Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Factors affecting availability for detection: An example using radio-collared Northern Bobwhite (Colinus virginianus)

  • Christopher M. Lituma ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    cml0017@mail.wvu.edu

    Current address: Division of Forestry and Natural Resources, West Virginia University, Morgantown, West Virginia, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Forestry, Wildlife and Fisheries, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee, United States of America

  • David A. Buehler,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Forestry, Wildlife and Fisheries, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee, United States of America

  • Evan P. Tanner,

    Roles Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Current address: Department of Natural Resource Ecology and Management, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, Oklahoma, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Forestry, Wildlife and Fisheries, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee, United States of America

  • Ashley M. Tanner,

    Roles Investigation, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Current address: Department of Natural Resource Ecology and Management, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, Oklahoma, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Forestry, Wildlife and Fisheries, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee, United States of America

  • Patrick D. Keyser,

    Roles Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Forestry, Wildlife and Fisheries, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee, United States of America

  • Craig A. Harper

    Roles Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Forestry, Wildlife and Fisheries, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee, United States of America

Abstract

Avian monitoring strategies are usually linked to bird singing or calling behavior. Individual availability for detection can change as a result of conspecific factors affecting bird behavior, though the magnitude of these effects is difficult to quantify. We evaluated behavioral and temporal factors affecting Northern Bobwhite (Colinus virginianus) breeding season individual availability for detection during three common survey times (3 min, 5 min, 10 min). We conducted 10-minute surveys associated with radio-collared male Northern Bobwhites on Peabody Wildlife Management Area, Kentucky, from 2010–2011. We homed to within 50 m of radio-collared males and recorded number of distinct Northern Bobwhite whistles (singing rate) per 1-minute interval, number of other males calling during the survey, minutes-since-sunrise, and day-of-season. We also recorded the number of minutes during a 10-minute survey that radio-collared male Northern Bobwhites called. We used logistic regression to estimate availability of radio-collared individuals for 3-minute, 5-minute, and 10-minute surveys. We also modeled number of minutes during 10-minute surveys that radio-collared Northern Bobwhites called, and we modeled singing rate. Individual availability for detection of radio-collared individuals during a 10-minute survey increased by 100% when at least 1 other Northern Bobwhite called during a survey (6.5% to 13.1%) and by 626% when 6 other Northern Bobwhites were calling (6.5% to 47.6%). Individual availability was 30% greater for 10-minute surveys than 5-minute surveys or 55% greater for 10-minute surveys than 3-minute surveys. Northern Bobwhite called most (2.8 ± 0.66 minutes/10-min survey) and at a greater rate (11.8 ± 1.3 calls/10-min period) when at least 5 other Northern Bobwhites called. Practitioners risk biasing population estimates low if individual availability is unaccounted for because species with low populations will not be stimulated by other calling males, are less likely to call, call less frequently, and call fewer times per minute, reducing their individual availability and likelihood to be counted on a survey even when they are present.

Introduction

Bird singing is a behavior used by many species to attract a mate [1], alert competitors to territorial boundaries [2], or to prevent extra pair copulations [3]. Bird songs are also the behavioral cue most frequently used by researchers for species identification in the field, and some species are only detected if they are singing [4]. As a result, detection probability becomes an integral analytical adjustment for any contemporary monitoring program or population assessment strategy [5].

The species detection process (P) is the product of three major components (P = pp × pa × pd): the probability that an individual bird associated with the sample area is present (pp), available for detection (i.e., calling, visible, etc.) during the survey period (pa), and the probability it is detected by an observer given it is present and available (pd) [6, 7]. Availability (pa) is the most difficult component of the detection process to assess directly, and can have a greater effect on population estimates than other factors, including observer ability [7, 8]. Distance sampling methods [9] generate estimates of pd, and removal sampling [10] and time-of-detection [11] methods estimate pa × pd. Logistic regression methods are confounded because pp × pa × pd are inseparable though occupancy models are capable of separating pp from detection probability pa × pd [5]. To estimate directly, factors affecting species availability for detection (pa), the species of interest must be present at a survey location (pp = 1), and it must be detectable within a reasonable distance, which can vary by species (pd = 1).

Additionally, variability in abundance during surveys can affect species detection probabilities as: Pd│a = 1 –(1 –r)N, where N = number of individuals in a sampling unit (abundance), and r is intrinsic (or individual) detection probability [12, 13]. In this case r is assumed to be constant because factors contributing to known heterogeneity in r related to individual behavior are difficult to quantify, though much attention has been paid to variables affecting species detection probability as a whole (time since sunrise, season, temperature, observer, species breeding condition etc.). Assuming r is constant, local abundance (N) will positively affect species detection probability because observers are more likely to hear a particular individual if more individuals are present and calling [12, 14, 15]. Density dependent effects such as changes in behavior in response to conspecific stimuli would cause variation in individual detection probability (r) and affect species detection probability, more specifically individual availability (ra), though this response is difficult to quantify directly [1619].

