Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Effect of smoking status on neuronal responses to graphic cigarette warning labels

  • Tobias Rüther ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    tobias.ruether@med.uni-muenchen.de

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany

  • Yannick Schultz,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany

  • Christina Wirth,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany

  • Agnieszka Chrobok,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany

  • Andrea Rabenstein,

    Roles Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany

  • Daniel Keeser,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany, Institute of Clinical Radiology, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany

  • Birgit Ertl-Wagner,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Clinical Radiology, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany

  • Oliver Pogarell ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Oliver Pogarell, Susanne Karch

    Roles Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany

  • Susanne Karch

    Contributed equally to this work with: Oliver Pogarell, Susanne Karch

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry und Psychotherapy, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany

Abstract

Background

Smoking is responsible for a large proportion of cancer, respiratory and cardiovascular deaths. Nevertheless the health risks of smoking are still underestimated in many smokers. The present study aimed to examine neurobiological responses to graphical warnings on cigarette packings in non-smokers and patients with tobacco dependence.

Methods

Twenty non-smokers and twenty-four patients with tobacco dependence participated in a functional MRI study during that pictures of different categories were presented ((a) EU-warning pictures, (b) text-only warnings, (c) neutral pictures with short information). Patients contributed twice in the experiment (after 10 hours nicotine withdrawal / about 5 minutes after nicotine consumption).

Results

Smokers during withdrawal demonstrated increased neuronal responses predominantly in subcortical, temporal and frontal brain regions that are associated with emotional and cognitive processes during the presentation of graphical warnings compared to neutral pictures. In smokers after smoking and non-smokers, the differences between graphical warnings and neutral pictures were increased compared to smokers during withdrawal. The comparison of the graphical warnings with text-only labels demonstrated the importance of affective brain regions especially in smokers after smoking and in non-smokers. During withdrawal, the neural responses associated with graphical warnings and text-only labels differed only marginally.

Discussion and conclusion

The results suggest that emotional and cognitive reactions to graphical warnings are predominantly seen in smokers after smoking and in non-smokers. The impact of these pictures during withdrawal seems to be less pronounced; in this case, more unspecific processes seem to be important, including the projection of sensory signals to the cerebral cortex.

Introduction

Smoking is responsible for a large proportion of cancer, respiratory and cardiovascular deaths and is believed to be the single most important cause of death in Europe [1,2,3]. In 2015, the World Health Organization estimated a number of 6 million deaths attributable to smoking, and a number of 600,000 deaths of nonsmokers exposed to second-hand tobacco smoke (WHO 2015, http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs339/en/). In all likelihood most people are aware of the general harmfulness of smoking but many smokers still underestimate the risks of smoking and the associated range of illnesses for themselves and others [4](WHO 2011).

One strategy for prevention is warning labels on cigarette packaging whose implementation is progressing rapidly. While text-only messages are prevalent in most countries, many countries are now increasing warning signs, and a growing number has introduced graphic warning labels (GWL) [5]. Whereas in 2012 55 countries/jurisdictions had implemented picture warning requirements, their number has risen to 77 by September 2014. This means that picture warnings now reach 49% of the world’s population [5]. Since May 2016, all 28 EU countries require GWLs to cover 65% of packages front and back [6].

In literature, the prevailing view is that GWLs are more effective than text-only warnings in enhancing risk perception and health knowledge [7,8]. One important advantage is that pictorial warning labels have demonstrated to elicit enhanced emotional responses compared to text-only warnings [9]. Emotional responses to GWLs were negative including e.g. fear, disgust [10,11].

In particular, large and prominent GWLs have been proved to be effective [7,12]. It has been shown that large GWLs are a significant source of health information for both smokers and non-smokers [13]. Exposure to GWLs discourage smoking initiation [14,15], increasing health knowledge, perception and awareness of risks associated with smoking [14,15,16,17,18,19]. They reduce the appeal of cigarette packets [14], restrict intentions to stop smoking [17], encourage cessation [14,15,20,21,22], prevent relapse [23] and increase the use of quit lines [24]. It is assumed that GWLs provoke high emotions which support the implementation of behavioural change like quitting smoking [25].

While the beneficial effects of GWLs on behaviour correlates are well documented, research on the neurobiological basis for the efficacy of GWLs is still rare. In the last few years, neuroimaging studies focused on neurobiological correlates of smoking cue-related information. The neurobiology of smoking cue reactivity has been summarised by a meta-analysis indicating that the most important areas in smoking cue reactivity are the precuneus, the posterior and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the dorsal and medial prefrontal cortex (DLPFC, MPFC), the superior and inferior parietal lobules, the insula and the dorsal striatum [26]. GWLs appear to be the counterpart of smoking cues but little is known about their neurobiological effect so far. Newman-Norlund and colleagues (2014) indicated that pictorial warnings activate large-scale neural networks including amygdala, insula, visual association cortex, hippocampus, fusiform gyrus, precentral gyrus, supplementary motor area, pars triangularis, pars opercularis, pars orbitalis and fusiform gyrus [27]. Another study demonstrated an association between frontoinsular neural activity and craving reduction in response to GWLs [28]. The amygdala was most robustly activated by warnings that included personal suffering from smoking-related consequences followed by warnings, including graphic representation of physical consequences of smoking and symbolic representations of risk [27].

