Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Parental breeding age effects on descendants’ longevity interact over 2 generations in matrilines and patrilines

  • Zachariah Wylde ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    wyldescience@gmail.com

    Affiliation Evolution & Ecology Research Centre, School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

  • Foteini Spagopoulou,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Uppsala Centre for Evolution and Genomics, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden

  • Amy K. Hooper,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Evolution & Ecology Research Centre, School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

  • Alexei A. Maklakov,

    Roles Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Uppsala Centre for Evolution and Genomics, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden, School of Biological Sciences, University of East Anglia, Norwich Research Park, Norwich, United Kingdom

  • Russell Bonduriansky

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Evolution & Ecology Research Centre, School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

Abstract

Individuals within populations vary enormously in mortality risk and longevity, but the causes of this variation remain poorly understood. A potentially important and phylogenetically widespread source of such variation is maternal age at breeding, which typically has negative effects on offspring longevity. Here, we show that paternal age can affect offspring longevity as strongly as maternal age does and that breeding age effects can interact over 2 generations in both matrilines and patrilines. We manipulated maternal and paternal ages at breeding over 2 generations in the neriid fly Telostylinus angusticollis. To determine whether breeding age effects can be modulated by the environment, we also manipulated larval diet and male competitive environment in the first generation. We found separate and interactive effects of parental and grand-parental ages at breeding on descendants’ mortality rate and life span in both matrilines and patrilines. These breeding age effects were not modulated by grand-parental larval diet quality or competitive environment. Our findings suggest that variation in maternal and paternal ages at breeding could contribute substantially to intrapopulation variation in mortality and longevity.

Introduction

In many species, offspring of older mothers have a reduced mean life span, a phenomenon known as the ‘Lansing’ effect [1] or maternal age effect. Maternal age effects have been observed in a great variety of organisms, including yeast, plants, nematodes, rotifers, insects, birds, and mammals [26]. Although most studies have focused on offspring life span, some studies show that maternal age at breeding can also affect offspring juvenile viability and adult reproductive performance [711]. A few studies have also reported effects of paternal age at breeding on offspring performance [2,5,6]. Parental age effects represent a potentially important source of variation in individual mortality risk, longevity, and fitness, but many aspects of these effects remain poorly understood.

Parental age effects could be caused by the accumulation of mutations in the germline [12]. In humans, mutations accumulate at a constant rate in the male germline and at an accelerating rate in the female germline [13]. Parental age effects could also be mediated by nongenetic factors. Recent studies on mice, monkeys, and humans have shown that patterns of DNA methylation across the genome change with age—a pattern known as the ‘epigenetic clock’ [1418], and some of these altered epigenetic factors could be transmitted across generations [1923]. Older parents could also transmit altered microRNAs or other factors such as proteins to offspring via the gametes [24,25]. For example, in mice, the transmission of proteins in the egg cytoplasm is thought to mediate maternal age effects on offspring [26], and more recent evidence suggests a role for sperm microRNAs in paternal effects [2731]. Although such effects are best characterised in mammals, age-related changes in gamete quality also occur in arthropods, and such effects could contribute to parental age effects. For example, in the parasitoid wasp Eupelmus vuilletti, increasing maternal age is associated with reduced egg size and altered egg composition [32]. Likewise, in Daphnia pulex, maternal age is associated with changes in egg provisioning, with effects on offspring longevity and life history [33]. The transmission of dysregulated epigenetic or cytoplasmic factors from old-breeding parents to their offspring could mediate parental age effects in many species [34].

Maternal and paternal effects are likely to be mediated by different factors and can have distinct effects on offspring [35,36]. However, relatively few studies have tested experimentally for effects of paternal age at breeding, and even fewer studies have directly compared the effects of maternal and paternal age at breeding on offspring performance. Experimental evidence in mice shows that offspring of older fathers have a reduced life span and suggests that this effect could be mediated by epigenetic (DNA methylation) changes within sperm of gene promoters involved in evolutionarily conserved pathways of life span regulation [37]. In Drosophila melanogaster, both maternal and paternal age effects have been reported [5]. Similar effects may occur in other species (including humans), although much of the evidence is correlational. For example, in the wandering albatross, paternal but not maternal age affected juvenile survival of offspring [11]. A recent long-term study on a natural population of house sparrows showed that paternal breeding has a similar effect size on life span and reproductive success to female breeding age and that these effects are transferred to offspring in a sex-specific manner [6]. In humans, advanced paternal age at breeding is associated with reduced sperm quality and testicular functions, and such effects appear to be mediated by both epigenetic changes and genetic mutations [38]. Advanced paternal age is also associated with reduced performance on standardised tests in children, whereas the effect of maternal age was more complex [39]. Likewise, parental age, and the difference between maternal and paternal ages, are associated with risk of autism spectrum disorder [40].

Parental age effects could interact with environmental factors such as diet and stress [8,41]. For example, a restricted maternal diet mitigated the effects of advanced maternal age at breeding on offspring longevity in rotifers [42]. In mice, a fat-restricted maternal diet did not influence maternal age effects [16], but maternal age effects were mitigated by rapamycin [43]. In the butterfly Pieris brassicae, effects of parental age at breeding on offspring performance were influenced by stress [2]. However, the role of environment in modulating effects of parental age remains largely unexplored.

Perhaps the most important gap in understanding of parental age effects is the potential for such effects to accumulate and interact over multiple generations. In Drospohila serrata, offspring juvenile viability decreased with increasing maternal and grand-maternal ages at breeding [8], but it remains unclear whether such cumulative effects can occur in partrilines or in other species. If such multigenerational effects are widespread, they could make an important contribution to variation in mortality and longevity and, potentially, play a role in the evolution of ageing [5,34].

Here, we examined 3 aspects of parental age effects that have received little attention in previous research by (1) comparing the effects of both male and female age at breeding on descendants, (2) testing for interactions of age at breeding with key environmental factors (diet and competitive environment), and (3) investigating the potential for effects of age at breeding to accumulate over generations. We addressed these questions in the neriid fly Telostylinus angusticollis (Enderlein), a species endemic to New South Wales and Southern Queensland, Australia. Both larval and adult nutrition affect mortality rate and life span in this species [44,45]. Larval access to dietary protein has a nonlinear effect on adult longevity [44], but high overall macronutrient (protein and carbohydrate) abundance at the larval stage accelerates larval growth and development while also promoting rapid ageing in males [46,47]. Adult protein restriction extends life [45] and can interact with larval diet to influence reproductive ageing [48]. However, effects of parental age at breeding on offspring performance have not been investigated previously in this species.

We reared individuals of the grand-parental (F1) generation on either a high-nutrient or low-nutrient larval diet and then allowed adult females and males from these larval diet treatments to breed at 15 and 35 days of age. Neriid males fight other males for access to territories and females, and such male-male interactions could affect male ageing [47]. We therefore investigated the potential for male-male interactions to affect paternal age effects by manipulating F1 male competitive environment. Female and male offspring (F2) were reared on a standard larval diet (with a nutrient concentration intermediate between the high-nutrient and low-nutrient diets) and then allowed to breed at 15-day age intervals between ages 15 and 60 days. We quantified the adult longevity of grand-offspring (F3) and used these data to test for effects of grand-parental ages at breeding, grand-parental environment, and parental ages at breeding on grand-offspring life span, mortality rate, and actuarial ageing rate.

