Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Time and tide: Seasonal, diel and tidal rhythms in Wadden Sea Harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena)

  • Beate Zein,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    Affiliations Institute for Terrestrial and Aquatic Wildlife Research, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Foundation, Büsum, Germany, School of Geography & Geosciences, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife, Scotland, United Kingdom

  • Benno Woelfing,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute for Terrestrial and Aquatic Wildlife Research, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Foundation, Büsum, Germany

  • Michael Dähne,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation German Oceanographic Museum, Stralsund, Mecklenburg-Vorpommern, Germany

  • Tobias Schaffeld,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute for Terrestrial and Aquatic Wildlife Research, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Foundation, Büsum, Germany

  • Stefan Ludwig,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation German Navy Headquarters, Geo-Information Division, METOC Support, Rostock, Germany

  • Jacob Hansen Rye,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation The Danish Coastal Authority, Ministry of Environment and Food of Denmark, Lemvig, Denmark

  • Johannes Baltzer,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute for Terrestrial and Aquatic Wildlife Research, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Foundation, Büsum, Germany

  • Andreas Ruser ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    andreas.ruser@tiho-hannover.de

    Affiliation Institute for Terrestrial and Aquatic Wildlife Research, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Foundation, Büsum, Germany

  • Ursula Siebert

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute for Terrestrial and Aquatic Wildlife Research, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Foundation, Büsum, Germany

Abstract

Odontocetes have evolved a rich diversity of prey- and habitat-specific foraging strategies, which allows them to feed opportunistically on locally and temporally abundant prey. While habitat-specific foraging strategies have been documented for some odontocete species, this is less known for the harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena). We collected multiple years of acoustic data using echolocation click loggers to analyse porpoise occurrence and buzzing behaviour, indicating feeding, in the German Wadden Sea (North Sea). Seasonal, diel and tidal effects were studied using Generalised Estimating Equations (GEE-GAMs). Locally season, time of day and tidal time significantly influenced the probability of porpoise detections and detection of foraging sequences (buzzes). Hunting strategies, and therefore frequency of buzzes, were likely affected by prey distribution and large differences between POD locations indicated that porpoises used highly specific behaviour adapted to tide and time of day to efficiently feed on the available prey. Strong seasonal and spatial variation in diel and tidal effects underline the importance of long-term observations. Studies on porpoise behaviour are often based on short-term observations and might rather reflect a seasonal than a general pattern. The results of this study show clearly that significant changes in porpoise behaviour can be found in short and long-term observations. Here some features are based on short term determinants and others are stable over years and care should be taken about drawing general conclusions based on local patterns. Highly variable spatio-temporal patterns indicate a high flexibility of porpoises in a highly variable environment and address a challenge for complex conservation management plans.

Introduction

In a fast-changing environment with variable prey abundance, odontocetes have evolved highly adaptive foraging strategies. For an optimal foraging success, predators need to locate prey, which might be in localised patches due to biotic or abiotic factors [1,2]. While habitat-specific foraging strategies have been documented for some odontocete species, this is less known for the harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena). The dependency of predators’ distribution to prey availability should be especially high for harbour porpoises because they have a high energy demand due to their small size and their life in temperate waters [35].

In the North-East Atlantic, coastal and shelf waters are common habitats of the harbour porpoise [6]. In the North Sea it is the most common marine mammal [6] occurring both in offshore areas in the central, western and northern parts, and in coastal waters in the Wadden Sea in southern parts. There are major differences between those two habitats. The offshore areas of the North Sea are 30 to 200 metres deep and the Wadden Sea is mostly shallower than 20 metres. This affects the water temperatures throughout the year resulting in warmer waters in the Wadden Sea in spring and summer [7,8]. Other abiotic factors strongly affecting shallow areas of the North Sea are solar radiation, wind and tide [9]. Additionally, in coastal zones the effects of rivers, estuaries and tidal flats (Wadden Sea) lead to an increase in the concentration of organic matter [10] and hence might increase the prey availability, which is linked to porpoise distribution [1113].

Porpoise occurrence varies seasonally [14,15] and gradually over longer timescales [16] in the North Sea and indicate complex and variable pattern of porpoise occurrence. High porpoise occurrences have been detected in winter, spring and summer in different inshore areas in various countries [14,1627]. In offshore areas in Germany a peak of porpoise occurrence was found in autumn and winter [28] which might suggest a seasonal movement of harbour porpoises between inshore and offshore areas [29,30]. In general, coastal habitats are predominantly used in summer time in the North Sea [22,25] and a widespread distribution with a shift towards offshore areas is found in autumn [14,19,2931].

On a smaller scale, local porpoise abundance can change with changing abiotic factors such as fronts, day length, water depth, distance to coast and residual currents [15,30]. The occurrence of porpoises has been found to be influenced by currents [15,32] and tidally driven in- and outflows [3335]. Tides generally have an effect on the distribution of porpoises [3639] but the impact is highly specific to sites and tidal states [33,40]. Depending on the site monitored, animal detection probability has been found to peak at high tide [26,4143], but also flood tide [36,41,44], ebb tide [45], slack water [26] or with no tidal effects [46]. Another abiotic factor that has been found to influence porpoise occurrence are diel phases. Variable effects were found using acoustic methods [17,28,40,47]. Slight tendencies towards nocturnal detections and feeding behaviour are most common [26,4752] as well as for dusk and dawn [35,43,53].

Porpoises are traditionally monitored using visual surveys in large areas at large intervals [6,54] or in smaller areas at shorter intervals [14,15]. These visual monitoring methods have scale dependent advantages–large scale surveys may be the best solution to monitor the population, while shorter intervals will lead to improved power to detect short-term changes in occurrence. While porpoises can be observed visually at the surface at good sea conditions and during day light hours only, other methods are needed to detect porpoises more continuously. Therefore, we can detect echolocation signals used by toothed whales for navigation [55,56] and to find prey [5759]. The emitted high frequency clicks can be used to detect porpoises in monitoring studies [19,28,49,60]. Apart from occurrence, click sequences can give insight into porpoise behaviour [58,59,61]. Recordings of click sequences with very short inter-click intervals (< 10 milliseconds) can serve as a proxy to categorize foraging behaviour in acoustic data [47,49,59]. A method that can detect porpoises continuously with very limited spatial coverage, but high temporal resolution is stationary acoustic monitoring (SAM) [17,60]. It can collect data on 24-hour cycles and therefore allows for analysing diel cycles since it does not depend on daylight [52]. Therefore, it is a useful method to study short term and long term changes of porpoise behaviour.