Effects of conspecifics on bird calling are well documented in the literature. Birds are known to increase singing rates or counter singing in response to conspecifics [2022], and practitioners have used conspecific playback to elicit bird responses during surveys [23, 24]. Additionally, birds can be attracted to new areas by using conspecific playback [25, 26]. Calling conspecifics affected per capita Golden-cheeked Warbler (Setophaga chrysoparia) song rates recorded with Autonomous Recording Units (ARU) and were correlated with spot-mapped densities, but direct evidence of increased singing rates would provide stronger evidence of density effects on individual availability for detection (ra) [22]. Similarly, heterogeneity in individual availability for detection (ra) can affect abundance estimation in an N-mixture modeling framework [13]. More specifically, density dependent changes in r are influenced by scenario-driven conspecific responses of individuals to other individuals calling [13].

We used data from radio-collared Northern Bobwhite (Colinus virginianus) breeding season surveys to determine how individual availability for detection (ra) is affected by intraspecific behavior and temporal covariates (minutes-since-sunrise and day-of-year). Radio-telemetry allows an observer to know unambiguously if a species is present or absent (pp = 1), and a species like Northern Bobwhite has a loud and obvious call so detection probability by an observer is close to 1 (pd ~1). Therefore we have the unique opportunity to isolate factors that may affect species availability (pa) because of known presence and assured observer detection [27]. In addition to Northern Bobwhite calls being very conspicuous and easy to detect, we chose this species because management recommendations are commonly based on results from breeding season surveys [2830] and could have implications on the conservation of non-target species [29]. Thus variability in ra of Northern Bobwhite could influence management recommendations and would impact other species [31].

Finally, Northern Bobwhite have experienced long-term range-wide population declines [32] and improvements on monitoring strategies would have important conservation implications for this species of concern. The National Bobwhite Conservation Initiative (NBCI) encourages establishment of Northern Bobwhite conservation focal areas throughout its range as a part of the Coordinated Implementation Program (CIP). The CIP uses spring breeding-season counts as a method to monitor success of habitat implementation [33]. It is important to understand how individual availability for detection (ra) could affect the interpretation of survey results, relating to effectiveness of habitat implementation.

Our objective was to use Northern Bobwhite breeding season surveys to examine how individual availability for detection (ra) is affected by conspecific cues and compare results across three common survey time lengths (3 min, 5 min, 10 min) while also accounting for time-of-day and day-of-season effects. Based on review of the literature, we hypothesized that Northern Bobwhite individual availability for detection (ra) would increase as the number of calling conspecifics during a count increased, would decline as minutes-since-sunrise increased, and would decline as the breeding season progressed. We also hypothesized that there would be diminishing returns of increased detection probability on observer effort and time invested for extending survey times. To our knowledge, this is the first time non-simulated data have been used to quantify individual availability for detection (ra).

Materials and methods

Study area

We conducted radio-telemetry surveys on Peabody Wildlife Management Area located in the Central Hardwoods Bird Conservation Region [34]. Peabody Wildlife Management Area is an 18,854-ha reclaimed surface mine managed by Kentucky Department of Fish and Wildlife Resources in Ohio and Muhlenberg counties, Kentucky. Kentucky Department of Fish and Wildlife Resources is the authority who issued the permission to conduct this research; Northern Bobwhite are not considered to be endangered or protected. Herbaceous cover established during reclamation was dominated by sericea lespedeza (Lespedeza cuneata), but also included big bluestem (Andropogon gerardii), little bluestem (Schizachyrium scoparium), Indiangrass (Sorghastrum nutans), and switchgrass (Panicum virgatum). Our focal area for surveys was a 3,321-ha unit comprised predominantly of mixed deciduous forest (22%), open herbaceous (36%), native warm-season grass (8%), and shrub (25%) cover types [35, 36].

Radio-telemetry surveys

We used telemetry surveys conducted on Peabody Wildlife Management Area in 2010 and 2011 to document male Northern Bobwhite breeding season availability for aural detection by point count surveys. We randomly selected from a sample of >50 radio-tagged male Northern Bobwhites for location and observation, which were part of an ongoing telemetry study at Peabody Wildlife Management Area [35]. We located the observation point by homing in [37] to within approximately 50 m of the target male. We chose this distance to assure that the radio-collared bird would be detected if it called. We used signal strength and direction to estimate the distance (m) and azimuth to the hidden transmitter on the selected bird and to aid in homing [35]. Once the observation point was established, we waited 1 minute to allow for the potential disturbance of our arrival on the target individual to abate. After this acclimation period, we used a time-of-detection survey [11] by recording the calling behavior of the target (telemetry-located) radio-collared male for ten 1-minute segments. We recorded the number of times each individual radio-collared male called per minute. We also recorded all other male Northern Bobwhites within audible range (~200 m) [38] that called during the survey, and each minute within ten 1-minute segments that they called. After the 5th minute, we relocated the target male to confirm the correct male was being monitored before resuming the call surveys for the remaining 5 minutes. We confirmed the final location of the target male and visually estimated the distance of the individual from the survey point when the survey was completed. We conducted surveys during all times of the day (sunrise until 17:07) from May 3–July 10, 2010 and from June 1–Aug 1, 2011. During the survey, we recorded the number of other Northern Bobwhites calling and aurally detected by the observer, the time-of-day (minutes-since-sunrise), and the date (day-of-season).

