Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Speed change discrimination for motion in depth using constant world and retinal speeds

  • Abigail R. I. Lee ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    aril@st-andrews.ac.uk

    Affiliation School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife, United Kingdom

  • Justin M. Ales ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors are joint senior authors on this work.

    Affiliation School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife, United Kingdom

  • Julie M. Harris

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors are joint senior authors on this work.

    Affiliation School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife, United Kingdom

Abstract

Motion at constant speed in the world maps into retinal motion very differently for lateral motion and motion in depth. The former is close to linear, for the latter, constant speed objects accelerate on the retina as they approach. Motion in depth is frequently studied using speeds that are constant on the retina, and are thus not consistent with real-world constant motion. Our aim here was to test whether this matters: are we more sensitive to real-world motion? We measured speed change discrimination for objects undergoing accelerating retinal motion in depth (consistent with constant real-world speed), and constant retinal motion in depth (consistent with real-world deceleration). Our stimuli contained both looming and binocular disparity cues to motion in depth. We used a speed change discrimination task to obtain thresholds for conditions with and without binocular and looming motion in depth cues. We found that speed change discrimination thresholds were similar for accelerating retinal speed and constant retinal speed and were notably poor compared to classic speed discrimination thresholds. We conclude that the ecologically valid retinal acceleration in our stimuli neither helps, nor hinders, our ability to make judgements in a speed change discrimination task.

Introduction

The perception of motion is crucial for human vision: not only do we live in a world where important objects move, but we also move ourselves. Human perception of the motion around us has been studied in great detail. From this we know our visual system performs specific processing of motion speed and direction (for a recent review, see [1]). Because the retina can be thought of as a two-dimensional array, the vast majority of this work has been on motion constrained to the two dimensions that specify the fronto-parallel plane. In the world, however, motion actually occurs in three dimensions. It is therefore essential to understand motion in both two and three dimensions, and to understand the differences between them.

Differences between 2D and 3D motion

There are two key differences between 2D and 3D motion: (i) the mapping between speed in the world and on the retina, and (ii) the visual cues available for each. For 2D motion, speed in the world is proportional to speed on the retina, with the exact proportion depending on distance to the observer. When an object undergoes fairly small motions in the fronto-parallel plane (lateral motion), the distance to the observer stays relatively constant, therefore a constant speed in the world results in an effectively constant speed on the retina. However, when an object changes distance from the observer, this relationship breaks down. When an object moves towards or away from an observer at a constant speed in the real world, the image of that object on the retina accelerates or decelerates respectively, as shown in Fig 1A, and described in a recent review [2]. Fig 1A depicts 4 bars, each corresponding to the same object in the world at different distances from the retina, with the visual angle subtended by each bar’s retinal image in the right panel. The red bar, closest to the eye, subtends the largest retinal image. The black bar that is the furthest away from the eye creates the smallest retinal image. Whilst the distance between each bar is the same in the world, as a bar moves closer to the eye at a constant world speed, the distance moved between two locations in depth causes a greater change in visual angle, generating retinal acceleration as an object approaches the observer. This acceleration on the retina is not constant: it increases as the object approaches the observer. Many experiments on motion in depth have used constant retinal speed (for example, [39]). Such motion is consistent with the stimulus decelerating in the world. To represent a constant speed in the world, retinal motion must accelerate as an object approaches (see Fig 1B).

thumbnail
Fig 1. How constant world speed is translated into accelerating retinal speed.

(A) Illustrates how objects moving at a constant speed in depth in the world results in acceleration on the retina. Black, blue, purple and red lines/angles respectively, represent the object location/retinal projection at 4 consecutive time points. (B) Constant speed in the world (left) maps to accelerating speed on the retina (right). From a starting position of 117cm from the eye, objects travelling towards the eye at three different speeds accelerate, increasing rapidly in speed the closer to the eye the objects get.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.g001

For lateral motion, the major cue used to determine speed is the retinal speed of motion. Speed discrimination has been studied extensively for 2D lateral motion, and within an optimal range of speeds, people can discriminate between speeds that are only 5–7% different [1015].

For motion in depth, there are both binocular and monocular cues available. There are two distinct binocular cues to motion in depth: changing disparity (CD) and interocular velocity difference (IOVD; see [16] for a review). Even with both cues available, speed discrimination is significantly worse than the best possible thresholds for lateral motion [3,4,17].

The major monocular cue for motion in depth is looming, where the retinal image of an object moving at a constant speed in the world expands at an accelerating rate as the object approaches the observer (e.g. see Fig 1A). Speed discrimination thresholds of around 5% have been reported for looming motion [18]. Similarly, speed discrimination thresholds of around 7% have been reported using expanding optic flow fields [19].

Use of constant world and retinal speeds

There is a complication in the literature around the perception of motion in depth, namely that some studies have used constant world motion (e.g. [18,20,21]), and some have used constant retinal motion (e.g. [39]). This is potentially a problem because if there is a difference in how we discriminate the speed of motion in depth with a constant world speed or a constant retinal speed, we may have been making assumptions about motion in depth that are not appropriate for more ecologically valid stimuli.

Many experiments conducted to investigate the perception of motion in depth have used constant retinal speeds (e.g. [38]). Constant retinal speeds were also used in one of the few examples of where looming and binocular cues to motion in depth have been studied together [9].

Studies that have used accelerating retinal speed with motion in depth are less common. Examples using looming include Sekuler’s [18] speed discrimination study. Another study including looming, changing disparity and vergence information used accelerating retinal speed and reported that these cues contribute differently to our perception of position change and velocity of motion in depth [20]. A third study investigated time to contact for motion in depth with monocular and binocular cues [21]. Here, it was found that participants overestimated time to contact, suggesting they did not utilize the acceleration present in their stimulus.

Very few studies have compared motion perception for constant-world and constant-retinal motion. Adding self-acceleration to optic flow stimuli decreases the length of time before participants begin to feel vection, the feeling of self-motion, and increases the perceived speed of self-motion compared to when a constant velocity stimulus is used [22]. Additionally, if an accelerating stimulus, rather than a decelerating stimulus, is presented and followed by a speed discrimination task, speed discrimination thresholds are reduced, suggesting that if there is a “context” of acceleration, speed discrimination may be easier [23]. However, as described above, acceleration appears not to be useful when judging time to contact [21]. Thus, it is not clear whether the visual system is most sensitive to one or the other type of motion.