Knowledge on movements of porpoises in the North Sea is still limited and large-scale, long-term studies are necessary to unravel the distribution in time and space over annual cycles. In our study, we analysed a long-term passive acoustic data set to investigate harbour porpoise occurrences and feeding behaviour in the German Wadden Sea. We tested if (1) season, day time and/or tide drive changes in porpoise occurrence in the Wadden Sea and (2) if buzzing behaviour, as a proxy for feeding is influenced by the same factors.

Methods

Ethical statement

Permissions for the deployment of measurement positions were given from the Federal Waterways and Shipping Administration (WSV) and Agency for Coastal Defence, National Park and Marine Conservation Schleswig- Holstein and Lower Saxony. No other specific permissions were required for these locations and activities. Also, no ethical approval is required because no handlings of animals or experiments on animals were conducted and we confirm that the field studies did not involve endangered or protected species.

Data sampling

Data were recorded by static acoustic monitoring in the German Wadden Sea area at 17 locations (Fig 1, permission from Federal Waterways and Shipping Administration (WSV) and Agency for Coastal Defence, National Park and Marine Conservation Schleswig- Holstein and Lower Saxony) with autonomous acoustic data loggers (T-PODs and C-PODs; www.chelonia.co.uk). Stations had a minimum distance from at least 500 m from the next one and covered the coast line and important estuary areas (Fig 1). The recording effort from C-PODs and T-PODs are given in Figs 2 and 3, respectively.

thumbnail
Fig 1. POD locations.

T-POD stations (marked with a T and a triangle) were used in 2003–2006. C-POD stations (marked with a C and a circle) have been used since 2011 and are still running.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213348.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Timeline.

Time scale for the recordings of C PODs respectively.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213348.g002

T- and C-PODs were moored two to five metres above the seafloor. They had one to two marker buoys and were connected with an 18-mm nylon rope to concrete anchors. Lifting bodies were used to maintain the units in an approximately vertical position, even under strong tidal currents. The PODs were set to only record when the devices were in a vertical position directed upwards and switch off when the inclination angel was greater than 82 degrees. Every two to three months the PODs were exchanged and ropes and equipment inspected. The PODs were regularly calibrated every few months [62].

There are different underlying detection mechanisms for T- and C-PODs. T-PODs compare the sound energy of two frequency bands of 90 and 130 kHz. If a certain ratio is reached they record the time point and duration of the event [63]. In comparison, C-PODs record tonal sounds and log the time, centre frequency, sound pressure level, duration and bandwidth of each click (www.chelonia.co.uk /cpod_downloads.htm). The detection rate of T-and C-PODs at the same position was compared in the wild for over one year of recordings [39]. The C-PODs recorded about twice as many detection positive minutes per hour as T-PODs with approximately linear relationship. This might influence the number of recorded events but as POD types and positions were analysed completely separately, no differences in effect size should affect the model outputs. For both devices, we applied a limit of logging 4096 clicks per minute and logging was stopped for the rest of the minute when the threshold was reached. This limit was set to save battery and memory capacity in noisy areas. The data were regularly checked for the amount of potential data loss. The median of minutes a day with 4096 clicks was mostly just above 0 percent and generally shows low background noise.

Data analysis

Data were processed using the programs T-POD 8.19 and C-POD 2.044 (Chelonia Ltd). Built-in filtering algorithms (KERNO for C-POD and train classifier for T-POD) were used to select only clicks of harbour porpoises with a high likelihood (cetacean all and high/- moderate porpoise click trains respectively) of coming from a porpoise. All further analyses were carried out with the program R [64]. Inter-click-intervals (ICI) were calculated and data were scanned for buzzes which were defined as five consecutive clicks with ICI below ten milliseconds [47,59,65]. Data were grouped into click and buzz positive ten minutes (DP10MIN, BP10MIN) whereas BP10MIN was analysed from times when the porpoise was present (DP10MIN) only. Both were binary coded and not representing the specific number of detected clicks. DP10MIN and BP10MIN were then modelled dependent on abiotic variables (day time, tidal phase, day of year, year) that serve as a proxy for factors more meaningful for the animal (e.g. temperature, salinity, light availability).”

Environmental data

Tidal data were derived from the X-Tide software at the POD positions [66]. Sun rise and sun set data were taken for one central position at 54.5N and 7.5E and calculated using the package “Maptools” in R. Time sequences of ebb and flood tide change constantly over time and time of sun rise and sun set changes over the season. To standardize tides and daylight radian values were calculated. The values 0 and 2 Pi describe the phase of tide and day cycles and represent high water and dawn respectively, while and Pi represents low water and dusk.

Statistical analysis

Porpoise detections (expressed as the binary response variable DP10MIN, i.e. the probability of detecting at least one click train in a ten minute bin) and buzz occurrences relative to detections (expressed as the binary response variable BP10MIN, i.e. each the probability of detecting at least one buzz in DP10MIN) were analysed using time of day, tidal phase (referred to as tidal time) and the day of year (ordinal date numbers) as predictors. The buzz occurrence models (with the response variable BP10MIN) aim at estimating the likelihood that a porpoise present at the POD-position is feeding. Therefore 10-minute intervals in which no porpoise clicks were detected were excluded from this analysis. At each position analyses were conducted only when the recorded data covered more than 3 months. Separate models were calculated for each mooring position. This also meant that T-POD and C-POD data were not analysed within the same model. In order to correct for differences in harbour porpoise occurrence between years, year was included as a factor in all models that were based on multi-year records. We first calculated binary Generalized Additive Models (GAM). The cyclic predictors time of day, tidal time and day of year were modelled as cyclic cubic regression splines. In cases of stations where data was only available for part of the year, day of year was modelled as an ordinary cubic regression spline. Autocorrelation function (ACF) plots showed significant temporal autocorrelation of the residuals. Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE) can account for the observed temporal autocorrelation and have previously been used for the analysis of porpoise click detections [35,40]. Our general modelling approach followed their recommendations on incorporating splines in GEEs obtaining so-called GEE-GAMs. For specifying the correlation structure in the GEE-GAMs we grouped the data into blocks using a block size of 1000 minutes for T-PODs and 3000 minutes for C-PODs. These limits were selected based on a visual analysis of the ACF plots. We report full models for all mooring positions, i.e. all models include all predictor variables (i.e. day time, tidal phase, day of year and–if applicable–the factor year, which was included in all models for stations with multi-year recordings) and share the same internal covariate structure. No variable selection was performed, because all covariates were highly significant in the great majority of the models calculated for the different mooring positions. In such a situation, where all covariates are expected to have an effect on the response, it is generally desirable to control for the effect of a covariate even if it is not significant in a particular model [67]. Significance of the covariates was assessed using Wald’s tests. The models’ goodness of fit was evaluated based on the Area Under Curve (AUC) of the Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) and on presence-absence confusion matrices. Confusion matrices are two-way contingency tables that allow evaluation of model performance by tabulating the percentage of predicted presences and absences for both observed presences (in the first column) and observed absences (in the second column). For calculating the confusion matrices, the threshold for classifying a prediction as a presence was determined as the point at which the distance between the ROC curve and the 45° diagonal was maximal. ROC-AUC values can vary between zero and one with 0.5 representing the performance of a random predictor and one representing perfect prediction performance.

