Figures
Author summary
Episodic memory is a critical aspect of everyday life that allows us to recall events, make decisions, and navigate our environments. It is estimated that 1 in 25 Americans suffer from episodic memory loss from Alzheimer’s disease, dementia, or following a traumatic brain injury. Clinicians and researchers have recently tried to help supplement or repair memory in individuals with episodic memory disorders using electrical brain stimulation. However, although there is evidence of invasive brain stimulation improving memory, studies also show that it can have variable effects, and sometimes cause memory impairment. In this Essay, we investigate these differences and try to explain this variability. We propose that key aspects of the memory network are heterogeneous across individuals, requiring customized stimulation protocols to create successful brain stimulation systems for memory enhancement. We provide a plan for applying brain stimulation while overcoming this variability by using closed-loop stimulation protocols that are customized according to a person’s own anatomical structure, functional connectivity, and memory-related electrophysiological patterns.
Citation: Mohan UR, Jacobs J (2024) Why does invasive brain stimulation sometimes improve memory and sometimes impair it? PLoS Biol 22(10): e3002894. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002894
Published: October 25, 2024
This is an open access article, free of all copyright, and may be freely reproduced, distributed, transmitted, modified, built upon, or otherwise used by anyone for any lawful purpose. The work is made available under the Creative Commons CC0 public domain dedication.
Funding: This work was supported by an NSF CAREER award to J.J. and the NINDS Competitive Postdoctoral Fellowship Award for U.M. The funders had no role in the preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Direct electrical brain stimulation is an effective treatment for a number of neurological and behavioral disorders, including Parkinson’s disease, essential tremor, dystonia, and epileptic seizures [1–9]. Building on this success, in the past decades researchers have expanded the scope of invasive brain stimulation, using it to help patients with a much broader range of neuropsychiatric and cognitive disorders, including major depressive disorder [10,11], obsessive compulsive disorder [12], anorexia nervosa [13], addiction [14,15], schizophrenia [16,17], and memory disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease [18,19]. However, unlike the earlier success of deep brain stimulation for motor disorders, these efforts at using brain stimulation to treat neuropsychiatric and cognitive disorders have produced inconsistent effects [2,3,20,21]. But why is this the case?
Previous studies on the use of brain stimulation for memory enhancement generally used fixed stimulation protocols across individuals. These studies produced wide-ranging outcomes, with some reporting impaired memory performance from stimulation [22–31], and others showing enhancement [30,32–42] (Table 1). Across these studies, electrical brain stimulation was applied with various ranges of parameters, with substantial differences in location, frequency, duration, amplitude, and timing (Fig 1). As well as technical challenges, a significant scientific challenge is that we do not yet have a complete characterization of the neural and electrophysiological correlates of memory. Researchers have identified a number of different electrophysiological signals that correlate with memory encoding, such as theta-band oscillations, which are potential targets for enhancement with stimulation. However, we do not know which electrophysiological signals are most directly and causally relevant for forming new memories. This uncertainty regarding the neural basis of human memory encoding makes it hard to precisely design stimulation protocols that are optimized to drive memory enhancement.
(a) Different stimulation locations, frequencies, amplitudes, durations, and pulse widths have been shown to have varying effects on neural activity and in turn on memory function. When developing a stimulation protocol designed to modulate a specific memory-related neural signal, it is critical to first determine how the combination of these parameters will alter electrophysiology [29,37,38,42–52]. (b) Separate from the parameters that determine how current is delivered, the timing, or state in which an individual is in, impacts the effect of stimulation on memory. These can be considered in 2 different ways: timing with respect to the targeted memory process determined by the structure of the memory task [22,25–30,32,36,39–42], and timing from moment-to-moment, i.e., whether an individual’s neural signals show that they are in a good or bad memory processing state [36,53].
One approach that can be employed is to stimulate in a way that would recreate or supplement the brain’s natural memory-related signals in order to improve the brain’s own natural memory encoding or retrieval state. In this approach, stimulation is often targeted at the hippocampus, a critical structure for memory. Individuals are stimulated with a fixed set of stimulation pulses designed to broadly excite hippocampal neural activity [23–27,30,33] or reinstate the hippocampus’ own endogenous 4- to 8-Hz theta rhythm. Because theta oscillations are linked to memory encoding and synaptic plasticity (Table 1) [37,38,42], researchers hypothesized that theta-enhancing stimulation would improve memory accuracy. Although this method seems logical, the effects on memory performance from hippocampal stimulation are mixed, perhaps reflecting our lack of understanding of how memories are encoded.
In this Essay, we discuss why the behavioral and physiological effects of invasive brain stimulation are so variable across studies and individuals, explaining how fixed approaches for memory enhancement produce inconsistent results. In this context, we explain why a more flexible approach that accommodates complex and heterogenous brain patterns of individuals is necessary for consistent memory improvement. Finally, we describe a framework for tailoring customized stimulation for memory enhancement based on each person’s individually mapped physiology, with a focus on the hippocampal–cortical network.
Why are the effects of invasive brain stimulation so variable?
Given the importance of memory for everyday life, researchers have tried a number of approaches to apply electrical stimulation to enhance memory. Here, we focus on direct electrical stimulation with surgically implanted electrodes. Researchers have conducted studies with various types of stimulation, from continuous stimulation with direct currents to bursts of charge-balanced pulse trains with different parameters. Studies have also tested the effects of current applied at different times relative to behavior and across a range of target areas including both gray and white matter [37,42]. These different approaches and parameters have widely varying effects and need to be understood if we are to use invasive brain stimulation to effectively improve memory (Table 1).
How does the precise location of stimulation impact memory?
Memory is a complex process that involves a broad network of brain regions. Many studies have attempted to improve human memory by applying stimulation to different elements of the memory network [54], with the aim of enhancing synaptic plasticity and memory formation; however, a wide range of behavioral effects have been observed.