Results

Life span

F3 individuals (grand-offspring) from both matrilines and patrilines suffered similar negative effects of F1 (grand-parental) and F2 (parental) ages at breeding on life span (Table 1; Figs 1 and 2). F3 individuals descended from old-breeding grandmothers and grandfathers had 37.8% and 39.8% shorter lifespans, respectively, than F3 individuals descended from young-breeding grandmothers and grandfathers. There was no effect of F1 larval diet on F3 life span in either matrilines or patrilines, nor an F1 larval diet × F1 age interaction. There were also no main or interactive effects of F1 male competitive environment within patrilines (S3 Table). However, we detected an F1 × F2 age interaction within both matrilines and patrilines, whereby the negative effect of F1 age at breeding was diminished as F2 age at breeding increased (Fig 2). Within matrilines, we also detected an interaction of F1 age at breeding and F3 sex, whereby the negative effect of grandmothers’ age at breeding was stronger for F3 males than for F3 females. In patrilines, we also detected an F2 age × F2 sex interaction, such that F3 life span declined more steeply with increasing paternal (F2 male) age than with increasing maternal (F2 female) age. S1 Fig shows the combined effects of F1 and F2 breeding ages, F1 competitive environment (patrilines only), and F1 larval diet on F3 life span. Results were qualitatively similar for models including development time and body size (S4 Table). Overall, by comparison with previously published life span estimates for this species when maintained as individually housed virgin adults (e.g., male median = 37 d, female median = 36 d; [49]), the median lifespans of F3 individuals descended from young-breeding parents and grandparents are similar (male median = 25, female median = 36), whereas the median lifespans of F3 individuals descended from old-breeding parents and grandparents are substantially lower (male median = 10, female median = 15).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Effects of grand-parental (F1) breeding age and larval diet on grand-offspring (F3) life span in patrilines and matrilines.

The violin plot outline illustrates kernel probability density (width represents proportion of data located there). Within violin plots are box plots with median and interquartile range to illustrate data distribution. Underlying data can be found in the Dryad Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Interaction between effects of grand-parental and parental breeding ages on grand-offspring life span in patrilines and matrilines.

Black lines represent the lifespans of F3 descendants of F1 individuals paired at 15 days of age, and red lines represent the lifespans of F3 descendants of F1 individuals paired at 35 days of age. Bars represent SEM. Underlying data can be found in the Dryad Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw. F1, grand-parental generation; F3, grand-offspring.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.g002

thumbnail
Table 1. Tests of effects based on linear mixed models of F3 life span for patrilines and matrilines.

Significant effects are highlighted in bold. Negative effects of F1 and F2 age indicate that old grandparents and parents produced F3 individuals with reduced lifespans, negative effects of larval diet indicate that low-nutrient larval diet has a negative effect on F3 life span, and negative effects of sex indicate that the life span of male descendants was lower than that of females. Effect sizes represent marginal R2. Conditional whole-model R2 values were 47.72% for the patriline model and 54.78% for the matriline model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.t001

Mortality rate

Consistent with our results for life span, we found that baseline mortality rate (Gompertz bo parameter) of F3 individuals from both matrilines and patrilines was affected positively and similarly by F1 age at breeding but not affected by F1 larval diet (Fig 3). Individuals descended from grandparents that bred at age 35 d had higher baseline mortality rates, regardless of F1 larval diet treatment (High condition Old [HO]; Low condition Old [LO]; patrilines b0 HO = −3.5, b0 LO = −3.6; matrilines b0 HO = −3.8, b0 LO = −3.7) than individuals descended from grandparents that bred at age 15 d (High condition Young [HY]; Low condition Young [LY]; patrilines b0 HY = −4.4, b0 LY = −4.2; matrilines b0 HY = −4.6, b0 LY = −4.4). An effect of F1 age at breeding on the baseline mortality rate was supported by Kullback-Leibler discrepancy calibration (KLDC) values, which exceeded 0.98 for all comparisons of b0 parameters for F3 descendants of young-breeding versus old-breeding F1 individuals within and across larval diet treatments in both patrilines and matrilines (S6 and S8 Tables).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Effects of grand-parental larval diet and breeding age on estimated age-specific survival and mortality rates for grand-offspring of patrilines and matrilines as fitted by the simple Gompertz mortality model.

b0 is the baseline mortality rate (scale) parameter, and b1 is the rate of actuarial ageing (shape) parameter. Posterior distributions are shown for b0 and b1 in the left panels. Panels on the right illustrate how these estimates translate to survival and mortality rates over time. The shaded areas in the survival plots represent 95% confidence intervals. Underlying data can be found in the Dryad Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw. F1, grand-parental generation; F3, grand-offspring.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.g003

Grand-parental and parental breeding ages interacted in their effects on F3 baseline mortality rates (b0), particularly within patrilines (Fig 4). F3 individuals descended from young grandparents (F1) experienced increasingly high baseline mortality as parental (F2) age at breeding increased, and this effect was especially strong in patrilines (S10 and S11 Tables). By contrast, for F3 individuals descended from old-breeding grandparents, there were no consistent effects of parental age at breeding.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Effects of F1 breeding age and F2 breeding age on estimated age-specific survival and mortality rates for grand-offspring of patrilines and matrilines as fitted by the simple Gompertz mortality model.

b0 is the baseline mortality rate (scale) parameter, and b1 is the rate of actuarial ageing (shape) parameter. Posterior distributions are shown for b0 and b1 in the left panels. Panels on the right illustrate how these estimates translate to survival and mortality rates over time. Shaded areas in the survival plots represent 95% confidence intervals. Underlying data can be found in the Dryad Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw. F1, grand-parental generation; F2, female and male offspring; F3, grand-offspring.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.g004

For actuarial ageing rates (Gompertz b1 parameter), evidence of treatment effects was weaker, and patterns were less consistent. Individuals descended from grandparents that bred at age 35 days had similar actuarial ageing rates, regardless of F1 larval diet treatment (patrilines b1 HO = 0.032, b1 LO = 0.036; matrilines b1 HO = 0.031, b1 LO = 0.029), to individuals descended from grandparents that bred at age 15 d (patrilines b1 HY = 0.032, b1 LY = 0.029; matrilines b1 HY = 0.035, b1 LY = 0.034). In matrilines, KLDC values were <0.85 for all comparisons of b1 parameters for F3 descendants of young-breeding versus old-breeding F1 females (S8 Table). In patrilines, KLDC values marginally exceeded 0.85 for some comparisons of F3 descendants of young-breeding versus old-breeding F1 males within and across larval diet treatments, but the effect of F1 age at breeding on b1 was not consistent across larval diet treatments (S7 Table). There was little evidence that grand-parental and parental ages at breeding interacted in their effects on actuarial ageing rate (b1) in either matrilines or patrilines (Fig 4). Wider confidence limits for life span and age-dependent mortality rates for descendants of old-breeding F1 males reflect reduced sample size resulting from mortality between 15 and 35 days of age. For all other KLDC values of group comparisons refer to S2S5 Figs and S9S13 Tables.

Discussion

A recent model suggests that negative effects of parental age on offspring performance can readily evolve [50], but many aspects of such effects have received little attention in empirical research. Our results show that paternal age effects can be similar in magnitude to maternal age effects. The magnitude of the grand-maternal and grand-paternal effects detected in our study is comparable to longevity changes observed in multigenerational selection experiments in Drosophila melanogaster [51,52]. Our mortality rate analyses suggest that decreased life span of grand-offspring of older grandparents and parents results largely from elevated baseline mortality rather than from a higher rate of increase in mortality rate with age (i.e., actuarial ageing). Actuarial ageing could result from the accumulation of somatic damage with age [53]. Previous studies of T. angusticollis showed that males reared on a high-nutrient larval diet accumulated damage more rapidly with age than males reared on a low-nutrient larval diet [46] and exhibited more rapid actuarial and reproductive ageing [47]. Here, we show that declining offspring longevity and increasing offspring mortality rate represent additional manifestations of ageing in T. angusticollis males and females. However, breeding age effects on offspring life span and mortality were unaffected by grand-parental larval diet. Interestingly, although we found largely similar effects of grand-paternal versus grand-maternal and paternal versus maternal ages at breeding on offspring baseline mortality rate, we also found some evidence of effects on actuarial ageing rate in patrilines but not in matrilines. These differences suggest that male and female breeding age effects could be mediated by different factors and could have different effects on offspring life history.

Our findings suggest that the effect of ancestors’ age at breeding could contribute substantially to within-population variation in longevity. However, the importance of these effects in natural populations remains unclear. T. angusticollis has a much shorter mean life span in the wild than in the laboratory, and wild males also exhibit very rapid actuarial ageing [49]. The short average life span and rapid ageing observed in natural populations of this species is consistent with findings for other insects in the wild [5456]. Given the very high background mortality rate experienced by T. angusticollis in the wild, it is possible that longevity of flies in natural populations is not strongly affected by parental age effects. However, it is also possible that maternal and paternal age effects are accelerated along with the overall rate of ageing in wild populations as a result of environmental stresses such as parasites and temperature fluctuations. If so, then parental age effects could have a substantial effect on fitness in natural populations, despite a short life expectancy. It is also possible that offspring of old-breeding parents or grandparents might respond by increasing their early-life reproductive effort, thereby partly mitigating the effects of reduced life span. For example, in Daphnia pulex, older mothers produce offspring with shortened life spans but these offspring achieve increased early-life reproductive output [33]. We found little evidence that age at breeding effects on life span were mediated by body size or development time, because inclusion of these traits as covariates in life span models did not qualitatively alter the results.

