Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Smoking mediates the relationship between SES and brain volume: The CARDIA study

  • Ryan J. Dougherty ,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    rdoughe7@jhu.edu

    Affiliations Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland, United States of America, National Institute on Aging, Bethesda, Maryland, United States of America

  • Justine Moonen,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation National Institute on Aging, Bethesda, Maryland, United States of America

  • Kristine Yaffe,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, California, United States of America, San Francisco VA Health Care System, San Francisco, California, United States of America

  • Stephen Sidney,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Kaiser Permanente Division of Research, Oakland, California, United States of America, Kaiser Permanente of Northern California, Oakland, California, United States of America

  • Christos Davatzikos,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Mohamad Habes,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Lenore J. Launer

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation National Institute on Aging, Bethesda, Maryland, United States of America

Abstract

Objective

Investigate whether socioeconomic status (SES) was related to brain volume in aging related regions, and if so, determine whether this relationship was mediated by lifestyle factors that are known to associate with risk of dementia in a population-based sample of community dwelling middle-aged adults.

Methods

We studied 645 (41% black) participants (mean age 55.3±3.5) from the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) study who underwent brain magnetic resonance imaging. SES was operationalized as a composite measure of annual income and years of education. Gray matter volume was estimated within the insular cortex, thalamus, cingulate, frontal, inferior parietal, and lateral temporal cortex. These regions are vulnerable to age-related atrophy captured by the Spatial Pattern of Atrophy for Recognition of Brain Aging (SPARE-BA) index. Lifestyle factors of interest included physical activity, cognitive activity (e.g. book/newspaper reading), smoking status, alcohol consumption, and diet. Multivariable linear regressions tested the association between SES and brain volume. Sobel mediation analyses determined if this association was mediated by lifestyle factors. All models were age, sex, and race adjusted.

Results

Higher SES was positively associated with brain volume (β = .109 SE = .039; p < .01) and smoking status significantly mediated this relationship (z = 2.57). With respect to brain volume, smoking accounted for 27% of the variance (β = -.179 SE = .065; p < .01) that was previously attributed to SES.

Conclusion

Targeting smoking cessation could be an efficacious means to reduce the health disparity of low SES on brain volume and may decrease vulnerability for dementia.

Introduction

Low socioeconomic status (SES) has been linked to a variety of negative lifestyle factors [15] and greater risk of developing dementia [610]. It has been well documented that pathophysiological processes of dementia, including brain atrophy, begin to develop during midlife, decades prior to cognitive impairment [1113]. Importantly, lifestyle factors associated with SES have shown to influence brain volume [1416] and dementia risk [1719]. Recent research suggests SES may be related to alterations in brain health [2022], however, prior studies have not investigated modifiable lifestyle factors that may contribute to the observed associations.

Epidemiological cohort studies have identified specific brain regions vulnerable to early age-related atrophy including the insular cortex, thalamus, cingulate cortex, frontal inferior parietal, and lateral temporal cortex [14, 15]. Investigating whether SES is associated with brain volume in a community-based middle age population will provide novel epidemiological health information. Further, examining lifestyle factors which may contribute to this relationship is particularly relevant as midlife is a period of aging when behaviors may be modified to decrease one’s risk of dementia [1719]. There is a need to identify modifiable lifestyle factors that may be highest yield when targeted early to mitigate the deleterious health effects of SES disparities. The purpose of this study was to i) determine the relationship between SES and brain volume in regions susceptible to age related atrophy and, ii) ascertain whether the relationship was mediated by modifiable lifestyle factors.

Methods

Participants

The Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) is a longitudinal study which began in 1985–1986 when 5,115 participants (18–30 years old) were recruited by equal distribution of sex, age, education, and race with the aim to study determinants of cardiovascular disease in four U.S. cities (Birmingham, AL; Chicago, IL; Minneapolis, MN; and Oakland, CA [23]. Participants have completed numerous follow-up appointments with the most recent at the year 30 exam (2015–2016), when a subset of participants underwent magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) for the CARDIA Brain MRI Sub-study. This MRI sub-study was designed to characterize the morphology, pathology, physiology, and function of the brain in this cohort. The sample for the CARDIA Brain MRI sub-study were balanced within four strata of ethnicity/race (black, white) and sex from three of the CARDIA field centers (Birmingham, AL; Minneapolis, MN; and Oakland, CA).

Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents

The MRI protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the participating sites including the University of Alabama Birmingham IRB, University of Minnesota IRB, Kaiser Permanente Northern California IRB, and the University of Pennsylvania IRB. Additional IRB approval was granted through the NIH Office of Human Subjects Research Protection for the Intramural Research Program, and the National Institute on Aging. All participants were provided and signed a separate written informed consent for the CARDIA Brain MRI Sub-study.

Socioeconomic status

SES was operationalized as a composite score of total years of formal education and household income over the past 12 months. Because education and income are salient indicators of SES [6, 7] and national data demonstrates weak-moderate correlations between these measures [24], investigators routinely use a composite index of SES [3, 20, 25, 26]. Similar to previous research, education and income were categorized into approximately equal tertiles (education: 1 = ≤ 12; 2 = 13–16, 3 = ≥ 17; income: 1 = ≤ 49,999; 2 = 50,000–99,999; 3 = ≥ 100,000) and were summed to create a SES score which ranged from 2 (low SES) to 6 (high SES) [26].

Neuroimaging protocol

The MRI scans were acquired in the axial plane on 3T scanners located at each CARDIA study sites; a Siemens 3T Tim Trio/VB 15 platform in Minneapolis and in Oakland; a Philips 3T Achieva/2.6.3.6 platform in Birmingham. Standard quality assurance protocols which were previously developed for the Functional Bioinformatics Research Network (FBIRN), and the Alzheimer’s disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) were used with the following thresholds: FBIRN—Siemens scanners Signal-to-Fluctuation-Noise-Ratio (SFNR) >220, Radius of Decorrelation (RDC)>3.1, Philips scanners SFNR>220, RDC>2.4; ADNI—Signal-to-Noise-Ratio (SNR) >300, Maximum Distortion >2.0. The structural images were acquired with 3D T1 and T2 sequences. Scan acquisition parameters have been previously described [27], and were processed using previously described methods [2830]. In brief, structural images were processed using an automated multispectral computer algorithm which classified all supratentorial brain tissue into gray matter, white matter, and cerebral spinal fluid. After correction of intensity inhomogeneities [31] a multi-atlas skull stripping algorithm was applied for the removal of extra-cranial tissues [32]. Each T1-weighted scan is automatically segmented into a set of anatomical gray matter regions of interest (ROIs) using a mutli-atlas label fusion method, MUSE [33]. A total of 663 images were visually checked for incidental findings, motion artifacts, and other quality issues. For the current study, gray matter ROIs that correspond to the Spatial Pattern of Atrophy for Recognition of Brain Aging (SPARE-BA) index were chosen a priori due to these regions demonstrating early age-related atrophy patterns [14, 15]. These ROIs which include the insular cortex, thalamus, cingulate, frontal, inferior parietal, and lateral temporal cortex were summed together and then expressed as a percentage of intracranial volume (ICV) to account for differences in head size. The cerebellum was chosen as a control region.

Questionnaire data

In addition to the MRI, all participants completed a variety of health-related questionnaires and measurements (e.g. height, weight) at the year 30 exam. Modifiable lifestyle factors that were investigated as potential mediating variables include physical activity, cognitive activity, smoking status, alcohol consumption, and diet. All exam materials (i.e. protocols, questionnaires) can be found on the public CARDIA website (https://www.cardia.dopm.uab.edu). Physical activity was measured by the CARDIA physical activity questionnaire. This interviewer-administered questionnaire inquires on vigorous (e.g. jogging, racket sports, heavy lifting on the job & sport), leisurely (e.g. non-strenuous sport, hiking, home exercising & gardening), and work-related (e.g. sitting frequency) activities over the previous year [34]. Scores were converted into total physical activity intensity scores which reflect the estimated number of kilocalories expended per activity, and expressed in exercise units [35]. Cognitive activity was assessed by the CARDIA Cognitive Activity Scale where participants rated how often they performed 10 cognitive activities (e.g. read newspapers, play cards) over the past 12 months. Each of the 10 cognitive activity items ranged from ‘never’ (0) to ‘daily’ (6) and were averaged into a total cognitive activity score (range: 0–6). Smoking status was determined by an interview questionnaire. The participants were classified as a ‘non-smoker’ (0), ‘former smoker’ (1), or ‘current smoker’ (2). Diet was measured based on a self-report question which inquired on the nutritional quality of their diet. Participants responded to one of four categories which ranged from low (1) to high (4) dietary quality (range: 1–4). Alcohol consumption was determined by an interviewer-administered questionnaire inquiring how many drinks (beer, wine, liquor) per week are typically consumed. The total number of drinks per week were then relativized (beer [16.7 mL/drink] + wine [17.0 mL/drink] + liquor [16.7 mL/drink]) into total milliliters of alcohol consumed. Median imputation was used to account for missing questionnaire data.

