Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Effect of barriers and distance on song, genetic, and morphological divergence in the highland endemic Timberline Wren (Thryorchilus browni, Troglodytidae)

  • Andrés Camacho-Alpízar,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Escuela de Biología, Universidad de Costa Rica, San José, Costa Rica

  • Eric J. Fuchs ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    e.j.fuchs@gmail.com

    Affiliation Escuela de Biología, Universidad de Costa Rica, San José, Costa Rica

  • Gilbert Barrantes

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Escuela de Biología, Universidad de Costa Rica, San José, Costa Rica

Abstract

Populations may become isolated by distance, geographic barriers or both. Isolated populations often diverge in behavioral, morphological and genetic traits as a result of reduced inter-population gene flow. Highland species commonly present naturally fragmented distributions that confine populations to the highest mountain peaks, isolated by mountain passes and distance. The endemic Timberline Wren (Thryorchilus browni) inhabits the highlands of the Talamanca mountain range, including western Panama, and the highest peak in the Central Volcanic mountain range of Costa Rica. Using microsatellites and song recordings we studied the effect of a geographic barrier and distance on song, genetic and morphological divergence among four populations in Costa Rica. A lowland mountain pass resulted in the largest genetic, vocal, and morphological (bill length) differences among populations, likely due to reduce the gene flow. Cultural drift and assortative mating by females selecting songs from their own population likely accentuates the effect of isolation and limited gene flow between populations. This pattern of population divergence has been found in other Neotropical highland birds, but over larger geographical scales. We conclude that mountain passes and distance both reduce gene flow between populations in recently-isolated highland species with restricted distributions.

Introduction

For most species, absent or limited gene flow will result in some degree of inter-population divergence [1]. Multiple factors, including morphology and behavior, may restrict gene flow [2,3]; however, spatial separation and barriers are often considered the two main factors limiting gene flow among populations [1,4]. In the absence of physical or ecological barriers, dispersal limitation restricts gene flow in animals, increasing genetic structure and phenotypic differences among populations [5,6]. Gene flow distances will depend on the social structure and mating system of each species [7,8]. Species with limited dispersal will most likely remain within the limits of the population, and in these cases, geographic distance is expected to correlate with population divergence [9]. Alternatively, barriers are expected to increase genetic and phenotypic divergence, independently of the distance among populations.

In birds, the spatial distribution of song dialects could also affect dispersal and consequently gene flow patterns. Oscine birds -and a few other groups- learn to sing early in life and have dialects which are restricted to small geographic areas [10]. Oscine females prefer males with matching songs learned within the same population [1114]. Thus, the spatial distribution of dialects is expected to limit gene flow, and differences in both song dialect and allele frequencies are often correlated with geographic distance. Mating systems are also thought to affect population genetic structure in different animals [15,16]. In particular, avian mating systems can be strongly correlated with their social structure and dialect evolution [17]. For instance, in Zonotrichia leucophrys oriantha, the spatial distribution of dialects limits gene flow across populations but does not act as an impermeable barrier [18]. In other cases, differences in song dialects can restrict gene flow similar to geographic barriers [1926].

The highland avifauna of Costa Rica and western Panama includes a large number of endemic species with naturally fragmented distributions [27,28]. Climate shifts during inter-glacial periods at the end of the Pleistocene confined areas with suitable habitat for some avian species to the highest mountain elevations, thus fragmenting their once continuous distribution and forming “sky islands” [28]. These species inhabit almost exclusively the highest mountains, resulting in a series of isolated populations sometimes separated by deep mountain passes that limit the movement of individuals between populations. Some highland populations are not separated by geographic barriers, but rather are continuously distributed over relatively larger areas on top of mountain ranges. In these cases, dispersal between populations or demes is limited only by distance and the dispersal capabilities of each species. Bird populations inhabiting the neotropical highlands are excellent systems to study the effects of topographic barriers and dispersal limitation on vocal, genetic, and morphological divergence of bird populations on relatively small geographic scales [27,29,30].

The Timberline Wren (Thryorchilus browni; Troglodytidae) is a highland endemic with a fragmented distribution, confined to the highest mountain peaks above 2800m a.s.l. of the Talamanca Mountain Range in Costa Rica and western Panama, and the highest volcano of the Central Volcanic Mountain Range in Costa Rica. A deep mountain pass separates these two mountain ranges. T. browni is a territorial species that forages in dense undergrowth within montane forests and the páramo [31]. Its song is likely learned as in other members of the Troglodytidae family and consists of many highly variable elements. The geographic distribution of the Timberline Wren allows us to test for the effects of topographic barriers and distance on song dialect divergence, population structure, and morphological divergence. Topographic barriers and distance should both limit gene flow; however, we expected higher divergence in all traits between populations separated by topographic barriers. Differences in allele frequencies should increase with spatial separation as expected in isolation-by-distance (IBD) for populations within the Talamanca mountain range.

