Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

A multifactorial approach of nutritional, intellectual, brain development, cardiovascular risk, socio-economic, demographic and educational variables affecting the scholastic achievement in Chilean students: An eight- year follow-up study

  • Daniza M. Ivanovic ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    daniza@inta.uchile.cl

    Affiliation Laboratory of Nutrition and Neurological Sciences, Human Nutrition Area, Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology Dr. Fernando Monckeberg Barros (INTA), University of Chile, Santiago, Chile

  • Atilio F. Almagià,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Laboratory of Physical Anthropology and Human Anatomy, Institute of Biology, Faculty of Sciences, Pontifical Catholic University of Valparaíso, Valparaíso, Chile

  • Violeta C. Arancibia,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Center for Research in Education and Learning, University of Los Andes, Santiago, Chile

  • Camila V. Ibaceta,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliation Laboratory of Nutrition and Neurological Sciences, Human Nutrition Area, Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology Dr. Fernando Monckeberg Barros (INTA), University of Chile, Santiago, Chile

  • Vanessa F. Arias,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliations Laboratory of Nutrition and Neurological Sciences, Human Nutrition Area, Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology Dr. Fernando Monckeberg Barros (INTA), University of Chile, Santiago, Chile, School of Nutrition and Dietetics, Faculty of Medicine, Andres Bello University, Santiago, Chile

  • Tatiana R. Rojas,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliation Laboratory of Nutrition and Neurological Sciences, Human Nutrition Area, Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology Dr. Fernando Monckeberg Barros (INTA), University of Chile, Santiago, Chile

  • Ofelia C. Flores,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliations Laboratory of Nutrition and Neurological Sciences, Human Nutrition Area, Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology Dr. Fernando Monckeberg Barros (INTA), University of Chile, Santiago, Chile, School of Nutrition, Faculty of Medicine, University of Costa Rica, San José, Costa Rica

  • Francisca S. Villagrán,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliation Laboratory of Nutrition and Neurological Sciences, Human Nutrition Area, Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology Dr. Fernando Monckeberg Barros (INTA), University of Chile, Santiago, Chile

  • Liliana U. Tapia,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliation Laboratory of Nutrition and Neurological Sciences, Human Nutrition Area, Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology Dr. Fernando Monckeberg Barros (INTA), University of Chile, Santiago, Chile

  • Javiera A. Acevedo,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliation Laboratory of Nutrition and Neurological Sciences, Human Nutrition Area, Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology Dr. Fernando Monckeberg Barros (INTA), University of Chile, Santiago, Chile

  • Gladys I. Morales,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Public Health Department, Faculty of Medicine, University of La Frontera, Temuco, Chile

  • Víctor C. Martínez,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Commercial Engineering, Faculty of Economics and Business, University of Chile, Santiago, Chile

  • Cristián G. Larraín,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Radiology Department, Faculty of Medicine-German Clinic of Santiago, University of Development, Santiago, Chile

  • Claudio F. A. Silva,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Radiology Department, Faculty of Medicine-German Clinic of Santiago, University of Development, Santiago, Chile

  • Rodrigo B. Valenzuela,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Nutrition, Faculty of Medicine, University of Chile, Santiago, Chile

  • Cynthia R. Barrera,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Nutrition, Faculty of Medicine, University of Chile, Santiago, Chile

  • Pablo B. Billeke,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Neuroscience, Center for Research in Social Complexity (neuroSIS), Faculty of Government, University of Development, Santiago, Chile

  • Francisco M. Zamorano,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Division of Neuroscience, Center for Research in Social Complexity (neuroSIS), Faculty of Government, University of Development, Santiago, Chile, Advanced Quantitative Imaging Unit, Image Department, German Clinic of Santiago-University of Development, Santiago, Chile

  •  [ ... ],
  • Yasna Z. Orellana

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Laboratory of Nutrition and Neurological Sciences, Human Nutrition Area, Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology Dr. Fernando Monckeberg Barros (INTA), University of Chile, Santiago, Chile

  • [ view all ]
  • [ view less ]

Abstract

The aim of this study was to quantitate the relative impact of nutritional, intellectual, brain development, cardiovascular risk, socio-economic, demographic and educational variables on the results of the 2009 Quality Education Measurement System (SIMCE) tests of language and mathematics for scholastic achievement (SA) applying a multifactorial approach, in school-age children of the 2010 5th elementary school grade (5ESG) and of the 1st grade of high school (1HSG). The purposes were: i) to test the hypothesis that intellectual ability, the level of SA of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests, sex, parental schooling levels, and head circumference-for-age Z-score are the most relevant parameters associated with 2009 SIMCE outcomes; ii) to determine the predictive ability of the 2009 SIMCE results in determining the 2013 SIMCE outcomes for the 2010 5ESG cohort (when they graduated from elementary school, 8th grade) and for determining the 2013 University Selection Test (PSU) outcomes for the 2010 1HSG group (for university admission, when they graduated from high school, 4th grade); iii) to determine the association between the 2009 SIMCE results with the 2017 PSU outcomes for the 2010 5ESG group (for university admission, when they graduated from high school, 4th grade). A representative, proportional and stratified sample of 33 schools of the Metropolitan Region of Chile was randomly chosen. In these schools, 1,353 school-age children of both sexes, of the 2010 5ESG (n = 682; mean age = 10.8 years, SD = 0.6) and of the 2010 1HSG (n = 671; mean age = 14.8 years, SD = 0.6) participated. In both grades and tests, the findings confirm the hypotheses formulated. 2009 SIMCE outcomes were positively and significantly associated with 2013 SIMCE and with 2017 PSU and, with 2013 PSU outcomes in school-age children from 2010 5ESG and 1HSG, respectively. These findings may be useful for educational and health planning in Chile and countries in a comparable stage of development.

Introduction

One of the main national priorities in Chile is the improvement of the quality of the educational process. However, this is a multifactorial problem that depends on particular qualities of the child, his/her family and numerous factors related to the educational system such as the teachers’ academic background, their teaching methodologies and the infrastructure of the educational establishments [19].

All the factors mentioned above affect greatly the development of countries which is hampered by shortcomings in educational, socio-economic, socio-cultural, intellectual and nutritional conditions which affect the quality of life of their population. Few investigations have been carried out in Chile attempting to quantitate the relative impact of these factors on educational outcomes by applying standardized tests with national coverage. For instance, the long-term effects of nutritional status at an early age on scholastic achievement (SA) has not been sufficiently defined; additionally, research with a global approach of the predictors mentioned previously towards the follow-up of these studies is even scarcer [4,5,712]. Poor school-age children tend to become poor adults; this may be the expression of unsatisfactory educational outcomes. This may be explained by prevailing deficient home conditions and negative environments which do not stimulate adequately the learning processes, the development of intellectual capabilities and the fostering of health self-care [5,7,1216].

Educational inequalities result from the extent to which students with different socio-cultural origins have unequal probabilities of attaining satisfactory SA levels; as a consequence, cognitive abilities of children are very different across countries [1719]. Other authors have proposed that variables related to the family’s socio-economic status (SES) such as paternal schooling, and in particular maternal schooling, are consistent in explaining child SA, intelligence, and nutritional status, probably because mothers are the main source of intellectual stimulation and enrichment in the psycho-social environment and the health-related behaviour of the family [4,5,7,11,12,2024]. Besides maternal schooling, the family structure and stability and prenatal parental smoking have also been identified as important predictors of SA and child intelligence [25,26]. Other significant aspects that favour better SA arise from the presence of the father at home, the family size and the number of siblings [27,28]; however, other authors suggest that the apparent birth-order effect on SA or intelligence is an artefact of family size or that it is non-significant [29,30]. Studies by other authors report that sex differences in language and mathematics performance generally favour females and males, respectively, although recent data suggest that in many countries this gap is closing [3133].

