Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Sex differences in default mode and dorsal attention network engagement

  • Kelly M. Dumais,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations McLean Imaging Center, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Massachusetts, United States of America, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Sergey Chernyak,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations McLean Imaging Center, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Massachusetts, United States of America, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Lisa D. Nickerson ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Lisa D. Nickerson, Amy C. Janes

    Roles Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Software, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations McLean Imaging Center, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Massachusetts, United States of America, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Amy C. Janes

    Contributed equally to this work with: Lisa D. Nickerson, Amy C. Janes

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ajanes@mclean.harvard.edu

    Affiliations McLean Imaging Center, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Massachusetts, United States of America, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

Abstract

Focusing on sex differences is necessary to fully understand basic neurobiological processes such as the engagement of large-scale brain networks involved in attention. Prior work suggests that women show enhanced attention during tasks of reward/punishment relative to men. Yet, sex differences in the engagement of neural networks sub serving internal and external focus has been unexplored in regard to reward and punishment. Using data from a large sample (n = 190) of healthy participants from the Human Connectome Project, we investigated sex differences in default mode network (DMN), dorsal attention network (DAN), and frontal parietal network (FPN) activation during exposure to reward and punishment. To determine if sex differences are specific to valenced stimuli, we analyzed network activation during working memory. Results indicate that, relative to men, women have increased suppression of the DMN and greater activation of the DAN during exposure to reward and punishment. Given the relative roles of these networks in internal (DMN) and external (DAN) attention, this pattern of activation suggests that women have enhanced external attention to reward and punishment. In contrast, there were no sex differences in network activation during working memory, indicating that this sex difference is specific to the processing of reward and punishment. These findings suggest a neurobiological explanation for prior work showing women have greater sensitivity to reward/punishment and are more prone to psychiatric disorders characterized by enhanced attention to such stimuli. Furthermore, given the large sample from the Human Connectome Project, the current findings provide general implications for the study of sex as a biological variable in investigation of reward processes.

Introduction

Brain responses to both reward and punishment have been studied extensively, providing critical insight into normal and pathological brain states [14]. However, there has been limited investigation into how biological factors such as sex influence these processes. Such sex differences are likely, as women generally show more behavioral sensitivity to reward and punishment relative to men [57]. Neuroimaging investigation of sex differences in reward and punishment processing have largely focused on limbic brain regions typically associated with reward and emotion [89]. However, when considering sex differences in reward/punishment, there is evidence suggesting that sex differences exist outside traditional reward function. Specifically, event related potential studies suggest women engage more attentional resources when performing reward/punishment tasks [7, 10]. Such findings suggest that there may be sex differences in the engagement of large scale attentional neural networks during reward and punishment processing.

Attentional processes are modulated by the suppression and engagement of large-scale neural networks, such as the default mode network (DMN), dorsal attention network (DAN), and frontoparietal control network (FPN) [1113]. Specifically, the DMN, comprised mainly of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), inferior parietal lobe, and medial temporal lobe, modulates internally oriented attention, such as self-referential processing, recalling one’s past, and planning one’s future [14,15]. On the other hand, the DAN, comprised primarily of the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), frontal eye fields, middle temporal motion complex, and superior parietal lobe, modulates externally oriented attention, such as top-down attentional control [11]. Therefore, the DMN is typically suppressed and DAN activated during cognitive tasks requiring external attention, including those involving rewarding stimuli [1618]. The FPN modulates attentional processes by flexibly coupling with the DMN and/or DAN to support internal and external attention, respectively [13,19]. Prior work suggests that these networks may be differentially engaged in men and women during exposure to valenced stimuli. For example, core DMN regions, such as the mPFC and PCC, show greater activity in men versus women during exposure to monetary and appetitive drug stimuli [2022], which could be due to higher activation in men or increased suppression in women. In contrast, a core DAN region, the dlPFC, shows greater activity in women versus men during exposure to negative stimuli [23]. It is plausible that these prior regional findings represent sex differences in DMN and DAN engagement during exposure to reward/punishment.