Materials and methods

Study sites

We selected four sites above 2800 m a.s.l. in two Costa Rican mountain ranges. Three sites were located within the Talamanca Mountain Range: Cerro Buena Vista (CBV: 9° 33’N, 83° 44’W), Cerro Vueltas (CV: 09° 37’N, 83° 51’W) and Cerro Chirripó (CCH: 09° 28’N, 83° 29’W); and the fourth site was in the Irazú Volcano (IV: 09° 59’N, 83° 52’W), located in the Central Volcanic mountain range (Fig 1). Both mountain ranges are separated by a mountain pass at 1300 m a.s.l. Vegetation on all sites consists mainly of páramo and high elevation montane forests with canopies dominated by oak species (Quercus costaricensis and Q. bumelioides) and the undergrowth by bamboo thickets (Chusquea spp.), Ericaceae and Hypericum shrubs.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Distribution of the timberline wren in Costa Rica (dark orange area).

Sites include the Irazú Volcano in the Central Volcanic mountain range and the Talamanca Mountain Range above 2800 m a.s.l. The blue line indicates the position of the single barrier identified by Monmonier's algorithm for genetic and song divergence among populations. Black triangles indicate the location of the four study sites. IV = Irazú Volcano, CV = Cerro Vueltas, CBV = Cerro Buena Vista, CCH = Cerro Chirripó.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209508.g001

Data collection

We recorded songs from 36 adult T. browni adult males with undetermined age between January and June 2012 and 2013 with eight to ten individuals per site; adultness was determined by checking skull ossification, only adults have complete skull ossification. We recorded a single focal male per day from 0500 to 0600h and did not use playbacks before or during recordings. This method controls for possible daily variations in songs [32,33]. We used a Marantz PMD-660 digital recorder and a Sennheiser ME-66 shotgun microphone, with a WAVE recording format, a sampling rate of 44.1kHz, and an accuracy of 16 bits, for all recordings.

After each recording session, we captured each individual with a 6 m long mist nets. We color banded each individual and extracted a 5–10 uL blood sample from the brachial vein and preserved it in 95% alcohol at -20°C until DNA extraction. For each male, we measured culmen length from the front of the skull to the bill tip, bill width at front of nares, tarsus from the tibiotarsus joint to the distal end of the tarsometatarsus, tail length, flattened wing length and body mass. We captured and color banded all recorded individuals. For genetic analyses, we obtained blood samples from additional individuals for which no recordings were obtained. We did not use anesthesia and birds were released unharmed after data collection. All collections were done according to the guidelines of national law of animal care (Law # 7451) and the collecting permit RESOLUCIÓN-001-2012 issued by Sistema Nacional de Áreas de Protección to G. Barrantes and A. Camacho.

Song analysis

We analyzed song recordings with Raven Pro 1.4 (Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA). Spectrograms were produced using a Hann function, 700 samples and a FFT size of 1024 samples and an overlap of 75%. The song of the Timberline Wren consists of a sequence of elements that is repeated one or more times during a variable period of time (Fig 2). We defined a song element as the smallest unit that can be individualized from the spectrogram separated from other units by at least 50ms. In very few cases, some elements overlapped in time but not in frequency and were treated as different song elements. The elements followed the same order within a sequence in 99% of cases, but an individual can start its song at different points of the element sequence.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Spectrograms showing a portion of timberline wren songs from three different locations.

Note how a sequence of song elements is followed by an identical sequence. CBV = Cerro Buena Vista, CV = Cerro Vueltas, CCH = Cerro Chirripó, IV = Irazú Volcano.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209508.g002

We created a catalogue of song elements to determine the similarity in element composition shared among individuals and sites. One of the authors (ACA) visually identified each element based on their modulation pattern (frequency-temporal change) without prior knowledge of which individual was recorded [34]. For instance, two elements were considered the same if they coincided in their modulation or shape even though their frequency range differed because the frequency at which a certain element was sung could depend on external factors other than learning, e. g. body size, excitation or shifts due to habitat structure [35].

We calculated an index of shared elements between individuals using the Sorensen index, 2Ns/(R1+R2), where Ns is the number of shared elements, and R1 and R2 are the sizes of the respective repertoires for each pair of individuals. We subtracted this proportion of shared elements from 1 to obtain a dissimilarity index between individuals [34]. We calculated the dissimilarity between populations based on element shared using the same index, grouping each individual into their respective site.