Early anthropometric and nutritional measurements such as birth weight, birth length and breastfeeding are considered of great importance for later nutritional status and cognition [10,11,20,3440]. Our previous findings reveal that nutritional status is positively and significantly associated with socio-economic indicators, SA, dropout, intelligence, and brain development [27,11,13,20, 21,35]. Indicators of past nutrition (more than prenatal nutritional background and early nutritional measurements indicators), such as weight, height and head circumference, the anthropometric indicator of nutritional background and brain development, are of considerable importance during school age since nutrition affects both SA and intelligence [27,1114,20,41,42]; moreover, school dropout relates with head circumference but not with weight or height [41]. In addition, several publications show that obesity and physical fitness could be important factors not only for health but also for SA [4,5,12,4348].

The child’s intelligence represents the most relevant parameter that explains SA and is significantly associated with maternal schooling, maternal intelligence, head circumference and brain volume as well as with the antecedent of undernutrition in the first year of life [2,3,7,1114,20,21,3436,42,49,50]. This is how the findings from several authors confirm that differences in human brain size are relevant in explaining differences in SA and intelligence [27,1114,20,35,36,41,42,50,51]. As a consequence, children older for the school grade have significantly lower SA and intelligence parameters, deprived socio-economic conditions and lower level of education and occupation of parents, decreased brain measurements, suboptimal nutritional status and higher repetition and dropout rates [4,21,34]. Other results verify that SA and intelligence markers in girls are inversely and significantly associated with age at menarche [52].

In Chile, the quality of school education is evaluated by the Quality Education Measurement System (SIMCE) tests administered by the Agency for Education Quality, for elementary and high school students and by the University Selection Test (PSU), which evaluate the quality of education at the end of the high school cycle. These standardized tests have nationwide coverage [5355].

The full expression of the genetic potential of school- age children depends on multiple environmental factors by which, the educational process presents itself as a multifactorial phenomenon. In this context, SA will be the result of the interaction of child, family and educational system factors and children with cognitive, behavioural and educational problems must be detected at an early age [56]. This can be accomplished by adequate diagnoses which must be formulated to institute efforts for its evaluation and referral, if necessary, to improve their outcomes [57].

Building on this existing literature, the aim of this study was to quantitate the relative impact of nutritional, intellectual, brain development, cardiovascular risk, socio-economic, demographic and educational variables on the results of the 2009 SIMCE tests of language and mathematics. In this research, the impact of these variables on SA was carried out applying a multifactorial approach, in school-age children of the 2010 5th elementary school grade (2010 5ESG) and of the 1st grade of high school (2010 1HSG). The purposes were: i) to test the hypothesis that intellectual ability, the level of SA of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests, sex, parental schooling levels, and head circumference-for-age Z-score are the most relevant parameters associated with 2009 SIMCE outcomes; ii) to determine the predictive ability of the 2009 SIMCE results in determining the 2013 SIMCE outcomes for the 2010 5ESG cohort (when they graduated from elementary school, 8th grade) and for determining the 2013 PSU outcomes for the 2010 1HSG group (for university admission, when they graduated from high school, 4th grade); iii) to determine the association between the 2009 SIMCE results with the 2017 PSU outcomes for the 2010 5ESG group (for university admission, when they graduated from high school, 4th grade).

Material and methods

Study design

This is an observational, cross-sectional and follow-up study.

Study population

The target population, 187,860 children (39% of the Chilean school population), included all school-age children enrolled in the 5ESG (N = 91,663) and in the 1HSG (N = 96,197) in the Metropolitan Region of Chile in 2010 who took the SIMCE tests at the end of November 2009. They belonged to public, private-subsidized, and private non-subsidized schools from urban areas [58].

Sample selection

At the onset of March 2010, the Agency for Quality Education from the Ministry of Education provided us the results of the 2009 SIMCE tests for each school-age children. These results were stratified according to type of school (public, private-subsidized, and private non-subsidized), level of SA of educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests (high, medium and low) and sex. Population sampling was then performed to target a representative sample of students from the educational establishments from the urban areas of the Metropolitan Region of Chile. The sampling was carried out as follows: firstly, 33 educational establishments which represented 2.61% of the total population of the urban schools (N = 1,262), were randomly selected by proportional allocation according to their stratification by type of school and level of SA of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests and identified by: green (high), yellow (medium) or red (low) markers (Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Geographical distribution in the Metropolitan Region of Chile, of the educational establishments chosen in the sample with high (green), medium (yellow) and low (red) level of scholastic achievement in the 2009 SIMCE tests by type of school: Public (triangle); private subsidized (square); private non-subsidized (circle).

n = 33.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212279.g001

Secondly, in each of these 33 schools all students enrolled in both grades who took the 2009 SIMCE as well as their parents, the principals of the establishments and the language (L) and mathematics (M) teachers were invited to participate. A total of 1,353 school-age children of the 2010 5ESG (n = 682; males n = 379) and of the 2010 1HSG (n = 671; males n = 330), (95% of the original sample), as well as their parents, the school principals and teachers, agreed to participate and signed the informed consent form. Students’ ages ranged from 9.9y to 18.2y (mean 10.8; SD = 0.6y) and from 12.7y to 17.6y (mean = 14.8; SD = 0.6y), in the 2010 5ESG and in the 2010 1HSG, respectively.

Procedures of the field study

When Grant FONDECYT 1100435 was approved: i) the first part of the field study was carried out from March to September 2010, in order to collect data of the 2009 SIMCE outcomes and to assess the nutritional, intellectual, brain development, cardiovascular risk, socio-economic, demographic and educational variables. When Grant FONDECYT 1150524 was approved, the second part of the field study was carried out: ii) from March to May 2015, with the purpose to register, four years later, the 2013 SIMCE outcomes of the 2010 5ESG students who graduated from elementary school on 2013 and took the 2013 SIMCE tests and, the 2013 PSU outcomes of the 2010 1HSG students who graduated from high school on 2013, and took the PSU; and iii) eight years later, during June 2018, in order to register the 2017 PSU outcomes of the 2010 5ESG students who graduated from high school on 2017.

Ethical considerations

This study was approved by the Committee on Ethics in Studies in Humans of the Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology Dr. Fernando Monckeberg Barros (INTA), University of Chile, and ratified by the Committee on Bioethics of the National Fund for Scientific and Technological Development (FONDECYT), Chile. During 2010 and 2015 subjects' consent was obtained according to the norms for Human Experimentation, Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) [59].

Data collection conducted during 2010

2009 SIMCE outcomes.

SA was assessed through the 2009 SIMCE tests, which has national coverage and is administered by the Agency for Education Quality. The aim of this testing was to evaluate changes in the quality and equitableness of the educational process in the different areas covered by the national curriculum. Scores range between 0 and 400 and the results are expressed as mean ± SD in the L (LSA) and the M (MSA) SA tests. Data were also categorized in three ranges of achievements: high, medium and low in relation to the SA of the school-age children and the SA of the educational establishments, as defined by the Ministry of Education for these grades [53].

Nutritional status.