The grand-parental and parental age effects that we observed could be mediated by the accumulation of germline mutations with age. Because male and female germline cells develop differently in animals, including flies [5759], the male and female germlines could accumulate mutations at different rates [60,61]. In particular, the rate of age-dependent mutation accumulation is likely to reflect the number of germline cell divisions, and it has long been thought that males transmit more germline mutations because the male germline undergoes a larger number of cell divisions [62]. Interestingly, however, in Drosophila, the number of germline cell divisions is larger in females than in males at young ages but larger in males than in females at old ages [63]. This suggests that mutation-mediated maternal and paternal age effects could differ in relative magnitudes as a function of male and female age. If T. angusticollis exhibits a similar pattern of germline cell division to Drosophila, this could explain the somewhat stronger negative effect of grand-paternal age at breeding on grand-offspring life span, relative to the effect of grand-maternal age at breeding (Fig 1).

The rate of cell proliferation in the female germline also increases on a protein-rich diet in D. melanogaster [64], and dietary protein strongly stimulates female fecundity in T. angusticollis as well [45]. A protein-rich adult diet could therefore be expected to accentuate negative maternal breeding age effects on offspring performance and could also accentuate paternal breeding age effects if cell division in the male germline is also enhanced on a high-protein diet. Germline mutation rate can also be affected by investment in DNA repair, and D. melanogaster reared on low-nutrient food as larvae have lower rates of repair that result in increased germline mutation rate [65]. However, we found little evidence of effects of F1 larval diet on grand-offspring mortality and survival (Figs 2 and 4). Likewise, we did not detect an effect of male competitive environment (opportunity for combat interactions) or any interaction between this treatment and grand-paternal breeding age. This finding is consistent with the lack of any effect of male combat on male reproductive ageing [47] and suggests that agonistic interactions with other males do not affect the maintenance of the male germline.

A different (but nonexclusive) explanation for our findings is age-dependent transmission of epigenetic or cytoplasmic factors through the female and male germlines. DNA (cytosine) methylation contributes to the regulation of gene expression in many organisms [66], but flies have little cytosine methylation and its role in this group remains unclear [6770]. In D. melanogaster, DNA methylation is largely limited to the early stages of embryogenesis [71,72], but 2 studies suggest that DNA methylation can also persist in the germline [73,74]. In mammals, DNA methylation patterns undergo changes with age throughout the genome [75,76]. Such age-related changes in methylation (known as the ‘epigenetic clock’) could mediate parental age effects, because some DNA methylation patterns can be transmitted to offspring via both sperm and eggs (for a review, see the work by Ho and Burggren [77]). It is not known whether a DNA methylation ‘clock’ also occurs in flies.

Other epigenetic or cytoplasmic factors that change with age could also mediate the observed age-at-breeding effects. There is evidence of age-related cellular changes in the male and female germline. For example, as Drosophila males age, germline stem cells (GSCs) divide less frequently because of misorientation of centromeres [78]. Similarly, GSC division in female Drosophila declines with age, and this is accompanied by an increased rate of cell death in developing eggs [79]. RNA-mediated transmission of shortened telomeres could mediate breeding age effects in flies and other animals. Shortened telomeres are associated with cellular senescence in some taxa [80], and telomere length can be affected by noncoding telomeric repeat-containing RNAs (TERRA), which are transcriptionally active in Drosophila [81]. TERRAs are present in animal (including human) oocytes [82], and in female Drosophila, they affect blastoderm formation [83]. Other types of noncoding RNAs could also be involved. Flies maintain chromosome length through retrotranscription [84], which requires complex and specific chromatin structures [85]. Retrotransposon proliferation can promote mutagenesis [86]. RNA interference (RNAi) mechanisms control the silencing of retrotransposons in germline cells [87,88], and parental age effects could be mediated by the transmission of such small noncoding RNAs, with effects on chromatin states and gene expression in embryos [23]. Early development in Drosophila is thought to be governed by maternally inherited RNAs and proteins [89], but less is known about the effects of male-derived RNAs on offspring development. Although T. angusticollis males do not transmit nutritional nuptial gifts during copulation [90], males probably transfer a variety of microRNAs in the ejaculate. The complement of seminal and egg microRNAs could change with male and female age and affect embryo development.

Another possibility is that flies change their investment in gametes in response to the age or mating experience of their partner. A female may decrease investment per offspring when mated to an older male, whereas a male may reduce the quality or quantity of accessory gland proteins or sperm produced when mated with an older female, resulting in negative effects of parental age on offspring performance. Such responses to mate quality have been reported in Drosophila and other insects [9194] and might be mediated through cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs) that are known to change with age in flies [95,96]. In our experiment, increasing age was also associated with increasing mating experience. Individuals of both sexes might alter their investment in offspring based on their partner’s mating experience, because previously mated males might transfer smaller or lower-quality ejaculates. For example, male mating experience was negatively correlated to nuptial gift quality and sperm number in a bush cricket [97], and female reproductive output was lower when mated with sexually experienced males than when mating with virgin males across 25 species of Lepioptera [98]. Although T. angusticollis males appear to be able to replenish their ejaculate reserves very rapidly, the effects of age and mating experience cannot be decoupled statistically in our data and require further investigation.

We quantified effects of ancestors’ age at breeding in flies (F3) maintained as virgins in individual containers and supplied with ad libitum food and water. Housing T. angusticollis individuals in isolation and as virgins tends to increase their longevity (e.g., the work by Adler and Bonduriansky [99]), whereas ad libitum availability of dietary protein tends to reduce adult longevity [45]. Although our results suggest that larval diet and male competitive environment do not interact strongly with breeding age in affecting longevity of descendants, further work is required to determine whether housing, reproduction, or adult diet of descendants can interact with effects of parental and grand-parental ages at breeding.

Some individuals failed to produce viable offspring or did not survive to breed at older ages, and we cannot exclude the possibility that differential mortality or reproductive success biased the composition of our treatment groups. In particular, because T. angusticollis males reared on a nutrient-rich larval diet tend to exhibit an elevated adult mortality rate relative to males reared on a nutrient-poor larval diet [47], fewer F1 focal males from the rich-diet treatment survived to breed at age 35 days, resulting in a smaller sample size for that treatment combination. This resulted in somewhat wider confidence limits for life span and actuarial ageing rate for the F3 descendants of those males, but we cannot exclude the possibility that the elevated F1 mortality was also associated with differential natural selection on males reared on nutrient-rich versus nutrient-poor larval diets.

The interactive effects of grand-parental and parental ages at breeding that we observed suggest that the factors mediating these effects are stable across at least 2 generations. Priest and colleagues [5] suggested that parental age effects could play a role in the evolution of ageing by contributing to age-related decline in performance and generating selection for earlier reproduction. Bonduriansky and Day [34] argued that if such effects can accumulate over generations, an environmental change that brings about delayed breeding or causes a more rapid decline in offspring performance with parental age could result in a progressive decline in performance over several generations, resulting in phenotypic changes that resemble the evolution of accelerated ageing. Our results support these ideas by providing experimental evidence that parental age effects can have large effects on descendants’ longevity, can occur in both matrilines and patrilines and across contrasting environments, and can be transmitted over at least 2 generations. Further work is needed to understand the context-dependence and fitness consequences of such effects in natural populations.

Materials and methods

Source of experimental flies

Experiments were performed using a lab-reared stock of T. angusticollis that originated from individuals collected from Fred Hollows Reserve, Randwick, NSW, Australia (33°54′44.04″S 151°14′52.14″E). This stock was maintained as a large, outbred population with overlapping generations and periodically supplemented with wild-caught individuals from the same source population to maintain genetic diversity.