Green and colleagues (2016) revealed increased neuronal responses especially in the medial prefrontal cortex, the amygdala, the medial temporal lobe, and the occipital cortex during the presentation of GWLs in young adult smokers. There were no significant differences in response to warnings on branded versus plain cigarette packages. The self-reported motivation to quit smoking was significantly higher after viewing GWLs compared to control pictures [29]. Wang and colleagues (2015) demonstrated that GWLs causing high emotional reactions were associated with increased responses e.g. in the amygdala, the hippocampus, the inferior frontal gyri and the insula. The neurobiological variations were accompanied by a greater reduction in the craving to smoke [25]. Overall brain regions activated during the presentation of GWLs are assumed to be involved in cognitive and affective decision-making and memory formation [25,27,29] as well as visual information processing. Particularly the amygdala is associated with emotion processing and emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli [30,31]. The amygdala response is associated with quitting smoking [32].

The aim of the present study was to investigate the neuronal responses to the new German GWLs in non-smokers and smokers, especially taking into account the smoking status of smokers (smokers after deprivation versus smoker after smoking). Withdrawal can lead to withdrawal symptoms including craving and anxiety within several hours. Diagnostic criteria of the nicotine withdrawal syndrome are irritability, decreased frustration tolerance, anger, aggression, attention deficit, restlessness, dysphoric mood [33]. Avoidance of the negative state produced by nicotine withdrawal represents a motivational component that promotes continued tobacco use and relapse after smoking cessation. With the modest success rate of currently available smoking cessation therapies, understanding mechanisms involved in the nicotine withdrawal syndrome are crucial for developing successful treatments [34]. Overall, the available literature indicates that the nicotine withdrawal syndrome is complex, and involves a range of neurobiological mechanisms [34].

While undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), the new GWLs, old text-only warnings and neutral pictures were presented to the participants.

We hypothesised that especially regions associated with emotion processing [e.g. the amygdala [35,36]], craving suppression [including the DLPFC [37] and the insula [38]] would show significant responses during the presentation of the new German GWLs. These responses are supposed to be increased during the presentation of the new GWLs compared to the text-only warnings and neutral pictures. In addition, we hypothesised that neural responses associated with new GWLs would be increased in smokers compared to healthy subjects because of the stronger personal relevance of the information for smokers.

Materials and methods

Sample

24 smokers (male: n = 12; female: n = 12) and 20 non-smokers (male: n = 10; female: n = 10) aged between 23 and 53 years were recruited via online advertisements and were screened for eligibility in a telephone interview. One inclusion criterion was the age between 18 and 65; only smokers with a Fagerström score of > 3 were included. Participants who were not able to maintain the smoking deprivation of 10 hours, who reported a current/past neurological or psychiatric disorder and who met standard fMRI exclusion criteria (e.g. pregnancy, metal, claustrophobia) were not included in the study. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant after procedures had been fully explained. The consent procedures were approved by the ethics committee of the Ludwig-Maximilians-University.

Smokers and non-smokers did not differ significantly in all assessed, smoking-unrelated characteristics (age, sex, level of education, verbal intelligence, personality, impulsiveness, depressive symptoms). Please see Table 1 for further information.

Assessment of behavioural and smoking related data

Several questionnaires were used for the assessment of behavioural and smoking related data. The smoking urge was assessed by the German version of the Questionnaire of Smoking Urges (QSU-G [39], aggression by the Aggression Questionnaire (AQ, [40,41]), impulsiveness by the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11 [42], intelligence by a German verbal intelligence test (Wortschatztest (WST) [43]), depressive symptoms by the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II [44]) and nicotine dependence by The Fagerström Test of Nicotine Dependence (FTND [45,46]). Further assessed information included age, sex, level of education and number of cigarettes smoked per day, and is summarised in Table 1.

Procedure of graphic warning label task

The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Department of the Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich. FMRI measurements took place at the Institute of Clinical Radiology, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich. At the first session, participants provided written informed consent. The smoking status was confirmed by carbon monoxide (CO) in expired air by a Bedfont Smokerlyzer and by salivary cotinine levels. A basic questionnaire (demographics, smoking behaviour, BIS-11, WST, NEO), QSU and AQ were completed and the fMRI session was started.

Non-smokers and smokers participated at the fMRI sessions twice on separate days: for smokers (a) after 10 hours nicotine withdrawal; (b) directly (about 5 minutes) after nicotine consumption. The sequence of measurements (a-b vs. b-a) was counterbalanced between subjects.