In general, the behavioral effect of applying electrical stimulation to a region often corresponds with the broader functional role of that region (Fig 1A). For instance, stimulation in sensory regions induces perceptual phosphenes [55], the sensation of light without light actually entering the eye. However, the effects of stimulation even in a particular brain region can vary dramatically according to the specific positioning of the stimulating electrode(s). In some cases, stimulation at nearby sites within a region can produce highly variable, even opposite [56–59], behavioral effects. For instance, when direct electrical stimulation was applied to a specific cortical location, a patient spontaneously recalled memories from high school; however, when stimulation was applied at all neighboring locations it did not evoke high school-related memories [59]. To explain why the effects of invasive brain stimulation are so spatially specific, it is helpful to consider multi-scale interactions between the network and local physiology because stimulating different combinations of neuron populations and pathways can have complex network-level effects beyond the stimulation site [60].
The importance of the precise stimulation location is most evident from a set of recent memory-modulation studies that measured the positioning of individual stimulation electrodes with respect to white matter pathways (Fig 1A). White matter in the brain consists of bundles of axons that create structural pathways for communication between brain regions [61]. Due to the interconnection of regions by white matter tracts, stimulating nearby specific bundles can have particularly strong impacts. Two studies reported greater improvement in memory performance from stimulating in or close to the white matter pathways of the medial temporal lobe (MTL) rather than in gray matter (Table 1) [37,62]. This trend of different stimulation effects in white matter versus gray matter were also observed in the lateral temporal cortex [63]. These findings could be explained by the enhanced ability of white matter stimulation to modulate both local and distant neural activity [43,51,63,64], thus allowing for the regulation of broader networks that drive cognitive states [44–47,65,66]. Further, when stimulating gray matter directly, currents can impact neighboring cell bodies, and the effects are likely to be inhibitory and spatially limited compared to white matter [52,67,68]. Thus, to improve targeting of white matter with electrical stimulation, recent modeling work shows promise for selecting stimulation locations based on individual patient tractography [69,70].
Despite some apparent successes, merely stimulating the white matter elements of the memory network does not always lead to memory enhancement. For example, 2 studies [27,32] both targeted the white matter of the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex with 50-Hz stimulation and reported opposite effects on memory. This variability highlights the fact that effective stimulation for memory enhancement requires more than simply identifying a particular anatomical target. Therefore, even when applying stimulation in an optimal location, one may need to consider other aspects of the stimulation parameters with respect to task design, the type of memory process, and ongoing neural dynamics, because the wrong type of stimulation can be disruptive, even when applied even to the right target location (Table 1 and Fig 1B).
Which stimulation parameters are most effective for memory modulation?
Electrical stimulation can be applied to the brain with countless combinations of parameters, including different frequencies, amplitudes, and pulse-burst rates (Table 1 and Fig 1A). Identifying the optimal stimulation parameters is critical. The 2 stimulation parameters that often impact stimulation’s effects on underlying neural activity are the frequency and amplitude. Generally, applying low-frequency stimulation inhibits local neural activity, while high-frequency stimulation (100 Hz and above) can both excite or inhibit neural activity depending on the target region (Fig 2A) [43,51,71,72]. In contrast to frequency, where effects can be positive or negative, the effect of amplitude is generally simpler. At a given stimulation site, increasing amplitude increases the magnitude of the effect that occurred from lower-amplitude stimulation (Fig 2B) [51,64,72].
(a) Percent of recording electrodes showing significant high frequency activity increases and decreases for each stimulation frequency for depth and surface stimulation sites. Adapted from [43]. (b) Increase in inhibitory and excitatory effects determined by spiking rate with stimulation amplitude. Adapted from [50].
Beyond these general trends, there is additional variability across regions in the effect of stimulation frequencies that differ in relation to the functional role of the local circuit. In some brain regions, stimulation at particular frequencies can improve memory by up-regulating specific local processes. There is evidence of memory improvement from stimulation at higher frequencies [43], bursts of stimulation at 4- to 8-Hz theta frequencies [37,42,73], and lower amplitudes applied in deep structures. In particular, key hippocampal signals, such as ripple oscillations and 4- to 8-Hz theta rhythms, are vital for synaptic plasticity and memory in encoding, consolidation, and retrieval of information in spatial navigation, working memory, and long-term memory formation [74–77]. There is evidence that bursts of stimulation at theta frequencies can improve memory, likely by entraining and enhancing the brain’s own theta-band oscillations [78,79]. Meanwhile, 200-Hz stimulation in the temporal cortex has been used to target the spectral tilt in the power spectrum, which represents the prevalence of high- and low-frequency components in a neural signal. This biomarker, which is associated with memory-related increases in high-frequency activity, is thought to represent synaptic excitation/inhibition balance [80–83]. The ability of these contrasting approaches to elicit comparable memory enhancements demonstrates that there are multiple distinct neurophysiological signatures of memory processes that form effective stimulation targets [76]. An important challenge going forward is to identify the optimal neural signal to modulate in cases where there are multiple potential stimulation targets at different frequencies.
In addition to frequency and amplitude, the pulse width and waveform also are important factors in controlling how stimulation affects the brain (Fig 1A) [84,85]. Most memory modulation studies have applied charge-balanced, biphasic, rectangular pulses (Table 1). These rectangle-shaped voltages patterns, especially when delivered continuously for many seconds, are quite different from the brain’s native electrical patterns. Although research is limited, there is reason to think that alternative stimulation waveforms, such as sinusoids [67], may be helpful because the currents may more closely align with endogenous electrophysiology. An additional useful approach may be varying the width of stimulation pulses, as varying pulse widths may elicit the ability to recruit different types and sizes of neurons [86].