In the experiment discussed here, we were interested in how the speed of motion in depth is processed. However, measuring speed discrimination is notoriously difficult because in most paradigms observers can use duration or distance rather than specifically responding to speed [24]. We therefore chose a speed discrimination task that observers could not perform by using distance or duration alone, namely a speed change discrimination task [13,25,26]. In this task, participants viewed a standard interval which travelled at either a constant speed in the world or on the retina, and an interval that travelled equal amounts slower and then faster than the speed in the standard interval. The average speed over the course of the speed change interval was the same as in the standard interval, and the distance travelled and duration were the same as in the standard interval.

For the speed change discrimination task we chose two main 3D motion stimuli types. Our World stimulus contained binocular and looming cues and travelled at constant world speeds (accelerating retinal speed). Our Retina stimulus contained binocular and size-change cues and travelled at constant retinal speeds. As well as these, we used two control stimuli. The World Control stimulus moved with accelerating retinal speed and contained lateral motion and looming information only. The Retina Control stimulus moved the same speed and distance on the retina as the Retina stimulus, but contained no cues to motion in depth and moved laterally. Using these stimuli, our aim was to investigate whether the visual system was more or less sensitive to speed changes in motion in depth for constant Real World or Retinal motion using a speed change discrimination task.

Methods

Participants

11 participants (8 female, 3 male) were recruited for this experiment. The number of participants to recruit was decided in advance of data collection. This study can be considered a classic small-N design typical of visual psychophysics using many measurements per participant; the advantages for using these designs has been discussed in some detail [27]. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were required to have a stereoacuity of at least 120 arcseconds as measured by a TNO test (16th edition). One participant was rejected for having a higher stereoacuity threshold, and another was rejected for failing to reach 80% correct responses in the highest speed change level of two conditions, leaving 9 participants (7 female, 2 male) to complete the study. Prior to the experiment start, all participants gave written informed consent. This consent procedure and all other procedures were approved by the St. Andrews University Teaching and Research Ethics Committee (UTREC; Approval code: PS11904) and adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Materials

Stimuli were presented using a Mac Pro on an Iiyama MM904UTA Vision Master Pro 455 cathode ray tube screen with a resolution of 1280x1024 and a refresh rate of 85Hz. The screen was luminance calibrated using a Cambridge Research Systems ColorCal MK II colorimeter and size calibrated by manually measuring the size of a square of a set number of pixels on the screen to obtain an accurate pixel per centimeter conversion. The screen was viewed through a four-mirror stereoscope from a distance of 97cm (including the distances between the mirrors of the stereoscope).

Stimulus design

All stimuli were generated using MATLAB R2014b (The MathWorks Inc., 2014) with Psychtoolbox-3 [2830]. All code used to generate the stimuli and run the experiment, has been deposited in an open access repository and available via https://osf.io/7mg63/. Two different stimulus types were used in these experiments. Both stimuli were presented on a grey screen with a luminance of 29.9 cd/m2. The first consisted of a drifting grating subtending 4 degrees of visual angle with a spatial frequency of 1 cycle per degree moving laterally from left to right at a constant retinal speed. This drifting grating stimulus was used exclusively in 2 training blocks.

The other stimulus was a pair of vertical white lines with a luminance of 59.9 cd/m2 that could either move laterally or in depth, with constant world or constant retinal speed. Stimuli moving towards the observer contained both binocular (from changing binocular disparity and interocular velocity difference) and looming (from the increasing separation between the lines) cues to motion in depth. The lines themselves did not change size and remained a constant 0.95 arcmin wide on the screen. These lines were 13.0 degrees tall. An aperture frame of close-to uniformly distributed luminance noise (individual pixels given randomly chosen grey levels) 1.58 degrees wide with a minimum luminance of 0.09 cd/m2 and a maximum luminance of 59.9 cd/m2 was also presented to aid fixation in the plane of the screen. The vertical line stimulus spanned the full height of the monitor that was within the aperture. Stimuli were presented with a central white fixation cross (37.9 arcmin tall, 37.9 arcmin wide) with a luminance of 59.9 cd/m2. A 56.9 by 56.9 arcmin white box with a luminance of 59.9 cd/m2 appeared around the fixation cross when participants were to give their responses. The moving line stimulus was used in 1 training condition and 8 experimental conditions. The moving line stimulus, and the drifting grating stimulus used for training, are shown in Fig 2. Examples of the moving line stimulus are shown in S1 and S2 Figs.

thumbnail
Fig 2. The stimuli presented to participants.

(A) Shows the drifting grating stimulus used only for training purposes. (B) Shows the moving line stimulus. The left side of the image is viewed only by the left eye. The right side of the image is viewed only by the right eye. The two halves of both stimuli are fused into a single image by the mirror stereoscope.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.g002

Each experimental trial contained two intervals. One was labeled as the ‘standard’ speed change interval, where the lines moved at a constant speed. The other was the ‘variable’ interval, which contained an instantaneous speed change during the trial. Four stimulus types and associated experimental conditions were designed:

  1. (I). World: constant world speed of motion in depth. Here each eye’s view contained lines moving with accelerating motion on the retina. Fig 3A shows how the speed changed on the retina during the standard interval, and Fig 3B shows an example of retinal speed during a variable interval. Both binocular and looming cues to motion in depth were present.
  2. (II). World Control: lateral motion consistent with one eye’s view from the “World” stimulus, but presented to both eyes, so containing looming but not binocular cues to motion in depth (Fig 2A and 2B show retinal motion for different World Control intervals).
  3. (III). Retina: constant retinal speed of motion in depth. Here, each eye’s view contained lines moving at a constant retinal speed, consistent with decelerating motion in the world (retinal motion for a standard speed change interval is shown in Fig 3C and a variable speed change interval in Fig 3D). The lines moved apart from each other at a constant retinal speed.
  4. (IV). Retina Control: lateral motion with speeds consistent with one eye’s view from the “Retina” stimulus, but presented to both eyes. There were no binocular cues to motion in depth and no looming or size-change cues (Fig 2C and 2D shows the retinal motion).
thumbnail
Fig 3. The structure of speed change intervals represented as a speed-time graph for retinal speed.