Prenatal nutritional background and early nutritional measurements such as birth weight, birth length and duration of breastfeeding were reported by the mothers who presented the document that was delivered to them by the hospital registry where their children were born. Measurements of weight, height and head circumference were carried out at school using standardized procedures; all the instruments were verified before measuring each subject [60]. The postnatal nutritional status was expressed as height-for-age Z-score according to NCHS-CDC tables [61,62]. Head circumference-for-age Z-score was assessed using the tables of D. Ivanovic, Olivares, Castro, and Ivanovic, 1995 [63], Nellhaus, 1968 [64,65], Roche, Mukherjee, Guo, and Moore, 1987 [66] and Tanner, 1984 [67]. Head circumference-for-age Z-score values were similar when applying these four tables (correlation coefficient between these patterns 0.98) [63]. The current nutritional status was expressed as body mass index (BMI, weight/height2) and compared to the NCHS-CDC tables and expressed as body mass index Z-score (Z-BMI) [61,62].

BMI was calculated using biological age derived from the Tanner stages [67]. Birth weight and birth length were used as indices of prenatal nutrition, height-for-age Z-score and head circumference-for-age Z-score served as indicators of postnatal nutrition and BMI was used as an index of current nutritional status.

Intellectual ability.

Intellectual ability was assessed with the standard version of the Raven's Progressive Matrices Test in book form, with a general scale for children 12 years or above that had been standardized for Chilean school-age children [68,69]. The test was administered collectively in the classroom by an educational psychologist and scores were recorded in a percentile scale according to age, with the following grading: Grade I = superior intellectual ability; Grade II = above average; Grade III = average; Grade IV = below average and Grade V = intellectually defective. WHO experts on its application in developing countries have recommended Raven’s test because its results are not affected by culture [70].

Brain development.

Brain development was assessed through the measurement of head circumference and expressed as Z-score for-age, as previously stated. Head circumference measurement is a simple method to assess brain growth and has been defined as an anthropometric indicator of both nutritional background and brain development [49,60,71].

Cardiovascular risk factors.

In addition to BMI waist circumference was measured using standard procedures and abdominal obesity was calculated using a percentile scale [72]. Systolic and diastolic blood pressures were classified as normal or high in a percentile scale [73]. Diabetes and smoking (number of cigarettes per day) were also registered. The cardiovascular risk index was expressed into a single score as number of risk factors and they were also considered individually.

SES.

SES was measured applying a score based on Graffar's modified parameters which consider schooling and occupation of the household head, and characteristics of the housing (building materials, ownership, water supply and ownership of durable goods) [74]. Graffar's modified scale has been adapted for Chilean urban and rural populations and classifies the population into five socio-economic strata: 1 = high; 2 = medium-high; 3 = medium; 4 = medium-low; and 5 = low.

Demographic variables.

Some demographic characteristics such as the student’s age, sex, age of menarche, number of siblings, order of place among siblings, number of family members, crowding (persons/bedroom) and promiscuity (persons/bed) were registered.

Variables dependent on the educational system.

Teachers were invited to answer a questionnaire that evaluates their academic background and teaching methodologies. An index was calculated for these variables, which finally was expressed as scores and categorized as adequate, regular and deficient. The principals of the schools were interviewed to assess the infrastructure of school. This information was confirmed by the investigators and was expressed as scores categorized as adequate, regular and deficient. Other educational variables considered were the type of school and the level of SA of the educational establishment in the 2009 SIMCE tests.

Follow-up study

Data collection conducted during 2015

2013 SIMCE outcomes.

Four years later, during 2013, 2010 5ESG school-age children were in the 8th grade of elementary school and graduated from elementary school. They took the 2013 SIMCE on November of this year both LSA and MSA scores and data were also categorized as we explained previously [53]. The aim was to establish whether the 2009 and 2013 SIMCE outcomes were interrelated.

2013 PSU outcomes.

Four years later, during 2013, 2010 1HSG school-age children were in the 4th grade of high school and graduated from high school. They took the 2013 PSU, on November of this year. LSA and MSA scores in the 2013 PSU were also registered and these data were provided by the Department of Evaluation, Measurement and Educational Registry (DEMRE) of the University of Chile and, by the Studies Centre of the Ministry of Education during 2015 and were also graded as follows both in LSA and MSA tests: low SA (< p25; score < 450), medium SA (≥ p25 and ≤ p 75; 450–620) and high SA (> p75; > 620). In fact, scores below 450 barred students from applying to higher education [54]. The purpose was to determine to what extent SA in the 2009 SIMCE would predict PSU outcomes four years later. The PSU, the baccalaureate examination with national coverage for admission to university, has minimum and maximum scores of 150 and 850, respectively. The L test considers 90 items and the M test 60 [54].

Data collection conducted during 2018

2017 PSU outcomes.

Eight years later, at the end of November 2017, 2010 5ESG school-age children were in the 4th grade of high school and graduated from high school and they took the 2017 PSU, both LSA and MSA scores. L and M tests consider 80 items each. LSA and MSA scores in the 2017 PSU were provided by the Department of Evaluation, Measurement and Educational Registry (DEMRE) of the University of Chile and, by the Studies Centre of the Ministry of Education at the onset of June 2018, as part of the development of Grant FONDECYT 1150524. [55]. Results were expressed in the same categories of the 2013 PSU.

Statistical analysis.

Data analysis included chi-squared test for ordinal variables and ANOVA for comparison of means. Pearson and Spearman correlation coefficients were used for quantitative and ordinal variables, respectively. In the linear regression analysis, the stepwise procedure was used to establish the most important independent variables that could affect LSA and MSA in the SIMCE tests (dependent variable). The determination coefficient (R2) was calculated to measure the fit of the regression models [75]. For all hypothesis tests the level of significance was 0.05. Data were processed using the software Stata 14.

Results

Distribution of the sample according to SES

The distribution of the sample according to SES categories was as follows: 0.3% belonged to high SES, 14.7% to medium-high SES, 38.3% to medium SES, 45.6% to medium-low SES and 1.1% to low SES.

SIMCE outcomes according to sex

In school-age children from the 2010 5ESG, LSA and MSA did not differ between males and females. However, in the 2010 1HSG, males achieved higher LSA scores (286.1 ± 48.6 (n = 330)) than females (274.1 ± 49.70 (n = 341)); (F = 8.35; p = .0040) and the same was observed for MSA scores (308.3 ± 50.7 (n = 330)) and (277.1 ± 51.6 (n = 341)), respectively (F = 52.75; p < .0001).

Distribution of the sample by type of school attended and the level of SA of the educational establishment

Differences were found in the distribution of students depending on the type of school attended and the level of SA of the educational establishments since in the private non-subsidized schools group most of the school-age children were attending to educational establishments with high SA (68.8%) compared with public schools (48.4%) and private-subsidized schools (42.1%) (Fig 2; p < .001).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Distribution of the sample by the level of scholastic achievement of the educational establishments (EESA) in the 2009 SIMCE tests and type of school.

Note. P = public schools; PS = private subsidized schools; PNS = private non-subsidized schools. (n = 1,353). X2o (4df) = 36.381 > X2t (4df) p < .001 = 18.465.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212279.g002

Associations between the level of SA of the educational establishments, SES and type of school

SA level of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests was also positively associated with SES (Fig 3; p < .001) and SES significantly differed according to type of school attended (Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Distribution of the sample by the level of scholastic achievement of the educational establishments (EESA) in the 2009 SIMCE tests and socio-economic status (SES).

(n = 1,353). X2o (4df) = 291.256 > X2t (4df) p < .001 = 18.465.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212279.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Distribution of the sample by type of school and socio-economic status (SES).