There were three categories of visual stimulation: (1) 32 warning pictures with the EU warning notices (date: 10.09.2013), (2) 16 warning notices (old text-only warning labels), (3) 32 neutral pictures with short information. The design was kept according to the official EU warning notices. Fig 1 shows an example of the three stimuli categories.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Example pictures of the three stimulus categories A) graphical warning notices (translation: Protect children, don’t let them breath in your tobacco smoke); B) warning notices (translation: Smokers die earlier); C) neutral pictures (translation: Wood can be made into anything).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201360.g001

In order to determine the neutrality of the neutral pictures the emotional valence and arousal has been rated in a sample of 21 subjects using the Self Assessment Manikin scores. The subject can select any of the 5 figures comprising each scale, or between any two figures, which results in a 9-point rating scale for each dimension. Ratings are scored such that 9 represents a high rating on each dimension (i.e., high pleasure, high arousal), and 1 represents a low rating on each dimension (i.e., low pleasure, low arousal). The mean valence score for the neutral pictures was m = 5.47 (standard deviation: 0.31; minimum: 4.63; maximum: 5.97). The mean arousal for the neutral pictures was m = 2.42 (standard deviation: 0.44; minimum: 1.34; maximum: 3.25).

The pictures were presented for 6 seconds each. The picture sequences differed between the first and the second fMRI session in order to prevent that subjects know which picture will be presented next. The same picture sequences were used for smokers and non-smokers. The subjects were instructed to view the pictures on the screen.

We especially focused on neuronal alterations that are linked to motivational processes within the emotion network. As we supposed that the combination of cognitive tasks with emotional information could be used by some participants as a distraction strategy in order to prevent dealing with the emotional content of the pictures and/or to control emotional responses, we did not include any behavioural task.

After the fMRI session, participants were asked again to complete the QSU and the AQ.

MRI data acquisition and analysis

FMRI-imaging was performed in a 3 Tesla Philips scanner with echoplanar capability. A three-dimensional MPRAGE data set (T1-weighted) was acquired for each subject for anatomical referencing. For functional BOLD imaging during the presentation of pictures a T2* weighted EPI sequence was acquired in the same position as the anatomical images (repetition time (TR): 2000 ms; echo time (TE): 30 ms; 36 axial slices; matrix size: 1,65 x 1,65; slice thickness: 3 mm).

The post-processing and analysis of the fMRI data was carried out by the BrainVoyager software package (Brain Innovation, Maastricht, Netherlands). The first 4 images were excluded from any further analysis due to relaxation time effects. The preprocessing of the functional data included high-pass filtering (cut-off: three cycles in time course) to low frequency signal drift inherent in echo planar imaging, a slice scan time correction, spatial smoothing (Gaussian filter with FWHM 8.0 mm), and a 3D motion correction. In addition, the functional images were transferred to a standard Talairach brain. Significant BOLD activity was determined by a cross correlation of MR image pixel intensity with an expected hemodynamic response function. Voxelwise t-tests were used to identify those brain areas where the signal change was significantly different between the different experimental conditions (new GWLs, text-only labels, neutral pictures). For each participant the conditions new GWL, text-only label and neutral pictures were calculated as regressors.

The results of non-smokers were compared to those of smokers after nicotine consumption as well as smokers during withdrawal. The data of all smokers and non-smokers were calculated in the same GLM.

Statistical analysis

Nominal data (demographics) were compared by Chi-square tests and continuous data by one-way ANOVAs. Paired t-tests were conducted to compare the impact of the fMRI session on smoking urge (craving), intention to smoke and aggression. All tests of significance used an alpha level of 0.05 and were reported as 2 tailed. Statistical analyses were performed with the software SPSS version 23.0 for Windows.

Results

Functional MRI results

New GWLs versus neutral pictures.

In the comparison of the new GWLs with neutral pictures, smokers after smoking showed increased BOLD responses especially in subcortical, temporal and frontal regions including amygdala/parahippocampal gyrus, superior and middle temporal gyrus, e.g. thalamus/globus pallidus/caudate, insula, hippocampus, middle/superior/inferior frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate cortex, precentral/postcentral gyrus. In addition, enhanced responses were shown e.g. in the superior parietal gyrus, postcentral and precentral gyrus, precuneus, superior/middle temporal gyrus. Decreased responses during the new GWLs compared to the neutral cues were small (e.g. middle/superior frontal gyrus) (see Table 2, Fig 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Smokers after smoking: Comparison of new GWLs and neutral cues (Fixed effects analysis q(FDR) < 0.01, T: 2.92–8.0; p < 0.003547).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201360.t002

thumbnail
Fig 2. Comparison of neuronal responses during the presentation of the new GWLs minus neutral pictures.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201360.g002

During withdrawal smokers demonstrated enhanced neuronal responses while the new GWLs were presented, compared to the neutral cues predominantly in the amygdala/hippocampus, middle/inferior temporal/occipital gyrus, the inferior parietal gyrus/postcentral gyrus and the inferior/middle frontal region. The responses in the caudate/cuneus as well as the superior/middle frontal gyrus were decreased during the new GWLs compared to the neutral condition (Table 3, Fig 2).

thumbnail
Table 3. Smokers during withdrawal: Comparison of new GWLs and neutral cues (Fixed effects analysis q(FDR) < 0.01, T: 3.35–8.0; p <0.000825).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201360.t003

Non-smokers showed enhanced BOLD responses during the presentation of new GWLs compared to neutral cues e.g. in amygdala/parahippocampal gyrus/hippocampus, middle occipital/temporal gyrus, anterior cingulate cortex, superior/middle and inferior frontal gyrus and inferior parietal cortex/postcentral gyrus. Decreased responses were small and predominantly located in the cuneus/lingual gyrus (Table 4, Fig 2).

thumbnail
Table 4. Non-smokers: Comparison of new GWLs and neutral cues (Fixed effects analysis q(FDR) < 0.01, T: 3.12–8.0, p < 0.001792).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201360.t004

New GWLs versus old text-only labels.

