis of circadian pacemaker neurons in Drosophila melanogaster. J Neurosci. 2006;26(9):2531–43. pmid:16510731
  20. 20. Renn SC, Park JH, Rosbash M, Hall JC, Taghert PH. A pdf neuropeptide gene mutation and ablation of PDF neurons each cause severe abnormalities of behavioral circadian rhythms in Drosophila. Cell. 1999;99(7):791–802. pmid:10619432
  21. 21. Park JH, Helfrich-Förster C, Lee G, Liu L, Rosbash M, Hall JC. Differential regulation of circadian pacemaker output by separate clock genes in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97(7):3608–13. pmid:10725392
  22. 22. Stoleru D, Peng Y, Nawathean P, Rosbash M. A resetting signal between Drosophila pacemakers synchronizes morning and evening activity. Nature. 2005;438(7065):238–42. pmid:16281038
  23. 23. Stoleru D, Peng Y, Agosto J, Rosbash M. Coupled oscillators control morning and evening locomotor behaviour of Drosophila. Nature. 2004;431(7010):862–8. pmid:15483615
  24. 24. Fernandez MP, Pettibone HL, Bogart JT, Roell CJ, Davey CE, Pranevicius A, et al. Sites of circadian clock neuron plasticity mediate sensory integration and entrainment. Curr Biol. 2020;30(12):2225-2237.e5. pmid:32386535
  25. 25. Guo F, Chen X, Rosbash M. Temporal calcium profiling of specific circadian neurons in freely moving flies. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017;114(41):E8780–7. pmid:28973886
  26. 26. Lamaze A, Stanewsky R. DN1p or the “Fluffy” cerberus of clock outputs. Front Physiol. 2020;10:1540. pmid:31969832
  27. 27. Collins B, Kane EA, Reeves DC, Akabas MH, Blau J. Balance of activity between LN(v)s and glutamatergic dorsal clock neurons promotes robust circadian rhythms in Drosophila. Neuron. 2012;74(4):706–18. pmid:22632728
  28. 28. Yoshii T, Wülbeck C, Sehadova H, Veleri S, Bichler D, Stanewsky R, et al. The neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor adjusts period and phase of Drosophila’s clock. J Neurosci. 2009;29(8):2597–610. pmid:19244536
  29. 29. Peng Y, Stoleru D, Levine JD, Hall JC, Rosbash M. Drosophila free-running rhythms require intercellular communication. PLoS Biol. 2003;1(1):E13. pmid:12975658
  30. 30. Liang X, Holy TE, Taghert PH. A series of suppressive signals within the Drosophila circadian neural circuit generates sequential daily outputs. Neuron. 2017;94(6):1173-1189.e4. pmid:28552314
  31. 31. Mertens I, Vandingenen A, Johnson EC, Shafer OT, Li W, Trigg JS, et al. PDF receptor signaling in Drosophila contributes to both circadian and geotactic behaviors. Neuron. 2005;48(2):213–9. pmid:16242402
  32. 32. Hyun S, Lee Y, Hong S-T, Bang S, Paik D, Kang J, et al. Drosophila GPCR Han is a receptor for the circadian clock neuropeptide PDF. Neuron. 2005;48(2):267–78. pmid:16242407
  33. 33. Lear BC, Merrill CE, Lin J-M, Schroeder A, Zhang L, Allada R. A G protein-coupled receptor, groom-of-PDF, is required for PDF neuron action in circadian behavior. Neuron. 2005;48(2):221–7. pmid:16242403
  34. 34. Kim WJ, Jan LY, Jan YN. A PDF/NPF neuropeptide signaling circuitry of male Drosophila melanogaster controls rival-induced prolonged mating. Neuron. 2013;80(5):1190–205. pmid:24314729
  35. 35. Krupp JJ, Billeter J-C, Wong A, Choi C, Nitabach MN, Levine JD. Pigment-dispersing factor modulates pheromone production in clock cells that influence mating in drosophila. Neuron. 2013;79(1):54–68. pmid:23849197
  36. 36. Inami S, Sato S, Kondo S, Tanimoto H, Kitamoto T, Sakai T. Environmental light is required for maintenance of long-term memory in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 2020;40(7):1427–39. pmid:31932417
  37. 37. Toma DP, White KP, Hirsch J, Greenspan RJ. Identification of genes involved in Drosophila melanogaster geotaxis, a complex behavioral trait. Nat Genet. 2002;31(4):349–53. pmid:12042820