Analyses

We assessed the effects of behavioral and temporal covariates on individual availability for detection during a 10-minute survey, number of minutes during the 10-minute survey that Northern Bobwhites called, and the number of distinct Northern Bobwhite whistles during the 10-minute survey (singing rate). Because we recorded data as minute-specific observations, we also modeled individual availability for detection for two other commonly used point-count time lengths; 3 and 5 minutes. In this case, detection probability directly estimates the probability of individual availability to call (did the individual sing or not sing during the length of the count) for the radio-collared individual during a 3-minute, 5-minute, and 10-minute survey (ra). We estimated breeding season individual availability for detection (ra) using mixed-effects logistic regression in program R [39] via package lme4 [40] for 3-minute, 5-minute, and 10-minute intervals. We knew the individual was present (pp = 1) via radio telemetry and we assumed the observer would detect the individual if it called (pd ~ 1) because of the proximity (50 m) of the observer to the focal bird. We modeled the number of minutes during the 10-minute survey that Northern Bobwhite called using the same package in R, but assumed a Poisson distribution to describe the number of minutes (1–10 min). Last, singing rate during the 10-minute survey was modeled assuming a zero-inflated Poisson (ZIP) distribution because we were concerned about the amount of zeros in the data, and was implemented in package pscl [41] in program R. We treated bird ID as a random effect, to account for a potential lack of independence because some individual males had multiple point counts associated with them. We were unable to fit random effects as part of the singing rate ZIP analysis because models would not converge in package pscl. We evaluated the significance of random effects using a chi-square test to compare the top mixed-effects model with and without the random effects parameter. In all instances, we used package AICcmodavg [42] for model comparison with Akaike’s Information Criterion adjusted for small sample sizes (AICc).

For all analyses, we grouped surveys based on the year they were conducted (temporal), and included the number of other Northern Bobwhites calling (behavioral) at the time of the survey, minutes-since-sunrise (temporal), and day-of-season (temporal) as covariates. We considered each point count as an independent event because they were recorded on separate days for any given male. We quantified minutes-since-sunrise based on a 24-hr period, we considered May 3, 2010 as the first sampling day and as the start value of 0 for day-of-season. We did not include observer effects because we trained all observers for one week prior to allowing them to independently track via radio-telemetry and conduct point counts. Additionally, target birds were so close (~50 m) to observers that it would be very unlikely for an observer to not hear a target bird call.

We fitted a suite of 22 a priori models based on our specific objectives for evaluating availability for detection during 3-minute, 5-minute, and 10-minute surveys. We fitted a suite of 23 models to evaluate the number of minutes Northern Bobwhite called during a 10-minute survey, and we fitted a suite of 25 models to evaluate singing rate during a 10-minute survey. We included additive linear and quadratic models for each of the covariates and all combinations of the covariates. We included quadratic models because we suspected non-linear covariate relationships. We also included an interaction term for year × day-of-season because we suspected that our discrepancy in survey timing could affect individual availability for detection. We considered models with a ΔAICc ≤ 2 most competitive in explaining variability among our model set and given our data [43]. We assumed equal availability for detection among 1-minute intervals because intervals were equal in duration [10, 44]. All results are presented as means ± SE, and we used top-model mean covariate values to derive predictions from models; we did not model average. Rather than presenting complete model sets for all analyses, we only present the four most parsimonious models, the interaction model, and constant model.

We assumed population closure and no double-counting [11, 45] because radio-telemetry results suggested that Northern Bobwhite, on average, did not move significant distances (<7 m) during a 10-minute survey, and we relocated individuals immediately following each survey to ensure monitoring of the target individual. Northern Bobwhite movement during surveys was less than the recorded telemetry estimation error (9.33 ± 0.65 m) for 23 technicians [35], therefore “movement” during the survey could have been a product of human error.

Results

In 2010, 5 observers conducted 128 point count surveys and detected 258 unmarked Northern Bobwhite males associated with monitoring 32 radio-collared males, and in 2011, 6 observers conducted 148 point count surveys and detected 358 unmarked Northern Bobwhite males associated with monitoring 34 radio-collared males during 10-min surveys. The mean number of point count surveys associated with each male was 4.37 (± 0.39). We recorded calls on 109 point counts (39.5%) during 10-min survey periods. The farthest distance a radio-collared male moved during the 10-min survey period was 60 m ( = 6.2 ± 0.61 m, n = 276). On average, not including the radio-collared individual, observers detected 1.5 (± 0.09, n = 276) Northern Bobwhite males during 3-min surveys, 1.7 (± 0.1, n = 276) Northern Bobwhite males during 5-min surveys, and 2.2 (± 0.1, n = 276) Northern Bobwhite males during 10-min surveys. Mean minutes-since-sunrise during surveys was 214 min (± 7 min) and mean day-of-season that surveys were conducted was day June 16.