To determine whether there are sex differences in network engagement during the processing of reward/punishment, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data from a large group of healthy participants from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) [24]. We analyzed DMN, DAN, and FPN activity during the incentive processing task, which involves exposure to monetary reward and punishment. Because women generally show greater attention to valenced stimuli [7,10,25,26], we hypothesize that women will show greater suppression of brain networks related to internal attentional processes (i.e., greater DMN suppression) and greater activation of brain networks related to external attentional processes (i.e., greater DAN activation) during reward and punishment exposure. To determine if sex differences are specific to the processing of valenced stimuli, we also analyzed network activity during an object n-back working memory task (0-back and 2-back) which is known to reliably suppress the DMN and activate the DAN [18,27]. Men and women typically show equivalent behavioral performance on similar n-back tasks [2831], though similar working memory performance could still be modulated by different brain regions in men versus women [32]. Indeed, a meta-analysis of sex differences during working memory have found that women show greater activation of limbic and prefrontal structures, such as the amygdala, left superior and right inferior frontal gyri, while men show greater activation of parietal areas such as the superior parietal lobule and left precuneus [33]. However, sex differences in attentional networks during working memory have not been investigated. Together, these results will clarify the neurobiological factors underlying sex-specific processing of reward and punishment, and whether these sex-specific processes are specific to reward/punishment or extend to other cognitive domains.

Methods

Participants

Secondary analysis of data collected from the first 500 participants in the Human Connectome Project (S500 data release) was done for the present study. Recruitment details for the HCP are provided in Van Essen et al. [24]. Study was approved by Washington University in the St. Louis’ Human Research Protection Office (IRB #201204036, "Mapping the Human Connectome: Structure, Function, and Heritability"). Briefly, individuals were excluded by the HCP if they reported having a significant history of psychiatric disorders, neurological or cardiovascular disease, if they were pregnant, or if they had unsafe metal in their body. Inclusion/exclusion criteria were assessed by a screening questionnaire developed explicitly for the HCP and using the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA) [34]. Additional criteria were used to exclude HCP participants from the current analyses, including testing positive for illicit drug use, having a breath alcohol level above zero, use of any tobacco products, irregular menstrual cycles, or head motion > 2.00 mm during the tasks. To attain two groups with equal numbers of men and women, the remaining participants were matched based on age and education level. The final group of participants included 190 individuals (n = 95 per sex, age range 22–36). See Table 1 for detailed participant demographics.

To assess for potential menstrual cycle phase effects, we also categorized women to be in either the follicular phase (n = 52) or luteal phase (n = 43) of their menstrual cycle. The regularly cycling women included in this study were considered to be in the follicular phase of their menstrual cycle if the number of days since their last period was 1–12 (for reported menstrual cycle lengths <25 days), 1–14 (for reported menstrual cycle lengths 25–35 days), and 1–17 (for reported menstrual cycle lengths > 35 days). All others were considered to be in the luteal phase of their menstrual cycle.

Experimental task design

Incentive processing.

The incentive processing task conducted by the HCP was adapted from Delgado et al. [35] and is described in detail in Barch et al. [36]. Participants were shown a card with a question mark (“?”) and were told the number on the back of the card was between 1–9. Participants guessed whether the number was either lower or higher than 5 by pressing one of two buttons on a response box. Participants were then given feedback to indicate whether they were correct and won money or incorrect and lost money. Although participants believed that they were correct or incorrect based on the accuracy of their responses, feedback was predetermined to create either a reward trial, a punishment trial, or a neutral trial. Feedback was either a green upward facing arrow and “+$1.00” to indicate a reward, a red downward facing arrow and “-$0.50” to indicate a punishment, or a bidirectional gray arrow pointing left and right and the number “5” to indicate neither a reward nor punishment (neutral trial). The experimental design was a block design with two 3 min 12 s runs. Each run had four 28 s blocks (2 reward blocks and 2 punishment blocks) interleaved with a 15 s fixation cross. Each block consisted of eight 3.5 s trials with presentation of the “?” for 1.5 s, feedback for 1.0 s, and an inter trial interval (ITI; fixation cross) for 1.0 s. Reward blocks consisted of 6 reward trials pseudo randomly interleaved with 2 out-of-set trials (1 neutral and 1 punishment trial, 2 neutral trials, or 2 punishment trials). Punishment blocks consisted of 6 punishment trials pseudo randomly interleaved with 2 out-of-set trials (1 reward and 1 neutral trial, 2 neutral trials, or 2 reward trials). Upon completion of the task, all participants were given a standardized amount of money. Since reward and punishment conditions were predetermined and not based on the accuracy of participant responses, there is no behavioral accuracy to be reported.

Working memory.