Larval rearing and diet manipulation

All larvae were reared in climate chambers at 25° C ± 2°C with a 12:12 photoperiod and moistened with deionised water every 2 days. We manipulated the quantity of resources available to larvae during development by rearing flies on either a high-nutrient, standard-nutrient, or low-nutrient larval diet. Diets were based on the work by Sentinella and colleagues [100] and were selected to generate considerable body size differences between treatment groups while minimising larval mortality and to preserve the protein to carbohydrate ratio of approximately 1:3 across diets. All diets consisted of a base of 170 g of cocopeat moistened with 600 mL of reverse osmosis-purified water. The high-nutrient larval diet consisted of 32.8 g of protein (Nature’s Way soy protein isolate; Pharm-a-Care, Warriewood, Australia) and 89 g of brown sugar (Woolworths Essentials Bonsucro brand); the standard larval diet consisted of 10.9 g of protein and 29.7 g sugar; the low-nutrient larval diet consisted of 5.5 g of protein and 14.8 g sugar. These nutrients were mixed into the cocopeat and water using a hand-held blender and frozen at −20°C until the day of use. Males and females of the F1 generation were reared on either a high- or low-nutrient larval diet and standardised for larval density (40 eggs per 200 g of larval food). All larvae of the F2 and F3 generations were reared on a standard larval diet (see the work by Adler and colleagues [45] for further details). Following the first adult emergence from each larval container, adult flies were collected for 10 days, and the rest were discarded.

F1 adult housing and competitive environment

F1 males were subjected to a “low” or “high” competition environment. Each adult focal male was paired with a competitor male reared on a standard larval diet inside an enclosure containing a petri dish with larval medium (which stimulates territory defence behaviours in T. angusticollis males). Males in the “high” competition environment were able to move freely around the arena and engage in combat interactions with the competitor male, whereas males in the “low” competition environment were separated by mesh so that they could perceive the competitor’s chemical and perhaps visual cues but have no physical contact. All focal F1 females were kept in a similar housing as the “low” competitive environment males where each focal female was paired with a female reared on a standard larval diet. All housing containers had a layer of moistened cocopeat on the bottom, and dishes of oviposition medium (on which adult flies also feed) to stimulate ovary development in females.

F1 adult male and female age-at-breeding manipulation

The age at breeding was manipulated for F1 focal individuals by pairing at ‘young’ (15 ± 1 days old) and ‘old’ (35 ± 1 days old) ages with an opposite-sex individual reared on the standard larval diet and standardised for age (15 ± 1 days old). These ages were selected because, in T. angusticollis, adults become fully reproductively mature by 10 to 15 days of age under laboratory conditions, whereas median longevity of individually housed, captive flies is 37 days for males and 36 days for females, and mortality rate begins to increase rapidly in both sexes after 30 days of age [49]. Thus, at 15 days old, both sexes are considered to be at their prime, whereas, at 35 days old, both sexes are well past their prime. Each focal F1 adult was thus paired twice, each time with a different mate, to produce broods of F2 offspring at ‘young’ and ‘old’ ages (Fig 5). Mating pairs were kept in 60 mL glass vials under standardised light and temperature (approximately 23°C) for 1 hour, and females were then placed into 250 mL enclosures with mesh coverings and a moistened cocopeat substrate and were allowed to oviposit for 96 h into a petri dish containing oviposition medium. After 48 h, a fresh oviposition dish was provided. A total of 20 eggs were sampled randomly from each female and transferred to 100 g of standard larval medium.

thumbnail
Fig 5.

Experimental design: Patrilines (a) consist of descendants of F1 males, whereas matrilines (b) consist of descendants of F1 females. F1 individuals were reared on either a high- or low-nutrient larval diet. Adult F1 males were also maintained in high- or low-competition social environments (S4 Table). F1 males and females were then mated at 15 days or 35 days of age, and all offspring (F2) were reared on a standard larval diet. From each F1 breeding bout, 1 male and 1 female of the F2 generation were paired with a standard mate at 15-day intervals up to 60 days of age. Grand-offspring (F3) were all reared on standard larval diet and housed individually until death. Sample sizes (number of F1 or F2 focal individuals that produced offspring and number of F3 individuals for which longevity was quantified) are shown for each combination of treatment and sex. F1, grand-parental generation; F2, female and male offspring; F3, grand-offspring.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.g005

F2 adult male and female age-at-breeding manipulation

One F2 male and one F2 female focal individual were randomly sampled for breeding from each F1 larval container. Thus, where possible, each F1 focal individual contributed one F2 offspring of each sex from a reproductive bout at 15 days of age and one F2 offspring of each sex from a reproductive bout at 35 days of age. Each F2 focal individual was paired with a partner of the opposite sex (raised on a standard diet and 15 ± 1 days old on the day of pairing) at 4 ages (where possible): 15 d, 30 d, 45 d, and 60 d. The flies were allowed 1 hour to mate, after which eggs were collected from each female and maintained as described above.

F3 rearing and quantification of life span

From each reproductive bout of each F2 individual, one male and female of the F3 generation were obtained (where possible) and housed individually in a 120 mL container fitted with a feeding tube containing a sugar-yeast mixture and drinking tube containing water (with both food and water provided ad libitum), and a substrate of moistened cocopeat to maintain humidity. F3 housing containers were maintained at ambient room temperature (23°C ± 4°C) and checked daily for mortality until all individuals had died. To minimise spatial effects, containers were randomly moved to different locations every 2 days.

For all focal individuals, development time and body size were also recorded to investigate their possible roles in mediating treatment effects on life span and mortality rate (refer to S1 and S2 Tables for summary statistics). All F1 and F2 focal individuals were frozen at −20°C after their final reproductive bout (or prior natural death before day 60), and all F3 individuals were frozen after natural death. For all focal F1, F2, and F3 individuals, egg to adult development time was recorded as time from oviposition to adult emergence in days (± 1 day). Thorax length is a reliable proxy for body size in this species [101] and was measured for each F1, F2, and F3 focal individual from images taken using a Leica MS5 stereoscope equipped with a Leica DFC420 digital microscope camera. Measurements were made using FIJI open source software [102].

Life span analysis

We investigated treatment effects on F3 life span using R 3.3.2 [103] and the package “lme4” [104]. These analyses facilitate hypothesis testing by making it possible to test interactions within mixed-effects models. Because the life span of every individual was known, no censoring was required. Gaussian linear mixed models (LMM) were used, and all analyses were carried out separately for matrilines (i.e., descendants of F1 females) and patrilines (i.e., descendants of F1 males). Any effects of F1 age at breeding, larval diet, or male competitive environment therefore represent grand-maternal effects within matrilines and grand-paternal effects within patrilines. Within both matrilines and patrilines, we tested for effects of F2 age at breeding for both female parents (maternal age effects) and male parents (paternal age effects) and compared effects on F3 males and females (i.e., effect of F3 sex). For the patriline data set, F1 male competitive environment and its two-way interactions were tested by a likelihood ratio test (LRT) and were found to have no effect on any dependent variables. The patriline models were then refitted without F1 competitive environment. This resulted in identical model structure for patrilines and matrilines, facilitating comparison of matrilineal and patrilineal results. Qualitatively identical results are obtained without F1 competitive environment as a predictor in the patriline models (S3 Table).

Our final models thus included F1 (grand-parental) larval diet and age at breeding, F2 parental age at breeding, F2 sex and F3 sex as fixed effects. F2 breeding age was fitted as a continuous predictor, whereas the other factors were fitted as categorical predictors. F1 and F2 individual ID, replicate F1 larval container, and emergence date were included as random effects. We also fitted models with F1, F2, and F3 body sizes and development times as fixed covariates in order to determine whether these traits mediate treatment effects on F3 life span (S4 Table). Treatment effects on F3 body size and development time were also tested using similar models to those described above, and results of those analyses are shown in S6 and S7 Figs, S14 and S15 Tables, and discussed in S1 Text. Estimates and F-ratios were obtained using the packages “lme4” [104] and “lmerTest” [105], whereas p-values were obtained via “Type 3” likelihood ratio tests using the package “car”. To examine the relative effect size of each predictor, we also quantified marginal R2, which is variance explained by fixed factors, and conditional whole model R2 that includes variation explained by random factors from our LMM using the methods developed in [106].

