Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Systematic heterogenisation to improve reproducibility in animal studies

A recent study published in PLOS Biology investigated whether the systematic use of multiple experimenters boosts the reproducibility of behavioural assays in mice. These findings open up prospects for solutions to reproducibility issues in animal research.

Reproducibility defies scientists in animal research. The cause of poor reproducibility ranges from technical issues to the unintended consequences of scientific practices (publication bias, perverse incentives, and so on) [1]. Additionally, the same species-specific cognitive and emotional systems that make laboratory animals useful for biological research bring variation to the studies [2]. Biology interacts with the environment adding variability layers to animal experiments [3]. The scientific community has pursued solutions to mitigate inconsistencies and avoid research waste in animal science [49]. A recent article published in PLOS Biology by von Kortzfleisch and colleagues [10] reported a strategy to minimise the incidence of contradictory results in mice behavioural assays, which was partially successful. Building upon this research [10], we conceived some hypothetical studies seeking to investigate systematic heterogenisation approaches to improve reproducibility in animal studies (Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Design of hypothetical studies aiming to investigate systematic heterogenisation approaches to improve reproducibility in animal studies.

Behavioural studies were replicated using single- or multilaboratory configurations, following homogenised and heterogenised designs. The estrous cycle, sex, or behavioural strategy can be used as a heterogenisation factor. Experimental groups in the homogenised design would be homogeneous (e.g., single estrous phase, single sex, and single behavioural strategy), while in the heterogenised design, groups would be heterogeneous (e.g., mixture of estrous phases, mixture of sexes, and mixture of behavioural strategies). The reproducibility of the studies was estimated by comparing the consistency of the outcomes within homogenised or heterogenised designs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001629.g001

Scientists strive to keep their procedures standardised as much as possible, to eliminate variation, obtain accurate results in the long run, and extract the maximum information using a minimum number of animals. A homogenous population of animals (of the same sex, age, strain, etc.) kept under the same controlled conditions (food, water, temperature, humidity, etc.) tested simultaneously across experiments, preferentially by the same experimenters, are examples of standardisation. Nevertheless, biological variation precludes complete homogenisation of animal studies, making repeatability imperfect, even in the same laboratory over time [4]. Furthermore, homogenising laboratory conditions brings consistency in replica studies at the cost of generalisation, yielding results that are often idiosyncratic to a particular laboratory, which may damage reproducibility among laboratories [3,5,6]. In this context, systematic heterogenisation has emerged as a practical alternative to incomplete standardisation and a reasonable solution to the homogenisation–generalisation impasse.

Theoretically, adding a known source of variation to the experimental design boosts reproducibility once the portion of unknown variance in the study diminishes [5,6]. Proof-of-concept studies have found positive effects of systematic heterogenisation on reproducibility in some experimental settings [7,8], but not in others [9,10]. For example, Bodden and colleagues [7] observed in a simulation that the inclusion of 2 different testing times improved the reproducibility between replica experiments in the same laboratory. Likewise, von Kortzfleisch and colleagues [8] observed better reproducibility by splitting the experiment into several “mini-experiments” spread over different time points a few weeks apart in a single laboratory study. Contrastingly, heterogenised designs provide modest improvements in reproducibility across laboratories [9,10]. For example, systematically varying the age and cage enrichment of mice or the number of experimenters was insufficient to overcome the large variation between laboratories for most outcomes [9,10]. Experimenters, and other handlers of experimental animals, seem promising heterogenisation factors since they are distinctive elements in a study contributing to idiosyncratic results obtained in a laboratory. In this framework, results by Kortzfleisch and colleagues [10] showing that “experimenters” explained on average 5% of the experimental variation appear counterintuitive deserving a second look.

Homogeneous design used the same person as experimenter across the experiments in each laboratory (A, B, or C), whereas the heterogenised design included several experimenters within laboratories (A, B, and C) [10]. In the homogenous design, differences between the 2 strains of female mice (C57BL/6J-DBA/2N) varied in direction, magnitude, and statistical significance through laboratories for some outcomes. For example, rearing in a new cage varied from significantly higher in DBA/2N in “Lab A” to significantly higher in C57BL/6J in “Lab C,” while small difference was observed in “Lab B.” Time in the centre of the open field was significantly higher in C57BL/6J than in DBA/2N in laboratories A and C, while a small difference appears in “Lab B.” These discrepant results, leading to opposing conclusions concerning the differences between the 2 strains of mice across laboratories, remained in the heterogenised design. Heterogenised or homogenised designs were also similar in terms of consistency across laboratories, coverage probability, or proportion of accurate results. Systematic heterogenisation of experimenters failed to increase within-study variance above between-laboratory variation for behavioural or physiological outcomes [10].