In the comparison of the new GWLs with old text-only labels, smokers after smoking showed increased BOLD responses especially in subcortical areas, including the parahippocampal gyrus/fusiform gyrus, globus pallidus) as well as parietal areas (e.g. superior/inferior parietal lobule, precuneus). By contrast, the activation in frontal and temporal brain regions, including the superior, middle and inferior frontal gyrus, the anterior cingulate gyrus and the insula are reduced (see Table 5, Fig 3).

thumbnail
Table 5. Smokers after smoking: Comparison of new GWLs and old text-only labels (Fixed effects analysis q(FDR) < 0.01, T: 3.29–8.0, p<0.001020).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201360.t005

thumbnail
Fig 3. Comparison of neuronal responses during the presentation of the new GWLs minus old text-only labels.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201360.g003

Smokers during withdrawal demonstrated very small differences between both conditions: the BOLD responses were slightly increased during the presentation of the new GWLs in the parahippocampal gyrus/fusiform gyrus and occipital regions and slightly decreased, predominantly in the cuneus/precuneus as well as the middle/superior frontal gyrus (see Table 6, Fig 3).

thumbnail
Table 6. Smokers during withdrawal: Comparison of new GWLs and old text-only labels (Fixed effects analysis q(FDR)< 0.01, T: 3.72–8.0; p < 0.000198).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201360.t006

Non-smokers showed increased BOLD responses during the presentation of the new GWLs compared to text-only labels especially in subcortical areas (e.g. parahippocampal gyrus, amygdala, thalamus, hippocampus, putamen, caudate, globus pallidus), frontal brain regions (including the middle and inferior frontal gyrus), parietal areas (e.g. superior parietal lobule/precuneus), temporal areas (e.g. middle/superior temporal gyrus) and occipital areas (e.g. cuneus/precuneus, superior/inferior occipital gyrus). Responses especially in the lingual gyrus/inferior occipital gyrus were decreased (see Table 7, Fig 3).

thumbnail
Table 7. Non-smokers: Comparison of new GWLs and old text-only labels (Fixed effects analysis q(FDR)< 0.01, T: 3.08–8.0; p < 0.002090).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201360.t007

Smoking urge and aggression.

Craving increased in the group of smokers after smoking after the fMRI session compared to before (T (22) = -2.65, p = .015; Table 8). Difference between pre and post fMRI for the intention to smoke in all smokers was not significant, nor for craving in smokers during withdrawal (p > .185). The level of aggression did not change after the fMRI session for any group (p > .536).

thumbnail
Table 8. Results of the QSU in smokers and non-smokers before and after the acquisition of the fMRI data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201360.t008

The results also showed that craving and intention to smoke were significantly higher for smokers during withdrawal than for smokers after smoking before the fMRI session (T < -2.65, p < .015). After the fMRI session, craving was also significantly higher in smokers during withdrawal than in smokers after smoking (T (21) = -2.11, p = .047). Aggression scores did not differ significantly between smokers after smoking and smokers during withdrawal, neither before nor after the fMRI session (|ts| < 1.70, p > .103).

Discussion

The present study investigated the neuronal responses to the new German GWLs in non-smokers and smokers. We focused especially on the effect of smoking status on neurobiological responses comparing smokers after smoking and smokers during withdrawal. While undergoing fMRI, new GWLs, old text-only warnings and neutral pictures were presented to the participants. To our knowledge the effect of smoking status on BOLD responses has not been considered so far: up to now, neuronal responses of smokers during withdrawal have not been examined (e.g. Newman-Norlund et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2015).

New GWLs vs neutral pictures

The presentation of the new GWLs warnings to smokers after smoking resulted in increased activations especially in subcortical, temporal and frontal brain regions including amygdala, hippocampus, caudate, thalamus, medial frontal regions and the DLPFC compared to neutral pictures. These regions are especially associated with emotional processing (e.g. amygdala, hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, insula) as well as cognitive processes (e.g. medial/prefrontal cortex/DLPFC) including attention and working memory. The insula is often related to a network that includes the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex and is important for the conversion of sensory information into emotions [25,47]. In addition, the importance of the insula for the perception of hazards of smoking has been demonstrated [48]. In addition, brain regions which are thought to be associated with visual processing (e.g. occipital cortex, precuneus) and earlier stages of processing of emotional stimuli (e.g. fusiform cortex) [49] seem to be involved.

Smokers during withdrawal also showed increased responses predominantly in frontal, temporal/occipital and subcortical areas (e.g. amygdala/hippocampus, inferior/middle frontal gyrus, middle/inferior temporal gyrus); however, the differences between GWLs and neutral pictures were smaller than in smokers after smoking. By contrast, the findings of non-smokers were comparable to those of smokers after smoking: areas of emotional processing, areas of visual processing and areas of cognitive processing showed pronounced BOLD responses during the presentation of GWLs.