Mortality rate analysis

To gain a better understanding of treatment effects on F3 life span, we also investigated effects on F3 mortality rates. We used the Bayesian Survival Trajectory Analysis, implemented with the package “BaSTA” [107]. BaSTA utilises a Bayesian approach based on Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) estimation of age-specific mortality rate distributions. Our data are uncensored, and the date of adult emergence is known for all individuals, allowing us to obtain reliable population estimates of the mortality distribution [108]. In order to find the mortality rate distribution that best fits our data, we first used the package “flexsurv” [109] on a combined data set comprising both patrilines and matrilines. We compared the simple and Makeham versions of the Gompertz and Weibull models, as well as the logistic and exponential models, using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC). This analysis showed that a simple Gompertz distribution provided the best fit to our data (S5 Table). Mortality rate was therefore modeled as

Survival probability was modeled as

The Gompertz mortality rate function includes a scale parameter, b0 (often called the “baseline mortality rate”), and a shape parameter, b1, that describes the dependency of mortality on age (x) and is often interpreted as the rate of actuarial ageing, which reflects the rate of increase in mortality rate with age [110113].

We used BaSTA to estimate and compare parameters of the simple Gompertz model for our experimental treatment groups. We performed 4 parallel BaSTA simulations, each proceeding for 2,200,000 iterations, with a burn-in of 200,000 chains, and took an MCMC chain sample every 4,000 iterations. Our models generated parameter estimates that converged with low serial autocorrelations (<5%) and robust posterior distributions of bo and b1 (N = 2,000), allowing for robust comparisons between treatment groups.

We compared parameter estimates for various treatment groups based on differences between their posterior distributions, using the KLDC implemented in BaSTA. Values near 0.5 suggest nominal differences between distributions, whereas values close to 1 indicate a sizeable divergence. KLDC thresholds can vary depending on interpretation and can range between 0.65 and 1 [114116]. We considered a relatively conservative KLDC value >0.85 to indicate a difference between the posterior distributions of the treatment groups being compared. We report Gompertz bo parameter estimates on a log scale and refer to F1 treatment combinations as HO, HY, LO, and LY.

Data are deposited in the Dryad repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw [117].

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Combined effects of F1, F2 breeding ages and F1 larval diet quality on mean F3 life span.

Black lines represent F3 individuals descended from F1 males and females bred at a young age (15 days old) and red lines signify individuals descended from old (35 days old) grandparents. In patrilines only, individuals descended from F1 males that were subjected to either a high or low competitive environment are represented by a solid or dotted line, respectively. F3 grand-offspring of F1 grandparents reared on a high-nutrient larval diet are represented by a circle, and low-nutrient larval diest is represented by a triangle. All points represent means. Bars represent SEM. Underlying data can be found in the Dryad Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw. F1, grand-parental generation; F2, female and male offspring; F3, grand-offspring.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s001

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Values of the KLDC for patrilines, comparing parameter posterior distributions between treatment groups.

Underlying data can be found in the Dryad Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw. HO, High Nutrient Old Breeding treatment; HY, High Nutrient Young Breeding treatment; KLDC, Kullback-Leibler discrepancy calibration; LO, Low Nutrient Old Breeding treatment; LY, Low Nutrient Young Breeding treatment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s002

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Values of the KLDC for matrilines, comparing parameter posterior distributions between our treatment groups.

Underlying data can be found in the Dryad Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw. HO, High Nutrient Old Breeding treatment; HY, High Nutrient Young Breeding treatment; KLDC, Kullback-Leibler discrepancy calibration; LO, Low Nutrient Old Breeding treatment; LY, Low Nutrient Young Breeding treatment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s003

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Values of the KLDC for patrilines, comparing parameter posterior distributions between treatment groups.

Underlying data can be found in the Dryad Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw. F1, grand-parental generation; F2, female and male offspring; KLDC, Kullback-Leibler discrepancy calibration; OO, Old F1 breeding age Old F2 breeding age; OY, Old F1 breeding age Young F2 breeding age; YO, Young F1 breeding age Old F2 breeding age treatment; YVO, Young F1 breeding age Very old F2 breeding age; YY, Young F1 breeding age Young F2 breeding age.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s004

(TIF)

S5 Fig. Values of the KLDC for matrilines, comparing parameter posterior distributions between our treatment groups.

Underlying data can be found in the Dryad Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw. F1, grand-parental generation; F2, female and male offspring; KLDC, Kullback-Leibler discrepancy calibration; OO, Old F1 breeding age Old F2 breeding age; OY, Old F1 breeding age Young F2 breeding age; YO, Young F1 breeding age Old F2 breeding age treatment; YVO, Young F1 breeding age Very old F2 breeding age; YY, Young F1 breeding age Young F2 breeding age.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s005

(TIF)

S6 Fig. Effects of F2 breeding age and F2 sex on F3 body size in patrilines.

Solid grey lines represent F3 individuals descended from F2 females and solid black lines represent F3 individuals descended from F2 males. Bars represent SEM. Underlying data can be found in the Dryad Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw. F2, female and male offspring; F3, grand-offspring.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s006

(TIF)

S7 Fig. Effects of F1 larval diet and age at breeding on F3 body size in patrilines and matrilines.

Solid grey lines represent effects of F1 individuals reared on reared on a poor larval diet, and solid black lines represent the effects of F1 individuals reared on a rich larval diet. Bars represent SEM. Underlying data can be found in the Dryad Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw. F1, grand-parental generation; F3, grand-offspring.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s007

(TIF)

S1 Table. Factorial summary of mean F3 life span, development time, and thorax length for patrilines.

F3, grand-offspring.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s008

(XLSX)

S2 Table. Factorial summary of mean F3 life span, development time, and thorax length for matrilines.

F3, grand-offspring.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s009

(XLSX)

S3 Table. Linear mixed-effects models of F3 life span for patrilines including F1 competitive environment.

Negative effects for F1 larval diet indicate that grandparents reared on a high-nutrient larval diet produced grand-offspring with a relatively longer life span than descendants of grandparents reared on a low-nutrient larval diet. Negative effects of F1 and F2 age indicate that old grandparents and parents produced F3 individuals with reduced lifespans, negative effects of larval diet indicate that low-nutrient larval diet has a negative effect on F3 life span, and negative effects of sex indicate that the life span of male descendants was lower than that of females. Significance codes: p = 0.0001 ‘***’, p = 0.001 ‘**’, p = 0.01 ‘*’, p = 0.05 ‘.’, p = 0.1. F1, grand-parental generation; F2, female and male offspring; F3, grand-offspring

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s010

(XLSX)

S4 Table. Linear mixed-effects models of F3 life span for patrilines and matrilines, with thorax length and development time of all focal individuals included as covariates.

Negative effects for F1 larval diet indicate that grandparents reared on a high-nutrient larval diet produced grand-offspring with a relatively longer life span than descendants of grandparents reared on a low-nutrient larval diet. Negative effects of F1 and F2 age indicate that old grandparents and parents produced F3 individuals with reduced lifespans, negative effects of larval diet indicate that low-nutrient larval diet has a negative effect on F3 life span, and negative effects of sex indicate that the life span of male descendants was lower than that of females. Significance codes: p = 0.0001 ‘***’, p = 0.001 ‘**’, p = 0.01 ‘*’, p = 0.05 ‘.’, p = 0.1. F1, grand-parental generation; F2, female and male offspring; F3, grand-offspring

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s011

(XLSX)

S5 Table. Model selection results based on ‘flexsurv’ package.

The simple Gompertz model provided the best fit to our data based on the Aikaike Information Criterion and was used for further analyses using BaSTA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s012

(XLSX)

S6 Table. Parameter estimates for each treatment group for the best fitting model (Gompertz with simple shape) for grand-paternal effects of F1 larval diet × F1 breeding age.

F1, grand-parental generation

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s013

(XLSX)

S7 Table. Mean KLDC values for patrilines, comparing parameter posterior distributions between F1 treatment groups.

F1, grand-parental generation; KLDC, Kullback-Leibler discrepancy calibration

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s014

(XLSX)

S8 Table. Parameter estimates for each treatment group for the best fitting model (Gompertz with Simple shape) for grand-maternal effects of F1 larval diet × F1 breeding age.

F1, grand-parental generation

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s015

(XLSX)

S9 Table. Mean KLDC values for matrilines, comparing parameter posterior distributions between F1 treatment groups.