Statistical analyses

Multivariable linear regression was used to test the association between SES and brain volume while adjusting for age, race and sex. Pearson correlations tested relationships between SES, brain volume, and modifiable lifestyle factors. Mediation analysis requires the following assumptions to be satisfied i) the independent variable (SES) must be associated with the dependent variable (brain volume), ii) the mediator variable (lifestyle factors) must be associated with the independent variable and the dependent variable, and iii) the mediator variable must significantly reduce the variance explained by the independent variable on the dependent variable [36]. After identification of potential mediators, our mediation analyses involved testing three regression models to determine whether the indirect effect of SES through modifiable lifestyle factors had a significant effect on brain volume [37]. Model 1 predicted brain volume from SES. Model 2 predicted lifestyle factors from SES. Model 3 predicted brain volume from both SES and lifestyle factors simultaneously. The Sobel test was used to determine whether the mediation effect was significant [38]. In brief, the Sobel test uses the beta coefficients and standard errors from the models to determine if the mediation effect on the dependent variable is significantly different than zero [37, 38]. The magnitude of mediation was determined by the percent reduction in the beta coefficient for SES predicting brain volume after inclusion of the mediating variable using the following formula: (βModel 1 − βModel 3) / (βModel 1) × 100. All statistical regression models were age, race, and sex adjusted with the significance level set at 0.05. Our statistical package was IBM SPSS, version 26.

Results

A total of 645 participants (40.6% black; mean age 55.3 years ± 3.5) with processed and quality checked structural brain imaging data were included in the study. Brain volume was negatively associated with age (r = -.20; p < .001). The SES scores were normally distributed and the percentage of participants in each category is as follows: 2: 12%, 3: 22%, 4: 24%, 5: 30%, 6: 12%. Additional participant characteristics are listed in Table 1. The percentage of missing questionnaire data for each measured variable ranged from 1–3% and sensitivity analyses determined median imputation did not influence any of the reported findings (p >.05).

SES and brain volume

The overall regression model linking SES to brain volume was significant (F(4,640) = 25.75, p < .001). SES was significantly and positively associated with brain volume (β = .109 SE = .039; p < .01) while accounting for variance explained by age, sex, and race (Table 2). Having established that SES was significantly associated with brain volume, we proceeded determining whether this association was being mediated by any of the measured modifiable lifestyle factors (aim 2). To identify potential mediators, we investigated the individual relationships between each of the five lifestyle factors with SES and brain volume. Bivariate correlation analysis revealed all the measured lifestyle factors were correlated with either SES or brain volume (p < .05; Table 3), however smoking was the only variable negatively correlated with both SES (r = -.296, p < .001) and brain volume (r = -.128, p < .01). The Sobel Mediation analysis determined smoking status was a significant mediator between SES and brain volume (z = 2.57, p = .01; Fig 1). In the mediation model, smoking was negatively associated with SES (β = -.165 SE = .023) and brain volume (β = -.179 SE = .065) which consequently explained 27% of the variance initially attributed to SES with respect to brain volume (Table 4). There was no detected mediation with respect to SES and cerebellum volume.

thumbnail
Table 2. Multivariable linear regression Model 1: SES and brain volume.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239548.t002

thumbnail
Table 4. Multivariable linear regression Model 3: SES, smoking, and brain volume.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239548.t004

Discussion

In this population-based sample of biracial community dwelling middle-aged adults, SES was positively associated with brain volume in regions vulnerable to early age-related atrophy. Notably, we demonstrated smoking status significantly mediated this relationship, that is, lower SES was associated with greater smoking prevalence, which in turn had a negative effect on brain volume. We did not detect mediation from the other four modifiable lifestyle factors. This finding suggests smoking cessation may be a high yield target for early intervention to mitigate the adverse effect of low SES on brain health. In contrast, there was no observed effect with cerebellum volume, suggesting SES and smoking have specific effects with respect to gray matter volume in regions that are predictive of future cognitive impairment.