Note. P = public; PS = private subsidized; PNS = private non-subsidized. (n = 1,353). X2o (4df) = 268.808 > X2t (4df) p < .001 = 18.465.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212279.g004

Correlation coefficients between the 2009 SIMCE outcomes both LSA and MSA (dependent variables) and psychological, nutritional, cardiovascular risk, socio-economic, socio-cultural, demographic, family, and educational factors (independent variables) by grade

Table 1 shows the correlation coefficients between LSA and MSA in the 2009 SIMCE tests (dependent variables) and independent variables by school grade. Intellectual ability positively correlated with 2009 SIMCE outcomes in LSA and MSA in 2010 5ESG (p < .0001). Prenatal nutritional background and early anthropometric measurements such as birth weight and birth length did not correlate with 2009 SIMCE outcomes; the exception was the duration of breastfeeding in MSA of 2010 5ESG (p = .0364). Postnatal nutritional background markers height-for-age Z-score and head circumference-for-age Z-score, correlated positively and significantly with the 2009 SIMCE outcomes in both grades and tests. The correlations being higher for MSA than LSA, especially for head circumference-for-age Z-score in school-age children from both grades (p < .0001). A negative correlation between the 2009 SIMCE and current nutritional status expressed as Z-BMI was found only for LSA in school-age children from the 2010 1HSG (p = .0019). With regard to cardiovascular risk factors, negative and significant correlations were found only in the 2010 1HSG for both tests, with the exception of tobacco use for LSA in school-age children from the 2010 1HSG. Positive and significant correlations between 2009 SIMCE test outcomes and socio-economic and socio-cultural variables were observed with the exception of the property of housing in the 2010 5ESG and in MSA, in the 2010 1HSG. As for the demographic and family variables, negative and significant correlations were observed between student’s age, the number of siblings, the place occupied by the student among his siblings (only in 2010 1HSG), the number of family members (only in 2010 5ESG), the existence of crowding and promiscuity (only in 2010 1HSG). School-age children whose parents were married obtained higher scores in LSA and MSA, but differences were significant only for MSA in both grades.

thumbnail
Table 1. Correlation coefficients between scholastic achievement (SA) in the Education Quality Measurement System (2009 SIMCE) tests of language (LSA) and mathematics (MSA) (dependent variables) and psychological, nutritional, cardiovascular risk, socio-economic, socio-cultural, demographic, family, and educational indicators (independent variables) by grade.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212279.t001

With regard to the variables related to the educational system, positive correlations were observed between 2009 SIMCE outcomes and the SA level of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE and school infrastructure in both grades and tests (p < .0001).

The teacher’s academic background was comparable in the different types of schools since all had a professional degree but only 16% had post graduate studies. As a consequence, only in 2010 1HSG L teachers academic background positively correlated with LSA (p < .0001) and only in 2010 5ESG, L teaching methodologies positively correlated with LSA (p = .0055). Although for M teacher’s academic background was not associated with MSA, positive correlations were found between M teaching methodologies and MSA both in 2010 5ESG (p = .0002) and 1HSG (p < .0001).

Multiple regression analysis between LSA and MSA in the 2009 SIMCE (dependent variables) and most relevant independent variables in school-age children of the 2010 5ESG

The multiple regression models considered all independent variables significantly associated with the dependent variables, LSA and MSA. The stepwise procedure, as we explained previously in the statistical analysis, chooses those independent variables significantly associated with LSA and MSA. Multiple regression models between LSA and MSA in the 2009 SIMCE test (dependent variables) and the independent variables choose for the statistical procedure in school-age children from 2010 5ESG (Table 2) revealed that for LSA, the level of SA of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests (high), sex (females), paternal schooling (≥ 14y), head circumference-for-age Z-score (> 0) and intellectual ability (grades I and II) were the independent variables associated with LSA results (R2 = .354; p < .0001). For MSA, the level of SA of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests (high), head circumference-for-age Z-score (> 0), intellectual ability (grades I and II) and paternal schooling (≥ 14y) were the independent variables entered in the statistical regression model (R 2 = .463; p < .00001).

thumbnail
Table 2. Multiple regression models between language (LSA) and mathematics (MSA) scholastic achievement (SA) in the Education Quality Measurement System (2009 SIMCE) tests (dependent variables) and most relevant independent variables in school-age children of the 2010 5ESG.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212279.t002

Multiple regression analysis between LSA and MSA in the 2009 SIMCE (dependent variables) and most relevant independent variables in school-age children of the 2010 1HSG

In school-age children from 2010 1HSG (Table 3), the level of SA of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests (high), maternal schooling (≥ 14y), and intellectual ability (grades I and II) were the independent variables more relevant associated with LSA in the 2009 SIMCE (R 2 = .3892; p < .00001). In MSA, the level of SA of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests (high), maternal schooling (≥ 14y), sex (males) and intellectual ability (grades I and II), were the independent variables entered in the statistical regression model (R 2 = .500; p < .00001).

thumbnail
Table 3. Multiple regression models between language (LSA) and mathematics (MSA) scholastic achievement (SA) in the Education Quality Measurement System (2009 SIMCE) tests (dependent variables) and most relevant independent variables in school-age children of the 2010 1HSG.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212279.t003

Association between 2009 SIMCE with 2013 SIMCE and 2017 PSU tests both LSA and MSA of school-age children of the 2010 5ESG

Four years later, LSA and MSA in the 2009 SIMCE of school-age children of the 2010 5ESG were found to be positively associated with LSA (A) and MSA (B) in the 2013 SIMCE when they graduated from 8th grade of elementary school (p < .001 and p < .001, respectively) (Fig 5). This was also observed, eight years later, with LSA (C) and MSA D), in the 2017 PSU for university admission, when they graduated from high school (p < .001 and p < .001, respectively).

thumbnail
Fig 5.

Association between the 2009 Quality Education Measurement System (2009 SIMCE) test of school-age children of the 2010 5ESG, four years later, with the 2013 SIMCE test, when they graduated from 8th grade of elementary school, both language (LSA) (A) and mathematics (MSA) (B) and, eight years later, with the 2017 University Selection Test (2017 PSU) outcomes, for university admission, when they graduated from high school, both LSA (C) and MSA (D). Note. A. (X2o (4df) = 167.580 > X2t (4df) p < .001 = 18.465). B. (X2o (4df) = 219.503 > X2t (4df) p < .001 = 18.465). C. (X2o (4df) = 116.899 > X2t (4df) p < .001 = 18.465). D. (X2o (4df) = 95.113 > X2t (4df) p < .001 = 18.465).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212279.g005

Association between 2009 SIMCE and 2013 PSU tests both LSA and MSA of school-age children of the 2010 1HSG

Similarly, LSA and MSA in the 2009 SIMCE of school-age children of the 2010 1HSG were found to be positively associated with their 2013 PSU outcomes both LSA (A) and MSA (B), which they took at the end of high school for admission to university (p < .001 and p < .001, respectively) (Fig 6).

thumbnail
Fig 6.

Association between the 2009 Quality Education Measurement System (2009 SIMCE) test of school-age children of the 2010 1HSG, four years later, with the 2013 University Selection Test (2013 PSU) outcomes, for university admission, when they graduated from high school, both language (LSA) (A) and mathematics (MSA) (B). Note. LSA: (X2o (4df) = 195.763 > X2t (4df) p < .001 = 18.465). MSA: (X2o (4df) = 291.332 > X2t (4df) p < .001 = 18.465).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212279.g006

Discussion

The findings of this study reveal, that the level of SA of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests to which school-age children are subjected, the intellectual ability, parental schooling, head circumference and sex were the variables significantly associated in the statistical regression model, with their SA. These results were also observed in the sample of 2010 1HSG students when they graduated from high school four years later and took the 2013 PSU [56].