F1, grand-parental generation; KLDC, Kullback-Leibler discrepancy calibration

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s016

(XLSX)

S10 Table. Parameter estimates for each treatment group for the best fitting model (Gompertz with simple shape) for effects of F1 breeding age × F2 breeding age in patrilines.

F1, grand-parental generation; F2, female and male offspring

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s017

(XLSX)

S11 Table. Mean KLDC values for patrilines comparing parameter posterior distributions between treatment groups.

KLDC, Kullback-Leibler discrepancy calibration

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s018

(XLSX)

S12 Table. Parameter estimates for each treatment group for the best fitting model (Gompertz with Simple shape) for effects of F1 breeding age × F2 breeding age in matrilines.

F1, grand-parental generation; F2, female and male offspring

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s019

(XLSX)

S13 Table. Mean KLDC values for matrilines, comparing parameter posterior distributions between our treatment groups.

KLDC, Kullback-Leibler discrepancy calibration

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s020

(XLSX)

S14 Table. Linear mixed-effects model of F3 body size.

Significant effects are highlighted in bold. Significance codes: p = 0.0001 ‘***’, p = 0.001 ‘**’, p = 0.01 ‘*’, p = 0.05 ‘.’, p = 0.1. F3, grand-offspring

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s021

(XLSX)

S15 Table. Linear mixed-effects model of F3 development time.

Significant effects are highlighted in bold. Solid black lines represent the development time of F3 offspring descended from F2 males, and solid grey lines are individuals derived from F2 females. Bars represent SEM. SEM for descendants of F2 females at 60 days is missing because of a small sample size. Significance codes: p = 0.0001 ‘***’, p = 0.001 ‘**’, p = 0.01 ‘*’, p = 0.05 ‘.’, p = 0.1.F1, grand-parental generation; F2, female and male offspring; F3, grand-offspring

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s022

(XLSX)

S1 Text. Discussion of the effects influencing F3 body size and development time.