In recent years, researchers have identified a number of new, important electrophysiological patterns that may be useful targets for memory enhancement in humans, such as sharp-wave ripples and traveling waves of memory-related oscillations [87]. Up-regulating these signals with naturalistic stimulation may be more effective for memory improvement. Along these lines, one study experimented with stimulation that more closely resembled actual electrophysiological rhythms, with sine-wave-shaped stimulation delivered in-phase and anti-phase inducing memory enhancement and impairment, respectively [33]. A more recent study demonstrated impairment of memory retrieval by targeting separate nodes of the memory retrieval network with precisely timed theta-burst stimulation [29]. The specific phase lag that impaired memory the most varied depending on the stimulation target nodes of the network, emphasizing the importance of precisely timing stimulation to match the brain’s intrinsic signals. Thus, the efficacy of stimulation for cognitive enhancement may depend on finding the optimal combination of waveform shape, timing, frequency, amplitude, pulse width, and temporal patterning for a particular target brain region. Together, these studies open pathways for future work, such as designing stimulation waveforms to modulate endogenous neural signals and determining how to best deliver stimulation at multiple locations simultaneously.
When should stimulation be applied for memory enhancement?
In order for stimulation to improve memory, it is not only important to choose the correct stimulation parameters and location, but also to find the right time to stimulate. The correct stimulation applied at the wrong time can be problematic; for instance, one study showed that stimulation applied in the period between encoding and recall impaired memory to a greater degree than when applied directly during either one of these 2 periods individually [28]. This amplified disruption implies an important functional process occurs between the encoding and retrieval phases of memory tasks (Fig 1B). Other research adds to this perspective showing that the effects of stimulation depend on the state of the memory network at the time of stimulation. One study showed that if a patient was already in a good memory encoding state, stimulation was not necessary [36]. However, if a patient was in a poor memory encoding state, stimulation could facilitate better memory performance [36]. Researchers have often assumed that stimulation during encoding would either disrupt or enhance neural mechanisms involved in memory formation, while stimulation during retrieval would modulate recall mechanisms (Table 1). However, studies applying stimulation between these explicitly defined encoding and retrieval periods indicate the importance of less understood dynamic memory processing during intermediary states.
Separate from wakeful encoding and retrieval memory states, one study modulated memory performance by stimulating during periods of sleep that occurred between task sessions [41]. Instead of aiming to induce a good memory encoding or retrieval state when a subject was trying to encode or retrieve information, this approach enhanced how newly encoded information was consolidated in order to be recognized more accurately the next day. Interestingly, even as stimulation was applied during unconscious sleep states, it changed neural activity in distinct ways from awake states [88]. The different effects of stimulation in wakefulness versus sleep call for further exploration of the underlying neural dynamics across diverse states to better predict large-scale responses to stimulation.
An additional challenge in designing brain stimulation protocols is that the same type of stimulation can have varying effects on different types of memory [30,38]. For instance, one case demonstrated that 50-Hz stimulation in the hippocampus improved associative memory even as it disrupted memory for individual items [30]. In line with this concept, a separate study demonstrated that multi-site stimulation targeting nodes of a network impaired spatial memory retrieval, but not temporal memory retrieval [29]. These findings further emphasize the importance of ensuring that stimulation is tailored according to current behavioral events and the brain’s ongoing functional processes.
How do the effects of stimulation vary across individuals?
Interindividual differences in anatomy and connectivity
Beyond parameter- and location-related effects, there are also substantial individual differences in the neural and behavioral effects of stimulation [56]. Individual humans have unique anatomy, connectivity, and functional organization, which influence how electrical stimulation interacts with local neural activity and brain circuits to modulate behavior. Early work [56,57] showed that when direct electrical stimulation was applied to similar locations on the surface of the sensory cortex, different patients experienced opposite sensations of involuntary vocalization or a temporary inability to speak, and in another case, numbness or feeling sensations without outside stimuli. These variations may be caused by various factors, such as differences in the exact location of neural populations, the density of neurotransmitter receptors, and the strength of synaptic connections. In addition, evidence from nonhuman primates suggests that specific patterns of stimulation can simultaneously induce both inhibitory and excitatory effects in different downstream regions [72]. A patient’s own individualized microarchitecture, or neural microstructure surrounding the stimulating electrodes, including the arrangement and density of cells, proximity to white matter tracts, functionally connected regions, and cortical layers, affect the way current flows through neural tissue and how neural activity changes in different downstream regions [42,43,45,51,52,63,89–93].
These diverse sources of variation create a challenge in designing brain stimulation protocols to alter brain activity in targeted ways that achieve desired electrophysiological changes and behavioral outcomes. Much of the apparent variability in therapeutic efficacy in treating neuropsychiatric disorders [21,94] likely stems from the largely uncharacterized and complex interactions between stimulation region, frequency, amplitude, pulse width, duration, and location relative to individual patients’ grey and white matter.
Researchers and clinicians should consider this individual- and region-level variability when crafting stimulation protocols. For instance, if the goal is to inhibit a specific brain region, one may design a stimulation protocol that targets gray matter with low-frequency stimulation; however, if the goal is to modulate large-scale connectivity, a stimulation protocol that targets a white-matter bundle in the relevant network with high-frequency pulses may be more effective. These optimal locations for each patient can be informed by mapping procedures, such as cortico-cortical evoked potential responses to single pulse electrical stimulation and structural connectivity from diffusion tensor imaging [51,63]. The neural and therapeutic impact of some of these interactions have been explored in animals and by using models [67–70], and in deep brain stimulation to treat Parkinson’s disease, providing a preliminary understanding of how combinations of stimulation parameters interact when influencing neural activity.
Interindividual differences in memory-related brain oscillations
An additional factor that may modulate the effects of brain stimulation in a given region are the local electrophysiological characteristics of activity, in particular endogenous neural oscillations.
Key neural signals related to memory are neural oscillations in the theta and alpha band [95,96]. However, the frequencies of ongoing theta and alpha oscillations vary widely between subjects and across cortical regions [87,97]. In the human hippocampus, for example, theta- and alpha-like oscillations range from 2 to 16 Hz [98], with narrow-band theta oscillations at 4 to 8 Hz being associated with successful memory encoding, particularly when synchronized between regions. In addition, there is also evidence that “slow theta” oscillations at 3 Hz could relate to episodic memory [95]. A different memory-related pattern is high-gamma activity, often occurring at specific phases of low-frequency oscillations, which has also been associated with a wide range of memory and attention processes [82,99]. A specific type of high-frequency oscillation event, known as a ripple, has been shown to underlie the binding and retrieval of memories [100,101].