Song data analysis

We tested for difference in shared song elements across sites using a Permutational Multivariate Analysis of Variance [36] as implemented in the Vegan package in R 3.1.1 [37,38]. We analyzed the effect of site on the previously described song dissimilarity index and F-test significance was based on 10 000 permutations of the raw data. To test for morphological differences among sites, we conducted a MANOVA using site as the independent factor.

We also used the Partitioning Around Medoids (PAM), k-medoids unsupervised machine-learning algorithm to estimate the optimal number of K clusters that group individuals based on song dissimilarity. This algorithm is less sensitive to outliers than the traditional k-means clustering [39]. We calculated the PAM algorithm for different cluster numbers (from K = 1 to K = 6) and chose the configuration that maximized the average silhouette as suggested by Kaufman and Rousseeuw [39] and implemented in the factoextra and cluster R packages.

Song acoustic analysis

We analyzed 187 songs of 39 T. browni adult males to test the effects of a barrier and distance on six acoustical traits: sequence duration (s), peak frequency (Hz), high frequency (Hz), low frequency (Hz), frequency range (Hz), and the number of elements per sequence. These traits were measured for each element sequence in a song to obtain the mean value of each variable for each song, using Raven Pro 1.4, with spectrograms produced with a Hann function, 700 samples and a FFT size of 1024 samples and an overlap of 75%.

We conducted a principal component analysis to visualize the distribution of populations in a multidimensional space defined by song features. We also used a discriminant function analysis to assess whether or not song traits are assigned to their own populations using the MASS package for R statistical language (Version 3. 2. 2) [40].

DNA extraction and SSR analysis

DNA was extracted from blood samples using a DNeasy Blood & Tissue Kit (Qiagen, Valencia, CA, USA) and a FastDNA Spin Kit (MPBiochemicals, Santa Ana, CA, USA). We determined multilocus genotypes for all samples using six microsatellites isolated for other Troglodytidae: Troglodytes aedon (TA-A5-2, TA-A5-15, TA-C6-7, TA-B4-2, TA-C3(B)-2 [41]) and Thryophilus pleurostictus (ThPl-14 [42]). We used the standard PCR protocols of the Top Taq Master Kit (Quiagen). Annealing temperatures for Th-Pl-14 were raised to 61°C to improve amplification quality. Multilocus genotypes were scored using GeneMarker 2.4.2 software (Soft Genetics, State College, PA, USA).

We used GenAlEx 6.0 [43] to estimate observed and expected heterozygosities (Ho, He), number of alleles (A) and number of effective alleles (Ae), as estimators of intrapopulation genetic diversity. We also used Micro-Checker Version 2.2.3 [42] to test for the presence of null alleles and quantified genetic structure with Fst statistics. Significance of Fst values was determined by 10 000 bootstraps of individuals among sites as implemented in FSTAT v2.9.3 [44].

We tested if individuals could be grouped into different clusters based on their allele frequencies using the Bayesian clustering algorithm implemented in Structure 2.3.4 [45]. The most likely number of clusters (K) was determined by changing K between 1 and 6, and using 20 independent runs for each K value. All runs used the admixture model with correlated allele frequencies and consisted of 20 000 burn-in steps followed by 200 000 MCMC steps. To determine the most likely number of K clusters, we used Structure Harvester [46,47].

Geographic barriers and distance effects on song and genetic divergence

To test for isolation-by-distance, we estimated the correlation between Fst values and the shortest linear geographic distance between populations with a Mantel test; Fst estimates were transformed to Fs/(1-Fst) as suggested by Rousset [48]. We performed a similar analysis to correlate pairwise song dissimilarities between populations and geographic distance, and to correlate linearized Fst values and repertoire dissimilarity between sites. We also conducted a partial Mantel correlation, to test for the correlation between genetic structure and song dissimilarities, while controlling for the distance among sites. Significance of Mantel correlations was determined from 10 000 permutations using the Vegan package for R statistical language (Version 3. 2. 2).

To identify possible barriers to gene flow, we used the Monmonier algorithm [49] implemented in the adegenet package [50]. Individuals were initially connected using Delaunay triangulation; since all the individuals share the same population coordinates, small differences were introduced with the jitter R function. The song dissimilarity index matrix was used to identify barriers between populations based on vocal divergence. Barriers to gene flow were identified using Euclidean distances between individual genotypes. Random noise was eliminated using Principal Coordinate Analysis with the dudi.pco function from the ade4 package. The Monmonier algorithm estimates boundaries based on maximum pairwise genetic distances or song dissimilarities.