Additionally, separate models of buzzing behaviour (BP10MIN as dependent) were calculated for each of the four calendric seasons as replacement for day of year, to investigate whether generalizable patterns for the seasons can be found.

Results

At all POD stations echolocation clicks of harbour porpoises were regularly recorded and recording effort was 9888 days from 17 different positions. At four POD sites T11, T13, T15 and T18 recorded data was less than 3 months and they were therefore excluded from further analyses. Results of the GEE-GAM models indicate highly significant effects of day of the year, tide and time of the day on harbour porpoise detections at most POD sites (Table 1).

At most sites, the predictor variable with highest chi-squared value and thus highest explanatory power is day of year for porpoise detections (Table 1). A peak in detections can be found around day 100 (March to May) for 5 stations while all others showed a highly variable seasonal variation. Several years of observations exist only for the C-POD stations and effects of year could be shown at most POD locations. On a smaller scale, we found that effects of tide and daytime on porpoise detections were highly variable and site specific. At most sites detections were slightly elevated at high tide but also peaks of detections at low tide and at flood tide were found (Table 1 and Figs 4 and S3 and S6). Diel patterns were found for most of the POD stations, with detection peaks at night, day or twilight (Table 1 and Figs 4 and S2 and S5).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Model results.

Effects of season, daytime and tide on the probability that either a click is detected (DP10MIN) or a buzz (BP10MIN) is observed in a 10-minute interval. Estimates of the GEE-GAM model are plotted for four selected sites (C1, C2, T8 and C3). Grey areas represent associated 95% confidence intervals. The values for day time of 0 and 6 are representing dawn and the transition between the white and grey background is equal to dusk. Day-time is with a white background and night-time is shaded in grey. Similarly, for tide 0 and 6 are representing high water and the transition between white and grey areas are equal to low water. Ebb tide is with a white background and flood tide is shaded in grey. (For all positions see supplement).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213348.g004

Visual comparison of the model outputs for porpoise detections and buzzing behaviour show variable effects. Over days of the year, at some positions an increase in buzzes was detected even when detections of porpoises were going down (C1, T8, C4, C6). On the opposite, detection of buzzes also increased with increasing porpoise detections (C2, C3, C5, T14, T16; Figs 4 and S1 and S4).

BP10MIN were significantly influenced by most factors but very complex patterns were found across the different stations (Table 1). When visually grouping the station as closer to the coast (C1, C2, C3, C5, C6, T8, T9, T10, T12, Fig 1) and further away (C4, T7, T14, T16, T17, Fig 1) similar pattern for day of the year (C1, C3, C5, C6 and T14, T16, Figs 4 and S4) and tide (C2, C3, C5, T8, Figs 4 and S5) were found. A clear trend for detections are in-between high and low tides when current speed was likely to be highest (C2, C4, C6, T12, T14, T16, Fig 4). However, high probabilities for buzzes at high tide were also detected at two sites (C1, T7). A very site-specific pattern of diel behaviour could be seen for the different stations (Fig 4) showing nocturnal, diurnal and dusk/dawn pattern.

At the neighbouring POD positions C1 and C2, tide showed opposite effects on DP10MIN and BP10MIN (Fig 4). These two PODs were in close proximity to each other but were differently affected by the tides.

The factor year was significant at most stations for DP10MIN and BP10MIN indicating different patterns over the years. Only at one station for porpoise detections and at two stations for buzzing porpoises the effect of the year was not significant indicating stable pattern over the years of POD deployment.

The season-specific effects of tide and daytime on porpoise detection probability were analysed (Figs 5 and 6) for the 3 C-POD sites with the most complete coverage of all seasons (Table 1). At these stations the impact of the tide and time of the day changed over season. At position C1 there was a clear nocturnal effect from spring to autumn, which was not detected in winter. At this time of the year, however, the effect size of tide increased at the specified position.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Seasonal tidal effects on detected buzzes.

Effect of tidal time on the probability that a buzz is observed in a 10-minute interval (BP10MIN). Estimates of GEE-GAM models for each calendric season and POD position (C1, C3, C5) are shown. Grey areas represent associated 95% confidence intervals. The values for tide 0 and 6 are representing high water and the transition between white and grey areas are equal to low water. Ebb tide is with a white background and flood tide is shaded in grey. Model significance is symbolled with asterisk (* = p ≤ 0.05, ** = p ≤ 0.01, *** = p ≤ 0.001).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213348.g005

thumbnail
Fig 6. Seasonal daytime effects on detected buzzes.

Effect of the time of day on the probability that a buzz is observed in a 10-minute interval (BP10MIN). Estimates of GEE-GAM models for each calendric season and POD position (C1, C3, C5) are shown. Grey areas represent associated 95% confidence intervals. The values for day time of 0 and 6 are representing dawn and the transition between the white and grey background is equal to dusk. Day-time is with a white background and night-time is shaded in grey. Model significance is symbolled with asterisk (* = p ≤ 0.05, ** = p ≤ 0.01, *** = p ≤ 0.001).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213348.g006

The AUC-values in Table 1 are variable and range from 0.6 to 0.79 for DP10MIN and from 0.57 to 0.64 for BP10MIN. Generally, models calculated at stations with only significant factors have higher values in the AUC-values.

Discussion

We analysed long term acoustic data of harbour porpoises in the German Wadden Sea and found strong and statistically significant seasonal, tidal and daylight effects on detections and buzzing behaviour at the different deployment positions over the time period from 2003 to 2016.