Low SES has been shown to be a risk-factor for dementia [610]. In a prospective study, Goldbourt and colleagues reported adults of low midlife SES were 3 to 6-times more likely to develop dementia compared to age-matched adults of higher SES standing [8]. These results echo those of prior studies that have reported a 2–3 fold increase in dementia prevalence for those of low SES [7, 9, 10], but there have been reported contradictions in the available literature [39]. Because brain atrophy becomes apparent in midlife [11] and this decline is predictive of future dementia [12], investigating whether SES is related to brain volume during middle-age may provide insight into potential neurobiological mechanisms through which high SES protects against future dementia. There is a growing body of research that has investigated SES and brain health in cognitively healthy adults [2022]. Large scale studies (n ≥ 100) suggest higher SES in middle-late adulthood is a positive predictor of brain volume in several regions including the amygdala, hippocampus, cingulate, and temporal cortices [25, 40] (but see also [41]). Further, older adults of low SES have displayed accelerated rates of brain atrophy compared to those of higher SES [42]. Our findings from an epidemiological biracial middle-aged adult sample compliment and expand previous research by establishing a link between SES and brain volume within gray matter regions that have recently been shown to be sensitive to early age-related atrophy [14, 15].

The extant literature suggests SES may be associated with brain volume, yet these studies have not investigated modifiable lifestyle factors associated with SES, which may be mediating the observed associations. Because low SES is associated with a variety of unhealthy behaviors (e.g. poor diet, smoking, physical inactivity) [35] that have been previously shown to be associated with lower brain volume [1416], the findings reported in the literature may at least partly be due to modifiable lifestyle factors. In the present study, all of the lifestyle factors investigated were significantly associated with either SES or brain volume (Table 3). However, only smoking was identified as a potential mediator due to significant relationships with both SES and brain volume. Mediation analysis determined smoking status significantly mediated the relationship between SES and brain volume, accounting for 27% of the variance from the observed association (Tables 2 and 4). This finding is in agreement with Stringhini and colleagues who examined the role of modifiable lifestyle factors (i.e. smoking, alcohol consumption, diet, and physical activity) in relation to SES and mortality. As expected, adults of low SES had significantly higher mortality rates than those of high SES. When lifestyle factors were investigated, smoking was the strongest mediator, accounting for 32% of the variance between SES and mortality risk [43]. Although smoking appears to be a salient contributor to the deleterious effects of low SES it is important to note that these SES disparities were still present after accounting for smoking with respect to mortality [43], and brain volume (Table 4). These findings compliment national data which describe targeting lifestyle factors alone is not sufficient to overcome the health-related disparities associated with low SES [44].

Similar to SES, smoking is a significant risk factor for dementia [45]. In population-based studies of adults cognitively healthy at baseline assessment, smoking significantly increased the risk of developing dementia compared to non-smokers [45, 46]. Further, existing data suggests that smoking alone may account for 10% of the dementia cases nationwide [18]. The specific physiological mechanisms underlying the increased risk of developing dementia for smokers are relatively unknown [47], however, the preponderance of data suggests a connection between smoking and brain volume. Smoking has been shown to negatively influence indices of neurovascular function such as cerebral perfusion [48] (i.e. blood flow) and white matter integrity [27], which may precipitate brain volume decline. Compared to non-smokers, smokers exhibit greater age-related whole brain and cortical volume loss [49], and the higher frequency of smoking accelerates this decline [50]. In agreement with the present study, smoking has been shown to be negatively associated with brain volume in several cortical regions within the SPARE-BA index including the thalamus, cingulate, insular, frontal and temporal cortices [50, 51]. In fact, research that applied the SPARE-BA index demonstrated smoking status was negatively associated with brain volume in a large epidemiological sample of ~3,000 adult participants [14].

In our sample, smoking was negatively associated with SES which parallels studies that report disproportionately high smoking rates in adults of lower SES [52]. Although smoking rates in the United States have declined over the past 50 years [53], there are clear smoking cessation differences among varying levels of SES. In a study that utilized the National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys (NHANES), Kanjilal and colleagues investigated 30-year trends in smoking by SES levels among US adults. The authors reported a significant difference in smoking prevalence from years 1971–2002 among varying levels of SES, with the largest declines observed within the high SES group; the national averages decreased ~19%, whereas only modest declines were observed in the lowest SES group at ~6% [3]. Similarly, a Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) report which analyzed data from the 2008 National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) reported that from 1998–2008 adults of high SES had the greatest smoking cessation success compared to those of lower SES standing [52]. These data demonstrate individuals of lower SES have higher rates of smoking and are less likely to successfully abstain. A multitude of factors may contribute to the observed SES smoking disparity including access to treatment, social support, motivation, and life stressors [54]. With smoking prevalence disproportionately affecting those of lower SES, there is a need to identify successful strategies to promote cessation within SES disadvantaged populations.