(A) shows a ‘standard’ speed change interval (no change in speed) for the World and World Control conditions whilst (B) shows a ‘variable’ speed change interval (instantaneous speed changes during the trial) for the World and World Control conditions. (C) shows a standard speed change interval for the Retina and Retina Control conditions and (D) represents a maximum speed change interval in the Retina and Retina Control conditions. a represents the speed at the very beginning of the interval, whilst b represents the speed immediately before the step change in speed. c the speed immediately after the speed change, and d represents the speed at the very end of the interval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.g003

World and World Control stimuli.

For the World stimuli, we accurately rendered the retinal view for a real object moving in depth at a constant speed. Calculating the correct stimulus location to render was done using the following equations: (1) (2) Where θ is the visual angle, xw is the position of the object along the x axis of the real world, zw is the position of the object along the z axis of the real world, xs is the position of the object on the screen and D is the viewing distance (see Fig 4). Eq 1 converts world positions into a retinal angle. This retinal angle is then used in Eq 2 to find the position the object should appear at on the screen. For finding the correct stimulus location on the screen, Eqs 1 and 2 simplify to Eq 3: (3)

thumbnail
Fig 4. A visual representation of the values used to calculate the correct position for a 3D object to appear on a 2D screen.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.g004

As the retinal position is defined by the arctangent (see Eq 1), and the speed and acceleration are the derivative and second derivative of position respectively, this means the speed and acceleration of motion in depth are not constant. Instead, speed and acceleration increase as the stimulus approaches the retina, as is the case when viewing a real approaching object in the world. For the World Control stimulus, we took the left monocular half image from the World stimulus and displayed it to both eyes, thus the lines move from left to right but contain looming information. Like the World stimulus, the World Control stimulus contained accelerating retinal speed, as would be present if an object was approaching the observer. However, because the relationship between world and retinal speed is close to linear for 2D motion except at large eccentricities, and because the World Control accelerates as if the stimulus is moving towards the observer, the World Control stimulus is not representative of a 2D object moving at a constant world speed.

Retina and Retina Control stimuli.

For the Retina stimulus, each eye’s view was delivered at constant retinal speed, but in opposite directions, specifying binocular motion in depth. This is consistent with decelerating motion in the world. The stimulus contained size change information similar to the looming information in the World stimulus, but with size change at a constant retinal speed. The size-change cue was not identical to looming as looming by definition involves non-constant retinal speeds. The Retina Control stimulus consisted of lateral motion at a constant retinal speed, but with no looming information present. The speeds used in these two conditions were calculated by taking the mean of the speeds presented to the observer in the World conditions–see below for details.

Procedure

A 2-interval forced choice task was used. The participant’s goal was to identify the interval that contained a step change in speed (like those in Fig 2B and 2D), compared to an interval that did not have this step change (like those in Fig 2A and 2C). Using a speed change discrimination task such as this, rather than a traditional speed discrimination task, allows us to control against the use of both distance and duration cues in speed perception. This was accomplished by carefully choosing the speeds used in the changing stimulus so that the total duration and total distance travelled was identical in the standard and test intervals (see Fig 5). Thus, the first half of the stimulus was slower than the standard and the second half of the stimulus was faster than the standard.

thumbnail
Fig 5. A distance-time graph of the different levels used in a speed change discrimination experiment.

Demonstrates how the stimulus for each level of speed change discrimination stimuli travelled over the same total distance in the same total duration, in terms of the distance travelled in the real world.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.g005

In each interval, a static image of the stimulus appeared for 250ms before moving for 1 second. If a speed change were to occur in the interval it would always happen after 500ms of motion. The stimuli in each condition were presented using a 7-level method of constant stimuli design. Each condition was divided into three blocks with 10 trials per level, giving 70 trials per block and 210 per condition. Blocks were presented in a random order. No feedback was given during the main experiment, we aimed to avoid training participants from using something other than the speeds present to complete the task. In each trial participants heard one beep immediately before the first interval, and two beeps immediately before the second interval.

Training blocks.

The three training blocks were always presented in the same order. The first training block consisted of the drifting grating stimulus moving at a constant retinal speed and presented with audio feedback. If the participant correctly picked which grating changed speed they would hear a high-pitched beep, and if they incorrectly picked, they would hear a low-pitched beep. The second training block was visually identical to the first but had no audio feedback. The third block used the Retinal Control stimulus with the two moving lines and no feedback. In each training block participants viewed 30 trials with an “easy” speed change and 30 trials with a “hard” speed change, giving a total of 180 training trials. In the standard intervals of these training conditions, the stimuli moved at 283.4 arcmin/s on the retina. In the “hard” test intervals, the stimulus changed from moving at 212.6 arcmin/s to 354.1 arcmin/s. In the “easy” test intervals, the stimulus initially moved at 70.9 arcmin/s before jumping to 495.3 arcmin/s. Participants were required to obtain correct performance on at least 50 out of a total of 60 trials in the final training block to proceed to the main experiment. No participants were excluded based on their performance in the training blocks.

Main experiment.

For the main experiment, we used 8 experimental conditions. Our conditions were named in accordance with our four stimuli types listed above, each with “fast” and “slow” speeds. “Fast” speeds were based around a standard interval of approximately 40cm/s in the world while “slow” speeds were based around a standard interval of approximately 20cm/s in the world. We will refer to each condition using a short-hand term, for example, World-Fast is our fast-speed condition replicating 3D motion in the real world by using fast accelerating retinal speeds, and Retina Control-Slow is the control version of the 3D Retina Slow condition. As a result, the stimuli listed above gave rise to the following 8 experimental conditions: World-Fast, World-Slow; World Control-Fast, World Control-Slow; Retina-Fast, Retina-Slow, Retina Control-Fast, and Retina Control-Slow. A summary of the speeds in arcmin/s used in the Fast conditions is shown in Table 1, whilst a summary of the speeds in arcmin/s used in the Slow conditions is shown in Table 2. We include details for the standard interval, and for the maximum change test interval (level 7 in Fig 5).

thumbnail
Table 1. The speed(s) presented in the standard and maximum speed change level for each of the fast speed conditions before and after the speed change.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. The speed(s) presented in the standard and maximum speed change level for each of the slow speed conditions before and after the speed change.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.t002