In all instances, individual availability for detection (3-minute, 5-minute, and 10-minute), the number of minutes with a call during a 10-minute survey, and singing rate during a 10-minute survey was greatest in 2010, had a positive quadratic relationship to the number of other Northern Bobwhites calling (ABUN), and a negative relationship to minutes-since-sunrise (MSS; Figs 1 and 2, Tables 13). Random effects were only significant for the number of minutes calling models (p < 0.05). Day-of-season (DOS) was only retained in top models for singing rate during a 10-minute survey (Table 1). Individual availability for detection of Northern Bobwhite was greater during a 10-minute survey (0.40 ± 0.05), than during a 5-minute survey (0.31 ± 0.05), or during a 3-minute survey (0.23 ± 0.0). Individual availability for detection during a 10-minute survey was 150% greater in 2010 (61% ± 6.8) than 2011 (24% ± 5.6), and in 2011 increased by 100% when at least 1 other Northern Bobwhite called during a survey (6.5% ± 2.6 to 13.1% ± 3.7) and by 626% when at least 6 other Northern Bobwhites were calling (6.5% ± 2.6 to 47.6% ± 12.6). The magnitude of these effects was similar, albeit less substantial in 2010, and for 3-minute and 5-minute surveys (Fig 1). On average Northern Bobwhite called during 1 minute of a 10-minute survey, and called most in 2010 when at least 5 other Northern Bobwhites were calling (2.8 ± 0.66 minutes/10-min survey) and at sunrise (3.3 ± 0.88 minutes/10-min survey; Fig 3). Similarly, during a 10-minute survey in 2010, Northern Bobwhite singing rate was greater when at least 5 other Northern Bobwhites were calling (11.8 ± 1.3 calls/10 min) and at sunrise (18.9 ± 2.9 calls/10 min; Fig 4). Northern Bobwhite singing rate was also affected by day-of-season, and peaked on June 12 (5.5 ± 0.83 calls/10 min; Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Calling conspecifics and individual availability.

Effects of calling conspecifics on radio-collared male Northern Bobwhite individual availability for detection (ra) with 95% confidence intervals, from top models for 3-minute (A), 5-minute (B), and 10-minute (C) surveys from 2010–2011, Peabody Wildlife Management Area, KY.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190376.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Minutes-since-sunrise and individual availability.

Effects of minutes-since-sunrise on radio-collared male Northern Bobwhite individual availability for detection (ra) with 95% confidence intervals, from top models for 3-minute (A), 5-minute (B), and 10-minute (C) surveys from 2010–2011, Peabody Wildlife Management Area, KY.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190376.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Number of minutes with a call.

Effects of calling conspecifics and minutes-since-sunrise on the number of minutes with a call during a 10-minute survey with 95% confidence intervals, of radio-collared male Northern Bobwhite from 2010–2011, Peabody Wildlife Management Area, KY.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190376.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Singing rate.

Effects of the number of calling conspecifics, minutes-since-sunrise, and day-of-season on the number of radio-collared male Northern Bobwhite singing rate with 95% confidence intervals, during a 10-minute survey from 2010–2011, Peabody Wildlife Management Area, KY.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190376.g004

thumbnail
Table 1. Model selection results.

Top competitive models of individual availability (3-minute, 5-minute, 10-minute), minutes with a call, and singing rate for radio-collared Northern Bobwhite males from 2010–2011, Peabody Wildlife Management Area, KY.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190376.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Beta values and standard errors of covariates included in top competitive models of individual availability (3-minute, 5-minute, 10-minute) for Northern Bobwhite males from 2010–2011, Peabody Wildlife Management Area, KY.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190376.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Beta values and standard errors of covariates included in top competitive models for minutes with a call, and singing rate for radio-collared Northern Bobwhite males from 2010–2011, Peabody Wildlife Management Area, KY.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190376.t003

Discussion

We defined a new parameter, individual availability for detection (ra), and used field-collected data to relate the effects of calling conspecifics to breeding individual availability for detection for a bird species, the Northern Bobwhite. The activity of calling conspecifics was correlated with individual availability for detection during a 10-minute count and varied significantly, ranging from <7% in 2011 in the absence of conspecific stimuli, to >80% in 2010 if 6 other Northern Bobwhites were calling during a survey (Fig 1). The number of Northern Bobwhite calls was also significantly positively affected by stimuli of conspecifics, which was likely the greatest contributing factor to the increase in individual availability for detection. Not only were Northern Bobwhite more likely to call at least once during a 10-minute count as a result of conspecific stimuli, but they were also calling during more minutes, and at a greater rate (Figs 3 and 4). Though we did not account for some environmental factors (temperature, barometric pressure, wind speed, predator density, etc.), or behavioral factors (pairing status, nesting status, etc.) we included temporal variables (time-of-day and day-of-season) in analyses, which typically have a greater effect on avian detection probability. Because our results are correlative, we recognize that other unforeseen or unmeasured variables could be causing these results to be spurious. Future studies could use playback in conjunction with radio-collared birds to experimentally affirm our results, and include additional variables.

