Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The relationship between depression and cognitive function in adults with cardiovascular risk: Evidence from a randomised attention-controlled trial

  • Haley M. LaMonica ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Haley M. LaMonica, Daniel J. Biddle

    Roles Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    haley.lamonica@sydney.edu.au

    ‡ These authors share first authorship on this work.

    Affiliations Brain and Mind Centre, University of Sydney, Camperdown, Australia, Charles Perkins Centre, School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Camperdown, Australia, Central Clinical School, Sydney Medical School, University of Sydney, Camperdown, Australia

  • Daniel J. Biddle ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Haley M. LaMonica, Daniel J. Biddle

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors share first authorship on this work.

    Affiliation Brain and Mind Centre, University of Sydney, Camperdown, Australia

  • Sharon L. Naismith,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Brain and Mind Centre, University of Sydney, Camperdown, Australia, Charles Perkins Centre, School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Camperdown, Australia

  • Ian B. Hickie,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Brain and Mind Centre, University of Sydney, Camperdown, Australia, Central Clinical School, Sydney Medical School, University of Sydney, Camperdown, Australia

  • Paul Maruff,

    Roles Software, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Cogstate, Melbourne, Australia

  • Nicholas Glozier

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Brain and Mind Centre, University of Sydney, Camperdown, Australia, Central Clinical School, Sydney Medical School, University of Sydney, Camperdown, Australia

Abstract

Background and aim

This study assessed the association between depressive symptom severity and cognition in middle-to-older aged adults with mild-to-moderate depression and cardiovascular risk factors using an online test battery (CogState) and whether changes in depressive symptoms over 3 months were associated with changes in cognition.

Methods

Participants (mean age = 57.8) with cardiovascular risk and mild–to-moderate depressive symptoms completed measures of psychomotor speed, learning, and executive function prior to (n = 445)_and after (n = 334) online depression or attention control interventions. The symptom severity-cognition relationship was examined both cross-sectionally and prospectively.

Results

Participants exhibited significantly reduced psychomotor speed and variable impairments on measures of learning and executive functioning relative to normative data. However, there was no association of depression severity with cognition at baseline or of change in depressive symptoms with change in cognitive performance.

Limitations

Participants were well-educated, which may have protected against cognitive decline. Attrition may limit generalisability, though is unlikely to explain the lack of association between depression symptoms and cognition.

Conclusions

Adults with comorbid mild-to-moderate depressive symptoms and cardiovascular risks performed less well than age-matched normative data on three online cognitive tests; however, we were unable to show any symptom-cognition association cross-sectionally or longitudinally, despite significant improvements in depressive symptoms. This challenges the generalisability of such associations found in more severely unwell clinical samples to those with a broader depressive symptom profile, or suggests that underlying cardiovascular disease may account for the association seen in some clinical studies. This has implications for scaling up selective prevention of cognitive decline.

Introduction

It is recognized widely that Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) is associated with cognitive dysfunction [13] including impaired learning, working memory, processing speed, and executive functions [46]. The neuropsychological profile is, however, heterogeneous and varies with depressive symptom severity [7, 8], disease subtype [9], age of onset [10], etiology, co-morbidities and cerebrovascular disease (CVD) [8]. Regardless of the contributing factors, cognitive dysfunction in depression is associated with substantial disability and poorer quality of life [11], and frequently has been suggested as a selective target for interventions aimed at preventing cognitive decline and subsequent dementia [12].

There are three key issues that might limit such a strategy:

  1. Is the depression severity-cognition association present only in the small group with diagnosed MDD or is it applicable to the much larger group with mild to moderate depression symptoms?
  2. Does improvement in depression result in a significant improvement in cognitive function? The observed cognitive dysfunction of MDD often persists despite reductions in depressive symptoms [13, 14], and any improvements may not reach the levels of cognition that existed prior to onset of the depressive episode; the “scar hypothesis” [13, 15, 16]. Up to 75% of older adults whose symptoms of MDD have reduced following therapy with antidepressant medication can still be classified as having subtle cognitive dysfunction [17].
  3. Is the observed association between depression and cognition a reflection of an underlying common cause such as Cerebrovascular Disease (CVD)? ‘Vascular depression’ describes a syndrome whereby depression occurs for the first time in later life, associated with underlying CVD, marked cognitive impairment and poor prognosis. In this instance, CVD is thought to cause structural changes to CNS white matter [18] which themselves give rise to slowed processing speed, poor memory and executive dysfunction as well as an increased likelihood of progression to dementia [8]. Thus, any examination of depression and cognition in middle-to-older aged adults needs to take into account the presence of CVD and associated vascular risk factors.