Across all these rhythmic brain signals there is substantial individual variability in the frequency of oscillations related to various factors, including genetics [102], age [103,104], ongoing cognitive process, and neurological disorders [105]. For instance, younger individuals tend to exhibit oscillations at faster frequencies in certain bands [106]. Thus, because brain stimulation interacts with network oscillations, these varying oscillation frequencies have implications for the effective use of stimulation for memory. As stimulation at different frequencies has distinct effects [48,107], researchers may tailor the frequency of stimulation to an individual’s own natural oscillatory frequencies. Some studies that used theta-burst stimulation have employed this approach to amplify a subject’s own theta oscillations [38,49,107]. However, it remains an open question if patients could benefit from receiving customized stimulation in relation to neural biomarkers at other non-theta frequencies, such as memory-related high-frequency activity [81,82].
Neural oscillations vary not only in their frequency between patients, but also in their spatiotemporal patterns across the brain. An emerging discovery is that many brain oscillations are not only local but instead they propagate across the cortex as traveling waves. Traveling waves of memory-related oscillations are present in many individuals. Moreover, traveling waves exhibit individual differences across subjects, including variations in the strength of propagation, frequency, and the direction of wave propagation [87,108,109]. These differences remain to be understood, but could arise from variability in cortical geometries between patients, which constrain the function of large-scale neural patterns [110]. Given that traveling waves may correlate with the propagation of task-related information across the brain, individual-level variations in the topographies and direction of traveling waves are likely to interact with the effects of stimulation. Therefore, stimulation may be more effective if protocols are designed to target characteristics, such as frequency and axis of propagation, of ongoing traveling waves.
Conclusions and future directions
Although we have suggested useful future directions, there are several areas where more research is needed to create improved protocols for memory-related stimulation. The effects of stimulation on memory are diverse, with separate studies showing memory enhancement and disruption from stimulation. To explain this variability and help derive improved protocols, below we describe several general directions of future research that may be fruitful.
First, there should be more investigation into the sources of variability in human memory biomarkers and in effects of stimulation. Currently, one issue is that stimulating a fixed location with the same parameters in different patients or conditions leads to different neural and behavioral effects. We hypothesize that at least part of the heterogeneous effects of brain stimulation on human memory stem from uncontrolled variations in the brain’s anatomy, functional dynamics, pathology, and instantaneous state. We need to advance our measurements of these factors and then take them into account to guide stimulation location and parameters.
To advance towards a personalized stimulation framework for memory, we should, in particular, probe variability in the hippocampal-cortical network. The individual differences from the effects of stimulation can be substantial (with some subjects often showing effects in opposite directions [43]). To explain this variability, we should probe all possible sources, such as microarchitecture differences related to cell density or tractography, as well as macro-level features related to neural anatomy, memory biomarkers, and brain connectivity. If we have an improved sense of this variability in the hippocampal-cortical network it could lead to improved subject-specific stimulation frameworks for memory.
Further, it may be useful to identify factors that predict the effects of stimulation in individuals, perhaps by using genetic markers that influence neurotransmitter levels, characteristics of neural activity, and neural plasticity in combination with or novel neuroimaging [111]. This approach could identify patients who are most likely to benefit from optimization of stimulation therapy. Personalization of stimulation protocols could go further, including patient-specific customization based on the task, and phase of memory processing, frequency of neural oscillations, and adjusting stimulation parameters accordingly such that stimulation is applied in a real-time, closed-loop fashion [36,53,112].
Currently, much of our understanding of human brain function is derived from animal studies. However, for the purpose of creating human brain stimulation protocols to modulate memory, it seems that animal studies have limited utility because there seem to be key behavioral and electrophysiological differences in terms of how complex memory processes operates in humans. As one example, theta-band rhythms often appear robustly in rodents at 8 Hz, whereas these signals are slower and transient in humans [74]. Given these differences in such a key memory-related signal, we need continued research to understand the aspects in which brain stimulation in the human hippocampus can rely on rodent models versus the circumstances in which we need human-specific data and models.
An additional challenge going forward is to evaluate the degree to which the neuromodulation paradigms developed in the clinic translate to the real world. Most of the study paradigms that form the basis of our current understanding of human brain stimulation for memory are based on memory tasks running on laptop computers at a patient’s bedside. Memory in the real world, with distractions and many implicit tasks, may utilize different neural processes from computer-based paradigms. Thus, acute memory modulation in a clinical setting may not always be indicative of sustained improvements in real-world memory function. It will take creative unification of multiple research areas going forward to identify the aspects of brain stimulation research and paradigms that are most useful for memory enhancement in naturalistic settings.
References
- 1. Benabid A, Pollack P, Loveau A, Henry S, de Rougemont J. Combined (Thalamototmy and Stimulation) Stereotactic Surgery of the VM Thalamic Nucleus for Bilateral Parkinson Disease. Stereotact Funct Neurosurg. 1987;50(1–6).
- 2. Lang AE, Lozano AM. Parkinson’s Disease. N Engl J Med. 1998;339(15):1044–1053.
- 3. Lang AE, Lozano AM. Parkinson’s Disease. N Engl J Med. 1998;339(16):1130–1143.