The working memory task conducted by the HCP is described in detail in Barch et al. [36]. Participants were shown pictures as part of a 0-back working memory task and a 2-back working memory task. For the 0-back working memory task, a target cue was presented at the start of each block and participants were instructed to press a button on a response box whenever the stimulus was the same as the target cue. For the 2-back working memory task, participants were instructed to press a button on a response box whenever the current stimulus was the same as the stimulus two back. Stimuli were pictures of faces, places, tools and body parts, and each stimulus type was presented in separate blocks. The experimental design was a block design with two 5 min 1 s runs. Each run had eight 25 s blocks interleaved with a 15 s fixation cross. Within each run, ½ of the blocks used the 0-back working memory task and ½ of the blocks used the 2-back working memory task. Each block consisted of ten 2.5 s trials with presentation of the picture stimulus for 2 s, followed by a fixation cross ITI for 500 ms. At the start of each block, a 2.5 s cue indicated the task type (and target stimulus for 0-back).

fMRI data acquisition

HCP data were acquired with a 32-channel head coil on a Siemens 3T Skyra modified to include a Siemens SC72 gradient coil to achieve a maximum gradient strength of 100 mT/m. Whole brain images were acquired using echo-planar imaging (EPI) with the following parameters: repetition time (TR) = 720 ms, echo time (TE) = 33.1 ms, flip angle = 52°, BW = 2290 Hz/Px, in-plan FOV = 208 x 180 mm, 72 slices, 2 mm isotropic voxels, with a multi-band acceleration factor of 8. For each task, there were two runs, one with right-to-left phase encoding and one with left-to-right phase encoding to minimize signal dropout in the combined results. Details of scanner modifications and data acquisition parameters are further described in Ugurbil et al. [37].

fMRI data preprocessing and analyses

Imaging data were pre-processed and analyzed using FMRIB Software Library (FSL) 5.0.8 (http://fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl) [38] and group-level statistical analyses were implemented using Permutation Analysis of Linear Models (PALM) [39]. The current study used the “minimally preprocessed” Quarter 3 release of the HCP data, which included gradient unwarping, motion correction, fieldmap-based EPI distortion correction, brain-boundary-based registration of EPI to structural T1-weighted images, non-linear FNIRT registration to the MNI152, and grand-mean intensity normalization using tools from FSL and Freesurfer [40]. For the current analyses, data was also re-registered to subject specific T1-weighted scans, spatially smoothed with a 4 mm full-width half-maximum Gaussian kernel, and a high-pass temporal filter of 200 s was applied. First-level analyses were conducted on each participant’s individual task runs separately (2 runs per task). Using the general linear model (GLM), task-related regressors (corresponding to either reward or punishment blocks for the incentive processing task or the 0-back and 2-back blocks for the working memory task) were convolved with the gamma hemodynamic response function. For the working memory task, the task-related regressors were modeled to include only the correct trials for each participant. Confound regressors representing motion were also included in the models for each task. Contrasts between each task condition (reward, punishment, 0-back and 2-back) and the baseline fixation were calculated. Lower level individual runs within each task were then combined using a second level fixed effects analysis to generate the average brain activation map for each participant for each contrast. For each contrast of interest, a contrast “subject-series” was then created by concatenating the contrast maps for all subjects together into a 4D (volume x subjects) data matrix for further processing. This was done for each contrast independently.

To calculate network activation strength, we defined our networks of interest (DMN, DAN, FPN) using independent component analysis (ICA) via FSL’s Multivariate Exploratory Linear Optimized Decomposition into Independent Components (MELODIC) on an independent group of healthy participant resting state scans (see Supporting Information). Brain networks of interest identified from the group ICA are shown in Fig 1. The DMN was comprised of the mPFC, PCC, precuneus, parahippocampus, and lateral parietal lobe. The DAN was comprised of the frontal eye fields, inferior precentral sulcus, dlPFC, middle temporal motion complex, and left superior parietal cortex. The FPN was comprised of the lateral prefrontal cortex, anterior insula, precuneus, middle frontal gyrus, and anterior inferior parietal lobe. Multivariate spatial regression of the template ICA networks against the subject series for each contrast was done to calculate the strength of activation of each network during each task condition (reward, punishment, 0-back and 2-back, each versus baseline fixation) [41]. This analysis results in the network activation strengths for each subject for each contrast.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Network ICA maps.