Given these questions, we undertook a secondary analysis of a large trial of an online treatment that was shown to be effective for depressive symptoms in community-based adults with mild-to-moderate depression symptoms and self-reported history of CVD or cardiovascular risk factors who underwent cognitive testing at baseline and post intervention. We hypothesised that the depressive symptom severity-cognition association would be found in this group, and that improvements in depressive symptoms would be associated with improvement on cognitive testing despite the presence of cardiovascular risk for cognitive dysfunction.

Materials and methods

Study design

The protocol of the Cardiovascular Risk and E-Couch Depression Outcome (CREDO) trial has been published elsewhere [19] and the primary results of the CREDO study have been published [20]. CREDO was a double-blind, parallel group randomised controlled trial designed to compare the effectiveness of internet cognitive behavioural therapy (eCBT) relative to an online attention control in adults, aged 45- to 75-years, with self-reported CVD or significant CVD risk factors, and repeated evidence of mild–to-moderate depressive symptoms.

The trial complied with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association. Ethical approval was obtained from the University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee in June of 2009 and from the Australian National University Human Research Ethics Committee in 2010. Additionally, the trial was registered with the Australian and New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (ACTRN12610000085077). The 45 and Up Study had primary ethical approval from the University of New South Wales Human Research Ethics Committee.

Participants

Participants (n = 562) were recruited through the 45 and Up Study, a large-scale longitudinal population-based cohort study comprising over 260,000 men and women aged 45-years and over in New South Wales (NSW), Australia [21]. Comparative analyses between the 45 and Up Study and the NSW Population Health Survey revealed that the estimated relative risk of a range of health-related risk factors generalised to the wider population [22]. From July 2010 and January 2011, participants were randomly selected from the 45 and Up Study database using an algorithm to identify those that met the eligibility criteria described in detail in Glozier et al. (2013). In summary, participants were required to have provided a valid email address and to have a self-reported history of CVD or CVD risk factors and persistent or recurrent symptoms of depression. Following an online informed consent process, participants completed online baseline assessments and were randomised to either intervention arm using a customised, fully automated randomisation facility built into the trial website. Randomisation was stratified by depressive symptom severity. Participants were blinded to which programme was the ‘active’ intervention.

Interventions

The “active’ intervention was E-couch, an automated software program that offers 12 modules addressing mental health literacy, cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), interpersonal therapy (IPT), relaxation techniques and exercise programs targeting depression. This has been demonstrated to be effective compared to an attention control in reducing depressive symptoms in younger groups without comorbidities [23].

The attention control was HealthWatch, a 12-week online program in which participants were provided with information on a variety of topics including nutrition, physical activity, heart health, and pain. The attention control intervention was matched for contact (i.e., 12 modules) and was used to balance the effects of the expectation of therapeutic benefit offered in the active treatment arm. In previous trials, participants showed a small, although significant reduction in depressive symptoms using this program, potentially reflecting natural remission or some small effect [24].

Measures

Depressive symptoms, the primary pre-specified outcome measure for the trial, were measured using the PHQ-9 at baseline, 4-, 8-, and 12-weeks. The PHQ-9 is a nine item measure of DSM depression symptoms which has been found to be a reliable and valid screening measure, is widely used in previous community studies of people with depression, and is sensitive to change in clinical status [25, 26].

Three aspects of cognition were assessed at baseline and 12 weeks using an online version of Cogstate, a computerised neuropsychological test battery with established validity and sensitivity for the detection of subtle cognitive change in community cohorts in general and also in MDD [27]. For this trial, an internet version was utilised, which was downloaded onto participants’ computers to prevent bandwidth and connectivity interference. The test battery was chosen for brevity as well as capacity to assess neuropsychological skills commonly affected in older people with depression [3, 4]. The Detection Task required participants to respond when a card presented on the screen turned over the course of two minutes. Speed was the primary outcome, with lower scores reflecting better performance. Visual learning and memory was assessed with the One Card Learning Task. During this five minute, one-back style task, participants responded ‘yes’ or ‘no’ if a card presented on-screen was the same as a previous card. The score reflects accuracy of performance. Executive functioning was assessed with the Groton Maze Task. In this five minute, spatial problem-solving task, participants were shown a grid of tiles on-screen and had to use the mouse to find a hidden pathway based on trial and error feedback. The score reflects the number of errors, again with lower scores reflecting better performance.