Results

We recorded a total of 36 T. browni males at all four sites. Timberline Wren male songs showed high inter-individual variation, and not a single individual shared the same elements or element order with another individual within or between populations. We identified 301 different elements from which 64 were present in at least two populations and 19 were present in all four populations. The population with the largest number of elements was IV with 151, while CBV with 98 had the smallest number. The mean number of different elements per individual (± sd) was 30.3 ± 7.63, and ranged between 11 and 62 elements (Table 1), and the number of private elements per individual increased with repertoire size (r2 = 0.42, p<0.01).

thumbnail
Table 1. Song elements recorded in four mountainous sites in Costa Rica.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209508.t001

Song and morphological divergence

Thryorchilus browni males shared more song elements with individuals from their own populations than with other populations. Elements from IV differed the most when compared with other populations. Pairwise comparisons showed that IV and CCH shared fewer song elements (dissimilarity index score = 0.78) than any other pair of populations (F3,32 = 2.49, p < 0.001), and CBV and CV shared the largest number of elements (dissimilarity index score = 0.48). The optimal number of groups found by PAM analysis was K = 2. Most individuals from CBV, CV and CCH were grouped together, while all VI and 12% of CCH individuals were assigned to a second group (Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Group assignment for each timberline wren based on song dissimilarity.

Males were assigned to one of the two groups based on the partitioning around medioids (PAM) function. CBV = Cerro Buena Vista, CV = Cerro Vueltas, CCH = Cerro Chirripó, IV = Irazú Volcano.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209508.g003

Song acoustic traits of T. browni also differed across populations. The first component of the PCA (PC1) explained 39.2% of the variation in song traits, while the second component (PC2) accounted for 26.6%, with 65.8% of the total variation explained by the first two components. The highest loading variables for PC1 were sequence duration (0.47), frequency range (0.53) and number of elements per sequence (0.51), all loading positively (Table 2). For PC2 the highest loading values were peak frequency (-0.50), low frequency (-0.50) and high frequency (-0.57), all loading negatively (Table 2). T. browni populations did not show a clear clustering pattern along PC1, but they did show some separation along PC2, with IV songs mostly in the positive values of PC2, and CCH songs on the negative values (Fig 4). The separation of IV and CCH is a consequence of IV songs having the lowest values for peak frequency, low frequency and high frequency, while the songs of CCH have the highest values for the same acoustic variables (Table 3). The discriminant function analysis, based on the song features of each male, classified correctly (in its own population)59.5% of the males from CBV, 36.4% of CV males, 68.6% of CCH males, and 81,4% of IV males, showing that IV is the most distinct population in terms of the acoustic traits of T. browni songs.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Principal component analysis plot using the first two components of six T. browni song acoustic traits.

CBV = Cerro Buena Vista, CV = Cerro Vueltas, CCH = Cerro Chirripó, IV = Irazú Volcano.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209508.g004

thumbnail
Table 2. Loading scores of the first two principal components from the T. browni songs’ acoustic traits.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209508.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Summary of mean (± sd) T. browni song traits for each study site.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209508.t003

Bill length was the only morphological variable that differed among sites (F3,53 = 2.21, p < 0.001). Individuals from VI had longer bills than those from the other populations (Fig 5); however, the optimal number of groups based on all morphological data was K = 1.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Bill length of timberline wrens from four populations in Costa Rica: CBV = Cerro Buena Vista, CV = Cerro Vueltas, CCH = Cerro Chirripó, IV = Irazú Volcano.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209508.g005

Genetic structure

We did not find evidence for null alleles across loci and only one locus in one population deviated from HW expectations (TA-B4-2 in CBV). CBV and CCH had the highest mean number of alleles per locus, while IV had the lowest (Table 4). The populations with the largest number of private alleles were CCH and IV with four in each population. IV had the lowest genetic diversity estimated across populations.

thumbnail
Table 4. Mean (± sd) genetic diversity estimates for six microsatellite loci from the timberline wren in four populations in Costa Rica.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209508.t004

We found significant genetic structure between populations of the Timberline Wren (Fst = 0.134, p < 0.001). Pairwise comparisons of Fst estimates ranged between 0.01 (CBV-CV) and 0.178 (CCH-VI; Table 5). STRUCTURE HARVESTER [47] determined that the most likely configuration clustered individuals into two clusters (K = 2), the first composed primarily by individuals from CBV, CV and CCH (Talamanca Mountain Range) while the second clustered mostly IV individuals (Central Volcanic Mountain Range; Fig 6).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Individual assignment of timberline wren genotypes to one of two clusters defined by STRUCTURE and the evanno method.