The speeds in arcmin/s for the World-Fast condition standard level correspond to a constant world speed of 40 cm/s in depth. The speeds for the maximum change level from this condition correspond to world speeds of 10 cm/s in depth before the change and 70 cm/s in depth after the change. The world speeds for each level of this condition are shown in Table 3. For the World-Slow condition, the standard level speed corresponds to a world speed of 20 cm/s and the maximum change level speeds correspond to a speed of 5 cm/s before the speed change and 35 cm/s after the speed change. The world speeds for the levels of the World-Slow condition are shown in Table 4. S1 and S2 Figs demonstrate a speed change interval and a standard interval respectively from the World-Fast condition.

thumbnail
Table 3. The constant world speeds for each level of the World-Fast condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. The constant world speeds for each level of the World-Slow condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.t004

Analysis

Percent correct responses were recorded as a function of proportion speed change at the changepoint, which was calculated using Eq 4: (4) where c is the speed immediately after the speed change and b is the speed immediately before the speed change, as shown on the graphs in Fig 3. Percent correct responses were analyzed in this way to avoid biasing the results due to the increased speed in the Fast conditions, or due to the higher end speeds of the World and World Control conditions compared to the Retina and Retina Control conditions. From participant responses, 75% correct thresholds for determining which interval contained the speed change were found for each condition by fitting cumulative normal psychometric functions using MATLAB R2014b (The MathWorks Inc., 2014) with the Palamedes toolbox [31]. A fixed guess rate of 0.5 and a fixed lapse rate of 0 was used for all psychometric function fits. Psychometric functions for all 8 conditions from a representative participant can be found in S3 Fig.

Statistics

Following psychometric analysis, JASP Version 0.9.1 (JASP Team, 2018) was used to conduct multiple 2-way repeated measures ANOVAs on our data. The first ANOVA involved a comparison of the World and Retina thresholds, as well as a comparison of the corresponding Slow and Fast thresholds. The second ANOVA compared the World and World Control thresholds along with the corresponding Slow and Fast thresholds. The final ANOVA compared the Retina and Retina Control thresholds and the corresponding Fast and Slow thresholds. We performed 3 2-way repeated measures ANOVAs rather than a single 3-way repeated measures ANOVA because we wanted to test something different with each comparison. With the comparison between the World and Retina conditions we wanted to compare speed change discrimination thresholds for constant world and constant retinal speeds. For our World and World Control conditions, we were interested in what effect removing binocular cues to motion in depth would have on the speed change discrimination thresholds. For our Retina and Retina Control conditions, we wanted to observe the effect of removing all cues to motion in depth on the speed change discrimination thresholds. It would not be possible to make these comparisons with a 3-way repeated measures ANOVA. In order to be able to determine if the null or alternate hypothesis was more likely, Bayesian repeated measures ANOVAs were also conducted. These provide different models to explore which differences within the ANOVA factors explain the data. The prior used was a Cauchy distribution, centered on zero, with a width of 0.5, which is the default prior for Bayesian repeated measures ANOVAs in JASP Version 0.9.1. For a review of the rationale for using Bayesian statistics in comparison to frequentist statistics see [32]. We are reporting Bayes Factors in two ways. BF10 values indicate the ratio of the likelihood of the alternative compared to the null. BF01 values give the ratio of the likelihood of the null compared to the alternate. These quantities are the inverse of each other.

The data collected in this experiment and the code written to analyze the data has been deposited in an open access repository and is available at https://osf.io/7mg63/.

Results

A summary of the proportion speed change thresholds for each of our 8 conditions is shown in Fig 6. The results of this experiment are then considered in terms of the comparison between our World and Retina conditions, our World and World Control conditions and our Retina and Retina Control conditions.

thumbnail
Fig 6. 75% proportion speed change thresholds.

Blue bars represent World conditions while red bars represent Retina conditions. Lighter coloring indicates a control condition. Error bars are between-subject confidence intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.g006

World versus Retina

The 75% thresholds across observers, described in terms of the proportion speed change at the changepoint, were very similar across all four World and Retina conditions (see Fig 6). We found no significant difference between the World and Retina conditions (F(1,8) = 0.959, p = 0.356, η2 = 0.107) or the Fast and Slow conditions (F(1,8) = 0.324, p = 0.585, η2 = 0.039). We also found no interaction effect between our World vs. Retina and Fast vs. Slow comparisons (F(1,8) = 0.461, p = 0.516, η2 = 0.054).

We also conducted Bayesian statistical analyses to obtain Bayes Factors for the World vs. Retina and corresponding Fast vs. Slow data. These allow us to investigate more carefully the likelihood of the data being consistent with the alternative hypothesis (difference between thresholds across conditions) or the null hypothesis (no difference between thresholds across conditions). The Bayesian repeated measures ANOVA conducted here provides Bayes Factors for 4 different models which may explain the data: (1) A World vs. Retina model, which assumes that only the difference between the World and Retina conditions explains the data. (2) A Fast vs. Slow model, which assumes that only the differences between the Fast and Slow conditions explain the data. (3) A World vs. Retina and Fast vs. Slow model, which assumes that the linear combination of differences between World and Retina and Fast and Slow explain this data. (4) A World vs. Retina, Fast vs. Slow and interaction model, which assumes that the data can be explained by the differences between the World and Retina conditions, the Fast and Slow conditions and an additional interaction between these factors. A Bayes factor of 3 or larger is considered substantial evidence that a set of results support either the null (if using a BF01 value that is weighted toward the null) or alternative (if using a BF10 value that is weighted towards the alternative) hypothesis, while a Bayes factor between 1 and 3 only indicates weak or anecdotal evidence in support of a hypothesis [3335].

A Bayes Factor (BF01) of 1.596 was found for the World vs. Retina model, indicating anecdotal support for the null hypothesis and that these results are 1.596 times more likely under the null hypothesis. A Bayes Factor (BF01) of 2.981 was found for the Fast vs. Slow model, indicating these findings are 2.981 times more likely under the case of the null hypothesis, and anecdotal support for the null hypothesis. A Bayes Factor (BF01) of 4.802 was found for the World vs. Retina and Fast vs. Slow model, indicating substantial support for the null hypothesis. A Bayes Factor (BF01) of 10.582 was found for the World vs. Retina, Fast vs. Slow and interaction model, which indicates strong evidence for the null hypothesis [33,35]. For each tested model it is more likely to observe the data if the null hypothesis was true than if the alternate hypothesis was true. These Bayes factors suggest that either the visual system is as sensitive to changes in constant world speed or constant retinal speed or any difference in sensitivity is small.