As part of their baseline assessment through the 45 and Up Study, participants completed a questionnaire about demographic and social characteristics, personal health behaviours, and general health-related data [21]. These data, the coding of which is standard and detailed in [20] were included in the present study for the purposes of characterising the sample and to assess attrition bias and potential confounding.

Analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to summarise the baseline demographic and health characteristics of the study sample, and their association with the measures of cognition. One-sample t-tests were then used to compare performance on the cognitive tests between the CREDO sample and age-matched normative data based on a healthy population of subjects enrolled in clinical trials as well as research and academic studies [28].

Hypothesis one: Depressive symptom severity-cognition association.

Cognitive function values were then converted to normalised scores based on the above-mentioned age-matched normative data [28], and Spearman correlations were used to estimate the relationship between depression severity and cognition at baseline. Pearson product-moment correlations were used to assess the relationship between normally-distributed cognitive and binary sociodemographic and clinical characteristics. Linear regression using the enter method was also used to explore this relationship adjusting for potential confounders. At step 1 depression was entered alone, at step 2 age, gender, education (post-school versus no post-school), psychotropic medication, and intervention arm were entered, and finally diagnosed and treated cardiovascular disease was entered at step 3.

Before conducting linear regression models, major assumptions of the approach were assessed. Homoscedasticity and normality of residuals was confirmed through inspection of scatterplots and graphs, respectively. Lack of association between independent variables and residuals was confirmed through examining correlations for continuous independent variables and t-tests for binary independent variables. Potential multicollinearity was assessed via examining variance inflation factors (VIFs). As residuals for the cross-sectional analysis of executive function were non-normal, executive function was further transformed into its natural logarithm for this analysis.

Outliers, defined as cases scoring +/-3 standard deviations from the mean on the outcome or independent variable of interest for univariate analyses (t-tests and correlations), and standardized residuals <-3 or >3 for multivariate analyses (regressions), were excluded.

Hypothesis two: Change in cognitive function.

For those with baseline and follow-up cognitive function data, paired t-tests were used to assess change in cognitive variables and depression over the course of the study. Change scores were then calculated for each normalised cognitive function variable, along with depressive symptoms, by subtracting follow-up scores from baseline scores. Linear regression was used to estimate associations between changes in depressive symptoms and changes in cognitive function and then adjusted, in a stepwise fashion as above, for age, gender, education, depression symptoms, medication and intervention arm, and then the presence of diagnosed CVD and risk factors. Outliers (cases with standardized residuals <-3 or >3, were excluded from these analyses. In sensitivity analyses the regression models were re-run with MDD, current depression symptom severity (centered), and their interaction, entered at step 1, to gauge whether the depression-cognition relationship was apparent for those with probable MDD at baseline. Probable MDD was defined as scores of 2 or more on either questions 1 or 2 of the PHQ-9, and a total of 5 or more items with scores of 2 or 3 [29]. In a further sensitivity analysis designed to take into account status at baseline, latent change scores were created for depression and cognitive function tasks by regressing status at baseline on status post-intervention. The residuals, which equate to the estimated change in each variable not explained by status at baseline, were entered into regression models in the same manner described above. Results are reported such that positive coefficients equate to better cognitive performance or improvement (i.e. faster reaction times, greater learning accuracy, fewer executive function task errors).

Results

As referenced above, the primary results of the CREDO trial have been published previously [20]. For the purposes of this study, the data from both intervention arms was pooled.