Our Northern Bobwhite availability for detection during a 10-minute count (0.40) was less than that derived from other researchers using field data (~0.9) [7]. The reason for this discrepancy is likely because we directly estimated individual availability for detection through real-time telemetry-based calling surveys conducted on a large sample of individual males, across two years, while accounting for temporal and behavioral factors. Previously, a combination of double-observer and time-to-detection methodologies was used to quantitatively derive species availability estimates, rather than directly quantify individual availability for detection [7].

The concept of individual heterogeneity affecting availability for individual capture or individual detection has always complicated species population modeling [12, 46, 47]. Variability in individual song rate for birds is well documented, specifically relating to breeding condition, time-of-day, or day-of-season [21, 48]. These extrinsic factors are easily accounted for as part of the species detection process through inclusion of covariates. Intrinsic (individual) heterogeneity as part of the detection process is more difficult to account for, but can be indirectly modeled in N-mixture and occupancy models [12, 47]. If individual availability for detection is high (>0.5), or heterogeneity in individual availability for detection is random, then parameter estimates remain unbiased. However, if individual availability for detection is low (<0.2), non-random or directional, then it can produce biased parameter estimates [47]. When there is a positive density-dependent relationship on individual availability for detection, then estimated abundance will be biased low as a result of low individual availability, i.e. fewer individuals are singing and thus less likely to be detected during surveys.

Many species of birds respond positively to auditory cues of conspecifics [16, 49, 50]. This is especially true for bird species that use visual and auditory cues from conspecifics as indicators for mate selection and reproduction [51, 52]. Some species use cues from conspecifics as alarm behaviors in the presence of a threat or predator, to locate food or water resources, or to assess habitat quality [24, 53, 54]. These changes in behavior in response to conspecifics can be problematic in experimental survey designs, specifically assessments of species spatial distributions and species populations. For instance, low density areas of Golden-cheeked Warblers had lower detection probabilities [22]. Similarly, counter-singing rates of Black-throated Blue Warblers (Setophaga caerulescens) were lower in experimentally reduced density areas [55]. Male Eurasian Eagle Owls (Bubo bubo) used conspecific cues to dictate calling rate, and in low-density areas male calling rates were reduced [20].

Although Northern Bobwhite do not defend distinct territories per se, other researchers have commented on conspecifics positively affecting calling behavior in the fall and during the breeding season [56, 57]. In the fall, Northern Bobwhite use covey calls to maintain contact with surrounding coveys, and the rate of these calls is positively related to the number of other coveys calling [57]. Female Northern Bobwhite playback can elicit breeding male calling responses, but these responses are confounded by the uncontrolled presence of other males calling during the playback and impractical for non-playback surveys because females rarely vocalize [56]. Similarly, playback recordings of male Northern Bobwhite breeding vocalizations elicited increased calling rates [17], but results were confounded by the lack of experimental control for surrounding “real” males calling so the reported response could be misinterpreted as a result of playback rather than a response to other males. Also, individuals were not marked and could not be identified, so analyses could not produce estimates of individual availability for detection. Playback has been used to increase detection probabilities for a number of bird species [1619, 58, 59], but all of these studies have similar practical limitations on surveys where individuals are unknown and the design is not paired with controlled populations.

Annual variability in species populations is accepted as inherent in population parameters of interest (abundance, density, population size), and often accounted for through a detection probability. Unaccounted for variability in the observation processes, however, could bias estimates of annual change. We documented significant annual differences in individual availability for detection, minutes during which a Northern Bobwhite called, and singing rate of Northern Bobwhite. We can only speculate about why calling behavior changed so dramatically between years but the ramifications of this result are substantial. We hypothesize that the observed dramatic decline in individual availability for detection during 2011 was in response to noise created by a 13-year brood (Brood XIX) cicada (Maigcicada spp.) emergence which occurred during that year in west-central Kentucky [60]. Northern Bobwhite individuals could have been responding to the increased background noise associated with the cicadas and singing less [61], or individuals could have been exploiting an abundant food resource foraging for the cicadas, and spent less time singing [62]. We did not collect any formal data about cicada abundance influencing background noise levels, so our hypothesis is purely speculative and requires further examination.

Regardless of the cause for annual differences, the magnitude of this difference is larger than we expected. Implications are that actual changes in Northern Bobwhite populations over time could easily be masked by the annual variability in individual calling behavior. For instance, based on mean minutes-since-sunrise and mean number of other calling males, Northern Bobwhite individual availability for detection during a 10-minute survey was 61% in 2010 compared to 24% in 2011. This simple effect would translate into an apparent adjusted population size that is approximately 150% greater in 2011 than 2010. This difference in population size is only a reflection of a behavioral change, and does not necessarily reflect any changes in the actual underlying populations.