The findings that GWLs activate brain regions that are involved in cognition, emotion and memory formation are consistent with current literature [25,50]: these studies demonstrated the importance of amygdala/hippocampus, insula and visual association cortices. The neural activity in regions which are involved in emotional and cognitive processing of warning messages is a predictor of positive cessation outcomes [32,51]. Numerous neuroscientific studies confirmed the prominent role of the amygdala in emotional processing in a number of sensory modalities [30,31,52] especially threat and fear [53,54]. Particularly high emotional salient GWLs were associated with activations in the amygdala and the hippocampus [25,27], probably indicating higher emotional responses as well as enhanced memory. The insula has shown to be another important brain region during the presentation of GWLs. This area is especially linked to disgust, e.g. induced by mutilation and contamination [55,56,57]. Interestingly the differences between neuronal responses to GWLs and neutral pictures were weaker in smokers after deprivation than in smokers after smoking, and in non-smokers. This may indicate that the effect of the new warnings labels on important brain areas of smokers is strongest in a limited period of time. After a certain time of deprivation, smokers seem to be less affected by the pictures.

New GWLs vs text-only labels

Former studies have already demonstrated enhanced emotional responses to pictorial warning labels compared to text-only warnings [9] as well as beneficial effects of GWLs on behaviour correlates. However, neurobiological aspects that may underly this difference have not been considered so far.

In the present study the comparison of neurobiological correlates of the new GWLs with the old text-only labels demonstrated in particular the importance of affective brain regions in smokers after smoking and in non-smokers: the differences between categories (new GWLs vs. text-only labels) were smaller than regarding the comparison of GWLs and neutral pictures.

Subcortical areas and occipital regions (e.g. parahippocampal gyrus, fusiform gyrus, lingual gyrus) as well as regions of the visual association cortex were predominantly activated in smokers after smoking during the presentation of new GWLs. By contrast, text-only labels were related to responses, e.g. in the posterior and anterior cingulate gyrus, medial frontal gyrus, insula/precentral gyrus. These responses may indicate enhanced higher cognitive processes, including working memory and response inhibition.

In non-smokers emotion-relevant areas (e.g. amygdala, parahippocampal areas, hippocampus) as well as areas related to cognitive processes (e.g. frontal regions) and brain regions involved in the transmission of sensory signals to the cerebral cortex (e.g. thalamus) were stronger linked to the presentation of GWLs than to text-only labels.

By contrast, during withdrawal, the neural responses during the presentation of GWLs and text-only labels differed only marginally: new GWLs were related to increased responses, e.g. in the parahippocampal area / lingual gyrus and the occipital gyrus. Text-only labels, by contrast, led to enhanced BOLD responses, e.g. in the cuneus/precuneus. Comparable responses are often seen during resting-state tasks.

Overall the results may lead to the presumption that new GWLs images are more emotional salient than textual information and lead to enhanced neuronal responses e.g. in brain regions that are related to the emotion network in non-smokers and smokers after smoking. In smokers during withdrawal, warning pictures seem to have little effect. One reason for the slight response in this group could be that craving for the drug is induced by mentioning cigarettes/smoking instead of considering the message in the warning.

One drawback of the comparison of new GWLs and old text-only warning is that these information do not only differ in their emotional intensity but also the use of verbal (text-only warnings) as compared to the combination of visual and verbal information (new GWLs).

Former studies have reported activation especially of the ventral anterior cingulate cortex including the subgenual cortex [58,59], in the amygdala [59,60] as well as the posterior cingulate cortex, the inferior and superior frontal cortex, the inferior and middle temporal gyrus and the thalamus [61] in response to emotionally salient words. The presentation of negative pictures is related to activations especially in the amygdala, the ventral striatum, the insula, the anterior cingulate cortex, the medial prefrontal cortex and the orbitofrontal cortex [62,63,64]. The direct comparison regarding neuronal responses between different modalities indicated that both pictures and words elicited emotional responses with no general superiority for either stimulus modality. However, emotional responses to pictures are modulated by perceptual stimulus features, such as picture complexity [65]. This may indicate that the neurobiological differences may be influenced by the stimulus complexity.

Behaviour level

With regard to smoking urge, groups differed significantly both in the intention to smoke and in craving before the fMRI session, assumable due to the 12 hours of withdrawal in the condition smokers during withdrawal. Smokers during withdrawal didn’t show any changes in the factor craving after the fMRI which is probably due to an already very high score before the task. This is in accordance with the findings on the neuronal level that smokers in a long period of withdrawal don’t experience any effect of the presented pictures.

Noticeably the group of smokers after smoking showed an increase in craving after the fMRI session. At first, this might seem surprising since the GWLs are supposed to decrease the urge to smoke. One possible explanation for this is that after over 60 minutes of deprivation (questionnaires and fMRI tasks) and additional confrontation of smoking cues, the craving for cigarettes rises naturally.

Limitations

One limitation of the study is the absence of any behavioural responses during fMRI data acquisition. The lack of behavioural responses makes it impossible to compare neuronal responses with behavioural data. In addition, it leads to an uncertainty over the mental processes of participants during the fMRI session. Apart from this it was not possible to ensure that each participant’s attention was spent on viewing the pictures.