Bars represent individuals and colors depict percentage admixture from two different clusters. Populations: CBV = Cerro Buena Vista, CV = Cerro Vueltas, CCH = Cerro Chirripó, IV = Irazú Volcano.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209508.g006

Barrier and distance effect on song and genetic divergence

We found significant evidence for isolation by distance on genetic divergence. Fst values increased significantly with increasing geographic distance (Mantel test: r = 0.818, p = 0.04; Table 5, Fig 7A), and with song dissimilarity (r = 0.902, p = 0.04; Fig 7C). Distance between populations was also correlated with song divergence (r = 0.874, p = 0.044, Fig 7B). The partial Mantel test between genetic and song dissimilarity while controlling for distance did not yield a significant correlation (r = 0.669, p = 0.168). Monmonier’s algorithm consistently found a barrier between IV and the remaining three populations for genetic differences and for song dissimilarities at the individual and population levels (Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 7. Correlation between linear geographic distance and timberline wren’s genetic and song dissimilarities.

Each point represents a direct comparison between two populations. Yellow dots represent pairwise comparisons between populations separated only by distance, and blue dots represent pairwise comparisons between populations separated by a geographic barrier. Correlation between Fst and linear geographic distance in km (A), song dissimilarity and linear geographic distance in km (B), and between song dissimilarity and Fst (C). Fst estimates were transformed to Fst / (1 –Fst).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209508.g007

Discussion

Populations of T. browni in the highlands of Costa Rica differed in male songs, genetic and morphological traits, and these differences were positively correlated with geographic distance between populations. However, the barrier created by a low elevation mountain pass between mountain ranges resulted in the most pronounced differences. Thus, in this system, mountain passes seem to act as effective gene flow barriers for Timberline Wrens. These populations likely became separated after the Pleistocene [28,5153] when highland vegetation withdrew to the highest sections of the mountains as climate changed gradually from temperate to tropical conditions.

Differences in song composition and genetic structure both increased with geographic distance between populations suggesting dispersal limitation for this species. Limited dispersal in T. browni could also be correlated with the forest stratum these birds inhabit. Understory-dwellers have been shown to be dispersal-limited and differ genetically among populations [5,6]. Additionally, males exhibit strong territory fidelity year-round (AC pers. observation), and by both mates during the breeding period, that may further limit dispersal in this species. In contrast, populations of the Sooty-capped bush tanager, Chlorospingus pileatus, a non-territorial species that forages at middle and high forest strata in highland forests and paramo, were not genetically structured and low elevation passes did not act as barriers to gene flow [30]. This suggests that species-specific life history traits could play an important role in determining gene flow patterns of highland bird species.

Results from STRUCTURE and Monmonier’s algorithms both indicated that low elevation mountain pass act as barriers limiting gene flow between populations of the Central Volcanic (IV) and Talamanca (CBV, CV, CCH) mountain ranges. Populations within Talamanca had lower genetic divergence than those found between IV and the nearest population in Talamanca. Isolation and the relatively smaller population size of IV have likely contributed to the observed differences between this populations and those from Talamanca via genetic drift or local adaptation to particular environments [2,5,12]. The genetic and song structure detected between populations of T. browni are congruent with other Neotropical highland birds for which geographic barriers have been proposed as a significant structuring factor [1926]. Limited gene flow across populations may also respond to cultural drift or female preferences for local songs. In several bird species, females prefer to mate with males that sing the local dialect, which has been proposed as a selective factor driving population divergence [1114].

Song diversity evolves faster than genetic differences [54,55, 56, 57], particularly in smaller populations [58]. This is evident in IV, the population with the largest number of unique song elements, as expected by significant isolation over long periods of time. Similarly, the acoustic traits (fundamental frequency, low frequency, high frequency) of IV male songs differ the most from those of other populations, and though differences in acoustic features are often attributed to habitat differences [2,35], these differences also correlate with geographic barriers (4). Spatial song divergence and assortative mating in T. browni could act as a prezygotic isolating mechanism, further increasing genetic and morphological divergence between populations [11]. The distribution of T. browni in the Central Volcanic mountain range is restricted to a reduced area of available habitat at the top of IV and limited dispersal and smaller population size could increase the effects of drift in this population, further reducing its genetic diversity. Climate change is expected to contract available habitat in mountainous regions, negatively impacting population sizes of highland residents. We found the lowest genetic diversity estimates at IV, and additional reductions in genetic diversity through genetic drift may increase the likelihood of local extinctions in this endemic highland species [59].

The morphology of T. browni varied little across these four populations. Bill length is the only trait that differed geographically, specifically between IV (longer bills) and populations in the Talamanca mountain range, a difference that was first described in 1910 [60]. Bill morphology often varies in response to dietary differences [61], which for T. browni consist mostly of small insects like flies and beetles [31]. Bill length is a morphological feature that varies geographically in many endemic highland birds [27,29,31]. The factors influencing variance in bill morphology of highland species remains elusive, though it is possible that differences in available food items across their geographical distribution drives changes in bill morphology; however, data is not currently available to test this hypothesis. Differences in bill morphology in T. browni may possibly contribute to significant differences in song acoustic traits between IV and other populations [35], but our data are insufficient to reach any conclusion. Further tests are needed to determine the role of morphological variation on song divergence in this species.