- 4. Koller W, Pahwa R, Busenbark K, Hubble J, Wilkinson S, Lang A, et al. High-frequency unilateral thalamic stimulation in the treatment of essential and parkinsonian tremor. Ann Neurol. 1997;42(3):292–299. pmid:9307249
- 5. Kumar R, Dagher A, Hutchison WD, Lang AE, Lozano AM. Globus pallidus deep brain stimulation for generalized dystonia: Clinical and PET investigation. Neurology. 1999;53(4):871–871. pmid:10489059
- 6. Coubes P, Roubertie A, Vayssiere N, Hemm S, Echenne B. Treatment of DYT1-generalised dystonia by stimulation of the internal globus pallidus. Lancet. 2000;355(9222):2220–2221. pmid:10881900
- 7. Yianni J, Bain P, Giladi N, Auca M, Gregory R, Joint C, et al. Globus pallidus internus deep brain stimulation for dystonic conditions: a prospective audit. Mov Disord. 2003;18(4):436–442. pmid:12671953
- 8. Fisher R, Salanova V, Witt T, Worth R, Henry T, Gross R, et al. Electrical stimulation of the anterior nucleus of thalamus for treatment of refractory epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2010;51(5):899–908. pmid:20331461
- 9. Fisher RS, Velasco AL. Electrical brain stimulation for epilepsy. Nat Rev Neurol. 2014;10(5):261. pmid:24709892
- 10. May CP, Rahhal T, Berry EM, Leighton EA. Aging, source memory, and emotion. Psychol Aging. 2005;20(4):571. pmid:16420132
- 11. Alagapan S, Choi KS, Heisig S, Riva-Posse P, Crowell A, Tiruvadi V, et al. Cingulate dynamics track depression recovery with deep brain stimulation. Nature. 2023;622(7981):130–138. pmid:37730990
- 12. Nuttin BJ, Gabri¨els LA, Cosyns PR, Meyerson BA, Andr´eewitch S, Sunaert SG, et al. Long-term electrical capsular stimulation in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neurosurgery. 2003;52(6):1263–1274. pmid:12762871
- 13. Lipsman N, Lam E, Volpini M, Sutandar K, Twose R, Giacobbe P, et al. Deep brain stimulation of the subcallosal cingulate for treatment-refractory anorexia nervosa: 1 year follow-up of an open-label trial. Lancet Psychiatry. 2017;4(4):285–294. pmid:28238701
- 14. Kuhn J, Lenartz D, Huff W, Lee S, Koulousakis A, Klosterkoetter J, et al. Remission of alcohol dependency following deep brain stimulation of the nucleus accumbens: valuable therapeutic implications? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78(10):1152–1153. pmid:17878197
- 15. Levy D, Shabat-Simon M, Shalev U, Barnea-Ygael N, Cooper A, Zangen A. Repeated electrical stimulation of reward-related brain regions affects cocaine but not? natural? reinforcement. J Neurosci. 2007;27(51):14179–14189. pmid:18094257
- 16. Kuhn J, Bodatsch M, Sturm V, Lenartz D, Klosterko¨tter J, Uhlhaas P, et al. Deep brain stimulation in schizophrenia. Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr. 2011;79(11):632–641.
- 17. Bakay RA. Deep brain stimulation for schizophrenia. Stereotact Funct Neurosurg. 2009;87(4):266–266. pmid:19556836
- 18. Kuhn J, Hardenacke K, Lenartz D, Gruendler T, Ullsperger M, Bartsch C, et al. Deep brain stimulation of the nucleus basalis of Meynert in Alzheimer?s dementia. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20(3):353. pmid:24798585
- 19. Lozano AM, Fosdick L, Chakravarty MM, Leoutsakos JM, Munro C, Oh E, et al. A phase II study of fornix deep brain stimulation in mild Alzheimer?s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;54(2):777–787. pmid:27567810
- 20. Gutman DA, Holtzheimer PE, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Mayberg HS. A tractography analysis of two deep brain stimulation white matter targets for depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65(4):276–282. pmid:19013554
- 21. Mayberg HS, Lozano AM, Voon V, McNeely HE, Seminowicz D, Hamani C, et al. Deep Brain Stimulation Surgery for Treatment Resistant Depression. Neuron. 2005;45:651–660.
- 22. Halgren E, Wilson CL, Stapleton JM. Human medial temporal-lobe stimulation disrupts both formation and retrieval of recent memories. Brain Cogn. 1985;4(3):287–95. pmid:4027062
- 23. Loring DW, Lee GP, Flanigin HF, Meador KJ, Smith JR, Gallagher BB, et al. Verbal memory performance following unilateral electrical stimulation of the human hippocampus. J Epilepsy. 1988;1(2):79–85.
- 24. Lee GP, Loring DW, Smith JR, Flanigin HF. Material specific learning during electrical stimulation of the human hippocampus. Cortex. 1990;26(3):433–442. pmid:2249443
- 25. Coleshill SG, Binnie CD, Morris RG, Alarcon G, van Emde Boas W, Velis DN, et al. Material-specific recognition memory deficits elicited by unilateral hippocampal electrical stimulation. J Neurosci. 2004;24(7):1612–16. pmid:14973245
- 26. Lacruz ME, Valent´ın A, Seoane JJG, Morris RG, Selway RP, Alarc´on G. Single pulse electrical stimulation of the hippocampus is sufficient to impair human episodic memory. Neuroscience. 2010;170(2):623–32. pmid:20643192
- 27. Jacobs J, Miller J, Lee SA, Coffey T, Watrous AJ, Sperling MR, et al. Direct electrical stimulation of the human entorhinal region and hippocampus impairs memory. Neuron. 2016;92(5):1–8. pmid:27930911
- 28. Merkow MB, Burke JF, Ramayya AG, Sharan AD, Sperling MR, Kahana MJ. Stimulation of the human medial temporal lobe between learning and recall selectively enhances forgetting. Brain Stimul. 2017;10(3):645–650. pmid:28073638
- 29. Kim K, Schedlbauer A, Rollo M, Karunakaran S, Ekstrom AD, Tandon N. Network-based brain stimulation selectively impairs spatial retrieval. Brain Stimul. 2018;11(1):213–221. pmid:29042188
- 30. Jun S, Lee SA, Kim JS, Jeong W, Chung CK. Task-dependent effects of intracranial hippocampal stimulation on human memory and hippocampal theta power. Brain Stimul. 2020;13(3):603–613. pmid:32289685