A high probability in detecting porpoises and buzzes in the Wadden Sea especially at POD positions close to the coast (Figs 4 and S1 and S4) was found in spring. This is in line with aerial observations and supports a seasonal on/offshore movement pattern of porpoises and a general movement of porpoises from other areas into the German North Sea in spring [14,16]. General seasonal movement [30] might reflect porpoise responses to different prey types and prey abundance [29]. Seasonal hot spots in prey abundance might additionally lead to a more aggregated porpoise distribution in spring and a more even distribution of porpoises in the North Sea in the rest of the year [6,29]. Our results show that the Wadden Sea might also be an important feeding and breeding habitat for porpoises with a peak in detected buzz positive intervals in spring. This is in accordance with previous analyses of stranded and bycaught porpoises in the German North Sea indicating a higher biomass of ingested prey in spring [29]. This time is just before the most common time of porpoises giving birth in June-July [68]. In this time period female cetaceans have to gain weight for the time of lactation [69], when they cannot gather enough prey to replenish their energy resources [70]. Shallow Wadden Sea areas have higher fluctuations in water temperature, creating areas of higher temperatures, compared to the neighbouring North Sea. This could be beneficial for animals due to decreased energy expenditure caused by maintenance of core temperature. Later in the year, we found a decrease in detections suggesting that most calves are not born in the Wadden Sea, or that some animals move elsewhere during that crucial time.

Nocturnal and diurnal pattern were found in porpoise detections and foraging behaviour in our data (Figs 4 and S2 and S5). These significant but very site-specific pattern could be directly linked to their prey and might be explained by differences in physical parameters, such as depth, slope and sea bed composition [45,71] of the POD sites. In the Baltic Sea, acoustic detections have been suggested to be linked to pelagic prey of porpoises, e.g. herring, that could be easier accessed at night [52]. Herring shows decreased predator avoidance mechanisms during the night, being closer to the surface in less aggregated schools and reduced swimming speed [72,73]. This could be reflected in porpoise behaviour as supported by a previous study [4] and therefore buzz rates may increase at certain times of the day due to porpoises following the vertical distribution of their prey. In contrast to these findings from the Baltic Sea a major prey item of harbour porpoises in the North Sea, the sandeel [29,74,75], shows a diurnal vertical migration pattern feeding on surface zooplankton during the day and buries itself in sand at night especially in spring time [76]. This would explain diurnal patterns in our data in addition to nocturnal patterns possibly linked to other prey species. Additionally, seasonal changes have diverse impacts on different prey fish types, and therefore might alter the distribution of the opportunistic feeding harbour porpoises in different habitats.

An alternative explanation to an increased echolocation at night could be to compensate the loss of visual information due to darkness [47]–a large advantage for nocturnal predators. A similar nocturnal increase in echolocation detection should then be expected in all odontocetes but that has not been found in studies of bottlenose dolphins [26] and Heaviside dolphins [77]. This was proved in captivity were echolocation activity did not increase when a porpoise was blindfolded [56] and may strengthen the hypothesis that different nocturnal and diurnal patterns are linked to the distribution of their prey in dependency of the habitats.

In coastal zones, inflows from rivers, estuaries and tidal flats increase the concentration of organic matter [10]. Therefore, strong tidal currents or different tidal stages could influence prey availability and porpoise abundance indirectly. Different water movement could more directly physically impact the distribution of their prey as the prey fish might be concentrated in areas with high differences in current speed. In the current study, we found a strong impact of tides on porpoise detection rates and detection of buzzes. Porpoises were mostly detected at high tide but also at times of high current speeds, at flood tide and at low tide (Figs 4, 5 and S3 and S6 and Table 1). Buzzes were mostly detected in high current speed and at ebb tide (C2, C4, C5, C6, T8, T14, T16; Figs 4 and S6). Our findings are supported by a short-term study which found an increase in porpoise detections and encounter duration when an energetic tidal jet occurred and clearly showed an attraction for porpoises to strong currents [32]. For general conclusions on porpoise distribution more complex measurements than tidal factors alone are needed and local differences in topography and tides may have various effects at different sites [40].

Occurrences of buzzes of porpoises were analysed in all detection positive intervals and a visual comparison of the occurrence of detections and buzzes show that they are not automatically linked. At some stations (C1, T8, C4, C6), an increase in buzzes was detected even when detection of porpoises was going down at different times over the year. However, at other stations (C2, C3, C5, T14, T16; Figs 4 and S1 and S4), detection of buzzes were increasing with increased porpoise detections. This shows that the method of passive acoustic monitoring with T/C-PODs is sensitive enough to indicate different behaviours and can help detecting buzzes as proxy for feeding behaviour in a localised position.

Potential feeding events, buzzes, were analysed for different calendaric seasons and showed a site-specific diel and tidal pattern. When the diel effect was less pronounced (C1 winter, Fig 6) the effect size of tide increased (Fig 5). This might generally suggest a stronger effect of currents and when current spends are low day time effect might be more pronounced. However, strong seasonal differences can be seen. This highlights the importance of yearlong observations to draw general conclusions on buzzing and detections.

Inter-annual differences of detections and buzzes at most POD sites show a variable porpoise behaviour over the years. This supports the idea that porpoise behaviour in the Wadden Sea is much more diverse than previously known. Generally, different patterns found in the long-term data set tend to be highly site specific. For example, even at neighbouring POD positions C1 and C2 an opposite effect of the tide on porpoise detection and buzzes can be found. These positions, even though they are in close proximity are differently affected by tides. C1 is right at the open coast of the island Sylt whereas C2 is sheltered behind the island in much calmer water. At the more exposed position C1 porpoises and buzzes were detected significantly more at high water, whereas an increase at low water was found at the more sheltered position C2. This may indicate that environmental factors with cyclic variation may lead to regular concentrations of porpoises or it could indicate that site fidelity of harbour porpoises is stronger than previously assumed. Site fidelity, in broad terms, is the inclination of an animal to remain or revisit an area formerly occupied [78]. In theory it optimizes the energy expenditure of an animal in a steady or fast changing environment, depending on prey availability and other biologically significant factors. Reports from a photo-identification study from Washington State (USA) suggest that at least some porpoises show high site fidelity [79] and this has also been hypothesized for porpoises from Morro Bay, California [80]. However, telemetry studies from the Danish Belt Sea and Skagerrak waters aimed at estimating population home ranges [81]. They found key habitats potentially associated with higher dispersal but for a completely different population and habitat. Since no porpoise was refitted with a logger after a year, when a seasonal migration would start over again the perspective on longer time scales is limited. Photo-ID or long-term telemetry studies focussing on the Wadden Sea are necessary to validate the hypothesis of strong site fidelity which currently cannot be substantiated using static acoustic monitoring due to the inability to identify individuals.