Conclusions

In recent years, Northern Bobwhite conservation has focused on identifying large areas of usable space where recovery efforts will be the most effective [63, 64]. These recovery efforts use data from breeding-season aural surveys to determine Northern Bobwhite densities or abundances, which are then used as a metric of restoration project success [65]. However, Northern Bobwhite in recovery areas with low populations will not be stimulated by other calling males, are less likely to call, call less frequently, and call fewer times per minute, reducing their individual availability and likelihood to be counted on a survey, biasing population estimates low if ra is not accounted for.

We recognize that the effects of conspecifics on individual availability for detection of Northern Bobwhite are not easily incorporated into routine point-count survey monitoring for many managers. Because Northern Bobwhite recovery projects rely on population density or abundance estimates as a metric of success, it may be necessary to account for this in the modeling process. One option for managers using breeding-season counts to assess population recovery or management effectiveness is to include an estimate of ra (Fig 1) in Pd│a = 1 –(1 –ra)N which corresponds to one of our commonly used survey times (3-min, 5-min, or 10-min). Alternatively, those tracking populations using estimates derived from N-mixture or occupancy models could account for positive density-dependent effects on ra by including our quadratic function in Pd│a = 1 –(1 –ra)N while estimating detection probabilities. Our results also suggest that another option for increasing Northern Bobwhite availability for detection is to extend survey times, though based on objectives, it may be better to conduct more surveys rather than longer ones. These conspecific effects likely affect other taxa, in particular birds. Additional studies using radio telemetry can be used to explicitly document the magnitude of effects [8], and then practitioners can weigh their importance.

Supporting information

S1 Dataset. Radio-collared Northern Bobwhite data.

These are raw detection histories and associated variables from radio-collared Northern Bobwhite individuals used for all analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190376.s001

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by the Kentucky Department of Fish and Wildlife Resources. We thank Eric Williams for his survey assistance on Peabody Wildlife Management Area, Kentucky, USA. We thank John Morgan and Ben Robinson of the Kentucky Department of Fish and Wildlife Resources for their unwavering support of this project. We thank Dr. Christopher T. Rota for his insightful review of the manuscript. We thank previous anonymous reviewers of this manuscript for their valued inputs. We also thank all of the dedicated work of the research technicians who conducted surveys and radio-tracked Northern Bobwhite. Trapping, banding, and handling protocols complied with the University of Tennessee Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee Permit 2042–0911.