- 31. Xie W, Chapeton JI, Bhasin S, Zawora C, Wittig JH Jr, Inati SK, et al. The medial temporal lobe supports the quality of visual short-term memory representation. Nat Hum Behav. 2023;7(4):627–641. pmid:36864132
- 32. Suthana N, Haneef Z, Stern J, Mukamel R, Behnke E, Knowlton B, et al. Memory enhancement and deep-brain stimulation of the entorhinal area. N Engl J Med. 2012;366:502–10. pmid:22316444
- 33. Fell J, Staresina BP, Do Lam AT, Widman G, Helmstaedter C, Elger CE, et al. Memory modulation by weak synchronous deep brain stimulation: a pilot study. Brain Stimul. 2013;6(3):270–273. pmid:22939277
- 34. Koubeissi MZ, Kahriman E, Syed TU, Miller J, Durand DM. Low-frequency electrical stimulation of a fiber tract in temporal lobe epilepsy. Ann Neurol. 2013;74(2):223–231. pmid:23613463
- 35. Miller JP, Sweet JA, Bailey CM, Munyon CN, Luders HO, Fastenau PS. Visual-spatial memory may be enhanced with theta burst deep brain stimulation of the fornix: a preliminary investigation with four cases. Brain. 2015;138(7):1833–1842. pmid:26106097
- 36. Ezzyat Y, Kragel JE, Burke JF, Levy DF, Lyalenko A, Wanda P, et al. Direct brain stimulation modulates encoding states and memory performance in humans. Curr Biol. 2017;27(9):1251–1258. pmid:28434860
- 37. Titiz AS, Hill MR, Mankin EA, Aghajan ZM, Eliashiv D, Tchemodanov N, et al. Theta-burst microstimulation in the human entorhinal area improves memory specificity. Elife. 2017;6. pmid:29063831
- 38. Inman CS, Manns JR, Bijanki KR, Bass DI, Hamann S, Drane DL, et al. Direct electrical stimulation of the amygdala enhances declarative memory in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115(1):98–103. pmid:29255054
- 39. Kucewicz MT, Berry BM, Kremen V, Miller LR, Khadjevand F, Ezzyat Y, et al. Electrical stimulation modulates high gamma activity and human memory performance. eNeuro. 2018. pmid:29404403
- 40. Hampson RE, Song D, Robinson BS, Fetterhoff D, Dakos AS, Roeder BM, et al. Developing a hippocampal neural prosthetic to facilitate human memory encoding and recall. J Neural Eng. 2018;15(3):036014. pmid:29589592
- 41. Geva-Sagiv M, Mankin EA, Eliashiv D, Epstein S, Cherry N, Kalender G, et al. Augmenting hippocampal–prefrontal neuronal synchrony during sleep enhances memory consolidation in humans. Nat Neurosci. 2023;26(6):1100–1110. pmid:37264156
- 42. Mankin EA, Aghajan ZM, Schuette P, Tran ME, Tchemodanov N, Titiz A, et al. Stimulation of the right entorhinal white matter enhances visual memory encoding in humans. Brain Stimul. 2021;14(1):131–140. pmid:33279717
- 43. Mohan UR, Watrous AJ, Miller JF, Lega BC, Sperling MR, Worrell GA, et al. The effects of direct brain stimulation in humans depend on frequency, amplitude, and white-matter proximity. Brain Stimul. 2020;13(5):1183–1195. pmid:32446925
- 44. Stiso J, Khambhati AN, Menara T, Kahn AE, Stein JM, Das SR, et al. White matter network architecture guides direct electrical stimulation through optimal state transitions. Cell Rep. 2019;28(10):2554–2566. pmid:31484068
- 45. Solomon EA, Kragel JE, Gross RE, Lega B, Sperling MR, Worrell G, et al. Medial temporal lobe functional connectivity predicts stimulation-induced theta power. Nat Commun. 2018;9(1):4437. pmid:30361627
- 46. Ezzyat Y, Kragel JE, Solomon EA, Lega BC, Aronson JP, Jobst BC, et al. Functional and anatomical connectivity predict brain stimulation’s mnemonic effects. Cereb Cortex. 2024;34(1):bhad427. pmid:38041253
- 47. Khambhati AN, Kahn AE, Costantini J, Ezzyat Y, Solomon EA, Gross RE, et al. Functional control of electrophysiological network architecture using direct neurostimulation in humans. Netw Neurosci. 2019;3(3):848–877. pmid:31410383
- 48. Alagapan S, Schmidt SL, Lefebvre J, Hadar E, Shin HW, Fr?hlich F. Modulation of cortical oscillations by low-frequency direct cortical stimulation is state-dependent. PLoS Biol. 2016;14(3):e1002424. pmid:27023427
- 49. Solomon EA, Sperling MR, Sharan AD, Wanda PA, Levy DF, Lyalenko A, et al. Theta-burst stimulation entrains frequency-specific oscillatory responses. Brain Stimul. 2021;14(5):1271–1284. pmid:34428553
- 50. Youssef D, Wittig JH, Jackson S, Inati SK, Zaghloul KA. Neuronal spiking responses to direct electrical microstimulation in the human cortex. J Neurosci. 2023;43(24):4448–4460. pmid:37188513
- 51. Paulk AC, Zelmann R, Crocker B, Widge AS, Dougherty DD, Eskandar EN, et al. Local and distant cortical responses to single pulse intracranial stimulation in the human brain are differentially modulated by specific stimulation parameters. Brain Stimul. 2022;15(2):491–508. pmid:35247646
- 52. McIntyre CC, Grill WM. Extracellular stimulation of central neurons: influence of stimulus waveform and frequency on neuronal output. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88(4):1592–1604. pmid:12364490
- 53. Ezzyat Y, Wanda P, Levy DF, Kadel A, Aka A, Pedisich I, et al. Closed-loop stimulation of temporal cortex rescues functional networks and improves memory. Nat Commun. 2018;9(1):365. pmid:29410414
- 54. Suthana N, Fried I. Deep brain stimulation for enhancement of learning and memory. Neuroimage. 2014;85:996–1002. pmid:23921099
- 55. Winawer J, Parvizi J. Linking electrical stimulation of human primary visual cortex, size of affected cortical area, neuronal responses, and subjective experience. Neuron. 2016;92(6):1213–1219. pmid:27939584
- 56. Borchers S, Himmelback M, Logothetis NK, Karanath H. Direct electrical stimulation of the human cortex—the gold standard for mapping brain functions? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(1):63–70.