Sagittal, coronal and axial images showing the independent components from the MELODIC analysis that correspond to the default mode network (DMN), dorsal attention network (DAN) and frontoparietal control network (FPN). Because two DMN sub-networks and two FPN networks (left and right) were identified, the maps of these networks were added together to show a single DMN and FPN. Networks are shown overlaid on the MNI standard brain image.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199049.g001

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were run using t-tests with non-parametric permutation testing via FSL’s PALM to account for family structure in the HCP data. The HCP focused on recruiting twins and siblings, so there is a significant amount of family structure. PALM has specialized functionality for analyzing HCP data, thus this tool was used for inference, and all results are reported with control of family-wise error, p<0.05. Demographic variables (age and education) were assessed using a two-group t-test using PALM (p<0.05, corrected for family structure). Behavioral scores (0-back accuracy and 2-back accuracy) were compared between males and females using a two-group t-test using PALM (p<0.05, corrected for family structure, number of tasks [2: 0-back, 2-back], and number of contrasts [2: males > females, females > males]). For the incentive processing and working memory tasks, sex differences in network activation were assessed for each task contrast using a two-group t-test using PALM (p<0.05, corrected for family structure, number of networks [3: DMN, DAN, FPN], and number of contrasts [2: males > females, females > males]). Influence of menstrual cycle phase (reported at the time of MRI data collection) on network activation was also assessed by a two-group t-test using PALM (p<0.05, corrected for family structure, number of networks [3: DMN, DAN, FPN], and number of contrasts [2: follicular > luteal, luteal > follicular]).

Results

Demographic variables and task behavior

As our sample was matched based on age and education, there were no sex differences in either variable (Table 1, p > 0.4 for all contrasts). There were no sex differences in accuracy on the 0-back (males: 91.3 ± 1.1%; females: 91.1 ± 1.0%) or 2-back (males: 84.8 ± 1.0%; females: 81.7 ± 1.2%) working memory tasks (p > 0.05 for all contrasts).

Sex differences in network activation

Incentive processing task.

Females showed greater suppression of the DMN during reward (t(188) = 3.16, p = 0.006) and punishment (t(188) = 3.22, p = 0.004) trials compared to males (Fig 2). On the other hand, females showed greater activation of the DAN during reward (t(188) = 3.07, p = 0.008) and punishment (t(188) = 2.80, p = 0.017) trials compared to males (Fig 2). There were no sex differences in FPN activation during either reward or punishment trials.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Network activation during reward and punishment trials of the incentive processing task.

Females show greater suppression of the default mode network (DMN) and greater activation of the dorsal attention network (DAN) during reward and punishment trials compared to males. There is no sex difference in frontoparietal control network (FPN) activation. * p < 0.05, **p<0.01, t-tests with non-parametric permutation testing via Permutation Analysis of Linear Models (PALM). Bars represent mean ± SEM.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199049.g002

Working memory task.

There were no sex differences in DMN, DAN, or FPN activation during the 0-back (p > 0.3 for all contrasts) or 2-back (p > 0.1 for all contrasts) working memory tasks (Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Network activation during the working memory task.

There are no sex differences in default mode network (DMN), dorsal attention network (DAN), or frontoparietal control network (FPN) activation during the 0-back or 2-back working memory tasks. Bars represent mean ± SEM.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199049.g003

Menstrual cycle phase effects

When comparing women in the follicular and luteal phases, no differences were found within any of the networks and contrasts of interest for either the incentive processing task (p > 0.5 for all contrasts) or the working memory task (p > 0.3 for all contrasts).

Discussion

Our results show that women have a pattern of neural activity indicative of enhanced attention to external valenced stimuli. Specifically, during the processing of reward and punishment, but not working memory, women showed increased suppression of the DMN and increased activation of the DAN compared to men. These results suggest that women may have neural processing biases toward stimuli representing reward and punishment.

Greater attention to reward and punishment in women versus men is inferred given the roles of the DMN and DAN in attentional processing. Specifically, the DMN is typically engaged during internally-focused attention, such as self-focused thought, autobiographical memory, and mind wandering [15,42,43]. On the other hand, the DAN plays a role in externally-focused attention, and modulates goal-oriented attentional control [11]. Therefore, greater suppression of internal attentional processes coupled with greater activation of external attentional processes may suggest that women are paying greater attention to reward and punishment stimuli compared to men. These data support previous behavioral and electrophysiological studies that show greater attention to valenced stimuli in women versus men in both healthy [7,10,25,26] and clinical [44] populations. We extend these findings by suggesting that sex-specific attention to valenced stimuli are in part modulated by sex differences in the engagement of the DMN and DAN.