F3, grand-offspring

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000556.s023

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors would thank E. Macartney, N. W. Burke, and F. Zajitschek for their helpful comments and discussions of earlier drafts of the manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Lansing AI. A transmissible, cumulative and reversible factor in aging. Gerontology. 1947;2: 228–239.
  2. 2. Ducatez S, Baguette M, Stevens VM, Legrand D, Fréville H. Complex Interactions Between Paternal And Maternal Effects: Parental Experience And Age At Reproduction Affect Fecundity And Offspring Performance In A Butterfly. Evolution. 2012;66: 3558–3569. pmid:23106718
  3. 3. Gavrilov LA, Gavrilova NS. Parental age at conception and offspring longevity. Rev Gerontol. 1997;7: 5–12.
  4. 4. Mousseau T, Fox CW. Maternal effects as adaptations. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998.
  5. 5. Priest N, Mackowiak B, Promislow DEL. The role of parental age effects on the evolution of aging. Evolution. 2002;56: 927–935. pmid:12093028
  6. 6. Schroeder J, Nakagawa S, Rees M, Mannarelli M-E, Burke T. Reduced fitness in progeny from old parents in a natural population. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112: 4021–5. pmid:25775600
  7. 7. Mousseau T. Maternal Effects In Insect Life Histories. Annu Rev Entomol. 1991;36: 511–534.
  8. 8. Hercus MJ, Hoffmann AA. Maternal and grandmaternal age influence offspring fitness in Drosophila. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2000;267: 2105–2110. pmid:11416916
  9. 9. Koch RE, Phillips JM, Camus MF, Dowling DK. Maternal age effects on fecundity and offspring egg-to-adult viability are not affected by mitochondrial haplotype. Ecol Evol. 2018;8: 10722–10732. pmid:30519401
  10. 10. Lippens C, Faivre B, Lechenault C, Sorci G. Aging parasites produce offspring with poor fitness prospects. Biol Lett. 2017;13. pmid:28202684
  11. 11. Fay R, Barbraud C, Delord K, Weimerskirch H. Paternal but not maternal age influences early-life performance of offspring in a long-lived seabird. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2016;283. pmid:27053738
  12. 12. Kirkwood TB, Austad SN. Why do we age? Nature. 2000;408: 233–238. pmid:11089980
  13. 13. Wong WSW, Solomon BD, Bodian DL, Kothiyal P, Eley G, Huddleston KC, et al. New observations on maternal age effect on germline de novo mutations. Nat Commun. 2016;7: 1–10. pmid:26781218
  14. 14. Horvath S. DNA methylation age of human tissues and cell types. Genome Biol. 2013;14. pmid:24138928
  15. 15. Koch CM, Wagner W. Epigenetic-aging-signature to determine age in different tissues. Aging. 2011;3: 1018–1027. 100395 [pii] pmid:22067257
  16. 16. Stubbs TM, Bonder MJ, Stark AT, Krueger F, Bolland DJ, Butcher GA, et al. Multi-tissue DNA methylation age predictor in mouse. Genome Biology. 2017.
  17. 17. Hannum G, Guinney J, Zhao L, Zhang L, Hughes G, Sadda SV, et al. Genome-wide Methylation Profiles Reveal Quantitative Views of Human Aging Rates. Mol Cell. 2013;49: 359–367. pmid:23177740
  18. 18. Bocklandt S, Lin W, Sehl ME, Sanchez FJ, Sinsheimer JS, Horvarth S, et al. Epigenetic Predictor of Age. PLoS ONE. 2011;6.
  19. 19. Curley JP, Mashoodh R, Champagne FA. Epigenetics and the origins of paternal effects. Horm Behav. 2011;59: 306–14. pmid:20620140
  20. 20. Crews D, Gore AC, Hsu TS, Dangleben NL, Spinetta M, Schallert T, et al. Transgenerational epigenetic imprints on mate preference. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104: 5942–5946. pmid:17389367
  21. 21. Franklin TB, Russig H, Weiss IC, Grff J, Linder N, Michalon A, et al. Epigenetic transmission of the impact of early stress across generations. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68: 408–415. pmid:20673872
  22. 22. Holeski LM, Jander G, Agrawal AA. Transgenerational defense induction and epigenetic inheritance in plants. Trends Ecol Evol. 2012;27: 618–626. pmid:22940222
  23. 23. Pauli A, Rinn JL, Schier AF. Non coding RNAs regulation in embryogenesis. Nat Rev Genet. 2011;12: 136–149. pmid:21245830
  24. 24. Chen Q, Yan W, Duan E. Epigenetic inheritance of acquired traits through sperm RNAs and sperm RNA modifications. Nat Rev Genet. 2016;17: 733. Available: pmid:27694809
  25. 25. Szyf M. Nongenetic inheritance and transgenerational epigenetics. Trends Mol Med. 2015;21: 134–144. pmid:25601643
  26. 26. Díaz H, Esponda P. Ageing-induced changes in the cortical granules of mouse eggs. Zygote. 2004;12: 95–103. pmid:15460103
  27. 27. Rodgers AB, Morgan CP, Leu NA, Bale TL. Transgenerational epigenetic programming via sperm microRNA recapitulates effects of paternal stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112: 13699–13704. pmid:26483456
  28. 28. Immler S. The sperm factor: paternal impact beyond genes. Heredity. 2018;121: 239–247. pmid:29959427
  29. 29. Chen Q, Yan M, Cao Z, Li X, Zhang Y, Shi J, et al. Sperm tsRNAs contribute to intergenerational inheritance of an acquired metabolic disorder. Science (80-). 2016;351: 397–400. pmid:26721680
  30. 30. Klosin A, Lehner B. Mechanisms, timescales and principles of trans-generational epigenetic inheritance in animals. Curr Opin Genet Dev. 2016;36: 41–49. pmid:27140512
  31. 31. Blake GET, Watson ED. Unravelling the complex mechanisms of transgenerational epigenetic inheritance. Curr Opin Chem Biol. 2016;33: 101–107. pmid:27327212
  32. 32. Giron D, Casas J. Mothers reduce egg provisioning with age. Ecol Lett. 2003;6: 273–277.
  33. 33. Plaistow SJ, Shirley C, Collin H, Cornell SJ, Harney ED. Offspring provisioning explains clone-specific maternal age effects on life history and life span in the water flea, Daphnia pulex. Am Nat. 2015;186: pmid:26655355
  34. 34. Bonduriansky R, Day T. Extended Heredity: A New Understanding of Inheritance and Evolution. Press PU, editor. 2018.
  35. 35. Crean A, Bonduriansky R. What is a paternal effect? Trends Ecol Evol. 2014;29: 554–559. pmid:25130305
  36. 36. Lock JE. Transgenerational effects of parent and grandparent gender on offspring development in a biparental beetle species. Biol Lett. 2012;8: 408–411. pmid:22090206
  37. 37. Xie K, Ryan DP, Pearson BL, Henzel KS, Neff F, Vidal RO, et al. Epigenetic alterations in longevity regulators, reduced life span, and exacerbated aging-related pathology in old father offspring mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2018;115: E2348–E2357. pmid:29467291
  38. 38. Sharma R, Agarwal A, Rohra VK, Assidi M, Abu-Elmagd M, Turki RF. Effects of increased paternal age on sperm quality, reproductive outcome and associated epigenetic risks to offspring. Reprod Biol Endocrinol. 2015;13: 1–20.
  39. 39. Saha S, Barnett AG, Foldi C, Burne TH, Eyles DW, Buka SL, et al. Advanced paternal age is associated with impaired neurocognitive outcomes during infancy and childhood. PLoS Med. 2009;6: 0303–0311. pmid:19278291
  40. 40. Sandin S, Schendel D, Magnusson P, Hultman C, Surén P, Susser E, et al. Autism risk associated with parental age and with increasing difference in age between the parents. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21: 693–700. pmid:26055426
  41. 41. Lynch M, Ennis R. Resource availability, maternal effects, and longevity. Exp Gerontol. 1983;18: 147–165. pmid:6884438
  42. 42. Gribble KE, Jarvis G, Bock M, Mark Welch DB. Maternal caloric restriction partially rescues the deleterious effects of advanced maternal age on offspring. Aging Cell. 2014;13: 623–630. pmid:24661622
  43. 43. Carnes BA, Riesch R, Schlupp I. The delayed impact of parental age on offspring mortality in Mice. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2012;67 A: 351–357. pmid:21835807
  44. 44. Runagall-McNaull A, Bonduriansky R, Crean AJ. Dietary protein and lifespan across the metamorphic boundary: protein-restricted larvae develop into short-lived adults. Sci Rep. 2015;5.
  45. 45. Adler MI, Cassidy EJ, Fricke C, Bonduriansky R. The lifespan-reproduction trade-off under dietary restriction is sex-specific and context-dependent. Exp Gerontol. 2013;48: 539–548. pmid:23542072
  46. 46. Adler MI, Telford M, Bonduriansky R. Phenotypes optimized for early-life reproduction exhibit faster somatic deterioration with age, revealing a latent cost of high condition. J Evol Biol. 2016;29: 2436–2446. pmid:27546615
  47. 47. Hooper AK, Spagopoulou F, Wylde Z, Maklakov AA, Bonduriansky R. Ontogenetic timing as a condition-dependent life history trait: High-condition males develop quickly, peak early and age fast. Evolution. 2017; 1–15. pmid:28067402
  48. 48. Macartney E, Crean A, Bonduriansky R. Adult dietary protein has age- and context-dependent effects on male post-copulatory performance. J Evol Biol. 2017;
  49. 49. Kawasaki N, Brassil CE, Brooks R, Bonduriansky R. Environmental effects on the expression of lifespan and aging: An extreme contrast between wild and captive cohorts of Telostylinus angusticollis (Diptera: Neriidae). Am Nat. 2008;172: 2379–2388.
  50. 50. Moorad JA, Nussey DH. Evolution of maternal effect senescence. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113: 362–367. pmid:26715745
  51. 51. Zwaan B, Bijlsma R, Hoekstra RE. Direct selection on life span in Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution. 1995;49: 649–659. pmid:28565142
  52. 52. Rose MR. Laboratory evolution of postponed senescence in Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution. 1984;38: 1004–1010. pmid:28555803
  53. 53. Kirkwood TBL, Holliday R. The Evolution of Ageing and Longevity. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 1979;205: 531–546. pmid:42059
  54. 54. Bonduriansky R, Brassil CE. Rapid and costly ageing in wild male flies. Nature. 2002;420: 2002.
  55. 55. Zajitschek F, Brassil CE, Bonduriansky R, Brooks RC. Sex effects on life span and senescence in the wild when dates of birth and death are unknown. Ecology. 2009;90: 1698–1707. pmid:19569384
  56. 56. Rodriguez-Munoz R, Bretman A, Slate J, Walling CA, Tregenza T. Natural and Sexual Selection in a Wild Insect Population. Science. 2010;328: 1269–1272. pmid:20522773
  57. 57. Danserau DA, Lasko PF. Germline Stem Cells. 2008;450.
  58. 58. Hennig W. Spermatogenesis in Drosophila. Int J Dev Biol. 1996;40: 167–176. pmid:8735926
  59. 59. Bastock R, St Johnston D. Drosophila oogenesis. Curr Biol. 2008;18: 1082–1087. pmid:19081037