Distinct patterns in acoustic communications calls corresponding to 5 differing behavioural classes have been identified in captive animals [82]. Four of these call types contain high click rates with ICIs below 10 ms, accompanied by a sudden drop in ICI. Free ranging harbour porpoises were shown to produce communication calls in 7.7 to 31.5% and receive communication calls from conspecifics in 1.7–3.6% (4 single animals) and 18.5–34.4% (2 mother calf pairs) of observation minutes, indicating that social interactions are of higher importance than previously assumed [83]. Foraging buzzes were furthermore shown to be clearly distinct from communication calls [83]. Unfortunately, the discrimination of communication and foraging buzzes [83] cannot be conducted in comparable manner for PODs since movement data and echoes of prey cannot be determined. Nevertheless, foraging buzzes are produced more frequently (17.8–75.8%, on average 52.6% of minutes) [84] than communication buzzes and harbour porpoise group sizes are typically small in the North Sea (average group size of 1.24 animals) [30]. Therefore, buzzes will represent the majority of the classified sequences within the here presented study and we suggest that increases in buzz occurrence should mostly reflect increases in individual feeding rate. The frequent presence of communication sequences and responding conspecifics could furthermore also be an indirect indicator of foraging, possibly representing cooperative foraging strategies [83]. Due to the described limitations with data logged by static acoustic recorders and additionally having data with incomplete click sequences, the sudden decrease of ICI below 10 ms is to date the best indicator for this method to classify foraging and has been regularly applied [26,47,49,52,85,86,87].

While using static acoustic monitoring throughout our study, we have differences in the acquisition of data. The biggest contrast between the different POD types is due to the different deployment times (Figs 2 and 3). At all C-POD positions data acquisition was over several years whereas the T-POD data was less than 300 days with a minimum recording time of 130 days. Therefore, the model output of T-POD data is restricted to the recording times of the PODs and rather reflect seasonal than general patterns especially for shorter recording times. For both POD types, the evaluation of the goodness of fit for the calculated models was done with AUC of the ROC. Our AUC-values are ranging between 0.6 to 0.79 for DP10MIN and from 0.57 to 0.64 for BP10MIN. This indicates that our predictor variables can only partly explain porpoise detections and that occurrence of buzzes are explained with less certainty than detections. Other factors that were not recorded might be influencing the behaviour of the porpoises as well. Compared to a study using a similar method for analysing porpoise detections [35] our study has similar or slightly lower AUC-values. In Benjamins study the AUC-values for porpoise detections ranged from 0.59 to 0.78 at one site and between 0.64 and 0.88 at the other site. Their recording effort was 58 and 55 days and therefore much shorter than in our study. The fact that in much shorter recording efforts AUC values are slightly higher might be another indicator that tidal or day time pattern might be relatively stable over some months [35] but more variable over years (our study).

Another difference between the two kinds of PODs are the different deployment depths which could lead to different detection rates as these are influenced by the distance of the POD to the sea floor [88]. Due to a general variation in dive depth of porpoises during pelagic feeding we assume that deployment depth in the tidal waters of the German North Sea is of lesser importance. However, the setup of the POD will mostly detect pelagic feeding and is likely to miss bottom grubbing [52].

Concluding, our results show that the Wadden Sea is an important habitat for harbour porpoises. It is used for feeding and especially at times before and in the breeding season. Porpoises use the Wadden Sea throughout the year but at different intensities at the different POD positions. Additionally, we found effects of time of the day and of different influences of the tides on detections and buzzing behaviour of porpoises. However, very site-specific results with annual differences of detections and buzzes indicate a high flexibility of porpoises in a highly variable environment. Factors like day time and tide were shown to vary in their influence on porpoises in different season at the same positions. Our results based on a multi-year dataset thus indicates that studies drawing conclusions on porpoise behaviour based on short-term datasets might rather reflect seasonal than general patterns and should be interpreted accordingly.

Our study gives new insights into harbour porpoise occurrence and buzzing behaviour in the German Wadden Sea and it also raises interesting research questions like how porpoises use specific situations efficiently for feeding, how they predict prey occurrence and if individuals visit the same sites in subsequent years.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Daily variance of porpoise detection.

All GEE-GAM results for DP10MIN probability at each POD position, shown are thin plate regression splines showing daily variance over year.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213348.s001

(PDF)

S2 Fig. Daytime dependency of porpoise detection.

All GEE-GAM results for DP10MIN probability as a function of time of the day at each POD position,the rad values of 0 and 2 Pi are representing dawn and Pi is equal to dusk.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213348.s002

(PDF)

S3 Fig. Tidal dependency of porpoise detection.

All GEE-GAM results for DP10MIN probability in dependency of tide at each POD position separate, the rad values for tide of 0/2 Pi are representing high water and Pi is equal to low water.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213348.s003

(PDF)

S4 Fig. Daily variance of porpoise buzzes.

All GEE-GAM results for BP10MIN probability at each POD position, thin plate regression splines show daily variance over year.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213348.s004

(PDF)

S5 Fig. Daytime dependency of porpoise buzzes.

All GEE-GAM results for BP10MIN probability in relation to time of the day at each POD position, the rad values of 0/2 Pi are representing dawn and Pi is equal to dusk.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213348.s005

(PDF)

S6 Fig. Tidal dependency of porpoise buzzes.

All GEE-GAM results for BP10MIN probability in relation to tide at each POD position separate, the rad values for tide of 0/2 Pi are representing high water and Pi is equal to low water.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213348.s006

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

The study was partly funded by the Schleswig-Holstein’s Government-Owned Company for Coastal Protection, National Parks and Ocean Protection, Wadden Sea National Park Administration of Lower Saxony (Richard Czeck) and the German Federal Ministry for the Environment, Nature Conservation and Nuclear Safety (FKZ: 03 275 20 and FKZ: 0329946B). This publication was supported by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Foundation within the funding programme Open Access Publishing. We thank Kai Eskildsen from the National Park Administration Schleswig-Holstein for the coordination of MINOSplus and the water and shipping authorities especially Wilhelmshaven and Tönning/Husum for their continued support of the project with the vessels M/V Schilling and M/V Oland. The University Oldenburg provided ship time on M/V Navicula and M/V Zephyr. Servicing of the devices was carried out by the authors and by Ole Meyer-Klaeden, Patrick Stührk, Abbo van Neer, Michel Böhmert, Heiko Charwat. We also thank the editor and reviewers for their valuable feedback. I would like to express special thanks to Steffen Mumme and Verena Peschko for their tremendous personal support.