World versus World Control

75% thresholds were higher for the World conditions compared to the World Control conditions (see Fig 6). World Control stimuli delivered significantly lower thresholds than the World condition(F(1,8) = 8.116, p = 0.022, η2 = 0.504). There was no significant difference between the Fast and Slow conditions (F(1,8) = 4.404, p = 0.069, η2 = 0.355), and no significant interaction between the World vs. World Control comparison and the Fast vs. Slow comparison (F(1,8) = 1.036, p = 0.338, η2 = 0.115).

Similar to for the World vs. Retina data, the Bayesian repeated measures ANOVA used here provided 4 models that could explain the World vs. World Control data. For the World vs. World Control model, a Bayes Factor (BF10) of 9.229 was found. This is considered substantial evidence in favor of the alternative hypothesis that the data is explained by a difference between the World and World Control conditions [3335]. It also suggests that this data is 9.229 times more likely to occur if the alternative hypothesis is true. A Bayes Factor (BF10) of 1.227 was found for the Fast vs. Slow model, indicating ‘anecdotal support’ for the alternative hypothesis that there was a difference between these Fast and Slow conditions [34]. A Bayes Factor (BF10) of 18.472 was found for the model that linearly combines World vs. World Control and Fast vs. Slow, indicating strong support for the alternative hypothesis [33,35]. A Bayes Factor (BF10) of 8.647 was found for the World vs. World Control, Fast vs. Slow and interaction model, suggesting substantial evidence for the alternative hypothesis. These findings indicate that the addition of binocular motion in depth information may make the speed change discrimination task more difficult. In all cases the alternate model is more likely than the null model. The model using a linear combination of World vs. World Control and Fast vs. Slow factors has the highest Bayes Factor, making it the most likely model given the data. In addition, the Bayes Factor decreases when the interaction term is added to this model indicating that there is evidence against including the interaction term.

Retina versus Retina Control

The Retina Control-Fast thresholds were lower than for the other Retina and Retina Control conditions (see Fig 6). We found a significant difference between the Retina and Retina Control conditions (F(1,8) = 5.484, p = 0.047, η2 = 0.407) and the corresponding Fast and Slow conditions (F(1,8) = 6.183, p = 0.038, η2 = 0.436). We additionally found a significant interaction effect between the Retina vs. Retina Control comparison and the Fast vs Slow comparison (F(1,8) = 46.360, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.853).

However, a Bayes Factor (BF10) of 1.532 for a Fast vs. Slow model from the Bayesian repeated measures ANOVA suggests only anecdotal support for the alternative hypothesis that there is a difference between the Fast and Slow conditions [34]. The Bayes Factor (BF10) for the Retina vs. Retina Control model was 3.885, suggesting substantial support for the alternative hypothesis that the data is explained by differences between the Retina and Retina control conditions [3335]. A Bayes Factor (BF10) of 8.370 was also found for the Retina vs. Retina Control and Fast vs. Slow model, indicating substantial support for the alternative hypothesis. A Bayes Factor (BF10) of 40.682 was found for the Retina vs. Retina Control, Fast vs. Slow model and interaction model, indicating very strong support for the alternative hypothesis that this data is explained by the differences between the Retina and Retina Control conditions, the Fast and Slow conditions and the interaction between these two factors [33,35]. This suggests that removing both monocular and binocular motion in depth information from stimuli with a constant retinal speed made the speed change discrimination task easier.

Discussion

We hypothesized that the visual system might be more sensitive to changes in constant world speeds, which accelerate on the retina, than to changes in constant retinal speeds. Our results show no significant difference between constant world speed and constant retinal speed conditions, as demonstrated by our World vs. Retina comparison. Bayes factors provide anecdotal evidence that there is no difference between the World and Retina speed conditions. However, these results suggest that when 3D motion cues are removed, thresholds are lower. Mixed evidence regarding whether it easier to do the task when the conditions contained fast speeds was also found. Below we discuss how our results compare with literature on the perception and use of acceleration in visual stimuli, speed discrimination at different speeds and comparisons between 2D and 3D motion perception. We also discuss the nature of speed change discrimination in more detail, and speculate about why speed change discrimination thresholds are high.

Perception of acceleration

Although motion with constant speed in the world has greater ecological validity, there is evidence that the acceleration in the more ecologically valid stimuli is difficult to perceive. It has been demonstrated that constant acceleration is difficult to perceive and discriminate, particularly in comparison to speed discrimination [3642].

Whilst there appears to be a consensus on the difficulty of acceleration discrimination, estimates of how large the speed change needs to be for the acceleration to be visible varies greatly between studies, anywhere from a 25% to 3 times increase in speed [36,37,39,40,43,44]. It is also important to note that these studies were looking at acceleration discrimination, with the majority of using constant acceleration in their stimuli. This is different to the acceleration present on the retina when we view objects moving towards us in the world, and in our World stimuli, which is non-constant. For example, with a task where participants had to report whether a ball was accelerating, decelerating or moving at a constant speed, Schmerler [36] reported that for acceleration to be detected on 50% of occasions, the end speed needed to be 2.3 to 3.2 times different from the initial speed. Meanwhile, Gottsdanker et al. [37] used a task where an accelerating or decelerating speed had to be discriminated from a stimulus moving with a constant velocity, and found a range of 75% threshold values of percentage speed change, from 26% to 157%, that varied according to presentation time and mean velocity. Others have also reported thresholds of around a 20–25% change in speed for acceleration discrimination [43,44]. Even the values reported in these studies of a 20–25% change in speed as a threshold for acceleration discrimination is considerably higher than thresholds found for speed discrimination, which can be as small as a 5–7% difference in speed [1012,15,18,19]. Despite the reported differences in acceleration thresholds, the literature suggests that acceleration discrimination is relatively difficult. This raises the question of whether accelerations like those present in our World and World Control conditions are perceived when they are available in stimuli. If they cannot, we may expect there to no difference between the World and Retina conditions, despite the increased ecological validity of the World conditions, as we have found here.