The level of SA of the educational establishments

The level of SA of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests, an educational system variable, was a significant predictor of 2009 SIMCE outcomes in the school-age children, and was found to be significantly associated with the type of school attended and the SES of the family. Therefore, school-age children from high and medium SES levels studied mainly in the educational establishments with high levels of SA in the 2009 SIMCE tests. Socio-economic disadvantage appears to entrench the educational disadvantage in Chile, with SES reflecting the quality of upbringing and schooling children receive, and thereby influencing their chance of attending university.

Schools with high levels of SA probably create a more stimulating environment, provide a more adequate infrastructure which favours the learning process and the parents themselves have higher levels of education and income. Students attending these schools develop higher levels of intellectual ability, have higher head circumference-for-age Z-scores, and are trained by teachers with better academic backgrounds who apply more efficient teaching methodologies [76,77].

Intellectual ability

Intellectual ability was one of the best predictors of SA and is explained to a significant point by maternal intellectual quotient, and by brain volume and nutritional status during the first year of life [7,13,20,35]; this has been observed independently of age, sex and socio-economic stratum [35]. Results from other studies carried out in Chilean school-age children to determine the interrelationships between intellectual ability and socio-economic, cultural, family, mass media exposure, demographic and educational factors showed that maternal schooling was the variable with the greatest explanatory power in intellectual ability variance [78]. These results are in agreement with other investigators who have emphasized that children’s intelligence is significantly associated with SA, maternal schooling, maternal intelligence, head circumference and brain volume as well as antecedents of undernutrition in the first year of life [2,3,7,1114,20,21,3436,42,4951].

Parental schooling level

Paternal and maternal schooling levels were the socio-economic and socio-cultural family variables which more accurately predict 2009 SIMCE outcomes as well as the results of intelligence testing in 2010 5ESG and 1HSG, respectively. This reflects the widely acknowledged opinion that intelligence is the best predictor of SA, as shown in this study, since brighter parents attain higher levels of education, occupy better-paying jobs and form families with higher incomes [7,1114,17,20]. It has been described that children from less advantaged families were up to twice as likely to be in the lowest quintile of mathematics and literacy scores. Around two-thirds of this elevated risk was ‘direct’ and the majority of the remainder was mediated by early cognitive ability and not self-regulation [79]. Maternal schooling is the strongest predictor of long-term SA and cognitive neurodevelopment in their children even among disadvantaged groups since, as previously stated, they are the main source of intellectual stimulation; maternal intelligence has been shown to be the best predictor of a child’s intelligence [7,11,12,14,21,2225,36,80].

Prenatal and postnatal nutritional background and current nutritional status

Head circumference, the anthropometric indicator of nutritional background and brain development was another important predictor of 2009 SIMCE outcomes in 2010 5ESG school-age children in both tests. This was not observed in school-age children from 2010 1HSG probably because a proportion of the deprived children (head circumference-for-age Z-score <0) had dropped out along these school years [41]. Head circumference has been defined as the most important anthropometric parameter associated with 2009 SIMCE outcomes [6] and agrees with other studies that stated that this is the most relevant anthropometric parameter, significantly associated with SA, parental schooling levels and especially with maternal schooling and intelligence: head circumference is positively and highly correlated with brain volume [27,1114,22,35,41,81]. Moreover, school dropout rates relate with head circumference but not with weight or height [41].

Birth weight and birth length, reflect the mother's nutritional conditions, besides being a predictor of later child growth [10,11]. Results from several authors reported that low birth weight, birth length and breastfeeding infants are at greater risk for SA and intelligence at school age although other investigators did not find any relationship study [4,5,11,12,20,3440]. In the current study, prenatal nutritional background and early nutritional measurements, were not found to be significantly associated with 2009 SIMCE outcomes and this does not agree with the results of other investigators [34]; the exception to these results was the negative correlation found for breastfeeding and MSA in 2010 5ESG school-age children. This could be explained because breastfeeding duration tends to be longer in families of the low SES in whom SA is significantly lower than in families of the high SES [82]. Although some authors report significant associations between breastfeeding, cognition and brain size, others emphasize that this association can be explained largely by socio-demographic factors, parental lifestyles and maternal intelligence [40,8285].

The impact of sex

Sex differences also contribute to explain the 2009 SIMCE outcomes in LSA only in school-age children of the 2010 5ESG (favouring females) and in MSA of school-age children from 2010 1HSG (favouring males). Our results are in agreement with those of other authors, although, as stated previously, recent data suggest that in many countries this gap is closing [3133].

Dietary intake habits, physical activity and cardiovascular risk factors

Some independent variables such as dietary intake habits and physical activity were previously tested as predictors for 2009 SIMCE outcomes but they did not contribute to explain the 2009 SIMCE results both LSA and MSA [86]. For Z-BMI a negative correlation was found only in LSA of school-age children from 2010 1HSG and some publications show an inverse and significant association between SA and obesity [4346]; nevertheless, other results suggest that obesity is not associated with academic performance [4,5,12,47,48]. Our recent findings in an eight-year follow-up study also show that obesity and SA in the SIMCE and PSU tests, both with nationally coverage in Chile, are not significantly associated [87]. Results also suggest that physical fitness in boys and obesity status in girls could be important factors not only for health status but also for SA, independent of socio-economic and behavioural backgrounds [45].

Cardiovascular risk factors negatively and significantly correlated with 2009 SIMCE outcomes only in school-age children of 2010 1HSG and in both 2009 SIMCE tests, with the exception of smoking, although these results did not contribute to explain the results. Other findings do not support the existence of consistent associations between blood pressure and subsequent performance in tests assessing various cognitive domains in adolescents [88].

Limitations of study

A limitation of the present study is that genetic and environmental factors that have been described by some authors as significant determinants for SA, intellectual ability, head circumference with brain size and other determinant variables establish complex interactions which were not measured in the present study and for these reasons further research is necessary [8991].

Recommendations for future research

Our results suggest that significant improvements in the quality of education are positively and significantly associated with improvements in all national socio-economic sectors of activity because education is a multifactorial problem influenced by economic, socio-cultural, family, nutritional status, psychological and educational system variables among others.

The purpose of the present investigation is to contribute to increase the existing evidence for the formulation of a theory about SA, which is difficult to establish, since its determinants vary from child to child [21]. The present study represents one of the most extensive explorations carried out in Chile focussing on the assessment of the determinants of SA. This evaluation is unique in terms of the range and number of variables measured and the combination of child (nutritional, intellectual, brain development, cardiovascular risk factors, socio-economic, demographic and educational characteristics), family and educational system factors. In summary, the level of SA of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests, intellectual ability, paternal and maternal schooling levels, head circumference and sex, are important predictors of 2009 SIMCE outcomes. These findings confirm the hypothesis that intellectual ability, the level of SA of the educational establishments in the 2009 SIMCE tests, sex, parental schooling levels and head circumference-for-age Z-score are the most relevant parameters associated with SA. The R-squared values were higher for MSA (46 to 50%, in school-age children of the 2010 5ESG and 1HSG, respectively) than LSA (35 to 39%, respectively). This can be explained because mathematics requires higher order thinking skills that need larger brain size and intellectual skills, since in both grades correlations between MSA and both intellectual ability and head circumference-for-age Z-score were higher than those registered with LSA; however, more research is needed to explore the extent to which brain structures are associated with learning [56]. Secondly, the findings of this study also reveal that 2009 SIMCE outcomes are a good predictor of 2013 SIMCE results in school-age children of the 2010 5ESG and, of PSU outcomes in both grades.

The results of this study should be considered as showing a statistical association and do not represent a direct cause and effect relationship. In Chile, SIMCE, a test with national coverage is an excellent predictor for the results in later SIMCE tests including the PSU scores, for university admission. These results also have clinical implications, since they allow the identification of risk groups with suboptimal learning, in order to implement all necessary measures to improve their outcomes starting from an early age. These findings may be useful in planning public policies in the education and health sectors in this and other countries.