- 57. Penfield W, Boldrey E. Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain. 1937;60(4):389–443.
- 58. Penfield W. Memory mechanisms. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1952;67(2):178–198. pmid:14893992
- 59. Jacobs J, Lega B, Anderson C. Explaining how brain stimulation can evoke memories. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012;24(3):553–563. pmid:22098266
- 60. Papadopoulos L, Lynn CW, Battaglia D, Bassett DS. Relations between large-scale brain connectivity and effects of regional stimulation depend on collective dynamical state. PLoS Comput Biol. 2020;16(9):e1008144. pmid:32886673
- 61. Filley CM, Fields RD. White matter and cognition: making the connection. J Neurophysiol. 2016;116(5):2093–2104. pmid:27512019
- 62. Mankin EA, Fried I. Modulation of human memory by deep brain stimulation of the entorhinal-hippocampal circuitry. Neuron. 2020;106(2):218–235. pmid:32325058
- 63. Crocker B, Ostrowski L, Williams ZM, Dougherty DD, Eskandar EN, Widge AS, et al. Local and distant responses to single pulse electrical stimulation reflect different forms of connectivity. Neuroimage. 2021;237:118094. pmid:33940142
- 64. Basu I, Robertson MM, Crocker B, Peled N, Farnes K, Vallejo-Lopez DI, et al. Consistent linear and non-linear responses to invasive electrical brain stimulation across individuals and primate species with implanted electrodes. Brain Stimul. 2019;12(4):877–892. pmid:30904423
- 65. Lujan JL, Chaturvedi A, Choi KS, Holtzheimer PE, Gross RE, Mayberg HS, et al. Tractography-activation models applied to subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation. Brain Stimul. 2013;6(5):737–739. pmid:23602025
- 66. Muldoon SF, Pasqualetti F, Gu S, Cieslak M, Grafton ST, Vettel JM, et al. Stimulation-based control of dynamic brain networks. PLoS Comput Biol. 2016;12(9):e1005076. pmid:27611328
- 67. McIntyre CC, Grill WM, Sherman DL, Thakor NV. Cellular effects of deep brain stimulation: model-based analysis of activation and inhibition. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91(4):1457–1469. pmid:14668299
- 68. Herrington TM, Cheng JJ, Eskandar EN. Mechanisms of deep brain stimulation. J Neurophysiol. 2015;115(1):19–38. pmid:26510756
- 69. Chaturvedi A, Butson CR, Lempka SF, Cooper SE, McIntyre CC. Patient-specific models of deep brain stimulation: influence of field model complexity on neural activation predictions. Brain Stimul. 2010;3(2):65–77. pmid:20607090
- 70.
Butson CR, Cooper SE, Henderson JM, McIntyre CC. Predicting the effects of deep brain stimulation with diffusion tensor based electric field models. In: Medical Image Computing and Computer-Assisted Intervention–MICCAI 2006: 9th International Conference, Copenhagen, Denmark, October 1–6, 2006. Proceedings, Part II 9. Springer; 2006. p. 429–437.
- 71. Manzouri F, Meisel C, Kunz L, Du¨mpelmann M, Stieglitz T, Schulze-Bonhage A. Low-frequency electrical stimulation reduces cortical excitability in the human brain. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2021;31:102778.
- 72. Logothetis NK, Augath M, Murayama Y, Rauch A, Sultan F, Goense J, et al. The effects of electrical microstimulation on cortical signal propagation. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(10):1283–1291. pmid:20818384
- 73. Miller JF, Fried I, Suthana N, Jacobs J. Repeating Spatial Activations in Human Entorhinal Cortex. Curr Biol. 2015. pmid:25843029
- 74. Jacobs J. Hippocampal theta oscillations are slower in humans than in rodents: implications for models of spatial navigation and memory. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014;369(1635):20130304. pmid:24366145
- 75. Battaglia FP, Benchenane K, Sirota A, Pennartz CM, Wiener SI. The hippocampus: hub of brain network communication for memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(7):310–318. pmid:21696996
- 76. Herweg NA, Solomon EA, Kahana MJ. Theta oscillations in human memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2020;24(3):208–227. pmid:32029359
- 77. Klimesch W. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res Rev. 1999;29:169–195. pmid:10209231
- 78. Miller JF, Watrous AJ, Tsitsiklis M, Lee SA, Sheth SA, Schevon CA, et al. Lateralized hippocampal oscillations underlie distinct aspects of human spatial memory and navigation. Nat Commun. 2018;9(1):2423. pmid:29930307
- 79. Solomon EA, Stein JM, Das S, Gorniak R, Sperling MR, Worrell G, et al. Dynamic theta networks in the human medial temporal lobe support episodic memory. Curr Biol. 2019;29(7):1100–1111. pmid:30905609
- 80. Kahana MJ, Ezzyat Y, Wanda PA, Solomon EA, Adamovich-Zeitlin R, Lega BC, et al. Biomarker-guided neuromodulation aids memory in traumatic brain injury. Brain Stimul. 2023;16(4):1086–1093. pmid:37414370
- 81. Burke JF, Zaghloul KA, Jacobs J, Williams RB, Sperling MR, Sharan AD, et al. Synchronous and Asynchronous Theta and Gamma Activity During Episodic Memory Formation. J Neurosci. 2013;33(1):292–304. pmid:23283342
- 82. Sederberg PB, Kahana MJ, Howard MW, Donner EJ, Madsen JR. Theta and gamma oscillations during encoding predict subsequent recall. J Neurosci. 2003;23(34):10809–10814. pmid:14645473
- 83. Gao R, Peterson EJ, Voytek B. Inferring synaptic excitation/inhibition balance from field potentials. Neuroimage. 2017 Sep 1;158:70–8. pmid:28676297
- 84. Kuncel AM, Grill WM. Selection of stimulus parameters for deep brain stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;115(11):2431–2441. pmid:15465430
- 85. Gilbert Z, Mason X, Sebastian R, Tang AM, Del Campo-Vera RM, Chen KH, et al. A review of neurophysiological effects and efficiency of waveform parameters in deep brain stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol. 2023. pmid:37208270
- 86. Grill WM, Mortimer JT. Stability of the input-output properties of chronically implanted multiple contact nerve cuff stimulating electrodes. IEEE Trans Rehabil Eng. 1998;6(4):364–373. pmid:9865883
- 87. Zhang H, Watrous AJ, Patel A, Jacobs J. Theta and alpha oscillations are traveling waves in the human neocortex. Neuron. 2018;98(6):1269–1281.e4. pmid:29887341
- 88. Zelmann R, Paulk AC, Tian F, Villegas GAB, Peralta JD, Crocker B, et al. Differential cortical network engagement during states of un/consciousness in humans. Neuron. 2023;111(21):3479–3495. pmid:37659409
- 89. Histed MH, Bonin V, Reid CR. Direct activation of sparse, distributed populations of cortical neurons by electrical microstimulation. Neuron. 2009;63:508–522. pmid:19709632
- 90. Histed MH, Ni AM, Maunsell JH. Insights into cortical mechanisms of behavior from microstimulation experiments. Prog Neurobiol. 2013;103:115–130. pmid:22307059
- 91. Nowak L, Bullier J. Axons, but not cell bodies, are activated by electrical stimulation in cortical gray matter I. Evidence from chronaxie measurements. Exp Brain Res. 1998;118(4):477–488.