Baseline demographics and health characteristics

Of the 562 recruited participants, 445 (79.2%) completed at least one baseline cognitive task. Baseline characteristics for these participants are presented in Table 1. Participants had a mean age of 58-years. The majority of participants were female (63.6%), spoke English at home (94.8%), and had more than a high school level of education (73.2%). Over half the sample (55.5%) had a prior diagnosis of depression. At baseline, the mean PHQ-9 score was 11.93 (SD: 3.4) and 24% (n = 107) of the participants met criteria for probable MDD. There were no statistically significant differences in any variable between arms indicating adequate randomisation [20]. There were few significant differences between those who completed at least one baseline cognitive task and those who did not. Those with baseline cognitive data were more likely to be female (63.6% vs 53.0%), partnered (74.5% vs 64.0%), taking psychotropic medication (35.96% vs 25.86%) and younger (M = 57.54 vs 59.48), than those who did not complete any baseline cognitive measures.

thumbnail
Table 1. Baseline characteristics of 445 CREDO participants, and their association with baseline cognition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203343.t001

Although cognitive function was worse in the older participants (slower psychomotor speed (ρ = -.194, p <.001), worse learning (ρ = -.104, p <.05), and worse executive function (ρ = -.123, p <.01)) there were few other associations of demographic, illness or social variables with the cognitive function measures. Of the 54 associations analysed in addition to age, only three were associated, and then with only one of the measures each, a number quite likely due to chance.

Cognitive function in mild-to-moderate depression compared to normative data

Compared to aged-matched groups from the CogState normative data (Table 2), the study participants showed a significant impairment in psychomotor speed in each age group. Similar results were seen in relation to learning accuracy, with significant deficits noted for participants aged 50 to 59 and 60 to 69, but not for those in their 70s. Those in their 60s made fewer errors on the executive functioning tasks than age-matched peers, however when outliers (n = 2) we included this difference was no longer significant. There were no significant differences in performance level on the executive functioning task for those subjects in their 50s, and 70s relative to age-matched peers. A one-way ANOVA suggested significant differences in depressive symptoms as a function of age (F(3,439) = 5.396, p = .001), and post-hoc contrasts indicated participants 70–79 years of age had less depressive symptoms at baseline than those 50–59 years of age (t = 2.15, p = 0.032).

thumbnail
Table 2. Participant cognitive test performance compared to Cogstate normative reference data, and participant baseline depressive symptoms by age group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203343.t002

Course of depressive symptoms and cognition over the study

Over the 12 weeks of the study, participants in both arms showed improvements in depressive symptoms of 3.66 points (95% CI: 3.05–4.27) with eCBT and 2.60 points (95% CI: 2.05–3.16) in the control group, with a significantly greater decline in the PHQ-9 for eCBT compared to control (1.06; 95% CI: 0.23–1.89; time by arm interaction p = .012).

The complete results relating to the effects of the web-based intervention on depression symptom severity were published previously [20]. Of participants with baseline cognitive function data (n = 445), 75% (n = 334) completed at least one of the cognitive measures at week 12 (see Table 3). Those who were missing week 12 cognitive data had lower baseline scores for the learning task (M = -.59, SD = .76) than completers (M = -.37, SD = .79, t(443) = -2.623, p = .009), and worse scores for executive function (M = -.05, SD = .95) than completers (M = .16, SD = .89, p = .035), but did not differ in baseline psychomotor speed or PHQ-9 scores. Those who provided only baseline data did not differ on most characteristics, but were less likely to speak English at home (89.2% vs 96.7%, X2 = 9.606, p = .002) and be obese (56.9% vs 44.4%, X2 = 5.078, p = .024), neither of which showed any association with baseline cognition. Of the cognitive measures only executive function showed any change over the 12 weeks of the study.

thumbnail
Table 3. Cognitive test scores and depressive symptoms over the study and paired t-tests assessing significance of change for those with baseline and follow-up data for at least one measure of cognitive function.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203343.t003

Cross-sectional association of depression severity, and cognition

Baseline depression severity was not associated with performance on any of the cognitive tasks, with correlations ranging from -.038 to .027, and p-values from .425 to .768 (see Table 1). In the linear regression models there were no associations between depression severity and cognitive function either with or without adjustment for potential confounders (see Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 4. The cross-sectional association of cognitive function and depressive symptoms at baseline, and association of change in cognitive function and change in depressive symptoms.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203343.t004