Use of acceleration information

There is evidence to suggest that not only is acceleration difficult to discriminate, but it is also not used when it is present in time-to-contact tasks [21,4448], or in manual interception tasks [49]. Using a task to discriminate an accelerating stimulus from a decelerating stimulus, Brouwer et al. [44] suggested that participants were likely comparing the start and end speeds of the stimuli rather than judging acceleration itself, concluding that people do not use acceleration during interception of approaching objects. There is evidence to suggest that we are better at intercepting or catching objects that have been accelerated by gravity, and that we may have an internal model for gravity [5052]. However, it also appears that we anticipate gravitational acceleration, which may suggest again that instead of perceiving the acceleration itself, our improved ability to intercept gravity-accelerated objects may be due to experience with gravity [53].

To sum up, the majority of evidence appears to suggest that acceleration is likely rarely used by the visual system, and when we are forced to make discriminations about it, these are usually very difficult. We found no clear difference in speed change thresholds between constant world speed (acceleration on the retina) and constant retinal speed. This may suggest that the acceleration on the retina in our World conditions may not have been used by the visual system.

Comparing lateral motion and motion in depth

We report a significant difference in the World vs. World Control and the Retina vs. Retina Control comparisons, with Bayes factors for both comparisons indicating substantial evidence that removing either binocular, or both binocular and monocular, cues to motion in depth from a stimulus made the task easier. These findings are consistent with a growing body of evidence that suggests several aspects of motion in depth are harder to perceive than their respective lateral motion equivalents [17,5460]. The idea of stereomotion suppression was first proposed by Tyler [55,60], who found that sensitivity to depth movements was reduced compared to lateral movements of the same size on the retina, and reasoned that it must be a result of the processes involved in fusing the two eyes’ images. It has been suggested that motion in depth thresholds may be higher due to the early averaging of motion signals between the two eyes, which would give an average motion of close to zero for motion in depth, making it harder to perceive [56,57]. Importantly, stereomotion suppression has also been demonstrated in speed discrimination tasks [17]. Brooks et al. [17] found that for their random dot stimuli, speed discrimination thresholds for lateral motion were a factor of 1.8 lower than those for the respective motion in depth stimuli. Our results support these previous findings of stereomotion suppression for 3D motion and indicate that this effect also exists for speed change discrimination.

Comparing faster and slower speeds

In our comparison between the Retina and Retina Control Fast and Slow conditions, we found that speed change discrimination was significantly easier with faster speeds. However, the Bayes factor indicated only anecdotal support for the hypothesis that there would be a difference between the slow and fast speeds. We additionally found there was no significant difference between the other Fast and Slow conditions.

Even the fast speeds used here were much slower than those typically used in the 2D motion literature. This is because large 3D motions translate into small motions on the retina, and because motion in depth experiments that include binocular disparity typically use slow retinal speeds to ensure that stimuli remain binocularly fused. However, there are several studies, for 2D and 3D motion, demonstrating that speed discrimination thresholds are worse at very slow speeds [3,1012,15]. It has been reported for a motion in depth experiment using speeds of 0.2–0.3 deg/s that speed discrimination thresholds were in the range of 10–20% [3].

For 2D motion, speed discrimination thresholds are best within a range of 4–64 deg/s, and become progressively worse as motion is slower than 4 deg/s or faster than 64 deg/s [10,12]. McKee [11] demonstrated that below 2 deg/s thresholds grew towards 8–9%. For speeds as low as 0.5 deg/s, thresholds approach 18% [15]. The speeds used in our study were generally much slower than this (Tables 1 and 2). Based on the literature on speed discrimination, we would expect thresholds for all of the speeds used in our experiments to be sub-optimal, but we would still expect that our Fast speed change discrimination thresholds would be lower than our Slow speed change discrimination thresholds unless at the Fast speeds participants had already reached close to minimum performance abilities.

However, almost all the available literature has used a speed discrimination task. Speed change discrimination has barely been studied, and never, to our knowledge, for motion in depth. In the next section, we review our current understanding of speed change perception in more detail.

The nature of speed change discrimination

Our is the first study to have explored speed change discrimination of motion in depth with binocular and monocular cues. Like other studies using a speed change task, thresholds were considerably higher than those reported previously for speed discrimination tasks. Such high thresholds have previously been observed for judgments of speed change discrimination of optic flow using random dot kinematogram stimuli [13,26]. For self-motion from optic flow, Monen et al. [26] found that dividing the two speeds into intervals separated in time cut the mean thresholds for their participants from around 49% to around 24%. Similarly, Snowden et al. [13] used random dot kinematograms moving in two dimensions and found that when the different speeds were in temporally-separate intervals, the speed change threshold could be as low as a 6%, but when the two speeds were presented in a single temporal interval and compared with another temporal interval with no speed change, the threshold rose to around 30%. It has also been found that speed discrimination thresholds can be up to three times worse when two speeds were presented with no time gap between the two stimuli [25].

There are several possible explanations for speed change discrimination having higher thresholds than classic speed discrimination. First, if temporal integration were needed to obtain a measure of speed, then speed change would be difficult to detect because the integration of speed information could average out the change. The notion of temporal integration being involved in making speed change discrimination tasks difficult is supported by Mateeff et al.’s [25] findings, because as the time between the speed presentations is reduced, thresholds increase. Others have also concluded that there is likely temporal integration of speeds when they are presented close together in time [61].

Second, spatial integration of speed may also play a role. It has been found that speed discrimination is more difficult when the task is to discriminate between two speeds for an object moving along one continuous trajectory, compared to having two stimuli travelling parallel to each other at different speeds [62]. Averaging over space would make it difficult to judge a speed change.

Third, speed change discrimination tasks prevent the use of both distance or duration cues. In contrast, in speed discrimination tasks it is very difficult to prevent participants from being able to use at least one of these cues [24]. It may be that many of the speed discrimination thresholds reported in the literature have not entirely eliminated all distance and duration cues and actually report combined thresholds for speed and distance or duration, rather than speed in isolation. Speed change discrimination thresholds may be poor because participants are unable to use duration or distance to help them to respond, and may struggle to discriminate between speeds in isolation. The cause of these elevated speed change discrimination thresholds is an interesting avenue for future research.