Despite the large proportion of explained variation by “mice strain,” “laboratory,” or interaction between “strain and laboratory,” residual variance appeared to be a major source of variation in 6 out of 10 outcomes [10]. Most of the variance in the study came from unknown sources, leaving the causes of the variation open to conjecture and subsequent studies. The authors discussed approaches for future studies to identify known and unknown background factors that integrate uncontrolled variation in behavioural studies. In line with their suggestions, we envisioned some designs for studies pursuing to investigate systematic heterogenisation of animal studies in single- or multilaboratory settings (Fig 1). For example, in experiments performed uniquely on females like theirs [10], the estrous cycle could explain a meaningful portion of the unknown variance. Therefore, the estrous cycle, divided into 2 (luteal phase and follicular) or 4 phases (proestrous, estrous, metestrous, and diestrous), could be a heterogenisation factor in future investigations. In this hypothetical study, the outcomes of females synchronised in a particular estrous phase (homogeneous design) were compared to those of females at different estrous phases (heterogeneous design).

Considering behavioural outcomes specially, it would be interesting to know whether the sex of laboratory animals [2] or their behavioural strategies [4] could add a significant amount of variation to these studies. Female readouts in behavioural tests, mainly validated in male animals, may reflect a behavioural strategy to deal with stressors or novelty distinct from that of males [2]. Upcoming studies considering sex or behavioural strategy as a heterogenisation factor could estimate the consistency of outcomes within homogeneous and heterogeneous designs. Sex is often divided into 2 distinct categories (male and female). Behavioural strategies can be divided into artificial categories according to the theoretical background that researchers intend to investigate (e.g., active or passive behaviours). Experimental groups in the homogenised design would be homogeneous (e.g., single sex and single behavioural strategy), while in the heterogenised design, groups would be heterogeneous (e.g., mixture of sexes and mixture of behavioural strategies). Successful systematic heterogenisation would reduce between-study variation, favouring generalisation and keeping within-studies variability under acceptable levels.

Acknowledgments

Text was edited for English language by Editage (www.editage.com). Artwork was prepared by authors using a Senior subscription to “Mind the Graph” online editor available under Creative Commons licence (www.mindthegraph.com).

References

  1. 1. Jarvis MF, Williams M. Irreproducibility in preclinical biomedical research: perceptions, uncertainties, and knowledge gaps. Trends Pharmacol Res. 2015;37:290–302.
  2. 2. Colom-Lapetina J, Li AJ, Pelegrina-Perez TC, Shansky RM. Behavioral diversity across classic rodent models is sex-dependent. Front Behav Neurosci. 2019;13:45. Available from: pmid:30894806
  3. 3. Crabbe JC, Wahlsten D, Dudek BC. Genetics of mouse behavior: interactions with laboratory environment. Science. 1999 Jun 4;284(5420):1670–2. pmid:10356397.
  4. 4. Suman PR, Zerbinatti N, Theindl LC, Domingues K, Lino de Oliveira C. Failure to detect the action of antidepressants in the forced swim test in Swiss mice. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 2018 Jun;30(3):158–67. Epub 2017 Dec 5. pmid:29202894.
  5. 5. Würbel H. Behaviour and the standardization fallacy. Nat Genet. 2000;26:263. pmid:11062457
  6. 6. Josef van der Staay F, Arndt SS, Nordquist RE. The standardization–generalization dilemma: a way out. Genes Brain Behav. 2010;9:849–55. pmid:20662940
  7. 7. Bodden C, von Kortzfleisch VT, Karwinkel F, Kaiser S, Sachser N, Richter SH. Heterogenising study samples across testing time improves reproducibility of behavioural data. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):8247. pmid:31160667
  8. 8. von Kortzfleisch VT, Karp NA, Palme R, Kaiser S, Sachser N, Richter SH. Improving reproducibility in animal research by splitting the study population into several ‘mini-experiments’. Sci Rep. 2020;10:16579. pmid:33024165
  9. 9. Richter SH, Garner JP, Zipser B, Lewejohann L, Sachser N, Touma C, et al. Effect of Population Heterogenization on the Reproducibility of Mouse Behavior: A Multi-Laboratory Study. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(1):e16461. pmid:21305027
  10. 10. von Kortzfleisch VT, Ambré O, Karp NA, Meyer N, Novak J, Palme R, et al. Do multiple experimenters improve the reproducibility of animal studies? PLoS Biol.