References

  1. 1. McGregor PK. The singer and the song-on the receiving end of bird song. Biol Rev Cambridge Philosophic Soc. 1991;66(1):57–81.
  2. 2. Krebs J, Ashcroft R, Webber M. Song repertories and territory defence in Great Tit. Nature. 1978;271(5645):539–42.
  3. 3. Westneat DF, Stewart IRK. Extra-pair paternity in birds: causes, correlates, and conflict. Annual Review of Ecology Evolution and Systematics. 2003;34:365–96.
  4. 4. Brewster JP, Simons TR. Testing the importance of auditory detections in avian point counts. Journal of Field Ornithology. 2009;80(2):178–82.
  5. 5. MacKenzie DI, Nichols JD, Lachman GB, Droege S, Royle JA, Langtimm CA. Estimating site occupancy rates when detection probabilities are less than one. Ecology. 2002;83(8):2248–55.
  6. 6. Nichols JD, Thomas L, Conn PB. Inferences about landbird abundance from count cata: recent advances and future directions. Thomson DL, Cooch EG, Conroy MJ, editors. New York: Springer; 2009. 201–35 p.
  7. 7. Riddle JD, Stanislav SJ, Pollock KH, Moorman CE, Perkins FS. Separating components of the detection process with combined methods: an example with Northern Bobwhite. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2010;74(6):1319–25.
  8. 8. Diefenbach DR, Marshall MR, Mattice JA, Brauning DW. Incorporating availability for detection in estimates of bird abundance. Auk. 2007;124(1):96–106.
  9. 9. Buckland ST, Anderson DR, Burnham KP, Laake JL, Borchers DL, Thomas L. Introduction to distance sampling. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2001.
  10. 10. Farnsworth GL, Pollock KH, Nichols JD, Simons TR, Hines JE, Sauer JR. A removal model for estimating detection probabilities from point-count surveys. The Auk. 2002;119(2):414–25.
  11. 11. Alldredge MW, Pollock KH, Simons TR, Collazo JA, Shriner SA. Time-of-detection method for estimating abundance from point-count surveys. The Auk. 2007;124(2):653–64.
  12. 12. Royle JA, Dorazio RM. Hierarchical modeling and inference in ecology. The analysis of data from populations, metapopulations and communicites. London, UK: Academic Press; 2008.
  13. 13. Veech JA, Ott JR, Troy JR. Intrinsic heterogeneity in detection probability and its effect on N-mixture models. Methods Ecol Evol. 2016;7(9):1019–28.
  14. 14. McCarthy MA, Moore JL, Morris WK, Parris KM, Garrard GE, Vesk PA, et al. The influence of abundance on detectability. Oikos. 2013;122(5):717–26.
  15. 15. Royle JA, Nichols JD. Estimating abundance from repeated presence-absence data or point counts. Ecology. 2003;84(3):777–90.
  16. 16. Conway CJ, Nadeau CP. Effects of broadcasting conspecific and heterospecific calls on detection of marsh birds in North America. Wetlands. 2010;30(2):358–68.
  17. 17. Hansen HM, Guthery FS. Calling behavior of Bobwhite males and the call-count index. Wildlife Society Bulletin. 2001;29(1):145–52.
  18. 18. Haug EA, Didiuk AB. Use of recorded calls to detect Burrowing Owls. Journal of Field Ornithology. 1993;64(2):188–94.
  19. 19. Kennedy PL, Stahlecker DW. Responsiveness of nesting Northern Goshawks to taped broadcasts of 3 conspecific calls. Journal of Wildlife Management. 1993;57(2):249–57.
  20. 20. Penteriani V, Gallardo M, Cazassus H. Conspecific density biases passive auditory surveys. Journal of Field Ornithology. 2002;73(4):387–91.
  21. 21. Sexton K, Murphy MT, Redmond LJ, Dolan AC. Dawn song of Eastern Kingbirds: intrapopulation variability and sociobiological correlates. Behaviour. 2007;144:1273–95.
  22. 22. Warren CC, Veech JA, Weckerly FW, O'Donnell L, Ott JR. Detection heterogeneity and abundance estimation in populations of Golden-Cheeked Warblers (Setophaga chrysoparia). Auk. 2013;130(4):677–88.
  23. 23. McNeil DJ, Otto CRV, Roloff GJ. Using audio lures to improve Golden-Winged Warbler (Vermivora chrysoptera) detection during point-count surveys. Wildlife Society Bulletin. 2014;38(3):586–90.
  24. 24. Rae LF, Whitaker DM, Warkentin IG. Variable effect of playback of chickadee mobbing calls on detection probability of boreal forest birds. Journal of Field Ornithology. 2015;86(1):51–64.
  25. 25. Farrell SL, Morrison ML, Campomizzi A, Wilkins RN. Conspecific cues and breeding habitat selection in an endangered woodland warbler. J Anim Ecol. 2012;81(5):1056–64. pmid:22548648
  26. 26. Virzi T, Boulton RL, Davis MJ, Gilroy JJ, Lockwood JL. Effectiveness of artificial song playback on influencing the settlement decisions of an endangered resident grassland passerine. Condor. 2012;114(4):846–55.
  27. 27. Murray LD, Gates RJ, Spinola RM. Evaluation of three methods to estimate density and detectability from roadside point counts. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2011;75(5):1072–81.
  28. 28. Cram DS, Masters RE, Guthery FS, Engle DM, Montague WG. Northern Bobwhite population and habitat response to pine-grassland restoration. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2002;66(4):1031–9.
  29. 29. Crosby AD, Elmore RD, Leslie DM, Will RE. Looking beyond rare species as umbrella species: Northern Bobwhites (Colinus virginianus) and conservation of grassland and shrubland birds. Biological Conservation. 2015;186:233–40.
  30. 30. Palmer WE, Wellendorf SD, Gillis JR, Bromley PT. Effect of field borders and nest-predator reduction on abundance of Northern Bobwhites. Wildlife Society Bulletin. 2005;33(4):1398–405.
  31. 31. Gallo T, Pejchar L. Improving habitat for game animals has mixed consequences for biodiversity conservation. Biological Conservation. 2016;197:47–52.
  32. 32. Sauer JR, Hines JE, Fallon JE, Pardicek KL Jr,D JZ, Link WA. The North American Breeding Bird Survey, results and analysis 1966–2013. Laurel (MD): USGS Patuxent Wildlife Research Center; 2014.