Association of change in cognitive function and change in depressive symptoms

Change in depression symptoms was not associated with change in any of the cognitive function variables (Table 4). There was no effect on this lack of association when adjusting for potential confounders. However, in the sensitivity analysis including an interaction between symptom severity and probable MDD, a significant interaction was observed for change in psychomotor speed at the first step of the model (t = 2.746, p = .006). The model became non-significant when covariates were added, and no covariates were significant predictors of change in psychomotor speed, so the first model (with change in depression, probable MDD, and their interaction) was retained. In a post-hoc split-file regression analysis it was found that only for those with probable MDD was change in depressive symptom severity associated positively with change in psychomotor speed. For these participants, change in depressive symptoms explained 12.6% of the variability in change in psychomotor speed, with each standard deviation increase in depression change associated with a .355 standard deviation increase in psychomotor speed change (t = 3.285, p = .002).

In the latent change sensitivity analysis, the results mirrored those of the initial analysis i.e. no prospective associations between depression and cognitive function were found.

Discussion

This study suggests that people with mild-to-moderate depressive symptoms and self-reported CVD or cardiovascular risk factors generally perform below an aged matched normative database on measures of psychomotor speed and learning accuracy. Participants aged 60 to 69-years and 70-to-79 years also exhibited executive dysfunction, but there was no evidence of executive dysfunction for those participants in their 50s. Contrary to our hypotheses there was no association at baseline between depression symptom severity and cognitive performance and no suggestion of potential (reverse) confounding by variables including age, gender, education, psychotropic medications, treatment arm, and diagnosed CVD. This null finding suggests that the symptom severity cognition association commonly observed in clinical samples [7] may not be evident in community settings where individuals may have a more heterogeneous and milder pattern of symptoms. Given that cognitive dysfunction is associated with greater disability in depression [2, 11] and that functional impairment can drive help seeking, such a discrepancy between settings might be expected. This does, however, have implications for the generalisation of findings from clinical settings.

As previously reported [20], over the course of the study, participants in both arms showed improvements in depressive symptoms, significantly greater in the active arm, although with a small effect size. However, only executive function improved over the course of the study and there was no association of depressive symptom reduction with cognitive improvement over the 12 weeks overall. Amongst the small group with probable MDD at baseline, the change in depressive symptom severity was associated with change in psychomotor speed, but not in learning or executive functions. These results may indicate a real lack of association between changes in depressive symptom and cognition in this group with predominantly mild-to-moderate depression, especially given there was no association of symptoms and cognition at baseline. The lack of association may also reflect the small size of the change in depression and that of the cognitive measures only executive function showed statistical improvement. However, although the mean changes were small, their marked variance could have reflected intra-individual correlations. It may be that in this study a mean change of only 3 or 4 on the PHQ-9 may be inadequate to lead to observable changes in neuropsychological test performance, or that there may be a time lag between changes in affective symptoms and cognition as has been observed with function [31].

In contrast to previous studies, we did not find that elevated symptoms of depression were associated with greater cognitive dysfunction nor did we find that a reduction in depression symptom severity was associated with cognitive improvement [7]. Furthermore, based on available data, we cannot conclude that the relationship between depression symptom severity and cognition was masked by a common cause, such as diagnosed CVD; however, it is possible that unmeasured confounders such as age of onset (early vs. late onset depression), duration of illness, episode frequency [3, 32], comorbid anxiety, underlying white matter change or other medical comorbidities may have mediated any relationship between depressive symptom severity and cognition, thus potentially concealing an association in this instance. The suggestion that there was however a symptom-cognition association in those with probable MDD is intriguing. Could it be that the frequently observed association is only present in those with a “disorder” and that they are somehow qualitatively different, as there was not much greater variance in the measures in this subgroup which was of a small sample size?

A further reason for the lack of association between depression symptoms and cognition in this study may be that any association is specific to certain cognitive domains. The meta-analysis [7] did suggest that the association was stronger in executive function than in processing speed, something observed previously in a cross-sectional study of older adults where increasing severity of depression was related to lower semantic fluency scores and poorer ability to shift cognitive set, but not to worsening performances on measures of learning and memory, phonemic fluency, or complex problem-solving [3]. Additionally, contradictory results have been found with regard to the relationship between depression severity and speed of information processing [10]. Therefore, it is plausible that the online cognitive tests may not have been sensitive to the cognitive deficits typical of individuals with depression, or those that might change with symptoms improvement. It is important to recognise that a neuropsychological evaluation in a clinical setting is a more comprehensive assessment of cognition and may be more sensitive to cognitive decrement than the brief, unsupervised online assessment used in this study.