Limitations of this study include its cross-sectional nature. Future studies would benefit from a prospective design to elucidate causality and directionality of the observed relationships between SES, smoking, and brain volume. Further, the modifiable lifestyle factors of interest were assessed through questionnaires, which are subject to recall and social desirability biases. For instance, physical activity was assessed via a self-report questionnaire which may be less sensitive than device measured physical activity (i.e. accelerometry). Due to the demographic make-up of the CARDIA Brain MRI Sub-study Cohort (i.e. ethnicity/race balanced from three CARDIA field centers) generalizability of our findings to other ethnic groups may be limited. Finally, all measures were collected at the year 30 exam period, therefore, we do not know whether changes in lifestyle factors over time have greater predictive value.

In summary, the present study provides evidence that within a biracial middle-aged cohort, smoking mediates the relationship between SES and brain volume within regions that are vulnerable to early age-related atrophy. This is an important finding as it is now well accepted that the neurobiology of dementia, including brain atrophy, begins during midlife [1113], a period of aging where lifestyle factors can be altered to decrease one’s risk of future dementia [1719]. While others have reported SES [21, 25, 40, 42] and smoking [14, 4951] are each separately associated with brain volume, our findings suggest smoking is a significant contributor to the SES and brain volume relationship. Targeting this particular modifiable lifestyle factor may be an efficacious means to mitigate the deleterious effects of low SES on brain volume. This study contributes to research investigating mechanisms by which low midlife SES may increase the risk of future dementia, and further, what modifiable lifestyle factors may be targeted to mitigate SES associated health care disparities.

Acknowledgments

Staff from the NHLBI (CARDIA sponsor) served on the executive and steering committees that made decisions on study design, methods, and data collection.