Sex-specific activation of attentional networks during reward and punishment processing may be due to sex differences in reward and punishment sensitivity. Specifically, meta-analyses show that women generally show greater reward and punishment sensitivity compared to men [5,6]. Greater reward sensitivity has been specifically linked with DMN suppression during exposure to reward cues [16]. Therefore, the greater DMN suppression in women in the current study may reflect the heightened reward sensitivity that is found in women versus men.

Though regions of the FPN also play a role in attention [11,45], there was no sex difference in FPN activity during exposure to reward and punishment. A major role of the FPN is to facilitate the interplay between the DMN and DAN. Specifically, the FPN is anatomically interposed between the DMN and DAN [19], and flexibly couples with either the DMN or DAN during internally driven versus externally driven goal-directed cognition, respectively [13]. Therefore, though both the FPN and DAN mediate attentional control, the DAN more directly modulates voluntary goal-directed stimulus response selection and serves to shift and maintain focus on relevant cues [11,27]. Therefore, the greater activation in women versus men in the DAN but not the FPN suggests that women show an enhanced voluntary shift in attention towards reward and punishment. While FPN activation did not differ between the sexes across all tasks, it was suppressed during the 0-back task while activated during the 2-back task and showed relatively little activation during the gambling task. It is plausible that these patterns relate to cognitive load. Specifically, the 2-back task is more challenging than the 0-back task, and FPN regions typically increase in activation during working memory and decision-making tasks [4648].

The lack of sex differences in network activation during working memory suggests that sex differences in attentional network activity are specific to the processing of valenced stimuli. However, a few studies have found sex differences in DMN activation during cognitive tasks that don’t include valenced stimuli. For example, women showed increased DMN suppression versus men during mental arithmetic [49] and cognitive interference [50]. Because greater DMN suppression is associated with increased task demands and difficulty [18,51], sex differences may emerge when task demands become more challenging for women than for men. Therefore, the mental arithmetic [49] and cognitive interference [50] tasks, but not the working memory task, may have been more challenging for women than men. Indeed, the lack of sex difference in n-back working memory performance in the current study and other studies [2831] may suggest that the working memory task had a similar level of difficulty for both men and women. However, cognitive effort was not assessed during any of these tasks. Whether sex differences in DMN and/or DAN activation are associated with sex differences in cognitive effort requires further testing.

Knowledge of sex differences in attentional network function during the processing of reward and punishment may help us gain a better understanding of sex biases found in numerous neuropsychiatric disorders. For example, depression and anxiety are more prevalent in women compared to men [52,53]. These disorders are characterized by dysfunctional reward processing [54,55], including attentional bias to negative information [5658]. It has been hypothesized that women’s enhanced attention to negative information may contribute to their greater prevalence of depression and anxiety [59,60]. It is then possible that sex differences in attentional network engagement during reward and punishment processing may in part underlie this increased vulnerability of women to develop these disorders. Further research is required to test this hypothesis.

There are several limitations of the current work that warrant discussion. First, the HCP dataset was collected to represent a normative sample of relatively young adults and thus there is some heterogeneity in the sample. For example, variables such as a family history of psychiatric/neurological disorders were not considered in the current study. Given the richness of the dataset there are many other variables that could have been taken into account, yet not overly restricting the dataset allows us to suggest the current findings represent sex differences within a general population. Nevertheless, future studies aimed at investigating other factors are warranted. In addition, though we found no difference in network activity between menstrual cycle phases, menstrual cycle was assessed by the HCP via self-report measures, which is an indirect measure of menstrual cycle phase and therefore subject to reporting errors. Given evidence of differences in attentional processing of negative stimuli across menstrual cycle phases [61], further investigation of the effect of menstrual cycle phase on network activation using a more direct assessment of menstrual cycle phase (i.e., hormone measurement) is warranted.

In conclusion, we found that attentional network engagement during the processing of reward and punishment is different in men and women. During exposure to reward and punishment, women showed increased suppression of the DMN, which modulates internally-oriented attention, and increased activation of the DAN, which modulates externally-oriented attention. Therefore, women show a pattern of neural activity that reflects enhanced attention to reward and punishment compared to men. Because of disturbances in reward function that are present in numerous sex-biased psychiatric disorders [54,55,62], a greater understanding of how sex influences reward and punishment processing may give insight into the etiology of these disorders.