  38. 38. Guo F, Yu J, Jung HJ, Abruzzi KC, Luo W, Griffith LC, et al. Circadian neuron feedback controls the Drosophila sleep—activity profile. Nature. 2016;536(7616):292–7. pmid:27479324
  39. 39. Chen D, Sitaraman D, Chen N, Jin X, Han C, Chen J, et al. Genetic and neuronal mechanisms governing the sex-specific interaction between sleep and sexual behaviors in Drosophila. Nat Commun. 2017;8(1):154. pmid:28754889
  40. 40. Kunst M, Hughes ME, Raccuglia D, Felix M, Li M, Barnett G, et al. Calcitonin gene-related peptide neurons mediate sleep-specific circadian output in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2014;24(22):2652–64. pmid:25455031
  41. 41. Helfrich-Förster C. Differential control of morning and evening components in the activity rhythm of Drosophila melanogaster—sex-specific differences suggest a different quality of activity. J Biol Rhythms. 2000;15(2):135–54. pmid:10762032
  42. 42. Palmateer CM, Artikis C, Brovero SG, Friedman B, Gresham A, Arbeitman MN. Single-cell transcriptome profiles of Drosophila fruitless-expressing neurons from both sexes. Elife. 2023;12:e78511. pmid:36724009
  43. 43. Fujii S, Amrein H. Ventral lateral and DN1 clock neurons mediate distinct properties of male sex drive rhythm in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(23):10590–5. pmid:20498055
  44. 44. Hanafusa S, Kawaguchi T, Umezaki Y, Tomioka K, Yoshii T. Sexual interactions influence the molecular oscillations in DN1 pacemaker neurons in Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS One. 2013;8(12):e84495. pmid:24367668
  45. 45. Lee G, Bahn JH, Park JH. Sex- and clock-controlled expression of the neuropeptide F gene in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(33):12580–5. pmid:16894172
  46. 46. Hermann C, Yoshii T, Dusik V, Helfrich-Förster C. Neuropeptide F immunoreactive clock neurons modify evening locomotor activity and free-running period in Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Neurol. 2012;520(5):970–87. pmid:21826659
  47. 47. Nässel DR, Zandawala M. Recent advances in neuropeptide signaling in Drosophila, from genes to physiology and behavior. Prog Neurobiol. 2019;179:101607. pmid:30905728
  48. 48. Akpoghiran O, Strich AK, Koh K. Effects of sex, mating status, and genetic background on circadian behavior in Drosophila. Front Neurosci. 2025;18:1532868. pmid:39844849
  49. 49. Kuwano R, Katsura M, Iwata M, Yokosako T, Yoshii T. Pigment-dispersing factor and CCHamide1 in the Drosophila circadian clock network. Chronobiol Int. 2023;40(3):284–99. pmid:36786215
  50. 50. Parisky KM, Agosto J, Pulver SR, Shang Y, Kuklin E, Hodge JJL, et al. PDF cells are a GABA-responsive wake-promoting component of the Drosophila sleep circuit. Neuron. 2008;60(4):672–82. pmid:19038223
  51. 51. Cornelissen G. Cosinor-based rhythmometry. Theor Biol Med Model. 2014;11:16. pmid:24725531
  52. 52. Im SH, Li W, Taghert PH. PDFR and CRY signaling converge in a subset of clock neurons to modulate the amplitude and phase of circadian behavior in Drosophila. PLoS One. 2011;6(4):e18974. pmid:21559487
  53. 53. Richhariya S, Shin D, Le JQ, Rosbash M. Dissecting neuron-specific functions of circadian genes using modified cell-specific CRISPR approaches. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2023;120(29):e2303779120. pmid:37428902
  54. 54. Gorostiza EA, Ceriani MF. Retrograde bone morphogenetic protein signaling shapes a key circadian pacemaker circuit. J Neurosci. 2013;33(2):687–96. pmid:23303947
  55. 55. Ruben M, Drapeau MD, Mizrak D, Blau J. A mechanism for circadian control of pacemaker neuron excitability. J Biol Rhythms. 2012;27(5):353–64. pmid:23010658
  56. 56. Yao Z, Shafer OT. The Drosophila circadian clock is a variably coupled network of multiple peptidergic units. Science. 2014;343(6178):1516–20. pmid:24675961