References

  1. 1. Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, et al. Socioeconomic status and health. The challenge of the gradient. Am Psychol. 1994;49(1):15–24. pmid:8122813
  2. 2. Adler NE, Stewart J. Health disparities across the lifespan: meaning, methods, and mechanisms. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1186:5–23. pmid:20201865
  3. 3. Kanjilal S, Gregg EW, Cheng YJ, et al. Socioeconomic status and trends in disparities in 4 major risk factors for cardiovascular disease among US adults, 1971–2002. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(21):2348–2355. pmid:17130388
  4. 4. Darmon N, Drewnowski A. Does social class predict diet quality? Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;87(5):1107–1117. pmid:18469226
  5. 5. Boone-Heinonen J, Diez Roux AV, Kiefe CI, Lewis CE, Guilkey DK, Gordon-Larsen P. Neighborhood socioeconomic status predictors of physical activity through young to middle adulthood: the CARDIA study. Soc Sci Med. 2011;72(5):641–649. pmid:21316829
  6. 6. Evans DA, Hebert LE, Beckett LA, et al. Education and other measures of socioeconomic status and risk of incident Alzheimer disease in a defined population of older persons. Arch Neurol. 1997;54(11):1399–1405. pmid:9362989
  7. 7. Karp A, Kåreholt I, Qiu C, Bellander T, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Relation of education and occupation-based socioeconomic status to incident Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159(2):175–183. pmid:14718220
  8. 8. Goldbourt U, Schnaider-Beeri M, Davidson M. Socioeconomic status in relationship to death of vascular disease and late-life dementia. J Neurol Sci. 2007;257(1–2):177–181. pmid:17395207
  9. 9. Stern Y, Gurland B, Tatemichi TK, Tang MX, Wilder D, Mayeux R. Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA. 1994;271(13):1004–1010. pmid:8139057
  10. 10. Anttila T, Helkala EL, Kivipelto M, et al. Midlife income, occupation, APOE status, and dementia: a population-based study. Neurology. 2002;59(6):887–893. pmid:12297572
  11. 11. Iturria-Medina Y, Sotero RC, Toussaint PJ, Mateos-Pérez JM, Evans AC, Initiative AsDN. Early role of vascular dysregulation on late-onset Alzheimer’s disease based on multifactorial data-driven analysis. Nat Commun. 2016;7:11934. pmid:27327500
  12. 12. Fox NC, Schott JM. Imaging cerebral atrophy: normal ageing to Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet. 2004;363(9406):392–394. pmid:15074306
  13. 13. Jack CR, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, et al. Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12(2):207–216. pmid:23332364
  14. 14. Habes M, Janowitz D, Erus G, et al. Advanced brain aging: relationship with epidemiologic and genetic risk factors, and overlap with Alzheimer disease atrophy patterns. Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6:e775. pmid:27045845
  15. 15. Habes M, Erus G, Toledo JB, et al. White matter hyperintensities and imaging patterns of brain ageing in the general population. Brain. 2016 Apr 1;139(4):1164–79. pmid:26912649
  16. 16. Dougherty RJ, Ellingson LD, Schultz SA, et al. Meeting physical activity recommendations may be protective against temporal lobe atrophy in older adults at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2016;4:14–17. pmid:27489874
  17. 17. Barnes DE, Yaffe K. The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. Lancet Neurol. 2011;10(9):819–828. pmid:21775213
  18. 18. Norton S, Matthews FE, Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Brayne C. Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based data. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(8):788–794. pmid:25030513
  19. 19. Yaffe K. Modifiable Risk Factors and Prevention of Dementia: What Is the Latest Evidence? JAMA Intern Med. 2018;178(2):281–282. pmid:29255907
  20. 20. Muscatell KA. Socioeconomic influences on brain function: implications for health. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2018;1428(1):14–32. pmid:29947055
  21. 21. Farah MJ. The Neuroscience of Socioeconomic Status: Correlates, Causes, and Consequences. Neuron. 2017;96(1):56–71. pmid:28957676
  22. 22. Brito NH, Noble KG. Socioeconomic status and structural brain development. Front Neurosci. 2014;8:276. pmid:25249931
  23. 23. Friedman GD, Cutter GR, Donahue RP, et al. CARDIA: study design, recruitment, and some characteristics of the examined subjects. J Clin Epidemiol. 1988;41(11):1105–1116. pmid:3204420
  24. 24. Braveman PA, Cubbin C, Egerter S, et al. Socioeconomic status in health research: one size does not fit all. JAMA. 2005;294(22):2879–2888. pmid:16352796
  25. 25. Shaked D, Millman ZB, Moody DLB, et al. Sociodemographic disparities in corticolimbic structures. PLoS One. 2019;14(5):e0216338. pmid:31071128
  26. 26. Cohen S, Schwartz JE, Epel E, Kirschbaum C, Sidney S, Seeman T. Socioeconomic status, race, and diurnal cortisol decline in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study. Psychosom Med. 2006;68(1):41–50. pmid:16449410
  27. 27. Launer LJ, Lewis CE, Schreiner PJ, et al. Vascular factors and multiple measures of early brain health: CARDIA brain MRI study. PLoS One. 2015;10(3):e0122138. pmid:25812012