The climatic and geological events that took place during and after the Pleistocene glacial cycle are presumably responsible for the isolation and divergence across populations of birds in the highlands of Costa Rica and western Panama [2730]. Low elevation mountain passes could act as barriers and restrict gene flow resulting in genetic, vocal, and morphological differences in highland endemic birds as we have shown here for T. browni in Costa Rica. In this species, distance was positively correlated with genetic and song structure, suggesting this species may have limited dispersal and highlights the effect of species-specific life history traits as important factors shaping the distribution of genetic diversity. Isolation by behavioral divergence through cultural drift may further increase the effects of genetic drift on smaller populations and impact their long-term survival. Distance and geographic barriers have been proposed as major factors driving the evolution and speciation of different taxa at large geographical scales [4,2026]. The present study shows that these factors are also responsible for intraspecific divergence in genetic, song, and morphological traits across populations, even for a species with restricted distributions within the highlands of southern Central America.

Supporting information

S1 Dataset. Genotypes, morphological measurements and acoustic data for Thryorchilus browni individuals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209508.s001

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank J. Chaves and L. Sandoval for insightful comments along the development of this research, as well as all the Universidad de Costa Rica undergraduates that helped with fieldwork. We would also like to thank deeply R. Cramer and G. Chavarría for kindly donating microsatellites and extraction kits used in this study.