References

  1. 1. MacArthur RH & Pianka E (1966). On optimal use of a patchy environment. Am. Nat. 100:603–609
  2. 2. Werner EE & Hall DJ (1974). Optimal Foraging and the Size Selection of Prey by the Bluegill Sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus). Ecology. 55 (5):1042
  3. 3. Koopman HN (1998) Topographical distribution of the blubber of harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). J Mammal 79: 260−270
  4. 4. Wisniewska DM, Johnson M, Teilmann J, Rojano-Doñate L, Shearer J, Sveegaard S, et al. (2016). Ultra-high foraging rates of harbour porpoises make them vulnerable to anthropogenic disturbance. Current Biology 26(11):1441–1446 pmid:27238281
  5. 5. Hoekendijk JP, Spitz J, Read AJ, Leopold MF, Fontaine MC (2018), Resilience of harbor porpoises to anthropogenic disturbance: Must they really feed continuously?. Mar Mam Sci 34:258–264
  6. 6. Hammond PS, Macleod K, PerBerggren D, Borchers DL, Burt L, Cañadas A, et al. (2013) Cetacean abundance and distribution in European Atlantic shelf waters to inform conservation and management. Biological Conservation 164:107–122
  7. 7. Onken R, Callies U, Vaessen B, Riethmüller R (2007) Indirect determination of the heat budget of tidal flats. Cont Shelf Res 27:1565–1676
  8. 8. van Aken HM (2008) Variability of the water temperature in the western Wadden Sea on tidal to centennial time scales. Jour Sea Res 60(4):227–234
  9. 9. Simpson JH & Hunter JR (1974) Fronts in the Irish Sea. Nature 250:404–406
  10. 10. Brockmann UH, Laane RWPM, Postma H (1990) Cycling of nutrient elements in the North Sea. Netherlands Journal of Sea Research 26:239–264
  11. 11. Sveegaard S, Nabe-Nielsen J, Stæhr KJ, Jensen TF, Mouritsen KN, Teilmann J (2012a) Spatial interactions between marine predators and their prey: herring abundance as a driver for the distributions of mackerel and harbour porpoise. MarEcol Prog Ser 468:245–253
  12. 12. Sveegaard S, Andreasen H, Mouritsen KN, Jeppesen JP, Teilmann J, Kinze CC (2012b) Correlation between the seasonal distribution of harbour porpoises and their prey in the Sound, Baltic Sea. Mar Biol 159:1029−1037
  13. 13. Borges L & Evans PGH (1996) Spatial distribution of the harbour porpoise and fish prey and their associations in southeast Shetland, N. Scotland. European research on cetaceans– 10. Proceedings of the 10th Annual Conference of the European Cetacean Society. Lisbon, Portugal. Cambridge, UK: European Cetacean Society
  14. 14. Gilles A, Scheidat M, Siebert U (2009) Seasonal distribution of harbour porpoises and possible interference of offshore wind farms in the German North Sea. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 383:295–307
  15. 15. Gilles A, Adler S, Kaschner K, Scheidat M, Siebert U (2011) Modelling harbour porpoise seasonal density as a function of the German Bight environment: implications for management. Endangered Species Research 14:157–169
  16. 16. Peschko V, Ronnenberg K, Siebert U, Gilles A (2016) Trends of harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) density in the southern North Sea. Ecological Indicators 60:174–183
  17. 17. Verfuß UK, Honnef CG, Meding A, Dähne M, Mundry R, Benke H (2007) Geographical and seasonal variation of harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) presence in the German Baltic Sea revealed by passive acoustic monitoring. J Mar Biol Asso UK 87(1):165
  18. 18. Camphuysen CJ (2004) The return of the harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) in Dutch coastal waters. Lutra 47:113–122
  19. 19. Geelhoed SCV, Scheidat M, van Bemmelen RSA, Aarts G (2013) Abundance of harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) on the Dutch Continental Shelf, aerial surveys in July 2010–March 2011. Lutra 56 (1):45–57
  20. 20. Haelters J, Kerckhof F, Jacques TG, Degraer S (2011) The harbour porpoise Phocoena phocoena in the Belgian part of the North Sea: trends in abundance and distribution. Belgian Journal of Zoology 141:75–84
  21. 21. Weir CR, Stockin KA, Pierce GJ (2007) Spatial and temporal trends in the distribution of harbour porpoises, white-beaked dolphins and minke whales off Aberdeenshire (UK), north-western North Sea. J Mar Biol Assoc UK 87(1):327–338
  22. 22. Siebert U, Gilles A, Lucke K, Ludwig M, Benke H, Kock KH, et al. (2006) A decade of harbour porpoise occurrence in German waters—analyses of aerial surveys, incidental sightings and strandings. J Sea Res 56:65–80
  23. 23. Thomsen F, Laczny M, Piper W (2006) A recovery of harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) in the southern North Sea? A case study off Eastern Frisia, Germany. Helgol Mar Res 60:189–195
  24. 24. Evans PGH, Anderwald P, Paines ME (2003) UK cetacean status review. Report to English Nature and Countryside Council for Wales. Oxford: Sea Watch Foundation. 162 p
  25. 25. De Boer MN, Simmonds MP, Reijnders PJH, Aarts G (2014) The Influence of Topographic and Dynamic Cyclic Variables on the Distribution of Small Cetaceans in a Shallow Coastal System. PLOS ONE 9(1): e86331 pmid:24466031
  26. 26. Nuuttila HK, Courtene-Jones W, Baulch S, Simon M, Evans PGH (2017) Don’t forget the porpoise: acoustic monitoring reveals fine scale temporal variation between bottlenose dolphin and harbour porpoise in Cardigan Bay SAC. Mar Biol 164:50 pmid:28275285
  27. 27. Cox SL, Witt MJ, Embling CB, Godley BJ, Hosegood PJ, Miller PI, et al. (2016) Temporal patterns in habitat use by small cetaceans at an oceanographically dynamic marine renewable energy test site in the Celtic Sea. Deep-Sea Res II 141:178–190
  28. 28. Dähne M, Gilles A, Lucke K, Peschko V, Adler S, Krügel K, et al. (2013 a) Effects of pile-driving on harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) at the first offshore wind farm in Germany. Environmental Research Letters 8:025002
  29. 29. Gilles A (2008) Characterisation of harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) habitat in German waters. Dissertation (doctoral thesis). Christian-Albrechts-Universität zu Kiel, 151 pp