There are some study limitations. Our measure of education was binary, and normative data was age-matched only, which may have masked some education-related variability that could affect cognitive performance. Also, this was an educated, computer-literate sample, which may have impacted on the relationships between depressive symptom severity and CVD risk with cognitive performance [33]. As such, they likely had greater cognitive reserve (usually assessed by educational attainment or occupational complexity) and thus have had some ‘buffer’ against cognitive decline due to depressive symptoms or CVD, although despite this they performed below an aged matched normative sample. Previous studies have shown that higher rates of cognitive reserve protect against deficits in verbal fluency [34] and verbal memory [35] in individuals with elevated symptoms of depression. The use of tools such as Cogstate in community-based research or primary care settings will be subject to similar selection bias compared to intensive testing in secondary or tertiary care samples. The same is also true of the attrition, whereby more of those we are most interested in following, i.e. with poorer cognition, were lost to follow-up, although aging in only one of the three domains. For many outcomes loss of the most severe might bias results to the null but with cognition, which tends to show a consistent decline over time, this may not be true. Further, this attrition may limit generalisability but does not explain the lack of association between depressive symptoms and cognitive function in this large sample. Finally, the measures of depressive symptoms [29] and cognition [27, 36, 37] are well-validated although our classification of probable MDD was based upon self-report [29] rather clinical interview, potentially leading to a misclassification bias, although more likely random misclassification. Nevertheless, it may be worthwhile to examine these relationships further in a more clinically robust sample of diagnosed patients with CVD and depression.

Conclusions

This large study of cognitive function suggests that even when technologically literate and well-educated, adults with mild-to-moderate depression and CVD risks in the community have poorer cognitive function than well peers, particularly in relation to psychomotor speed and learning. However, we were unable to find any of the hypothesised cross sectional or prospective associations of depressive symptoms and cognition in this community-based sample with milder depressive symptoms. This suggests that any such associations may be present only in those with actual MDD as seen in such secondary care samples. As such, alleviating depressive symptoms may lead to improvement in cognitive function only among those with probable MDD (as opposed to more mild symptoms); however, large-scale efforts to prevent progression from more mild symptoms to diagnosed MDD still seem worthwhile as a means to reduce the risk of future cognitive decline [38, 39].

Supporting information

S2 Data. Marginal distribution of outcomes and tests of assumptions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203343.s002

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

This trial was funded by the Cardiovascular Disease and Depression Strategic Research Program (Award Reference No. G08S 4048) funded by the National Heart Foundation of Australia and beyondblue: the national depression and anxiety initiative. Neither of the funders had any role in the study design, analysis or interpretation or the submission decision. The researchers are independent of the funders.

The 45 and Up Study is managed by the Sax Institute in collaboration with major partner Cancer Council New South Wales; and partners the Heart Foundation (NSW Division); NSW Ministry of Health; beyondblue: the national depression and anxiety initiative; Ageing, Disability and Home Care, NSW Family and Community Services; and the Australian Red Cross Blood Service.

The authors wish to thank the men and women participating in the 45 and Up Study and the trial participants.