  28. 28. Goldszal AF, Davatzikos C, Pham DL, Yan MX, Bryan RN, Resnick SM. An image-processing system for qualitative and quantitative volumetric analysis of brain images. J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1998;22(5):827–837. pmid:9754125
  29. 29. Shen D, Davatzikos C. HAMMER: hierarchical attribute matching mechanism for elastic registration. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2002;21(11):1421–1439. pmid:12575879
  30. 30. Zacharaki EI, Kanterakis S, Bryan RN, Davatzikos C. Measuring brain lesion progression with a supervised tissue classification system. Med Image Comput Comput Assist Interv. 2008;11(Pt 1):620–627. pmid:18979798
  31. 31. Tustison NJ, Avants BB, Cook PA, et al. N4ITK: improved N3 bias correction. IEEE transactions on medical imaging. 2010;29(6):1310–1320 pmid:20378467
  32. 32. Doshi J, Erus G, Ou Y, Gaonkar B, Davatzikos C. Multi-atlas skull-stripping. Academic radiology. 2013;20(12):1566–1576 pmid:24200484
  33. 33. Doshi J, Erus G, Ou Y, et al. MUSE: MUlti-atlas region Segmentation utilizing Ensembles of registration algorithms and parameters, and locally optimal atlas selection. NeuroImage. 2016;127:186–195. pmid:26679328
  34. 34. Sidney S, Jacobs DR, Haskell WL, et al. Comparison of two methods of assessing physical activity in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1991;133(12):1231–1245. pmid:2063831
  35. 35. Jacobs DR, Hahn LP, Haskell WL, Pirie P, Sidney S. Validity and Reliability of Short Physical Activity History: Cardia and the Minnesota Heart Health Program. J Cardiopulm Rehabil. 1989;9(11):448–459. pmid:29657358
  36. 36. Frazier PA, Tix AP, Barron KE. Testing moderator and mediator effects in counseling psychology research. Journal of counseling psychology. 2004;51(1)115–134.
  37. 37. Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51(6):1173–1182. pmid:3806354
  38. 38. Sobel ME. Aysmptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociological methodology. 1982;290–212.
  39. 39. Munoz DG, Ganapathy GR, Eliasziw M, Hachinski V. Educational attainment and socioeconomic status of patients with autopsy-confirmed Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2000;57(1):85–89. pmid:10634453
  40. 40. Liu Y, Julkunen V, Paajanen T, et al. Education increases reserve against Alzheimer’s disease—evidence from structural MRI analysis. Neuroradiology. 2012;54(9):929–938. pmid:22246242
  41. 41. Fotenos AF, Mintun MA, Snyder AZ, Morris JC, Buckner RL. Brain volume decline in aging: evidence for a relation between socioeconomic status, preclinical Alzheimer disease, and reserve. Arch Neurol. 2008;65(1):113–120. pmid:18195148
  42. 42. Elbejjani M, Fuhrer R, Abrahamowicz M, et al. Life-Course Socioeconomic Position and Hippocampal Atrophy in a Prospective Cohort of Older Adults. Psychosom Med. 2017;79(1):14–23. pmid:27428856
  43. 43. Stringhini S, Sabia S, Shipley M, et al. Association of socioeconomic position with health behaviors and mortality. JAMA. 2010;303(12):1159–1166. pmid:20332401
  44. 44. Lantz PM, House JS, Lepkowski JM, Williams DR, Mero RP, Chen J. Socioeconomic factors, health behaviors, and mortality: results from a nationally representative prospective study of US adults. JAMA. 1998;279(21):1703–1708. pmid:9624022
  45. 45. Merchant C, Tang MX, Albert S, Manly J, Stern Y, Mayeux R. The influence of smoking on the risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1999;52(7):1408–1412. pmid:10227626
  46. 46. Launer LJ, Andersen K, Dewey ME, EURODEM Incidence Research Group and Work Groups, et al. Rates and risk factors for dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: results from EURODEM pooled analyses. European Studies of Dementia. Neurology. 1999;52(1):78–84. pmid:9921852
  47. 47. Durazzo TC, Mattsson N, Weiner MW, Initiative AsDN Smoking and increased Alzheimer’s disease risk: a review of potential mechanisms. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(3 Suppl):S122–145. pmid:24924665
  48. 48. Durazzo TC, Meyerhoff DJ, Murray DE. Comparison of Regional Brain Perfusion Levels in Chronically Smoking and Non-Smoking Adults. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2015;12(7):8198–8213. pmid:26193290
  49. 49. Debette S, Seshadri S, Beiser A, et al. Midlife vascular risk factor exposure accelerates structural brain aging and cognitive decline. Neurology. 2011;77(5):461–468. pmid:21810696
  50. 50. Durazzo TC, Meyerhoff DJ, Yoder KK, Murray DE. Cigarette smoking is associated with amplified age-related volume loss in subcortical brain regions. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2017;177:228–236. pmid:28622625
  51. 51. Elbejjani M, Auer R, Jacobs DR, et al. Cigarette smoking and gray matter brain volumes in middle age adults: the CARDIA Brain MRI sub-study. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9(1):78. pmid:30741945
  52. 52. Dube SR, Asman K, Malarcher A, Carabollo R. Cigarette smoking among adults and trends in smoking cessation—United States, 2008. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2009;58(44):1227–1232. pmid:19910909
  53. 53. Schroeder SA, Koh HK. Tobacco control 50 years after the 1964 surgeon general’s report. JAMA. 2014;311(2):141–143. pmid:24399551
  54. 54. Hiscock R, Bauld L, Amos A, Fidler JA, Munafò M. Socioeconomic status and smoking: a review. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1248:107–123. pmid:22092035