Supporting information

S1 File. Supporting information.

Details regarding the definition of large-scale networks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199049.s001

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Blaise Frederick for his assistance with statistical analyses. Data were in part provided by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil).

References

  1. 1. Kujawa A, Carroll A, Mumper E, Mukherjee D, Kessel EM, Olino T, et al. A longitudinal examination of event-related potentials sensitive to monetary reward and loss feedback from late childhood to middle adolescence. Int J Psychophysiol. 2017 Nov 4.
  2. 2. Liu X, Hairston J, Schrier M, Fan J. Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci and Biobehav Rev. 2011 Apr;35(5):1219–36.
  3. 3. Must A, Szabó Z, Bódi N, Szász A, Janka Z, Kéri S. Sensitivity to reward and punishment and the prefrontal cortex in major depression. J Affect Disord. 2006 Feb;90(2–3):209–15. pmid:16412520
  4. 4. Sharma A, Wolf DH, Ciric R, Kable JW, Moore TM, Vandekar SN, et al. Common dimensional reward deficits across mood and psychotic disorders: A connectome-wide association study. Am J Psychiatry. 2017 Jul 1;174(7):657–66. pmid:28135847
  5. 5. Cross CP, Copping LT, Campbell A. Sex differences in impulsivity: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2011 Jan;137(1):97–130. pmid:21219058
  6. 6. Miettunen J, Veijola J, Lauronen E, Kantojärvi L, Joukamaa M. Sex differences in Cloninger’s temperament dimensions—a meta-analysis. Compr Psychiatry. 2007 Mar;48(2):161–9. pmid:17292707
  7. 7. Santesso DL, Dzyundzyak A, Segalowitz SJ. Age, sex and individual differences in punishment sensitivity: Factors influencing the feedback-related negativity. Psychophysiology. 2011 Jun 28;48(11):1481–9. pmid:21711354
  8. 8. Barman A, Richter S, Soch J, Deibele A, Richter A, Assmann A, et al. Gender-specific modulation of neural mechanisms underlying social reward processing by Autism Quotient. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015 Nov 3;10(11):1537–47. pmid:25944965
  9. 9. Spreckelmeyer KN, Krach S, Kohls G, Rademacher L, Irmak A, Konrad K, et al. Anticipation of monetary and social reward differently activates mesolimbic brain structures in men and women. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2009 May 26;4(2):158–65. pmid:19174537
  10. 10. Ding Y, Wang E, Zou Y, Song Y, Xiao X, Huang W, et al. Gender differences in reward and punishment for monetary and social feedback in children: An ERP study. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(3):e0174100. pmid:28346515
  11. 11. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002 Mar;3(3):201–15. pmid:11994752
  12. 12. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005 Jul 5;102(27):9673–8. pmid:15976020
  13. 13. Spreng RN, Stevens WD, Chamberlain JP, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL. Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. NeuroImage. 2010 Oct;53(1):303–17. pmid:20600998
  14. 14. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008 Apr 3;1124(1):1–38.
  15. 15. Andrews-Hanna JR, Smallwood J, Spreng RN. The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014 May;1316:29–52. pmid:24502540
  16. 16. Costumero V, Barrós-Loscertales A, Bustamante JC, Fuentes P, Rosell-Negre P, Ventura-Campos N, et al. A new window to understanding individual differences in reward sensitivity from attentional networks. Brain Struct Funct. 2015;220(3):1807–21. pmid:24696182
  17. 17. Krebs RM, Boehler CN, Roberts KC, Song AW, Woldorff MG. The involvement of the dopaminergic midbrain and cortico-striatal-thalamic circuits in the integration of reward prospect and attentional task demands. Cereb Cortex. 2012 Mar;22(3):607–15. pmid:21680848
  18. 18. McKiernan KA, Kaufman JN, Kucera-Thompson J, Binder JR. A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003 Apr 1;15(3):394–408. pmid:12729491
  19. 19. Vincent JL, Kahn I, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME, Buckner RL. Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2008 Dec;100(6):3328–42. pmid:18799601
  20. 20. Dumais KM, Franklin TR, Jagannathan K, Hager N, Gawrysiak M, Betts J, et al. Multi-site exploration of sex differences in brain reactivity to smoking cues: Consensus across sites and methodologies. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2017 Sep 1;178:469–76. pmid:28711813