  60. 60. Gao J-J, Pan X-R, Hu J, Ma L, Wu J-M, Shao Y-L, et al. Highly variable recessive lethal or nearly lethal mutation rates during germ-line development of male Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011;108: 15914–15919. pmid:21890796
  61. 61. Bachtrog D. Evidence for male-driven evolution in Drosophila. Mol Biol Evol. 2008;25: 617–619. pmid:18234707
  62. 62. Haldane JBS. The rate of spontaneous mutation of a human gene. J Genet. 1935;31: 317–326.
  63. 63. Bauer VL, Aquadro CF. Rates of DNA Sequence Evolution are Not Sex-Biased in Drosophila melanogaster and D. simulans. Mol Biol Evol. 1997;14: 1252–1257. pmid:9402735
  64. 64. Drummond-Barbosa D, Spradling AC. Stem cells and their progeny respond to nutritional changes during Drosophila oogenesis. Dev Biol. 2001;231: 265–278. pmid:11180967
  65. 65. Agrawal AF, Wang AD. Increased transmission of mutations by low-condition females: Evidence for condition-dependent DNA repair. PLoS Biol. 2008;6: 0389–0395. pmid:18271627
  66. 66. Jung M, Pfeifer GP. Aging and DNA methylation. BMC Biol. 2015;13. pmid:25637097
  67. 67. Regev A, Lamb MJ, Jablonka E. The Role of DNA Methylation in Invertebrates: Developmental Regulation\ror Genome Defense? Mol Biol Evol. 1998;15: 880–891.
  68. 68. Mandrioli M, Borsatti F. DNA methylation of fly genes and transposons. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2006;63: 1933–1936. pmid:16847580
  69. 69. Field LM, Lyko F, Mandrioli M, Prantera G. DNA methylation in insects. Insect Mol Biol. 2004;13: 109–115. pmid:15056357
  70. 70. Lyko F, Ramsahoye BH, Jaenisch R. DNA methylation in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature. 2000;408.
  71. 71. Ferres-Marco D, Gutierrez-Garcia I, Vallejo DM, Bolivar J, Gutierrez-Aviño FJ, Dominguez M. Epigenetic silencers and Notch collaborate to promote malignant tumours by Rb silencing. Nature. 2006;439: 430–436. pmid:16437107
  72. 72. Xing Y, Shi S, Le L, Lee CA, Silver-Morse L, Li WX. Evidence for transgenerational transmission of epigenetic tumor susceptibility in Drosophila. PLoS Genet. 2007;3: 1598–1606. pmid:17845077
  73. 73. Singh ND, Arndt PF, Petrov DA. Genomic heterogeneity of background substitutional patterns in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics. 2005;169: 709–722. pmid:15520267
  74. 74. Keller I, Bensasson D, Nichols RA. Transition-transversion bias is not universal: A counter example from grasshopper pseudogenes. PLoS Genet. 2007;3: 0185–0191. pmid:17274688
  75. 75. Horvarth S. DNA methylation age of human tissues and cell types. Genome Biol. 2013;14.
  76. 76. Law JA, Jacobsen SE. Establishing, maintaining and modifying DNA methylation patterns in plants and animals. Nat Rev Genet. 2010;11: 204–220. pmid:20142834
  77. 77. Ho DH, Burggren WW. Epigenetics and transgenerational transfer: a physiological perspective. J Exp Biol. 2010;213: 3–16. pmid:20008356
  78. 78. Cheng J, Türkel N, Hemati N, Fuller MT, Hunt AJ, Yamashita YM. Centrosome misorientation reduces stem cell division during ageing. Nature. 2008;456: 599–604. pmid:18923395
  79. 79. Zhao R, Xuan Y, Li X, Xi R. Age-related changes of germline stem cell activity, niche signaling activity and egg production in Drosophila. Aging Cell. 2008;7: 344–354. pmid:18267001
  80. 80. Harley CB. Telomerase is not an oncogene. Oncogene. 2002;21: 494–502. pmid:11850774
  81. 81. Azzalin C.M., Reichenbach P., Khoriauli L., Giulotto E. Telomeric repeat containing RNA and RNA surveillance factors at mammalian chromosome ends. Science (80-). 2007;318: 798–801.
  82. 82. Reig-Viader R, Brieño-Enríquez MA, Khoriauli L, Toran N, Cabero L, Giulotto E, et al. Telomeric repeat-containing RNA and telomerase in human fetal oocytes. Hum Reprod. 2013;28: 414–422. pmid:23049077
  83. 83. Olovnikov IA, Morgunova V V, Mironova AA, Kordyukova MY, Radion EI, Olenkina OM, et al. Interaction of Telomeric Retroelement HeT A Transcripts and Their Protein Product Gag in Early Embryogenesis of Drosophila. Bioschemistry (Mosc). 2016;81. pmid:27682174
  84. 84. Pardue M-L, DeBaryshe PG. Retrotransposons Provide an Evolutionarily Robust Non-Telomerase Mechanism to Maintain Telomeres. Annu Rev Genet. 2003;37: 485–511. pmid:14616071
  85. 85. Mason JM, Frydrychova RC, Biessmann H. Drosophila telomeres: an exception providing new insights. Bioessays. 2008;30: 25–37. pmid:18081009
  86. 86. Eickbush TH, Furano A V. Fruit flies and humans respond differently to retrotransposons. Curr Opin Genet Dev. 2002;12: 669–674. pmid:12433580
  87. 87. Vagin V V., Sigova A, Li C, Seitz H, Gvozdev V, Zamore PD. A distinct small RNA pathway silences selfish genetic elements in the germline. Science. 2006;313: 320–324. pmid:16809489
  88. 88. Yin H, Lin H. An epigenetic activation role of Piwi and a Piwi-associated piRNA in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature. 2007;450: 304–308. pmid:17952056
  89. 89. Salvany L, Aldaz S, Corsetti E, Azpiazu N. A new role for hth in the early pre-blastodermic divisions in Drosophila. Cell Cycle. 2009;8: 2748–2755. pmid:19652544
  90. 90. Bonduriansky R, Head M. Maternal and paternal condition effects on offspring phenotype in Telostylinus angusticollis (Diptera: Neriidae). J Evol Biol. 2007;20: 2379–2388. pmid:17956399
  91. 91. Avent TD, Price TAR, Wedell N. Age-based female preference in the fruit fly Drosophila pseudoobscura. Anim Behav. 2008;75: 1413–1421.
  92. 92. Fox CW. The influence of maternal age and mating frequency on egg size and offspring performance in Callosobruchus maculatus (Coleoptera: Bruchidae). Oecologia. 1993;96: 139–146. pmid:28313765
  93. 93. Mcnamara KB, van Lieshout E, Jones TM, Simmons LW. Age-dependent trade-offs between immunity and male, but not female, reproduction. J Anim Ecol. 2013;82: 235–244. pmid:22849327
  94. 94. Gray DA. Intrinsic factors affecting female choice in house crickets: Time cost, female age, nutritional condition, body size, and size-relative reproductive investment. J Insect Behav. 1999;12: 691–700.
  95. 95. Desena MS, Clark JM, Edman JD, Symington SB, Scott TW, Clark GG, et al. Potential for aging female Aedes aegypti (Diptera: Culicidae) by gas chromatographic analysis of cuticular hydrocarbons, including a field evaluation. J Med Entomol. 1999;36: 811–823. pmid:10593085
  96. 96. Braga MV, Pinto ZT, de Carvalho Queiroz MM, Blomquist GJ. Effect of age on cuticular hydrocarbon profiles in adult Chrysomya putoria (Diptera: Calliphoridae). Forensic Sci Int. 2016;259: e37–e47. pmid:26775199
  97. 97. Wedell N, Ritchie MG. Male age, mating status and nuptial gift quality in a bushcricket. Anim Behav. 2004;67: 1059–1065.
  98. 98. Torres-Vila LM, Jennions MD. Male mating history and female fecundity in the Lepidoptera: Do male virgins make better partners? Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2005;57: 318–326.
  99. 99. Adler MI, Bonduriansky R. The dissimilar costs of love and war: age-specific mortality as a function of the operational sex ratio. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2011;24: 1169–1177. pmid:21375650
  100. 100. Sentinella AT, Crean AJ, Bonduriansky R. Dietary protein mediates a trade-off between larval survival and the development of male secondary sexual traits. Funct Ecol. 2013;27: 1134–1144.
  101. 101. Bonduriansky R. Convergent evolution of sexual shape dimorphism in Diptera. J Morphol. 2006;267: 602–611. pmid:16477603
  102. 102. Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I. Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, Preibisch S, et al. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat Methods. 2012;9: 676–682. pmid:22743772
  103. 103. Team RC. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. [Internet]. 2012. Available: http://www.r-project.org/
  104. 104. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. J Stat Softw. 2015;67: 1–48.
  105. 105. Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB. {lmerTest} Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. J Stat Softw. 2017;82: 1–26.
  106. 106. Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol Evol. 2013;4: 133–142.
  107. 107. Colchero F, Jones OR, Rebke M. BaSTA: an R package for Bayesian estimation of age-specific mortality from incomplete mark-recapture/recovery data with covariates. Methods Ecol Evol. 2012; 466–470.
  108. 108. Colchero F, Clark JS. Bayesian inference on age-specific survival for censored and truncated data. J Anim Ecol. 2012;81: 139–149. pmid:21883202
  109. 109. Jackson C. flexsurv: A Platform for Parametric Survival Modeling in R. J Stat Softw. 2016;70: 1–33. pmid:29593450
  110. 110. Zou S, Carey JR, Liedo P, Ingram DK, Yu B. Prolongevity effects of a botanical with oregano and cranberry extracts in Mexican fruit flies: Examining interactions of diet restriction and age. Age. 2012;34: 269–279. pmid:21455602
  111. 111. Carey JR, Liedo P. Sex-specific life table aging rates in large medfly cohorts. Exp Gerontol. 1995;30: 315–325. pmid:7556510
  112. 112. Styer LM, Carey JR, Wang J-L, Scott TW. Mosquitoes do sensece: Departure from the paradigm of constant mortality. Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2007;76: 111–117. pmid:17255238
  113. 113. Wu D, Cysper JR, Yashin AI, Johnson TE. Multiple Mild Heat-Shocks Decrease The Gompertz Component of Mortality in C. elegans. Exp Gerontol. 2009;44: 607–612. pmid:19580861
  114. 114. Polačik M, Tomášek O, Kačer P, Methling C, Vrtílek M, Terzibasi Tozzini E, et al. Repeated intraspecific divergence in life span and aging of African annual fishes along an aridity gradient. Evolution. 2016;71: 386–402. pmid:27859247
  115. 115. Boonekamp JJ, Salomons M, Bouwhuis S, Dijkstra C, Verhulst S. Reproductive effort accelerates actuarial senescence in wild birds: an experimental study. Ecol Lett. 2014;17: 599–605. pmid:24818237
  116. 116. Zajitschek F, Jin T, Colchero F, Maklakov AA. Aging differently: Diet-and sex-dependent late-life mortality patterns in Drosophila melanogaster. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69: 666–674. pmid:24170671
  117. 117. Wylde Z (2019) Data from: Parental breeding age effects on descendants’ longevity interact over 2 generations in matrilines and patrilines. Dryad Digital Repository Openly available via https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2rbnzs7hw