  57. 57. Muskus MJ, Preuss F, Fan J-Y, Bjes ES, Price JL. Drosophila DBT lacking protein kinase activity produces long-period and arrhythmic circadian behavioral and molecular rhythms. Mol Cell Biol. 2007;27(23):8049–64. pmid:17893330
  58. 58. Kaneko M, Park JH, Cheng Y, Hardin PE, Hall JC. Disruption of synaptic transmission or clock-gene-product oscillations in circadian pacemaker cells of Drosophila cause abnormal behavioral rhythms. J Neurobiol. 2000;43(3):207–33. pmid:10842235
  59. 59. Blanchardon E, Grima B, Klarsfeld A, Chélot E, Hardin PE, Préat T, et al. Defining the role of Drosophila lateral neurons in the control of circadian rhythms in motor activity and eclosion by targeted genetic ablation and PERIOD protein overexpression. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;13(5):871–88. pmid:11264660
  60. 60. Umezaki Y, Yasuyama K, Nakagoshi H, Tomioka K. Blocking synaptic transmission with tetanus toxin light chain reveals modes of neurotransmission in the PDF-positive circadian clock neurons of Drosophila melanogaster. J Insect Physiol. 2011;57(9):1290–9. pmid:21708159
  61. 61. Yao Z, Bennett AJ, Clem JL, Shafer OT. The Drosophila clock neuron network features diverse coupling modes and requires network-wide coherence for robust circadian rhythms. Cell Rep. 2016;17(11):2873–81. pmid:27974202
  62. 62. Brown MP, Verma S, Palmer I, Zuniga AG, Rosensweig C, Keles MF, et al. A subclass of evening cells promotes the switch from arousal to sleep at dusk. bioRxiv. 2023:2023.08.28.555147. pmid:37693540
  63. 63. Delventhal R, O’Connor RM, Pantalia MM, Ulgherait M, Kim HX, Basturk MK, et al. Dissection of central clock function in Drosophila through cell-specific CRISPR-mediated clock gene disruption. Elife. 2019;8:e48308. pmid:31613218
  64. 64. Schlichting M, Díaz MM, Xin J, Rosbash M. Neuron-specific knockouts indicate the importance of network communication to Drosophila rhythmicity. Elife. 2019;8:e48301. pmid:31613223
  65. 65. Bulthuis N, Spontak KR, Kleeman B, Cavanaugh DJ. Neuronal activity in non-LNv clock cells is required to produce free-running rest:activity rhythms in Drosophila. J Biol Rhythms. 2019;34(3):249–71. pmid:30994046
  66. 66. Shansky RM, Murphy AZ. Considering sex as a biological variable will require a global shift in science culture. Nat Neurosci. 2021;24(4):457–64. pmid:33649507
  67. 67. Kokras N, Dalla C. Sex differences in animal models of psychiatric disorders. Br J Pharmacol. 2014;171(20):4595–619. pmid:24697577
  68. 68. Beery AK, Zucker I. Sex bias in neuroscience and biomedical research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):565–72. pmid:20620164
  69. 69. Joye DAM, Evans JA. Sex differences in daily timekeeping and circadian clock circuits. Semin Cell Dev Biol. 2022;126:45–55. pmid:33994299
  70. 70. Taghert PH, Nitabach MN. Peptide neuromodulation in invertebrate model systems. Neuron. 2012;76(1):82–97. pmid:23040808
  71. 71. Goda T, Umezaki Y, Alwattari F, Seo HW, Hamada FN. Neuropeptides PDF and DH31 hierarchically regulate free-running rhythmicity in Drosophila circadian locomotor activity. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):838. pmid:30696873
  72. 72. Helfrich-Förster C. Robust circadian rhythmicity of Drosophila melanogaster requires the presence of lateral neurons: a brain-behavioral study of disconnected mutants. J Comp Physiol A. 1998;182(4):435–53. pmid:9530835
  73. 73. Fernández MP, Berni J, Ceriani MF. Circadian remodeling of neuronal circuits involved in rhythmic behavior. PLoS Biol. 2008;6(3):e69. pmid:18366255
  74. 74. Biondi G, McCormick G, Fernandez MP. The Drosophila circadian clock gene cycle controls development of clock neurons. bioRxiv. 2023:2023.10.29.564626.