  21. 21. Konova AB, Moeller SJ, Parvaz MA, Froböse MI, Alia-Klein N, Goldstein RZ. Converging effects of cocaine addiction and sex on neural responses to monetary rewards. Psychiatry Res. 2016 Feb 28;248:110–8.
  22. 22. Wetherill RR, Young KA, Jagannathan K, Shin J, OBrien CP, Childress AR, et al. The impact of sex on brain responses to smoking cues: a perfusion fMRI study. Biol Sex Differ. 2013 Apr 29;4(1):9. pmid:23628003
  23. 23. Domes G, Schulze L, Böttger M, Grossmann A, Hauenstein K, Wirtz PH, et al. The neural correlates of sex differences in emotional reactivity and emotion regulation. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(5):758–69. pmid:19957268
  24. 24. Van Essen DC, Ugurbil K, Auerbach E, Barch D, Behrens TEJ, Bucholz R, et al. The Human Connectome Project: A data acquisition perspective. NeuroImage. 2012 Oct;62(4):2222–31. pmid:22366334
  25. 25. Lithari C, Frantzidis CA, Papadelis C, Vivas AB, Klados MA, Kourtidou-Papadeli C, et al. Are females more responsive to emotional stimuli? A neurophysiological study across arousal and valence dimensions. Brain Topogr. 2010 Mar;23(1):27–40. pmid:20043199
  26. 26. Pfabigan DM, Lamplmayr-Kragl E, Pintzinger NM, Sailer U, Tran US. Sex differences in event-related potentials and attentional biases to emotional facial stimuli. Front Psychol. 2014;5:1477. pmid:25566151
  27. 27. Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Shulman GL. Neural systems for visual orienting and their relationships to spatial working memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002 Apr 1;14(3):508–23. pmid:11970810
  28. 28. Evans KL, Hampson E. Sex differences on prefrontally-dependent cognitive tasks. Brain Cogn. 2015 Feb;93:42–53. pmid:25528435
  29. 29. Li T, Luo Q, Gong H. Gender-specific hemodynamics in prefrontal cortex during a verbal working memory task by near-infrared spectroscopy. Behav Brain Res. 2010 May 1;209(1):148–53. pmid:20117145
  30. 30. Schmidt H, Jogia J, Fast K, Christodoulou T, Haldane M, Kumari V, et al. No gender differences in brain activation during the N-back task: An fMRI study in healthy individuals. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009 Nov;30(11):3609–15. pmid:19387979
  31. 31. Schoofs D, Pabst S, Brand M, Wolf OT. Working memory is differentially affected by stress in men and women. Behav Brain Res. 2013 Mar 15;241:144–53. pmid:23238042
  32. 32. Goldstein JM, Jerram M, Poldrack R, Anagnoson R, Breiter HC, Makris N, et al. Sex differences in prefrontal cortical brain activity during fMRI of auditory verbal working memory. Neuropsychology. 2005;19(4):509–19. pmid:16060826
  33. 33. Hill AC, Laird AR, Robinson JL. Gender differences in working memory networks: a BrainMap meta-analysis. Biol Psychol. 2014 Oct;102:18–29. pmid:25042764
  34. 34. Bucholz KK, Cadoret R, Cloninger CR, Dinwiddie SH, Hesselbrock VM, Nurnberger JI, et al. A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: a report on the reliability of the SSAGA. J Stud Alcohol. 1994 Mar;55(2):149–58. pmid:8189735
  35. 35. Delgado MR, Nystrom LE, Fissell C, Noll DC, Fiez JA. Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in the striatum. J Neurophysiol. 2000 Dec;84(6):3072–7. pmid:11110834
  36. 36. Barch DM, Burgess GC, Harms MP, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL, Corbetta M, et al. Function in the human connectome: task-fMRI and individual differences in behavior. NeuroImage. 2013 Oct 15;80:169–89. pmid:23684877
  37. 37. Ugurbil K, Xu J, Auerbach EJ, Moeller S, Vu AT, Duarte-Carvajalino JM, et al. Pushing spatial and temporal resolution for functional and diffusion MRI in the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage. 2013 Oct 15;80:80–104. pmid:23702417
  38. 38. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage. 2004 Jan;23:S208–19. pmid:15501092
  39. 39. Winkler AM, Webster MA, Vidaurre D, Nichols TE, Smith SM. Multi-level block permutation. NeuroImage. 2015 Dec;123:253–68. pmid:26074200
  40. 40. Glasser MF, Sotiropoulos SN, Wilson JA, Coalson TS, Fischl B, Andersson JL, et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage. 2013 Oct 15;80:105–24. pmid:23668970
  41. 41. Nickerson LD. Harnessing human connectome project data for replication studies: A demonstration for resting-state task network correspondence. 2018; Vol. bioRxiv http://doi.org/10.1101/111021.