We decided to do not integrate a behavioural task in order to prevent participants to use this task in order to distract themselves from the emotional content of the pictures and associated emotional responses.

Another limitation of the findings results from the experimental design: the findings are limited to smokers that are able to withdraw from smoking for at least ten hours. We are not able to make any statements about smokers where this is not possible.

The differences regarding text-only and new GWL warings may be influenced by the use of verbal and combined verbal and visual information as well as stimulus complexity. These aspects should be carefully be taken into account in further studies.

Conclusions

The present study examined neural responses to GWL stimuli compared to text-only warnings and neutral pictures in smokers after smoking, smokers during withdrawal and non-smokers. GWLs elicited pronounced activations in a network of brain regions, including the visual association cortex (higher processing of visual information) and emotion-relevant regions including amygdala and insula). Functional differences between picture categories were predominantly present in smokers after smoking and in non-smokers. Smokers during withdrawal demonstrated only small differences between GWLs and neutral/text-only pictures. These results may indicate that the effect of GWLs on cognitive and emotional brain areas is more pronounced after smoking than during withdrawal. During withdrawal, more unspecific processes seem to be important, including the projection of sensory signals to the cerebral cortex and resting-state processes. Neuronal findings were in accordance with behavioural results: in smokers during withdrawal craving was not influenced by the experimental design. The increased craving in smokers after smoking might be related to the duration of non-smoking during the experimental setting.

Altogether, the results suggest that emotional and cognitive reactions to GWLs are predominantly seen in smokers after smoking and in non-smokers. The impact of these pictures during withdrawal seems to be less pronounced.