Supporting information

S1 Dataset. Total.zip.

This database includes the variables considered in this study (Excel).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212279.s001

(ZIP)

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the Agency for Quality Education, to the Studies Centre of the Ministry of Education of Chile and to the Department of Evaluation, Measurement and Educational Registry (DEMRE) of the University of Chile, for all the facilities given to carry out this research; to principals and teachers of the educational establishments and to parents and their children, which made ​​possible the realization of this study; to Dr Oscar Brunser, MD, for helpful comments and suggestions and to Mr Luis Santander for their valuable collaboration in the field study.

References

  1. 1. Chile. Ministry of Education. [Diagnosis of Chilean Education]. Santiago: Superintendence of Education. Document Nº 11.934; 1974. Spanish.
  2. 2. Ivanovic DM, Leiva BP, Castro CG, Olivares MG, Jansana JM, Castro V, et al. Brain Development Parameters and Intelligence in Chilean High School Graduates. Intelligence. 2004;32(5):461–479.
  3. 3. Ivanovic DM, Leiva BP, Pérez HT, Olivares MG, Díaz NS, Urrutia MSC, et al. Head size and intelligence, learning, nutritional status and brain development. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(8):1118–1131. pmid:15093150
  4. 4. Ivanovic DM, Rodríguez M del P, Pérez HT, Alvear J, Díaz NS, Leyton BD, et al. Twelve-year follow-up study of the impact of nutritional status at the onset of elementary school on later educational situation of Chilean school-age children. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2008;62(1):18–31. pmid:17311059
  5. 5. Ivanovic DM, Rodríguez M del P, Pérez H, Alvear J, Almagià AF, Toro TD, et al. Impact of nutritional status at the onset of elementary school on academic aptitude test achievement at the end of high school in a multifactorial approach. Br J Nutr. 2009;102(1):142–154. pmid:19138441
  6. 6. Ivanovic DM, Ibaceta CV, Orellana YZ, Calderón PM, Morales GI, Leyton BD, et al. Brain development and scholastic achievement in the Quality Education Measurement System (SIMCE) Tests in Chilean school-age children. Pediatr Res. 2014;75(3):464–470. pmid:24322172
  7. 7. Ivanovic D, Pérez H, Olivares M, Díaz N, Leyton B, Ivanovic R. Scholastic Achievement: A Multivariate Analysis of Nutritional, Intellectual, Socio-Economic, Socio-Cultural, Family and Demographic Variables in Chilean School-Age Children. Nutrition. 2004;20(10):878–889. pmid:15474876
  8. 8. Schiefelbein E, Farrel J. Eight Years of Their Lives: Through Schooling to the Labour Market in Chile. Ottawa, Canada: International Development Research Centre (IDRC); 1982.
  9. 9. Schiefelbein E, Simmons J. The Determinants of School Achievement: A Review of the Research for Developing Countries. Ottawa, Canada: International Development Research Centre (IDRC); 1981.
  10. 10. Hardy BJ. Birth weight and subsequent physical and intellectual development. N Engl J Med. 1973;289(18):973–974. pmid:4743624
  11. 11. Ivanovic D, Leiva B, Pérez H, Inzunza N, Almagià A, Toro T, et al. Long-term effects of severe undernutrition during the first year of life on brain development and learning in Chilean high school graduates. Nutrition. 2000;16(11/12):1056–1063.
  12. 12. Ivanovic D, Pérez H, Leiva B, Almagià A, Díaz N, Leyton B, et al. Neuropsychological parameters affecting the Academic Aptitude Test (AAT) achievement at the end of high school in 1996 and their impact on job status in 2002: A multifactorial approach in a follow-up study. In: Madsen LE, editor. Achievement Tests: Types, Interpretations and Uses. New York, United States of America: Nova Science Publishers, Inc; 2011. pp.139–190.
  13. 13. Ivanovic R, Forno H, Castro CG, Ivanovic D. Intellectual ability and nutritional status assessed through anthropometric measurements of Chilean school-age children from different socioeconomic status. Ecol Food Nutr. 2000;39(1):35–59.
  14. 14. Ivanovic D, Pérez H, Leiva B, Castro C, Castro V, Jansana JM, et al. Parental Intellectual quotient (IQ) and its Relation with their Offspring’s IQ. In: Columbus AM, editor. Advances in Psychology Research. New York, United States of America: Nova Science Publishers, Inc; 2011. pp. 53–74.
  15. 15. Yaqub S. Poor children grow into poor adults: harmful mechanisms or over-deterministic theory? J Int Dev. 2002;14(8):1081–1093.
  16. 16. Pollitt E. Poverty and Child Development: Relevance of Research in Developing Countries to the United States. Child Dev. 1994;65(2 Spec No):283–295.
  17. 17. Burhan NAS, Yunus MM, Tovar MEL, Burhan NMG. Why are cognitive abilities of children so different across countries? The link between major socioeconomic factors and PISA test scores. Pers Individ Dif. 2017;105:95–106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.09.043
  18. 18. Al-Mekhlafi HM, Mahdy MA, Sallam AA, Ariffin WA, Al-Mekhlafi AM, Amran AA, et al. Nutritional and socio-economic determinants of cognitive function and educational achievement of Aboriginal schoolchildren in rural Malaysia. Br J Nutr. 2011;106(7):1100–1106. pmid:21492493
  19. 19. Segretin MS, Lipina SJ, Hermida MJ, Sheffield TD, Nelson JM, Espy KA, et al. Predictors of cognitive enhancement after training in preschoolers from diverse socioeconomic backgrounds. Front Psychol. 2014;5:205. pmid:24659975
  20. 20. Ivanovic D, Almagià A, Toro T, Castro C, Pérez H, Urrutia MS, et al. [Impact of nutritional status on brain development, intelligence and school performance, within the framework of a multifactorial approach]. Revista Interamericana de Desarrollo Educativo, La Educación (OEA). 2000; 44(134–135)I-II:3–35. Spanish
  21. 21. Ivanovic R, Castro C, Ivanovic D. There is no theory about school performance. Journal of Education (Ministry of Education of Chile).1995;224:40–45. Spanish
  22. 22. Forns J, Julvez J, García-Esteban R, Guxens M, Ferrer M, Grellier J, et al. Maternal intelligence-mental health and child neuropsychological development at age 14 months. Gac Sanit. 2012;26(5):397–404. pmid:22284911
  23. 23. Torres DD. Understanding how family socioeconomic status mediates the maternal intelligence-child cognitive outcomes relationship: a moderated mediation analysis. Biodemography Soc Biol. 2013;59(2):157–177. pmid:24215257
  24. 24. Whitley E, Gale CR, Deary IJ, Kivimaki M, Singh-Manoux A, Batty GD. Influence of maternal and paternal IQ on offspring health and health behaviours: evidence for some trans-generational associations using the 1958 British birth cohort study. Eur Psychiatry. 2013;28(4):219–224. pmid:22541368
  25. 25. Rahu K, Rahu M, Pullmann H, Allik J. Effect of birth weight, maternal education and prenatal smoking on offspring intelligence at school age. Early Hum Dev. 2010;86(8):493–497. pmid:20634008
  26. 26. Gross S, Mettelman B, Dye TD, Slagle T. Impact of family structure and stability on academic outcome in preterm children at 10 years of age. J Pediatr. 2001;138(2):169–175. pmid:11174612