  42. 42. Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001 Mar 27;98(7):4259–64. pmid:11259662
  43. 43. Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim ASN. The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009 Mar;21(3):489–510. pmid:18510452
  44. 44. Kinney KL, Boffa JW, Amir N. Gender difference in attentional bias toward negative and positive stimuli in generalized anxiety disorder. Behav Ther. 2017 May;48(3):277–84. pmid:28390492
  45. 45. Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008 May 8;58(3):306–24. pmid:18466742
  46. 46. Kroger JK, Sabb FW, Fales CL, Bookheimer SY, Cohen MS, Holyoak KJ. Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. Cerebral Cortex. 2002 May;12(5):477–85. pmid:11950765
  47. 47. Koechlin E, Basso G, Pietrini P, Panzer S, Grafman J. The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature. 1999 May 13;399(6732):148–51. pmid:10335843
  48. 48. Gruber O. Effects of domain-specific interference on brain activation associated with verbal working memory task performance. Cerebral Cortex. 2001 Nov;11(11):1047–55. pmid:11590114
  49. 49. Keller K, Menon V. Gender differences in the functional and structural neuroanatomy of mathematical cognition. NeuroImage. 2009 Aug;47(1):342–52. pmid:19376239
  50. 50. Wang Z, Jacobs RH, Marsh R, Horga G, Qiao J, Warner V, et al. Sex-specific neural activity when resolving cognitive interference in individuals with or without prior internalizing disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2016 Mar 30;249:76–83.
  51. 51. Harrison BJ, Pujol J, Contreras-Rodríguez O, Soriano-Mas C, López-Solà M, Deus J, et al. Task-induced deactivation from rest extends beyond the default mode brain network. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(7):e22964. pmid:21829564
  52. 52. Kessler RC, Merikangas KR, Wang PS. Prevalence, comorbidity, and service utilization for mood disorders in the United States at the beginning of the twenty-first century. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2007;3:137–58. pmid:17716051
  53. 53. McLean CP, Anderson ER. Brave men and timid women? A review of the gender differences in fear and anxiety. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009 Aug;29(6):496–505. pmid:19541399
  54. 54. Admon R, Pizzagalli DA. Dysfunctional reward processing in depression. Curr Opin Psychol. 2015 Aug 1;4:114–8. pmid:26258159
  55. 55. DeVido J, Jones M, Geraci M, Hollon N, Blair RJR, Pine DS, et al. Stimulus-reinforcement-based decision making and anxiety: impairment in generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) but not in generalized social phobia (GSP). Psychol Med. 2009 Jul;39(7):1153–61. pmid:19102795
  56. 56. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull. 2007 Jan;133(1):1–24. pmid:17201568
  57. 57. Dainer-Best J, Trujillo LT, Schnyer DM, Beevers CG. Sustained engagement of attention is associated with increased negative self-referent processing in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychol. 2017 Oct;129:231–41. pmid:28893596
  58. 58. Lu S, Xu J, Li M, Xue J, Lu X, Feng L, et al. Attentional bias scores in patients with depression and effects of age: a controlled, eye-tracking study. J Int Med Res. 2016 Nov 11;45(5):1518–27.
  59. 59. Leach LS, Christensen H, Mackinnon AJ, Windsor TD, Butterworth P. Gender differences in depression and anxiety across the adult lifespan: the role of psychosocial mediators. Soc Psychiat Epidemiol. 2008 Jun 23;43(12):983–98.
  60. 60. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Emotion regulation and psychopathology: the role of gender. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012;8:161–87. pmid:22035243
  61. 61. Lusk BR, Carr AR, Ranson VA, Felmingham KL. Women in the midluteal phase of the menstrual cycle have difficulty suppressing the processing of negative emotional stimuli: An event-related potential study. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2017 Aug;17(4):886–903. pmid:28585019
  62. 62. Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Tomasi D, Telang F, Baler R. Addiction: Decreased reward sensitivity and increased expectation sensitivity conspire to overwhelm the brain’s control circuit. Bioessays. 2010 Aug 17;32(9):748–55. pmid:20730946