One further possible explanation is that our thresholds may be higher than those reported previously for speed discrimination as we used only naïve participants. With naïve observers, speed discrimination can also be poor: for example, Weber fractions around 0.15–0.35 for speed discrimination have been found using naïve observers [63]. These Weber fractions are closer to our mean proportion 75% thresholds of between 0.20 and 0.37.

Conclusions

We aimed to compare speed change discrimination thresholds with a constant retinal speed, as frequently used in motion in depth studies, to those with a constant world speed, as we see when an object approaches us in the real world. We have found no evidence for a difference in speed change discrimination between these two types of stimuli. We have found results suggesting that thresholds for motion in depth are worse than those when motion in depth cues are removed. Additionally, we report speed change discrimination thresholds that are considerably higher than thresholds previously reported in speed discrimination tasks, consistent with other investigations using speed change discrimination.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. The moving line stimulus in a speed change interval of the World-Fast condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.s001

(M4V)

S2 Fig. The moving line stimulus in a standard interval of the World-Fast condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.s002

(M4V)

S3 Fig. Psychometric functions from a representative individual participant.

Dots represent participant data and the line is the psychometric fit. (A) Shows the World Fast condition fit. (B) Is the fit for the World Slow condition. (C) Is the fit for the World Control Fast condition. (D) Shows the World Control Slow condition fit. (E) Shows the Retina Fast condition fit. (F) Shows the Retina Slow condition fit. (G) Is the fit for the Retina Control Fast condition. (H) Shows the Retina Control Slow condition fit.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214766.s003

(TIF)