  27. 27. Edwards RD, Roff J. Negative Effects of Paternal Age on Children’s Neurocognitive Outcomes Can Be Explained by Maternal Education and Number of Siblings. PLoS ONE. 2010;5(9): e12157. pmid:20856853
  28. 28. Sciara FJ. Effects of father absence on the educational achievement of urban black children. Child Stud J. 1975;5:45–55.
  29. 29. Downey DB. Number of Siblings and Intellectual Development The Resource Dilution Explanation. Am Psychol. 2001;56(6–7):497–504. pmid:11413873
  30. 30. Intelligence Kanazawa S., Order Birth, and Size Family. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2012;38(9):1157–1164.
  31. 31. Reilly D., Gender culture, and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoS ONE. 2012; 7(7):e39904. pmid:22808072
  32. 32. Stoet G, Geary DC. Sex differences in mathematics and reading achievement are inversely related: within- and across-nation assessment of 10 years of PISA data. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(3):e57988. pmid:23516422
  33. 33. Voyer D, Voyer S. Gender Differences in Scholastic Achievement: A Meta-Analysis. Psychol Bull. 2014;140(4):1174–1204. pmid:24773502
  34. 34. Heinonen K, Eriksson JG, Lahti J, Kajantie E, Pesonen AK, Tuovinen S, et al. Late preterm birth and neurocognitive performance in late adulthood: a birth cohort study. Pediatrics. 2015;135(4):e818–825. pmid:25733746
  35. 35. Ivanovic D, Leiva B, Pérez H, Almagià A, Toro T, Urrutia MS, et al. Nutritional status, brain development and scholastic achievement of Chilean high school graduates from high and low intellectual quotient and socioeconomic status. Br J Nutr. 2002;87(1):81–92. pmid:11895316
  36. 36. Eriksen HL, Kesmodel US, Underbjerg M, Kilburn TR, Bertrand J, Mortensen EL. Predictors of intelligence at the age of 5: family, pregnancy and birth characteristics, postnatal influences, and postnatal growth. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(11):e79200. pmid:24236109
  37. 37. Tanis JC, Van Braeckel KN, Kerstjens JM, Bocca-Tjeertes IF, Reijneveld SA, Bos AF. Functional outcomes at age 7 years of moderate preterm and full term children born small for gestational age. J Pediatr. 2015;166(3):552–558.e1. pmid:25575420
  38. 38. Ulvund SE, Smith L, Lindemann R. Psychological status at 8–9 years of age in children with birth weight below 1,501 grams. Tidsskr Nor Laegeforen. 2001;121(3):298–302. pmid:11242870
  39. 39. Victora CG, Horta BL, Loret de Mola C, Quevedo L, Pinheiro RT, Gigante DP, et al. Association between breastfeeding and intelligence, educational attainment, and income at 30 years of age: a prospective birth cohort study from Brazil. Lancet Glob Health. 2015;3(4):e199–205. pmid:25794674
  40. 40. Sajjad A, Tharner A, Kiefte-de Jong JC, Jaddoe VV, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, et al. Breastfeeding duration and non-verbal IQ in children. Epidemiol Community Health. 2015;69(8):775–781. pmid:25700532
  41. 41. Ivanovic D, Olivares M, Castro C, Ivanovic R. Nutrition and learning in Chilean school-age children. Chile’s Metropolitan Region. Survey 1986–1987. Nutrition. 1996;12(5):321–328. pmid:8875515
  42. 42. Toro T, Almagia A, Ivanovic D. [Anthropometric assessment and school achievement in school-age children from high school in Valparaiso, Chile]. Arch Latinoam Nutr. 1998;48(3): 201–209. Spanish. pmid:9951531
  43. 43. Booth JN, Tomporowski PD, Boyle JM, Ness AR, Joinson C, Leary SD, et al. Obesity impairs academic attainment in adolescence: findings from ALSPAC, a UK cohort. Int J Obes. 2014; 38(10):1335–1342. pmid:24614099
  44. 44. Carey FR, Singh GK, Brown HS 3rd, Wilkinson AV. Educational outcomes associated with childhood obesity in the United States: cross-sectional results from the 2011–2012 National Survey of Children's Health. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12 (Suppl 1): S3. pmid:26222699
  45. 45. Morita N, Nakajima T, Okita K, Ishihara T, Sagawa M, Yamatsu K. Relationships among fitness, obesity, screen time and academic achievement in Japanese adolescents. Physiol Behav. 2016;163:161–166. pmid:27150913
  46. 46. Kamijo K, Khan NA, Pontifex MB, Scudder MR, Drollette ES, Raine LB, et al. The relation of adiposity to cognitive control and scholastic achievement in preadolescent children. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2012;20(12):2406–2411. pmid:22546743
  47. 47. Abdelalim A, Ajaj N, Al-Tmimy A, Alyousefi M, Al-Rashaidan S, Hammoud MS, et al. Childhood Obesity and Academic Achievement among Male Students in Public Primary Schools in Kuwait. Med Princ Pract. 2012; 21(1):14–19. pmid:22024698
  48. 48. Veldwijk J, Fries MC, Bemelmans WJ, Haveman-Nies A, Smit HA, Koppelman GH, et al. Overweight and school performance among primary school children: the PIAMA birth cohort study. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2012;20(3):590–596. pmid:22030985
  49. 49. Miller LC, Joshi N, Lohani M, Singh R, Bhatta N, Rogers B, et al. Head growth of undernourished children in rural Nepal: Association with demographics, health and diet. Paediatr Int Child Health. 2016;36(2):91–101. pmid:27077633
  50. 50. Pietschnig J, Penke L, Wicherts JM, Zeiler M, Voracek M. Meta-analysis of associations between human brain volume and intelligence differences: How strong are they and what do they mean? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;57:411–432. pmid:26449760
  51. 51. Hair NL, Hanson JL, Wolfe BL, Pollak SD. Association of Child Poverty, Brain Development, and Academic Achievement. JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169(9):822–829. pmid:26192216
  52. 52. Johansson T, Ritzén EM. Very long-term follow-up of girls with early and late menarche. Endocr Dev. 2005;8:126–136. pmid:15722621
  53. 53. Chile. Chile's government. [SIMCE 2009 National results]. Santiago, Chile: SIMCE, Curriculum and Evaluation Unit (UCE), Ministry of Education; 2010.
  54. 54. University of Chile. [Statistical compendium year 2013 PSU: academic admission process]. Santiago, Chile: University of Chile, Vice-rectory of Academic Affairs, Department of Evaluation, Measurement and Educational Registry (DEMRE)]; 2015. Spanish
  55. 55. University of Chile. [Statistical compendium year 2018 PSU: academic admission process]. Santiago, Chile: University of Chile, Vice-rectory of Academic Affairs, Department of Evaluation, Measurement and Educational Registry (DEMRE)]; 2018. Spanish
  56. 56. Ivanovic D, Valenzuela R, Almagià A, Barrera C, Arancibia V, Larraín C, Silva C, Billeke P, Zamorano F, Villagrán F, Orellana Y, Martínez V. Impact of anthropometric nutritional parameters on the University Selection Test in Chile: A multifactorial approach. Nutrition. 2019; 57:74–83. pmid:30153583
  57. 57. Cheng ER, Palta M, Kotelchuck M, Poehlmann J, Witt WP. Cognitive delay and behavior problems prior to school age. Pediatrics. 2014;134(3):e749–757. pmid:25113290
  58. 58. Chile. Ministry of Education. [Enrollment of Chilean students in 4th and 8th grades of elementary school by geographical area, sex and type of school, in the Metropolitan Region of Chile. Santiago: Ministry of Education]; 2009. Spanish