References

  1. 1. Hermens DF, Hodge MAR, Naismith SL, Kaur M, Scott E, Hickie IB. Neuropsychological clustering highlights cognitive differences in young people presenting with depressive symptoms. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2011;17(02):267–76.
  2. 2. McIntyre RS, Cha DS, Soczynska JK, Woldeyohannes HO, Gallaugher LA, Kudlow P, et al. Cognitive deficits and functional outcomes in major depressive disorder: determinants, substrates, and treatment interventions. Depression and Anxiety. 2013;30(6):515–27. pmid:23468126
  3. 3. Naismith SL, Hickie IB, Turner K, Little CL, Winter V, Ward PB, et al. Neuropsychological performance in patients with depression is associated with clinical, etiological and genetic risk factors. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology. 2003;25(6):866–77. pmid:13680463
  4. 4. Hsu KJ, Young-Wolff KC, Kendler KS, Halberstadt LJ, Prescott CA. Neuropsychological deficits in major depression reflect genetic/familial risk more than clinical history: a monozygotic discordant twin-pair study. Psychiatry research. 2014;215(1):87–94. pmid:24262663
  5. 5. Jaeger J, Berns S, Uzelac S, Davis-Conway S. Neurocognitive deficits and disability in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry research. 2006;145(1):39–48. pmid:17045658
  6. 6. Naismith SL, Hickie IB, Ward PB, Scott E, Little C. Impaired implicit sequence learning in depression: a probe for frontostriatal dysfunction? Psychological Medicine. 2006;36(03):313–23.
  7. 7. McDermott LM, Ebmeier KP. A meta-analysis of depression severity and cognitive function. Journal of affective disorders. 2009;119(1):1–8.
  8. 8. Naismith SL, Norrie LM, Mowszowski L, Hickie IB. The neurobiology of depression in later-life: clinical, neuropsychological, neuroimaging and pathophysiological features. Progress in neurobiology. 2012;98(1):99–143. pmid:22609700
  9. 9. Lin K, Xu G, Lu W, Ouyang H, Dang Y, Lorenzo-Seva U, et al. Neuropsychological performance in melancholic, atypical and undifferentiated major depression during depressed and remitted states: a prospective longitudinal study. Journal of affective disorders. 2014;168:184–91. pmid:25061955
  10. 10. Herrmann LL, Goodwin GM, Ebmeier KP. The cognitive neuropsychology of depression in the elderly. Psychological medicine. 2007;37(12):1693–702. pmid:17610767
  11. 11. Naismith SL, Longley WA, Scott EM, Hickie IB. Disability in major depression related to self-rated and objectively-measured cognitive deficits: a preliminary study. BMC psychiatry. 2007;7(1):1.
  12. 12. Norton S, Matthews FE, Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Brayne C. Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based data. The Lancet Neurology. 2014;13(8):788–94. pmid:25030513
  13. 13. Herrera-Guzmán I, Herrera-Abarca JE, Gudayol-Ferré E, Herrera-Guzmán D, Gómez-Carbajal L, Peña-Olvira M, et al. Effects of selective serotonin reuptake and dual serotonergic–noradrenergic reuptake treatments on attention and executive functions in patients with major depressive disorder. Psychiatry research. 2010;177(3):323–9. pmid:20385412
  14. 14. Shilyansky C, Williams LM, Gyurak A, Harris A, Usherwood T, Etkin A. Effect of antidepressant treatment on cognitive impairments associated with depression: a randomised longitudinal study. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(5):425–35. pmid:26995298
  15. 15. Butters MA, Becker JT, Nebes RD, Zmuda MD, Mulsant BH, Pollock BG, et al. Changes in cognitive functioning following treatment of late-life depression. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;157(12):1949–54. pmid:11097959
  16. 16. Doraiswamy PM, Krishnan KRR, Oxman T, Jenkyn LR, Coffey DJ, Burt T, et al. Does antidepressant therapy improve cognition in elderly depressed patients? The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2003;58(12):M1137–M44.
  17. 17. Jayaweera HK, Hickie IB, Duffy SL, Hermens DF, Mowszowski L, Diamond K, et al. Mild Cognitive Impairment Subtypes in Older People With Depressive Symptoms Relationship With Clinical Variables and Hippocampal Change. Journal of geriatric psychiatry and neurology. 2015:0891988715573535.
  18. 18. HICKIE I, SCOTT E. Late-onset depressive disorders: a preventable variant of cerebrovascular disease? Psychological Medicine. 1998;28(05):1007–13.
  19. 19. Cockayne NL, Glozier N, Naismith SL, Christensen H, Neal B, Hickie IB. Internet-based treatment for older adults with depression and co-morbid cardiovascular disease: protocol for a randomised, double-blind, placebo controlled trial. BMC psychiatry. 2011;11(1):10.