References

  1. 1. Avise JC, Walker DE. Pleistocene phylogeographic effects on avian populations and the speciation process. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1998;265: 457–463. pmid:9569664
  2. 2. Podos J, Warren PS. The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong. Advances in the Study of Behavior. 2007;37: 403–458.
  3. 3. Bentley RA, Hahn MW, Shennan SJ. Random drift and culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2004;271: 1443–1450. pmid:15306315
  4. 4. Koetz AH, Westcott DA, Congdon BC. Geographical variation in song frequency and structure: the effects of vicariant isolation, habitat type and body size. Animal Behaviour. 2007;74: 1573–1583.
  5. 5. Burney CW, Brumfield RT. Ecology Predicts Levels of Genetic Differentiation in Neotropical Birds. The American Naturalist. 2009;174: 358–368. pmid:19627230
  6. 6. Smith BT, McCormack JE, Cuervo AM, Hickerson MJ, Aleixo A, Cadena CD, et al. The drivers of tropical speciation. Nature. 2014;515: 406–409. pmid:25209666
  7. 7. Wright S. Isolation by distance. Genetics. 1943;28: 114–138. pmid:17247074
  8. 8. Greenwood PJ. Mating systems, philopatry and dispersal in birds and mammals. Animal Behaviour. 1980;28: 1140–1162.
  9. 9. Wright S. Isolation by distance under diverse systems of mating. Genetics. 1946;31: 39–59. pmid:21009706
  10. 10. Baker MC, Cunningham M a. The Biology of Bird-Song Dialects. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1985;8: 85.
  11. 11. Danner JE, Danner RM, Bonier F, Martin PR, Small TW, Moore IT. Female, but Not Male, Tropical Sparrows Respond More Strongly to the Local Song Dialect: Implications for Population Divergence. The American Naturalist. 2011;178: 53–63. pmid:21670577
  12. 12. Slabbekoorn H, Smith TB. Bird song, ecology and speciation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2002;357: 493–503. pmid:12028787
  13. 13. O’Loghlen AL, Rothstein SI. Culturally correct song dialects are correlated with male age and female song preferences in wild populations of brown-headed cowbirds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1995;36: 251–259.
  14. 14. Searcy WA, Nowicki S, Hughes M. The response of male and female song sparrows to geographic variation in song. Condor. 1997;99: 651.
  15. 15. Wright S. The Interpretation of Population Structure by F-Statistics with Special Regard to Systems of Mating. Evolution. 1965;19: 395–420.
  16. 16. Ross KG. Molecular ecology of social behaviour: Analyses of breeding systems and genetic structure. Molecular Ecology. 2001;10: 265–284. pmid:11298944
  17. 17. Baker MC, Mewaldt RL. Song Dialects as Barriers to Dispersal in White-Crowned Sparrows, Zonotrichia leucophrys nuttalli. Evolution. 2012;32: 712–722.
  18. 18. MacDougall-Shackleton EA, MacDougall-Shackleton SA. Cultural and genetic evolution in mountain white-crowned sparrows: Song dialects are associated with population structure. Evolution. 2001;55: 2568–2575. pmid:11831670
  19. 19. Gutiérrez-Pinto N, Cuervo AM, Miranda J, Pérez-Emán JL, Brumfield RT, Cadena CD. Non-monophyly and deep genetic differentiation across low-elevation barriers in a Neotropical montane bird (Basileuterus tristriatus; Aves: Parulidae). Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution. 2012;64: 156–165. pmid:22484358
  20. 20. Maldonado-Sánchez D, Gutiérrez-Rodríguez C, Ornelas JF. Genetic divergence in the common bush-tanager Chlorospingus ophthalmicus (Aves: Emberizidae) throughout Mexican cloud forests: The role of geography, ecology and Pleistocene climatic fluctuations. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution. 2016;99: 76–88. pmid:26988412
  21. 21. Ortiz-Ramírez MF, Andersen MJ, Zaldívar-Riverón A, Ornelas JF, Navarro-Sig??enza AG. Geographic isolation drives divergence of uncorrelated genetic and song variation in the Ruddy-capped Nightingale-Thrush (Catharus frantzii; Aves: Turdidae). Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution. 2016;94: 74–86. pmid:26302950
  22. 22. Pérez-Emán JL. Molecular phylogenetics and biogeography of the Neotropical redstarts. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2005;37: 511–528. pmid:15935704
  23. 23. Sosa-López JR, Mennill DJ, Navarro-Sigüenza AG. Geographic variation and the evolution of song in Mesoamerican rufous-naped wrens Campylorhynchus rufinucha. Journal of Avian Biology. 2013;44: 27–38.
  24. 24. Sosa-López JR, González C, Navarro-Sigüenza AG. Vocal Geographic Variation In Mesoamerican Common Bush Tanagers (Chlorospingus ophthalmicus). The Wilson Journal of Ornithology. 2013;125: 24–33.
  25. 25. Valderrama E, Pérez-Emán JL, Brumfield RT, Cuervo AM, Cadena CD. The influence of the complex topography and dynamic history of the montane Neotropics on the evolutionary differentiation of a cloud forest bird (Premnoplex brunnescens, Furnariidae). Journal of Biogeography. 2014;41: 1533–1546.
  26. 26. Weir JT. Implications of genetic differentiation in neotropical montane forest birds. Annals of the Missouri Botanical Garden. 2009;96: 410–433.
  27. 27. Barrantes G, Loiselle B. Reproduction, habitat use, and natural history of the Black-and-Yellow Silky-flycatcher (Phainoptila melanoxantha), an endemic bird of the western Panama-Costa Rican highlands. Ornitología Neotropical. 2002;13: 121–136.
  28. 28. Barrantes G. The role of historical and local factors in determining species composition of the highland avifauna of Costa Rica and Western Panamá. Revista de Biologia Tropical. 2009;57: 333–349.