  33. 33. Palmer, WE, Terhune TM, and McKenzie DF, editors. The National Bobwhite Conservation Initiative: a range-wide plan for recovering bobwhites. National Bobwhite Technical Committee Technical Publication, ver 20, Knoxville, TN. The National Bobwhite Technical Committee. 2011.
  34. 34. Commission for Environmental Cooperation. A proposed framework for delineating ecologically-based planning implementation, and evaluation units for cooperative bird conservation in the US. 1997:25.
  35. 35. Brooke JM, Peters DC, Unger AM, Tanner EP, Harper CA, Keyser PD, et al. Habitat manipulation influences Northern Bobwhite resource selection on a reclaimed surface mine. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2015;79(8):1264–76.
  36. 36. Peters DC, Brooke JM, Tanner EP, Unger AM, Keyser PD, Harper CA, et al. Impact of Experimental Habitat Manipulation on Northern Bobwhite Survival. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2015;79(4):605–17.
  37. 37. White GC, Garrot RA. Analysis of wildlife radio-tracking data. San Diego (CA): Academic Press, Inc.; 1990.
  38. 38. Lituma CM. Regional assessment of the relationships of conservation prectices to Northern Bobwhite and other priority grassland breeding bird populations [dissertation]. Knoxville (TN): The University of Tennessee; 2014.
  39. 39. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R foundation for statistical computing, Vienna, Austria. R Core Team. 2014.
  40. 40. Bates D, Machler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. 2015;67(1):1–48.
  41. 41. Zeileis A, Kleiber C, Jackman S. Regression models for count data in R. Journal of Statistical Software. 2008;27(8).
  42. 42. Mazerolle MJ. AICcmodavg: Model selection and multimodel inference based on (Q)AIC(c). R package version 20–4. 2016.
  43. 43. Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and multi-model inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. 2nd ed. New York, NY, USA: Springer-Verlag; 2002.
  44. 44. Otis DL, Burnham KP, White GC, Anderson DR. Statistical-inference from capture data on closed animal populations. Wildl Monogr. 1978(62):7–135.
  45. 45. Huggins RM. On the statistical-analysis of capture experiments. Biometrika. 1989;76(1):133–40.
  46. 46. Lebreton JD, Burnham KP, Clobert J, Anderson DR. Modeling survival and testing biological hypotheses using marked animals—a unified approach with case-studies. Ecological Monographs. 1992;62(1):67–118.
  47. 47. Veech JA. A comparison of landscapes occupied by increasing and decreasing poulations of grassland birds. Conservation Biology. 2006;20:1422–32. pmid:17002760
  48. 48. Alkon PU. Social behavior and organization of a native Chukar (Alectoris chukar cypriotes) population. Wilson J Ornithol. 2015;127(2):181–99.
  49. 49. Campomizzi AJ, Butcher JA, Farrell SL, Snelgrove AG, Collier BA, Gutzwiller KJ, et al. Conspecific attraction is a missing component in wildlife habitat modeling. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2008;72(1):331–6.
  50. 50. Conway CJ, Simon JC. Comparison of detection probability associated with Burrowing Owl survey methods. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2003;67(3):501–11.
  51. 51. Mariette MM, Griffith SC. Conspecific attraction and nest site selection in a nomadic species, the Zebra Finch. Oikos. 2012;121(6):823–34.
  52. 52. Vabishchevich AP. Dawn singing in pied flycatchers: mated males sing highly versatile songs in the early morning. Ann Zool Fenn. 2011;48(6):376–80.
  53. 53. Ahlering MA, Faaborg J. Avian habitat management meets conspecific attraction: if you build it, will they come? The Auk. 2006;123(2):301–12.
  54. 54. Finity L, Nocera JJ. Vocal and visual conspecific cues can influence the behavior of Chimney Swifts at provisioned habitat. The Condor. 2012;14(2):323–8.
  55. 55. Sillett TS, Rodenhouse NL, Holmes RT. Experimentally reducing neighbor density affects reproduction and behavior of a migratory songbird. Ecology. 2004;85(9):2467–77.
  56. 56. Duren KR, Buler JJ, Jones WL, Williams CK. Effects of broadcasting calls during surveys to estimate density and occupancy of Northern Bobwhite. Wildlife Society Bulletin. 2012;36:16–20.
  57. 57. Wellendorf SD, Palmer WE, Bromley PT. Estimating calling rates of Northern Bobwhite coveys and measuring abundance. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2004;68(3):672–82.
  58. 58. Cox JA, Jones SR. Use of recorded vocalizations in winter surveys of Bachman's Sparrows. Journal of Field Ornithology. 2004;75(4):359–63.
  59. 59. Mori E, Menchetti M, Ferretti F. Seasonal and environmental influences on the calling behaviour of Eurasian Scops Owls. Bird Study. 2014;61(2):277–81.
  60. 60. Marshall DC. Periodical cicada (Homoptera: Cicadidae) life-cycle variations, the historical emergence record, and the geographic stability of brood distributions. Ann Entomol Soc Am. 2001;94(3):386–99.
  61. 61. Stanley CQ, Walter MH, Venkatraman MX, Wilkinson GS. Insect noise avoidance in the dawn chorus of Neotropical birds. Anim Behav. 2016;112:255–65.
  62. 62. Koenig WD, Liebhold AM. Effects of periodical cicada emergences on abundance and synchrony of avian populations. Ecology. 2005;86(7):1873–82.
  63. 63. Twedt DJ, Wilson RR, Keister AS. Spatial models of Northern Bobwhite populations for conservation planning. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2007;71(6):1808–18.
  64. 64. Williams CK, Guthery FS, Applegate RD, Peterson MJ. The Northern Bobwhite decline: scaling our management for the twenty-first century. Wildlife Society Bulletin. 2004;32(3):861–9.
  65. 65. Evans KO, Martin JA, Terhune TM. Incorporating effective monitoring into the National bobwhite conservation Initiative In: Palmer WE, Terhune TM, McKenzie DF, editors. The National bobwhite Conservation Initiative: A Range-wide Plan for Recovering Bobwhites. Knoxville (TN): National Bobwhite Technical Committee, Technical Publications, ver 2.0; 2011. p. 191–204.