Acknowledgments

Parts of this work were prepared in the context of the MD thesis of Yannick Schultz at the Faculty of Medicine, Ludwig-Maximilians-University, Munich. We thank Mije Hartmann who assisted with the proof-reading of the manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Ezzati M, Lopez AD. Estimates of global mortality attributable to smoking in 2000. Lancet. 2003; 362: 847–852. pmid:13678970
  2. 2. Sasco AJ, Secretan MB, Straif K. Tobacco smoking and cancer: a brief review of recent epidemiological evidence. Lung Cancer. 2004; 45 Suppl 2: S3–9.
  3. 3. Thun MJ, Carter BD, Feskanich D, Freedman ND, Prentice R, Lopez AD, et al. 50-year trends in smoking-related mortality in the United States. N Engl J Med. 2013; 368: 351–364. pmid:23343064
  4. 4. US Department of Health and Human Services (1989) Reducing the health consequences of smoking: 25 years of progress. A report of the surgeon general. Rockville, MD: US Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, Centers for Disease Control, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Office on smoking and Health.
  5. 5. Canadian Cancer Society. Cigarette Package Health Warnings: International Status Report. 2014; 4.
  6. 6. EU. Directive 2014/40/EU of the European Parliament and of the Council of 3 April 2014 on the appoximation of the laws, regulations and administrative provisions of the Member States concerning the manufacture, presentation and sale of tobacco and related products and repealing Directive 2001/37/EC. 2014.
  7. 7. Hammond D. Health warning messages on tobacco products: a review. Tob Control. 2011; 20: 327–337. pmid:21606180
  8. 8. Noar SM, Hall MG, Francis DB, Ribisl KM, Pepper JK, Brewer NT. Pictorial cigarette pack warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. Tobacco Control. 2016; 25: 341–354. pmid:25948713
  9. 9. Kees J, Buron S, Andrews JC, Kzup J. Tests of Graphic Visuals and Cigarette Package Warning Combinations: Implications for the Framework Convention on Tobacco Control. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing. 2006; 25: 212–223.
  10. 10. Bekalu MA, Ramanadhan S, Bigman CA, Nagler RH, Viswanath K. Graphic and Arousing? Emotional and Cognitive Reactions to Tobacco Graphic Health Warnings and Associated Quit-Related Outcomes Among Low SEP Population Groups. Health Commun. 2018; 1–9.
  11. 11. Droulers O, Gallopel-Morvan K, Lacoste-Badie S, Lajante M. The influence of threatening visual warnings on tobacco packaging: Measuring the impact of threat level, image size, and type of pack through psychophysiological and self-report methods. PLoS One. 2017; 12: e0184415. pmid:28910317
  12. 12. Fong GT, Hammond D, Hitchman SC. The impact of pictures on the effectiveness of tobacco warnings. Bull World Health Organ. 2009; 87: 640–643. pmid:19705020
  13. 13. Strahan EJ, White K, Fong GT, Fabrigar LR, Zanna MP, Cameron R. Enhancing the effectiveness of tobacco package warning labels: a social psychological perspective. Tob Control. 2002; 11: 183–190. pmid:12198266
  14. 14. Kees J, Burton S, Andrews JC, Kozup J. Understanding How Graphic Pictorial Warnings Work on Cigarette Packaging. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing. 2010; 29: 265–276.
  15. 15. O’Hegarty M, Pederson LL, Nelson DE, Mowery P, Gable JM, Wortley P. Reactions of young adult smokers to warning labels on cigarette packages. Am J Prev Med. 2006; 30: 467–473. pmid:16704939
  16. 16. Borland R, Wilson N, Fong GT, Hammond D, Cummings KM, Yong HH, et al. Impact of graphic and text warnings on cigarette packs: findings from four countries over five years. Tob Control. 2009; 18: 358–364. pmid:19561362
  17. 17. Hammond D, Fong GT, McDonald PW, Cameron R, Brown KS. Impact of the graphic Canadian warning labels on adult smoking behaviour. Tob Control. 2003; 12: 391–395. pmid:14660774
  18. 18. Hammond D, Fong GT, McNeill A, Borland R, Cummings KM. Effectiveness of cigarette warning labels in informing smokers about the risks of smoking: findings from the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Four Country Survey. Tobacco Control. 2006; 15: iii19–iii25. pmid:16754942
  19. 19. Thrasher JF, Hammond D, Fong GT, Arillo-Santillan E. Smokers’ reactions to cigarette package warnings with graphic imagery and with only text: a comparison between Mexico and Canada. Salud Publica Mex. 2007; 49 Suppl 2: S233–240.
  20. 20. Azagba S, Sharaf MF. The Effect of Graphic Cigarette Warning Labels on Smoking Behavior: Evidence from the Canadian Experience. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2013; 15: 708–717.
  21. 21. Hammond D, Fong GT, Borland R, Cummings KM, McNeill A, Driezen P. Text and graphic warnings on cigarette packages: findings from the international tobacco control four country study. Am J Prev Med. 2007; 32: 202–209. pmid:17296472
  22. 22. Villanti AC, Cantrell J, Pearson JL, Vallone DM, Rath JM. Perceptions and perceived impact of graphic cigarette health warning labels on smoking behavior among U.S. young adults. Nicotine Tob Res. 2014; 16: 469–477. pmid:24212476
  23. 23. Partos TR, Borland R, Yong HH, Thrasher J, Hammond D. Cigarette packet warning labels can prevent relapse: findings from the International Tobacco Control 4-Country policy evaluation cohort study. Tob Control. 2013; 22: e43–50. pmid:22535363
  24. 24. Miller CL, Hill DJ, Quester PG, Hiller JE. Impact on the Australian Quitline of new graphic cigarette pack warnings including the Quitline number. Tob Control. 2009; 18: 235–237. pmid:19211613
  25. 25. Wang AL, Lowen SB, Romer D, Giorno M, Langleben DD. Emotional reaction facilitates the brain and behavioural impact of graphic cigarette warning labels in smokers. Tob Control. 2015; 24: 225–232. pmid:25564288
  26. 26. Engelmann JM, Versace F, Robinson JD, Minnix JA, Lam CY, Cui Y, et al. Neural substrates of smoking cue reactivity: A meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neuroimage. 2012; 60: 252–262. pmid:22206965
  27. 27. Newman-Norlund RD, Thrasher JF, Fridriksson J, Brixius W, Froeliger B, Hammond D, et al. Neural biomarkers for assessing different types of imagery in pictorial health warning labels for cigarette packaging: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open. 2014; 4: e006411. pmid:25552613
  28. 28. Do KT, Galvan A. FDA cigarette warning labels lower craving and elicit frontoinsular activation in adolescent smokers. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015; 10: 1484–1496. pmid:25887154