  30. 30. Gilles A, Viquerat S, Becker EA, Forney KA, Geelhoed SCV, Haelters J, et al. (2016) Seasonal habitat-based density models for a marine top predator, the harbor porpoise, in a dynamic environment. Ecosphere 7(6):e01367.
  31. 31. Scheidat M, Verdaat H (2009) Distribution and density of harbour porpoises in Dutch North Sea waters. Technical Report. Wangeningen IMARES. Report Number C125/09
  32. 32. Benjamins S, Dale A, van Geel N, Wilson B (2016 a) Riding the tide: use of a moving tidal-stream habitat by harbour porpoises. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 549:275–288
  33. 33. Goodwin L (2008) Diurnal and tidal variations in habitat use of the harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) in Southwest Britain. Aquat Mamm 34(1):44–53
  34. 34. Embling CB, Gillibrand PA, Gordon J, Shrimpton J, Stevick PT, Hammond PS (2010) Using habitat models to identify suitable sites for marine protected areas for harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). Biological Conservation 143:267–279
  35. 35. Benjamins S, van Geel N, Hastie G, Elliott J, Wilson B (2016 b) Harbour porpoise distribution can vary at small spatiotemporal scales in energetic habitats, Deep Sea Res II 141:191–202
  36. 36. Johnston DW, Westgate AJ, Read AJ (2005) Effects of fine-scale oceanographic features on the distribution and movements of harbour porpoises Phocoena phocoena in the Bay of Fundy. Marine Ecology Progress Series 295:279–293
  37. 37. Pierpoint C (2008) Harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) foraging strategy at a high energy, nearshore site in south-west Wales, UK. J Mar Biol Assoc UK 88:1167–1173
  38. 38. Isojunno S, Matthiopoulos J, Evans PGH (2012) Harbour porpoise habitat preferences: robust spatio-temporal inferences from opportunistic data. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 448:155–170
  39. 39. Dähne M, Mayer-Klaeden O, Stührk P, Siebert U (2014) Akustisches Monitoring von Schweinswalen im Wattenmeer für den Landesbetrieb für Küstenschutz, Nationalpark und Meeresschutz Schleswig-Holstein und das Nationalparkamt Niedersachen. Available at: www.nationalpark-wattenmeer.de
  40. 40. Pirotta E, Thompson PM, Miller PI, Brookes KL, Cheney B, Barton TR, et al. (2014) Scale-dependent foraging ecology of a marine top predator modelled using passive acoustic data. Funct Ecol 26:206–217
  41. 41. Marubini F, Gimona A, Evans PGH, Wright PJ, Pierce GJ (2009) Habitat preferences and interannual variability in occurrence of the harbour porpoise Phocoena phocoena in the north-west of Scotland. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 381:297–310
  42. 42. Boonstra M, Radstake Y, Rebel K, Aarts G, Camphuysen CJ (2013) Harbour porpoises Phocoena phocoena in the Marsdiep area: new investigations in a historical study area. Lutra 56 (1):59–71
  43. 43. IJsseldijk LL, Camphuysen KCJ, Nauw JJ, Aarts G (2015) Going with the flow: tidal influence on the occurrence of the harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) in the Marsdiep area, The Netherlands. Journal of Sea Research 103:129–137
  44. 44. Zamon JE (2003) Mixed species aggregations feeding upon herring and sandlance schools in a nearshore archipelago depend on flooding tidal currents. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 261:243−255
  45. 45. Jones AR, Hosegood P, Wynn RB, De Boer MN, Butler- Cowdry S, Embling CB (2014) Fine-scale hydrodynamics influence the spatio-temporal distribution of harbour porpoises at a coastal hotspot. Prog Oceanogr 128: 30−48
  46. 46. Hall AM (2004) Seasonal abundance, distribution and prey species of harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) in southern Vancouver Island waters (T). Retrospective Theses and Dissertations. University of Victoria. 1919–2007
  47. 47. Carlström J (2005) Diel variation in echolocation behaviour of wild harbour porpoises. Mar Mamm Sci 21(1):1–12
  48. 48. Cox TM, Read AJ, Solow A, Tregenza N (2001) Will harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) habituate to pingers? J Cet Res Manage 3:81–86
  49. 49. Todd VLG, Pearse WD, Tregenza NC, Lepper PA, Todd IB (2009) Diel echolocation activity of harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) around North Sea offshore gas installations. ICES J Mar Sci 66:734–745
  50. 50. Linnenschmidt M, Teilmann J, Akamatsu T, Dietz R, Miller LA (2013) Biosonar, dive, and foraging activity of satellite tracked harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). Mar Mamm Sci 29:E77–E97
  51. 51. Wang Z, Akamatsu T, Wang K, Wang D (2014) The diel rhythms of biosonar behavior in the Yangtze finless porpoise (Neophocaena asiaeorientalis asiaeorientalis) in the port of the Yangtze River: The correlation between prey availability and boat traffic. Plos One 9(5):e97907 pmid:24823945
  52. 52. Schaffeld T, Bräger S, Gallus A, Dähne M, Krügel K, Hermann A, et al. (2016) Diel and seasonal patterns in acoustic presence and foraging behaviour of free-ranging harbour porpoises. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 547:257–272
  53. 53. Brandt MJ, Hansen S, Diederichs A, Nehls G (2014) Do man-made structures and water depth affect the diel rhythms in click recordings of harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena)? Mar Mamm Sci 30: 1109–1121
  54. 54. Hammond PS, Bearzi G, Bjørge A, Forney KA, Karczmarski L, Kasuya T, et al. (2008) Phocoena phocoena (Baltic Sea subpopulation). (errata version published in 2016) The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2008: e.T17031A98831650. Downloaded on 07 June 2017
  55. 55. Amundin M (1991) Sound production in odontocetes with emphasis on the harbour porpoise. PhD thesis, Stockholm University, Stockholm. 144pp
  56. 56. Verfuß UK, Miller LA & Schnitzler HU (2005) Spatial orientation in echolocating harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). J Exper Biol 208(17):3385–3394
  57. 57. Au WWL (1993) The sonar of dolphins. Springer, New York, NY