  59. 59. The World Medical Association. Declaration of Helsinki—Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. 59th WMA General Assembly, Seoul, Korea, October; 2008.
  60. 60. Gibson R. Principles of Nutritional Assessment. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. 1990.
  61. 61. De Onis M, Onyango AW, Borghi E, Siyam A, Nishida Ch, Siekmann J. Development of a WHO growth reference for school-aged children and adolescents. Bull World Health Organ. 2007;85(9):660–667. pmid:18026621
  62. 62. National Center for Health Statistics. Growth curves. CDC growth Charts for United States: Methods and development. www.cdc.gov/growthcharts; 2000.
  63. 63. Ivanovic D, Olivares M, Castro C, Ivanovic R. Head circumference in Chilean school children 5 to 18 years of age and socioeconomic status. Rev Med Chile. 1995;123(5):587–599. pmid:8525205
  64. 64. Nellhaus G. Head circumference from birth to eighteen years. Practical composite international and interracial graphs. Pediatrics. 1968;41(1):106–114. pmid:5635472
  65. 65. Weaver DD, Christian JC. Familial variation of head size and adjustment for parental head circumference. J Pediatr. 1980; 96(6):990–4. pmid:7189556.
  66. 66. Roche AF, Mukherjee D, Guo SM, Moore WM. Head circumference reference data: birth to 18 years. Pediatrics. 1987;79(5):706–712. pmid:3575026
  67. 67. Tanner J.M. Physical growth and development. In: Forfar JO, Arneil GC, editors. Textbook of Pediatrics. Edinburgh, Scotland: Churchill Livingstone; 1984. pp. 278–330.
  68. 68. Ivanovic R, Forno H, Durán MC, Hazbún J, Castro C, Ivanovic D. [Assessment of the intellectual ability (Raven's Progressive Matrices Test) in school children from 5 to 18 years old. I. General background, standards and recommendations. Metropolitan Region. Chile. 1986–1987]. Iberpsicología. 2003;8(1). (Full version of the work appeared in Rev Psicol Gen Apl. 2000;53(1):5–30). Spanish
  69. 69. Raven J. Standard Progressive Matrices Test. General Scale/Sets A, B, C, D, E. Buenos Aires, Argentina: Paidos; 2007.
  70. 70. Pollitt E. Evaluación de la conducta en los estudios sobre las consecuencias funcionales de la malnutrición. Descripción de métodos. En: OPS/OMS, editor. Ambiente, nutrición y desarrollo mental. Publicación científica No 450. Washington, Estados Unidos de Norteamérica: OPS/OMS; 1983. pp. 58–74.
  71. 71. Jelliffe DB. The assessment of the nutritional status of the community. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1966.
  72. 72. Fernández JR, Reeden DT, Petrobielli A, Allison DB. Waist circumference percentiles in nationally representative sample of African-American, European-American and Mexican American children and adolescent. J Pediatr. 2004;145(4):439–444. pmid:15480363
  73. 73. The fourth report on the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of high blood pressure in children and adolescents. National High Blood Pressure Education Program Working Group on High Blood Pressure in Children and Adolescents. Pediatrics. 2004;114(2 Suppl 4th Report):555–576.
  74. 74. Álvarez ML, Muzzo S, Ivanovic D. [Scale for measurement of socioeconomic level, in the health area]. Rev Med Chile.1985;113(3):243–249. Spanish. pmid:3915140
  75. 75. Guilford J, Fruchter B. [Fundamental statistics in psychology and education]. Ciudad de México, México: McGraw Hill;1984. Spanish.
  76. 76. Shashidhar S, Rao C, Hegde R. Factors affecting scholastic performances of adolescents. Indian J Pediatr. 2009;76(5):495–499. pmid:19390794
  77. 77. Guarino CM, Reckase MD, Wooldridge JM. Policy and Research Challenges of Moving Toward Best Practices in Using Student Test Scores to Evaluate Teacher Performance. J Res Educ Eff. 2015;8(1):1–7.
  78. 78. Ivanovic D, Forno H, Ivanovic R. Study of the intellectual capacity (Test of Raven's Progressive Matrices) in school children from 5 to 18 years old. II. Interrelationships with socio-economic, socio-cultural, family, mass media exposure, demographic and educational factors. Rev Psicol Gen Apl. 2001;54(3):443–466.
  79. 79. Pearce A, Sawyer AC, Chittleborough CR, Mittinty MN, Law C, Lynch JW. Do early life cognitive ability and self regulation skills explain socio-economic inequalities in academic achievement? An effect decomposition analysis in UK and Australian cohorts. Soc Sci Med. 2016;165:108–118. pmid:27500943
  80. 80. Ronfani L, Vecchi Brumatti L, Mariuz M, Tognin V, Bin M, Ferluga V, et al. The Complex Interaction between Home Environment, Socioeconomic Status, Maternal IQ and Early Child Neurocognitive Development: A Multivariate Analysis of Data Collected in a Newborn Cohort Study. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(5):e0127052. pmid:25996934
  81. 81. Silva LM, Jansen PW, Steegers EA, Jaddoe VW, Arends LR, Tiemeier H, et al. Mother's educational level and fetal growth: the genesis of health inequalities. Int J Epidemiol. 2010;39(5):1250–1261. pmid:20478844
  82. 82. Sánchez S, Castillo DC. Eating behavior disorders in Chilean infants, according to socioeconomic level. Arch Latinoam Nutr. 2008;58(4):357–362. pmid:19368296
  83. 83. Holme A, MacArthur C, Lancashire R. The effects of breastfeeding on cognitive and neurological development of children at 9 years. Child Care Health Dev. 2010;36(4):583–590. pmid:20184592
  84. 84. Isaacs EB, Fischl BR, Quinn BT, Chong WK, Gadian DG, Lucas A. Impact of breast milk on intelligence quotient, brain size, and white matter development. Pediatr Res. 2010;67(4):357–362. pmid:20035247
  85. 85. Jednoróg K, Altarelli I, Monzalvo K, Fluss J, Dubois J, Billard C, et al. The influence of socioeconomic status on children's brain structure. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(8):e42486. pmid:22880000
  86. 86. Ibaceta CV. Nutritional status and its association with scholastic achievement in the Quality Education Measurement System (SIMCE) Tests (Master's thesis). Santiago, Chile: University of Chile, Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology Dr. Fernando Monckeberg Barros (INTA); 2013.
  87. 87. Flores O, Orellana Y, Leyton B, Valenzuela R, Barrera C, Almagià A, Martínez V, Ivanovic D. Overnutrition and Scholastic Achievement: Is There a Relationship? An eight-year follow-up study. Obesity Facts. 2018;11(5):344–359. pmid:30308520
  88. 88. Lyngdoh T, Viswanathan B, Kobrosly R, van Wijngaarden E, Huber B, Davidson PW, et al. Blood pressure and cognitive function: a prospective analysis among adolescents in Seychelles. J Hypertens. 2013;31(6):1175–1182. pmid:23572201
  89. 89. Calvin CM, Deary IJ, Webbink D, Smith P, Fernandes C, Lee SH, et al. Multivariate genetic analyses of cognition and academic achievement from two population samples of 174,000 and 166,000 school children. Behav Genet. 2012;42(5):699–710. pmid:22700061
  90. 90. Pokropek A, Sikora J. Heritability, family, school and academic achievement in adolescence. Soc Sci Res. 2015;53:73–88. pmid:26188439
  91. 91. Batouli SA, Trollor JN, Wen W, Sachdev PS. The heritability of volumes of brain structures and its relationship to age: a review of twin and family studies. Ageing Res Rev. 2014;13:1–9. pmid:24211464