  20. 20. Glozier N, Christensen H, Naismith S, Cockayne N, Donkin L, Neal B, et al. Internet-delivered cognitive behavioural therapy for adults with mild to moderate depression and high cardiovascular disease risks: a randomised attention-controlled trial. PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e59139. pmid:23555624
  21. 21. Collaborators US. Cohort profile: the 45 and up study. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2008;37(5):941. pmid:17881411
  22. 22. Christensen H, Griffiths KM, Korten A. Web-based cognitive behavior therapy: analysis of site usage and changes in depression and anxiety scores. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 2002;4(1):e3. pmid:11956035
  23. 23. Griffiths KM, Mackinnon AJ, Crisp DA, Christensen H, Bennett K, Farrer L. The effectiveness of an online support group for members of the community with depression: a randomised controlled trial. PLoS One. 2012;7(12):e53244. pmid:23285271
  24. 24. Mackinnon A, Griffiths KM, Christensen H. Comparative randomised trial of online cognitive–behavioural therapy and an information website for depression: 12-month outcomes. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;192(2):130–4. pmid:18245031
  25. 25. Löwe B, Spitzer RL, Gräfe K, Kroenke K, Quenter A, Zipfel S, et al. Comparative validity of three screening questionnaires for DSM-IV depressive disorders and physicians’ diagnoses. Journal of affective disorders. 2004;78(2):131–40. pmid:14706723
  26. 26. Löwe B, Unützer J, Callahan CM, Perkins AJ, Kroenke K. Monitoring depression treatment outcomes with the patient health questionnaire-9. Medical care. 2004;42(12):1194–201. pmid:15550799
  27. 27. Falleti MG, Maruff P, Collie A, Darby DG. Practice effects associated with the repeated assessment of cognitive function using the CogState battery at 10-minute, one week and one month test-retest intervals. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology. 2006;28(7):1095–112. pmid:16840238
  28. 28. Cogstate. Cogstate Normative Data. Melbourne, Australia: Cogstate; 2013.
  29. 29. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The Phq‐9. Journal of general internal medicine. 2001;16(9):606–13.
  30. 30. Dawson DA, Grant BF, Stinson FS. The AUDIT-C: screening for alcohol use disorders and risk drinking in the presence of other psychiatric disorders. Comprehensive psychiatry. 2005;46(6):405–16. pmid:16275207
  31. 31. Velligan DI, Alphs L, Lancaster S, Morlock R, Mintz J. Association between changes on the Negative Symptom Assessment scale (NSA-16) and measures of functional outcome in schizophrenia. Psychiatry research. 2009;169(2):97–100. pmid:19664826
  32. 32. Woo YS, Rosenblat JD, Kakar R, Bahk W-M, McIntyre RS. Cognitive deficits as a mediator of poor occupational function in remitted major depressive disorder patients. Clinical Psychopharmacology and Neuroscience. 2016;14(1):1. pmid:26792035
  33. 33. Lee RS, Hermens DF, Porter MA, Redoblado-Hodge MA. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode major depressive disorder. Journal of affective disorders. 2012;140(2):113–24. pmid:22088608
  34. 34. Opdebeeck C, Nelis SM, Quinn C, Clare L. How does cognitive reserve impact on the relationships between mood, rumination, and cognitive function in later life? Aging & mental health. 2015;19(8):705–12.
  35. 35. McLaren ME, Szymkowicz SM, Kirton JW, Dotson VM. Impact of Education on Memory Deficits in Subclinical Depression. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2015;30(5):387–93. pmid:26109434
  36. 36. Lim YY, Ellis KA, Harrington K, Ames D, Martins RN, Masters CL, et al. Use of the CogState Brief Battery in the assessment of Alzheimer’s disease related cognitive impairment in the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2012;34(4):345–58. pmid:22248010
  37. 37. Lim YY, Jaeger J, Harrington K, Ashwood T, Ellis KA, Stöffler A, et al. Three-month stability of the CogState brief battery in healthy older adults, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease: results from the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers, and Lifestyle-rate of change substudy (AIBL-ROCS). Archives of clinical neuropsychology. 2013;28(4):320–30. pmid:23552802
  38. 38. Royall DR, Palmer RF. Alzheimer’s disease pathology does not mediate the association between depressive symptoms and subsequent cognitive decline. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2013;9(3):318–25.
  39. 39. Wilson RS, Capuano AW, Boyle PA, Hoganson GM, Hizel LP, Shah RC, et al. Clinical-pathologic study of depressive symptoms and cognitive decline in old age. Neurology. 2014;83(8):702–9. pmid:25080520