- 92. Nowak L, Bullier J. Axons, but not cell bodies, are activated by electrical stimulation in cortical gray matter II. Evidence from selective inactivation of cell bodies and axon initial segments. Exp Brain Res. 1998;118(4):489–500.
- 93. Brocker DT, Grill WM. Principles of electrical stimulation of neural tissue. Handb Clin Neurol. 2013;116:3–18. pmid:24112880
- 94. Holtzheimer PE, Husain MM, Lisanby SH, Taylor SF, Whitworth LA, McClintock S, et al. Subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression: a multisite, randomised, sham-controlled trial. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2017. pmid:28988904
- 95. Lega B, Jacobs J, Kahana MJ. Human hippocampal theta oscillations and the formation of episodic memories. Hippocampus. 2012;22(4):748–761. pmid:21538660
- 96. Klimesch W, Schimke H, Schwaiger J. Episodic and semantic memory: an analysis in the EEG theta and alpha band. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1994;91:428–441. pmid:7529682
- 97. Chapeton JI, Haque R, Wittig JH Jr, Inati SK, Zaghloul KA. Large-scale communication in the human brain is rhythmically modulated through alpha coherence. Curr Biol. 2019;29(17):2801–2811. pmid:31422882
- 98. Goyal A, Miller J, Qasim SE, Watrous AJ, Zhang H, Stein JM, et al. Functionally distinct high and low theta oscillations in the human hippocampus. Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):2469. pmid:32424312
- 99. Lega B, Burke J, Jacobs J, Kahana MJ. Slow-theta-to-gamma phase–amplitude coupling in human hippocampus supports the formation of new episodic memories. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26(1):268–278. pmid:25316340
- 100. Vaz AP, Inati SK, Brunel N, Zaghloul KA. Coupled ripple oscillations between the medial temporal lobe and neocortex retrieve human memory. Science. 2019 Mar 1;363(6430):975–8. pmid:30819961
- 101. Dickey CW, Verzhbinsky IA, Jiang X, Rosen BQ, Kajfez S, Stedelin B, et al. Widespread ripples synchronize human cortical activity during sleep, waking, and memory recall. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2022 Jul 12;119(28):e2107797119. pmid:35867767
- 102. Begleiter H, Porjesz B. Genetics of human brain oscillations. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006;60(2):162–171. pmid:16540194
- 103. Nikulin VV, Brismar T. Long-range temporal correlations in electroencephalographic oscillations: relation to topography, frequency band, age and gender. Neuroscience. 2005;130(2):549–558. pmid:15664711
- 104. Berthouze L, James LM, Farmer SF. Human EEG shows long-range temporal correlations of oscillation amplitude in Theta, Alpha and Beta bands across a wide age range. Clin Neurophysiol. 2010;121(8):1187–1197. pmid:20346732
- 105. Herrmann C, Demiralp T. Human EEG gamma oscillations in neuropsychiatric disorders. Clin Neurophysiol. 2005;116(12):2719–2733. pmid:16253555
- 106. Cellier D, Riddle J, Petersen I, Hwang K. The development of theta and alpha neural oscillations from ages 3 to 24 years. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;50:100969. pmid:34174512
- 107. Hermiller MS, VanHaerents S, Raij T, Voss JL. Frequency-specific noninvasive modulation of memory retrieval and its relationship with hippocampal network connectivity. Hippocampus. 2019 Jul;29(7):595–609. pmid:30447076
- 108. Mohan UR, Zhang H, Ermentrout B, Jacobs J. The direction of theta and alpha travelling waves modulates human memory processing. Nature Human Behaviour. 2024; p. 1–12. pmid:38459263
- 109. Kleen JK, Chung JE, Sellers KK, Zhou J, Triplett M, Lee K, et al. Bidirectional propagation of low frequency oscillations over the human hippocampal surface. Nat Commun. 2021;12(1):2764. pmid:33980852
- 110. Pang JC, Aquino KM, Oldehinkel M, Robinson PA, Fulcher BD, Breakspear M, et al. Geometric constraints on human brain function. Nature. 2023;618(7965):566–574. pmid:37258669
- 111. Flavell SW, Greenberg ME. Signaling mechanisms linking neuronal activity to gene expression and plasticity of the nervous system. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2008 Jul 21;31(1):563–90. pmid:18558867
- 112. Scangos KW, Khambhati AN, Daly PM, Makhoul GS, Sugrue LP, Zamanian H, et al. Closed-loop neuromodulation in an individual with treatment-resistant depression. Nat Med. 2021;27(10):1696–1700. pmid:34608328