  29. 29. Stiles FG. Systematics of the Southern Forms of Selasphorus Trochilidae. Auk. 1983;100: 311–325.
  30. 30. Chavarría-Pizarro T, Gutiérrez-Espeleta G, Fuchs EJ, Barrantes G. Genetic and morphological variation of the sooty-capped bush tanager (Chlorospingus pileatus), a highland endemic species from Costa Rica and western Panama. The Wilson Journal of Ornithology. 2010;122: 279–287.
  31. 31. Stiles FG, Skutch A. A guide to the birds of Costa Rica. New York: Cornell University Press; 1998.
  32. 32. Sandoval L, Mennill DJ. A quantitative description of vocalizations and vocal behaviour of the Rusty-crowned Ground-Sparrow (Melozone kieneri). Ornitología Neotropical. 2014;25: 219–230.
  33. 33. Sandoval L, Méndez C, Mennill DJ. Vocal behaviour of White-eared Ground-sparrows (Melozone leucotis) during the breeding season: repertoires, diel variation, behavioural contexts, and individual distinctiveness. J Ornithol. 2016;157: 1–12.
  34. 34. Vargas-Castro LE, Sánchez NV, Barrantes G. Repertoire Size and Syllable Sharing in the Song of the Clay-Colored Thrush (Turdus grayi). The Wilson Journal of Ornithology. 2012;124: 446–453.
  35. 35. Podos J. Correlated evolution of morphology and vocal signal signature in Darwin´s finches. Nature. 2001;409: 185–188. pmid:11196640
  36. 36. Anderson MJ. A new method for non parametric multivariate analysis of variance. Austral ecology. 2001;26: 32–46.
  37. 37. Core Team R. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014. 2016.
  38. 38. Oksanen AJ, Blanchet FG, Kindt R, Legen P, Minchin PR, Hara RBO, et al. Package ‘ vegan ‘ version 2.5–2. 2018.
  39. 39. Kaufman L, Rousseeuw PJ. Finding Groups in Data: An Introduction to Cluster Analysis. John Wiley & Sons; 2009.
  40. 40. Venables W. N., Ripley B. D. 2002. Modern Applied Statistics with S. Fourth Edition. Springer, New York. ISBN 0-387-95457-0
  41. 41. Cabe PR, Marshall KE. Microsatellite loci from the house wren (Troglodytes aedon). Molecular Ecology Notes. 2001;1: 155–156.
  42. 42. Brar RK, Schoenle LA, Stenzler LM, Hall ML, Vehrencamp SL, Lovette IJ. Eleven microsatellite loci isolated from the banded wren (Thryothorus pleurostictus). Molecular Ecology Notes. 2007;7: 69–71. pmid:18392115
  43. 43. Peakall R, Smouse PE. Genalex 6: genetic analysis in Excel. Population genetic software for teaching and research. Mol Ecol Notes. 2006;6: 288–295.
  44. 44. Goudet J. FSTAT (version 1.2): a computer program to calculate F-statistics. Journal of heredity. 1995;86: 485–486.
  45. 45. Falush D, Stephens M, Pritchard JK. Inference of population structure using multilocus genotype data: Linked loci and correlated allele frequencies. Genetics. 2003;164: 1567–1587. pmid:12930761
  46. 46. Earl DA, VonHoldt BM. STRUCTURE HARVESTER: A website and program for visualizing STRUCTURE output and implementing the Evanno method. Conservation Genetics Resources. 2012;4: 359–361.
  47. 47. Evanno G, Regnaut S, Goudet J. Detecting the number of clusters of individuals using the software STRUCTURE: a simulation study. Molecular Ecology. 2005;14: 2611–2620. pmid:15969739
  48. 48. Rousset F. Genetic Differentiation and Estimation of Gene Flow from F-Statistics Under Isolation by Distance. Genetics. 1997;145: 1219–1228. pmid:9093870
  49. 49. Monmonier MS. Maximum-Difference Barriers: An Alternative Numerical Regionalization Method. Geographical Analysis. 1973;5: 245–261.
  50. 50. Jombart T. adegenet: a R package for the multivariate analysis of genetic markers. Bioinformatics. 2008;24: 1403–1405. pmid:18397895
  51. 51. Hooghiemstra H, Cleef AM, Noldus CW, Kappelle M. Upper Quaternary vegetation dynamics and palaeoclimatology of the La Chonta bog area (Cordillera de Talamanca, Costa Rica). Journal of Quaternary Science. 1992;7: 205–225.
  52. 52. Islebe GA, Hooghiemstra H, van der Borg K. A cooling event during the Younger Dryas Chron in Costa Rica. Palaeogeography, Palaeoclimatology, Palaeoecology. 1995;117: 73–80.
  53. 53. Islebe GA, Hooghiemstra H. Vegetation and climate history of montane Costa Rica since the last glacial. Quaternary Science Reviews. 1997;16: 589–604.
  54. 54. West-Eberhard MJ. Sexual Selection, Social Competition, and Speciation. The Quarterly Review of Biology. 1983;58: 155–183.
  55. 55. Lynch A. The population memetics of birdsong. In: Kroodsman D, Miller E, editors. Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Ithaca, New York: Columbia University Press; 1996. pp. 181–197.
  56. 56. Gleason J. M., Ritchie M. G. 1998. Evolution of courtship song and reproductive isolation in the Drosophila willistoni species complex: Do sexual signals diverge the most quickly? Evolution 52:1493–1500. pmid:28565374
  57. 57. Oliveira C. C., Manfrin M. H., Fábio de M. Sene, and Etges W. J. 2013. Evolution of male courtship songs in the Drosophila buzzatii species cluster. pp. 137–164, (in) Michalak P. (ed.) Speciation: Natural Processes, Genetics and Biodiversity. Nova Science Publishers, New York.
  58. 58. Grant BR, Grant PR. Cultural Inheritance of Song and Its Role in the Evolution of Darwin’s Finches. Evolution. 1996;50: 2471–2487. pmid:28565664
  59. 59. Frankham R. Genetics and extinction. Biological Conservation. 2005;126: 131–140.
  60. 60. Carriker MA Jr. An annotated list of the birds of Costa Rica, including Cocos Island. Annals of the Carnegie Museum. 1910;6: 314–915.
  61. 61. Schluter D, Smith JNM. Natural Selection on Beak and Body Size in the Song Sparrow. Evolution. 40: 221–231. pmid:28556039