  58. 58. DeRuiter SL, Bahr A, Blanchet MA, Hansen SF, Kristensen JH, Madsen PT, et al. (2009) Acoustic behaviour of echolocating porpoises during prey capture. The Journal of experimental biology 212:3100–3107 pmid:19749102
  59. 59. Verfuß UK, Miller LA, Pilz PKD, Schnitzler HU (2009) Echolocation by two foraging harbour porpoises. J Exper Biol 212:823–834
  60. 60. Gallus A, Dähne M, Verfuß UK, Bräger S, Adler S, Siebert U, et al. (2012) Use of static passive acoustic monitoring to assess the status of the 'Critically Endangered' Baltic harbour porpoise in German waters. Endangered Species Research 18:265–278
  61. 61. Clausen KT, Beedholm K, DeRuiter S, Madsen PT, Wahlberg M (2011) Click communication in harbour porpoises Phocoena phocoena. Bioacoustics 20: 1−2
  62. 62. Dähne M, Verfuß UK, Brandecker A, Siebert U, Benke H (2013 b) Methodology and results of calibration of tonal click detectors for small odontocetes (C-PODs). J Acoust Soc Am 134(3):2514–2522 pmid:23968049
  63. 63. Verfuß UK, Dähne M, Gallus A, Jabbusch M, Benke H (2013) Determining the detection thresholds for harbor porpoise clicks of autonomous data loggers, the Timing Porpoise Detectors. J Acoust Soc Am, 134: 2462–2468 pmid:23968043
  64. 64. R Core Team (2014) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, URL http://www.R-project.org/
  65. 65. Koschinski S, Diederichs A, Amundin M (2008) Click train patterns of free-ranging harbour porpoises acquired using T-PODs may be useful as indicators of their behaviour. Journal of Cetacean Research and Management, 10(2):147–155
  66. 66. Flater D (2015) XTide version 2.2.7. http://www.flaterco.com/xtide/. 2015-08-09
  67. 67. Hocking RR (2013) Methods and applications of linear models:regression and the analysis of variance. John Wiley and Sons
  68. 68. Lockyer C (2003) Harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) in the North Atlantic: Biological parameters. NAMMCO Sci Publ 5:71–90
  69. 69. Lockyer C (1986) Body Fat Condition in Northeast Atlantic Fin Whales, Balaenoptera physalus, and Its Relationship with Reprodution and Food Resource. Can J Fish Aquat Sci 43:142–147
  70. 70. Yasui WY, Gaskin DE (1986) Energy budget of a small cetacean, the harbour porpoise, Phocoena phocoena (L.). Ophelia 25:183−197
  71. 71. Booth CG, Embling C, Gordon J, Calderan SV, Hammond PS (2013) Habitat preferences and distribution of the harbour porpoise Phocoena phocoena west of Scotland. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 478: 273−285
  72. 72. Cardinale M, Casini M, Arrhenius F, Håkansson N (2003). Diel spatial distribution and feeding activity of herring (Clupea harengus) and sprat (Sprattus sprattus) in the Baltic Sea. Aquatic Living Resources, 16(3), 283–292
  73. 73. Didrikas T & Hansson S (2008). Effects of light intensity on activity and pelagic dispersion of fish: studies with a seabed-mounted echosounder. ICES Jour Mar Sci, 66(2), 388–395
  74. 74. Benke H & Siebert U (1996) The current status of harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) in German waters. Reports of the International Whaling Commission, SC/47/SM49
  75. 75. Santos MB, Pierce GJ, Learmonth JA, Reid RJ, Ross HM, Patterson IAP, et al. (2004) Variability in the diet of harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) in Scottish waters 1992–2003. Mar Mamm Sci 20:1–27
  76. 76. Van der Kooij J, Scott BE, Mackinson S (2008) The effects of environmental factors on daytime sandeel distribution and abundance on the Dogger Bank. J Sea Res 60:201–209
  77. 77. Leeney RH, Carslake D, Elwen SH (2011) Using Static Acoustic Monitoring to Describe Echolocation Behaviour of Heaviside’s Dolphins (Cephalorhynchus heavisidii) in Namibia. Aquatic Mammals 37(2):151–160
  78. 78. White GC and Garrott RA (1990) Analysis of Wildlife Radio Tracking Data. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, New York
  79. 79. Elliser CR, MacIver KH, Green M (2017) Group characteristics, site fidelity, and photo‐identification of harbor porpoises, Phocoena phocoena, in Burrows Pass, Fidalgo Island, Washington. Mar Mamm Sci,
  80. 80. Forney KA, Southall BL, Slooten E, Dawson S, Read AJ, Baird RW, et al. (2017) Nowhere to go: Noise impact assessments for marine mammal populations with high site fidelity. Endanger Species Res 32:391–413
  81. 81. Sveegaard S, Teilmann J, Tougaard J, Dietz R, Mouritsen KN, Desportes G, et al. (2011) High-density areas for harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) identified by satellite tracking. Mar Mamm Sci. 27:230–246
  82. 82. Clausen KT, Wahlberg M, Beedholm K, Deruiter S, Madsen PT (2011) Click Communication in Harbour Porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). Bioacoustics 20(1):1–28
  83. 83. Sørensen PM, Wisniewska DM, Jensen FH, Johnson M, Teilmann J, Madsen PT (2018) Click communication in wild harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). Sci Rep 8(1): 9702 pmid:29946073
  84. 84. Wisniewska DM, Johnson M, Teilmann J, Siebert U, Galatius A, Dietz R, et al. (2018) High rates of vessel noise disrupt foraging in wild harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). Proc R Soc B, 285(1872):20172314 pmid:29445018
  85. 85. Wisniewska DM, Ratcliffe JM, Beedholm K, Christensen CB, Johnson M, Koblitz J, et al. (2015) Range-dependent flexibility in the acoustic field of view of echolocating porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) eLife 2015(4):1–29
  86. 86. Nuuttila HK, Bertelli CM, Mendzil A, Dearle N (2018) Seasonal and diel patterns in cetacean use and foraging at a potential marine renewable energy site. Mar Pollut Bull 129(2):633–644 pmid:29108737
  87. 87. Holdman AK, Haexl JH, Klinck H, Torres LG (2018) Acoustic monitoring reveals the times and tides of harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) distribution off central Oregon, USA. Marine Mammal Science 9999(9999):1–23
  88. 88. Kyhn L (2005) Detection of harbor porpoises using T-PODs. Master Thesis. University Aarhus