  75. 75. Sweeney ST, Broadie K, Keane J, Niemann H, O’Kane CJ. Targeted expression of tetanus toxin light chain in Drosophila specifically eliminates synaptic transmission and causes behavioral defects. Neuron. 1995;14(2):341–51. pmid:7857643
  76. 76. Kaneko M, Helfrich-Förster C, Hall JC. Spatial and temporal expression of the period and timeless genes in the developing nervous system of Drosophila: newly identified pacemaker candidates and novel features of clock gene product cycling. J Neurosci. 1997;17(17):6745–60. pmid:9254686
  77. 77. Hofbauer B, Zandawala M, Reinhard N, Rieger D, Werner C, Evers JF, et al. The neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor signals independently of Bruchpilot-labelled active zones in daily remodelled terminals of Drosophila clock neurons. Eur J Neurosci. 2024;59(10):2665–85. pmid:38414155
  78. 78. Johard HAD, Yoishii T, Dircksen H, Cusumano P, Rouyer F, Helfrich-Förster C, et al. Peptidergic clock neurons in Drosophila: ion transport peptide and short neuropeptide F in subsets of dorsal and ventral lateral neurons. J Comp Neurol. 2009;516(1):59–73. pmid:19565664
  79. 79. Todd WD, Fenselau H, Wang JL, Zhang R, Machado NL, Venner A, et al. A hypothalamic circuit for the circadian control of aggression. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21(5):717–24. pmid:29632359
  80. 80. Fujii S, Krishnan P, Hardin P, Amrein H. Nocturnal male sex drive in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2007;17(3):244–51. pmid:17276917
  81. 81. Sakai T, Ishida N. Circadian rhythms of female mating activity governed by clock genes in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(16):9221–5. pmid:11470898
  82. 82. T M, Hari Dass S, Sharma VK. Egg-laying rhythm in Drosophila melanogaster. J Genet. 2008;87(5):495–504. pmid:19147938
  83. 83. Zhang C, Daubnerova I, Jang Y-H, Kondo S, Žitňan D, Kim Y-J. The neuropeptide allatostatin C from clock-associated DN1p neurons generates the circadian rhythm for oogenesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2021;118(4):e2016878118. pmid:33479181
  84. 84. Stoleru D, Nawathean P, Fernández M de la P, Menet JS, Ceriani MF, Rosbash M. The Drosophila circadian network is a seasonal timer. Cell. 2007;129(1):207–19. pmid:17418796
  85. 85. Murad A, Emery-Le M, Emery P. A subset of dorsal neurons modulates circadian behavior and light responses in Drosophila. Neuron. 2007;53(5):689–701. pmid:17329209
  86. 86. Picot M, Cusumano P, Klarsfeld A, Ueda R, Rouyer F. Light activates output from evening neurons and inhibits output from morning neurons in the Drosophila circadian clock. PLoS Biol. 2007;5(11):e315. pmid:18044989
  87. 87. Zhang Y, Emery P. GW182 controls Drosophila circadian behavior and PDF-receptor signaling. Neuron. 2013;78(1):152–65. pmid:23583112
  88. 88. Persons JL, Abhilash L, Lopatkin AJ, Roelofs A, Bell EV, Fernandez MP, et al. PHASE: an open-source program for the analysis of Drosophila phase, activity, and sleep under entrainment. J Biol Rhythms. 2022;37(4):455–67. pmid:35727044
  89. 89. Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, et al. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat Methods. 2012;9(7):676–82. pmid:22743772
  90. 90. Lee Gierke C, Cornelissen G. Chronomics analysis toolkit (CATkit). Biological Rhythm Research. 2015;47(2):163–81.