  29. 29. Green AE, Mays D, Falk EB, Vallone D, Gallagher N, Richardson A, et al. Young Adult Smokers’ Neural Response to Graphic Cigarette Warning Labels. Addict Behav Rep. 2016; 3: 28–32. pmid:27019865
  30. 30. Phelps EA, LeDoux JE. Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron. 2005; 48: 175–187. pmid:16242399
  31. 31. Zald DH. The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2003; 41: 88–123. pmid:12505650
  32. 32. Jasinska AJ, Chua HF, Ho SS, Polk TA, Rozek LS, Strecher VJ. Amygdala response to smoking-cessation messages mediates the effects of serotonin transporter gene variation on quitting. Neuroimage. 2012; 60: 766–773. pmid:22245645
  33. 33. Sass H, Wittchen HU, Zaudig M (1996) Diagnostisches und Statistisches Manual Psychischer Störungen DSM-IV. Diagnostisches und Statistisches Manual Psychischer Störungen DSM-IV: Hogrefe.
  34. 34. Jackson KJ, Muldoon PP, De Biasi M, Damaj MI. New mechanisms and perspectives in nicotine withdrawal. Neuropharmacology. 2015; 96: 223–234. pmid:25433149
  35. 35. Due DL, Huettel SA, Hall WG, Rubin DC. Activation in mesolimbic and visuospatial neural circuits elicited by smoking cues: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Am J Psychiatry. 2002; 159: 954–960. pmid:12042183
  36. 36. David SP, Munafo MR, Johansen-Berg H, Smith SM, Rogers RD, Matthews PM, et al. Ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens activation to smoking-related pictorial cues in smokers and nonsmokers: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. 2005; 58: 488–494. pmid:16023086
  37. 37. Kober H, Mende-Siedlecki P, Kross EF, Weber J, Mischel W, Hart CL, et al. Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010; 107: 14811–14816. pmid:20679212
  38. 38. Naqvi NH, Rudrauf D, Damasio H, Bechara A. Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking. Science. 2007; 315: 531–534. pmid:17255515
  39. 39. Müller V, Mucha RF, Ackermann K, Pauli P. Die Erfassung des Cravings bei Rauchern mit einer deutschen Version des “Questionnaire on Smoking Urges”(QSU-G). Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie. 2001; 30: 164–171.
  40. 40. Werner R, von Collani G. Deutscher Aggressionsfragebogen. (2004). ZIS—GESIS Leibniz Institute for the Social Sciences.
  41. 41. Buss AH, Perry M. The aggression questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1992; 63: 452–459. pmid:1403624
  42. 42. Patton JH, Stanford MS. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of clinical psychology. 1995; 51: 768–774. pmid:8778124
  43. 43. Schmidt K-H, Metzler P (1992) Wortschatztest (WST). Weinheim: Beltz Test GmbH.
  44. 44. Hautzinger M, Beck AT, Keller F, Kühner C (2009) Beck Depressions-Inventar: BDI-II; Revision; Manual: Pearson.
  45. 45. Bleich S, Havermann-Reinecke U, Kornhuber J (2002) FTNA: Fagerström-Test für Nikotinabhängigkeit: Beltz Test.
  46. 46. Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerstrom KO. The Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict. 1991; 86: 1119–1127. pmid:1932883
  47. 47. Craig AD. Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003; 13: 500–505. pmid:12965300
  48. 48. Naqvi NH, Gaznick N, Tranel D, Bechara A. The insula: a critical neural substrate for craving and drug seeking under conflict and risk. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014; 1316: 53–70. pmid:24690001
  49. 49. Sabatinelli D, Frank DW, Wanger TJ, Dhamala M, Adhikari BM, Li X. The timing and directional connectivity of human frontoparietal and ventral visual attention networks in emotional scene perception. Neuroscience. 2014; 277: 229–238. pmid:25018086
  50. 50. Newman-Norlund RD, Thrasher JF, Fridriksson J, Brixius W, Froeliger B, Hammond D, et al. Neural biomarkers for assessing different types of imagery in pictorial health warning labels for cigarette packaging: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open. 2014; 4.
  51. 51. Falk EB, Berkman ET, Whalen D, Lieberman MD. Neural activity during health messaging predicts reductions in smoking above and beyond self-report. Health Psychol. 2011; 30: 177–185. pmid:21261410
  52. 52. Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage. 2002; 16: 331–348. pmid:12030820
  53. 53. Davis M. The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1992; 15: 353–375. pmid:1575447
  54. 54. LeDoux J. The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2003; 23: 727–738. pmid:14514027
  55. 55. Stark R, Zimmermann M, Kagerer S, Schienle A, Walter B, Weygandt M, et al. Hemodynamic brain correlates of disgust and fear ratings. Neuroimage. 2007; 37: 663–673. pmid:17574869
  56. 56. Schienle A, Schafer A, Hermann A, Walter B, Stark R, Vaitl D. fMRI responses to pictures of mutilation and contamination. Neurosci Lett. 2006; 393: 174–178. pmid:16239068
  57. 57. Murphy FC, Nimmo-Smith I, Lawrence AD. Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: a meta-analysis. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2003; 3: 207–233. pmid:14672157
  58. 58. Beauregard M, Chertkow H, Bub D, Murtha S, Dixon R, Evans A. The neural substrate for concrete, abstract, and emotional word lexica a positron emission tomography study. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997; 9: 441–461. pmid:23968210
  59. 59. Tabert MH, Borod JC, Tang CY, Lange G, Wei TC, Johnson R, et al. Differential amygdala activation during emotional decision and recognition memory tasks using unpleasant words: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2001; 39: 556–573. pmid:11257281
  60. 60. Strange BA, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ. Brain mechanisms for detecting perceptual, semantic, and emotional deviance. Neuroimage. 2000; 12: 425–433. pmid:10988036
  61. 61. Maddock RJ, Garrett AS, Buonocore MH. Posterior cingulate cortex activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence decision task. Hum Brain Mapp. 2003; 18: 30–41. pmid:12454910
  62. 62. Pessoa L, Ungerleider LG. Neuroimaging studies of attention and the processing of emotion-laden stimuli. Prog Brain Res. 2004; 144: 171–182. pmid:14650848
  63. 63. Garavan H, Pendergrass JC, Ross TJ, Stein EA, Risinger RC. Amygdala response to both positively and negatively valenced stimuli. Neuroreport. 2001; 12: 2779–2783. pmid:11522965
  64. 64. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Fitzsimmons JR, Cuthbert BN, Scott JD, Moulder B, et al. Emotional arousal and activation of the visual cortex: an fMRI analysis. Psychophysiology. 1998; 35: 199–210. pmid:9529946
  65. 65. Bayer M, Schacht A. Event-related brain responses to emotional words, pictures, and faces—a cross-domain comparison. Front Psychol. 2014; 5: 1106. pmid:25339927