References

  1. 1. Burr D, Thompson P. Motion psychophysics: 1985–2010. Vision Res 2011;51:1431–56. pmid:21324335
  2. 2. Cormack LK, Czuba TB, Knoll J, Huk AC. Binocular Mechanisms of 3D Motion Processing. Annu Rev Vis Sci 2017;3:297–318. pmid:28746813
  3. 3. Harris JM, Watamaniuk SNJ. Speed discrimination of motion-in-depth using binocular cues. Vision Res 1995;35:885–96. pmid:7762146
  4. 4. Brooks KR, Stone LS. Stereomotion speed perception: Contributions from both changing disparity and interocular velocity difference over a range of relative disparities. J Vis 2004;4:1061–79. pmid:15669911
  5. 5. Brooks KR, Mather G. Perceived speed of motion in depth is reduced in the periphery. Vision Res 2000;40:3507–16. pmid:11115678
  6. 6. Brooks KR. Interocular velocity difference contributes to stereomotion speed perception. J Vis 2002;2:218–31. pmid:12678584
  7. 7. Czuba TB, Rokers B, Huk AC, Cormack LK. Speed and Eccentricity Tuning Reveal a Central Role for the Velocity-Based Cue to 3D Visual Motion. J Neurophysiol 2010;104:2886–99. pmid:20881201
  8. 8. Wardle S, Alais D. Evidence for speed sensitivity to motion in depth from binocular cues. J Vis 2013;13:1–16.
  9. 9. Beverley KI, Regan D. Seperable aftereffects of changing-size and motion-in-depth: different neural mechanisms? Vision Res 1979;19:727–723. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(79)90251-7. pmid:547484
  10. 10. de Bruyn B, Orban GA. Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns. Vision Res 1988;28:1323–35. pmid:3256150
  11. 11. McKee SP. A local mechanism for differential velocity detection. Vision Res 1981;21:491–500. pmid:7269327
  12. 12. Orban GA, de Wolf J, Maes H. Factors influencing velocity coding in the human visual system. Vision Res 1984;24:33–9. pmid:6695505
  13. 13. Snowden RJ, Braddick OJ. The temporal integration and resolution of velocity signals. Vision Res 1991;31:907–14. pmid:2035273
  14. 14. McKee SP, Welch L. Sequential recruitment in the discrimination of velocity. J Opt Soc Am A 1985;2:243–51. pmid:3973756
  15. 15. Heidenreich SM, Turano KA. Speed discrimination under stabilized and normal viewing conditions. Vision Res 1996;36:1819–25. pmid:8759450
  16. 16. Harris JM, Nefs HT, Grafton CE. Binocular vision and motion-in-depth. Spat Vis 2008;21:531–47. pmid:19017481
  17. 17. Brooks KR, Stone LS. Stereomotion suppression and the perception of speed: accuracy and precision as a function of 3D trajectory. J Vis 2006;6:1214–23. pmid:17209730
  18. 18. Sekuler AB. Simple-pooling of unidirectional motion predicts speed discrimination for looming stimuli. Vision Res 1992;32:2277–88. pmid:1288004
  19. 19. von Grünau MW, Pilgrim K, Zhou R. Velocity discrimination thresholds for flowfield motions with moving observers. Vision Res 2007;47:2453–64. pmid:17651779
  20. 20. Brenner E, Van Den Berg AV, Van Damme WJ. Perceived motion in depth. Vision Res 1996;36:699–706. pmid:8762300
  21. 21. López-Moliner J, Maiche A, Estaún S. Perception of acceleration in motion-in-depth with only monocular and both monocular and binocular information. Psicológica 2003;24:93–108.
  22. 22. Palmisano S, Allison RS, Pekin F. Accelerating self-motion displays produce more compelling vection in depth. Perception 2008;37:22–34. pmid:18399245
  23. 23. Schlack A, Krekelberg B, Albright TD. Speed perception during acceleration and deceleration. J Vis 2008;8:9.1–11. pmid:18831632
  24. 24. McKee SP, Watamaniuk SNJ. The psychophysics of motion perception. In: Smith AT, Snowden RJ, editors. Vis. Detect. Motion, London: Academic Press; 1994, p. 85–114.
  25. 25. Mateeff S, Dimitrov G, Genova B, Likova L, Stefanova M, Hohnsbein J. The discrimination of abrupt changes in speed and direction of visual motion. Vision Res 2000;40:409–15. pmid:10820621
  26. 26. Monen J, Brenner E. Detecting changes in one’s own velocity from the optic flow. Perception 1994;23:681–90. pmid:7845761
  27. 27. Smith PL, Little DR. Small is beautiful: In defense of the small-N design. Psychon Bull Rev 2018;25:2083–101. pmid:29557067
  28. 28. Pelli DG. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into moview. Spat Vis 1997;10:437–42. pmid:9176953
  29. 29. Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D. What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception 2007;36.
  30. 30. Brainard DH. The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat Vis 1997;10:433–6. pmid:9176952
  31. 31. Prins N, Kingdom FAA. Palamedes: Matlab routines for analyzing psychophysical data. 2009.
  32. 32. Held L, Ott M. On P-Values and Bayes Factors. Annu Rev Stat Its Appl 2018;5:393–419.
  33. 33. Jeffreys H. Theory of Probability. 3rd ed. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 1961.
  34. 34. Dienes Z. Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Front Psychol 2014;5:1–17.
  35. 35. Robert CP, Chopin N, Rousseau J. Harold Jeffreys’s Theory of Probability Revisited. Stat Sci 2009;24:141–72.
  36. 36. Schmerler J. The visual perception of accelerated motion. Perception 1976;5:167–85. pmid:951166
  37. 37. Gottsdanker RM, Fricke JW, Lockard RB. Identifying the acceleration of visual targets. Br J Psychol 1961;52:31–42. pmid:13707460
  38. 38. Trewhella J, Edwards M, Ibbotson MR. Sensitivity to the acceleration of looming stimuli. Clin Exp Opthalmology 2003;31:258–61.
  39. 39. Calderone JB, Kaiser MK. Visual acceleration detection: effect of sign and motion orientation. Percept Psychophys 1989;45:391–4. pmid:2726400
  40. 40. Mueller AS, Timney B. Visual acceleration perception for simple and complex motion patterns. PLoS One 2016;11:1–10. pmid:26901879
  41. 41. Werkhoven P, Snippe HP, Alexander T. Visual processing of optic acceleration. Vision Res 1992;32:2313–29. pmid:1288008
  42. 42. Watamaniuk SNJ, Heinen SJ. Perceptual and oculomotor evidence of limitations on processing accelerating motion. J Vis 2003;3:698–709. pmid:14765954
  43. 43. Babler TG, Dannemiller JL. Role of image acceleration in judging landing location of free-falling projectiles. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1993;19:15–31. pmid:8440982
  44. 44. Brouwer A-M, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. Perception of acceleration with short presentation times: can acceleration be used in interception? Percept Psychophys 2002;64:1160–8. pmid:12489669
  45. 45. Brenner E, Rodriguez IA, Munoz VE, Schootemeijer S, Mahieu Y, Veerkamp K, et al. How Can People Be so Good at Intercepting Accelerating Objects if They Are so Poor at Visually Judging Acceleration? Iperception 2016;7. pmid:27482367
  46. 46. Benguigui N, Bennett SJ. Ocular pursuit and the estimation of time-to-contact with accelerating objects in prediction motion are controlled independently based on first-order estimates. Exp Brain Res 2010;202:327–39. pmid:20039024
  47. 47. Benguigui N, Ripoll H, Broderick MP. Time-to-contact estimation of accelerated stimuli is based on first-order information. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2003;29:1083–101. pmid:14640832
  48. 48. Kaiser MK, Hecht H. Time-to-passage judgments in nonconstant optical flow fields. Percept Psychophys 1995;57:817–25. pmid:7651806
  49. 49. Port NL, Lee D, Dassonville P, Georgopoulos AP. Manual interception of moving targets. I. Performance and movement initiation. Exp Brain Res 1997;116:406–20. pmid:9372290
  50. 50. Cesqui B, Mezzetti M, Lacquaniti F, D’Avella A. Gaze behavior in one-handed catching and its relation with interceptive performance: What the eyes can’t tell. PLoS One 2015;10:1–39. pmid:25793989
  51. 51. Mcintyre J, Zago M, Berthoz A, Lacquaniti F. Does the brain model Newton’s laws? Nat Neurosci 2001;4:693–4. https://doi.org/10.1038/89477 pmid:11426224
  52. 52. Russo M, Cesqui B, La Scaleia B, Ceccarelli F, Maselli A, Moscatelli A, et al. Intercepting virtual balls approaching under different gravity conditions: evidence for spatial prediction. J Neurophysiol 2017;118:2421–34. pmid:28768737
  53. 53. Senot P, Zago M, Lacquaniti F, McIntyre J. Anticipating the Effects of Gravity When Intercepting Moving Objects: Differentiating Up and Down Based on Nonvisual Cues. J Neurophysiol 2005;94:4471–80. pmid:16120661
  54. 54. Katz LN, Hennig JA, Cormack LK, Huk AC. A Distinct Mechanism of Temporal Integration for Motion through Depth. J Neurosci 2015;35:10212–6. pmid:26180197
  55. 55. Tyler CW. Characteristics of stereomovement suppression. Percept Psychophys 1975;17:225–30.
  56. 56. Harris JM, Rushton SK. Poor visibility of motion in depth is due to early motion averaging. Vision Res 2003;43:385–92. pmid:12535995
  57. 57. Harris JM, McKee SP, Watamaniuk SNJ. Visual search for motion-in-depth: stereomotion does not “pop out” from disparity noise. Nat Neurosci 1998;1:165–8. pmid:10195134
  58. 58. Sumnall JH, Harris JM. Binocular three-dimensional motion detection: contributions of lateral motion and stereomotion. J Opt Soc Am A 2000;17:687–96.
  59. 59. Cooper EA, van Ginkel M, Rokers B. Sensitivity and bias in the discrimination of two-dimensional and three-dimensional motion direction. J Vis 2016;16:1–11. pmid:27537702
  60. 60. Tyler CW. Stereoscopic Depth Movement: Two Eyes Less Sensitive than One. Science (80-) 1971;174:958–61.
  61. 61. Mateeff S, Dimitrov G, Hohnsbein J. Temporal thresholds and reaction time to changes in velocity of visual motion. Vision Res 1995;35:355–63. pmid:7892731
  62. 62. Verghese P, McKee SP. Motion grouping impairs speed discrimination. Vision Res 2006;46:1540–6. pmid:16168457
  63. 63. Manning C, Thomas RT, Braddick OJ. Can speed be judged independent of direction? J Vis 2018;18:1–16. pmid:30029225