Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Pacing the phasing of leg and arm movements in breaststroke swimming to minimize intra-cyclic velocity fluctuations

  • Josje van Houwelingen ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    j.v.houwelingen@tue.nl

    Affiliation Department of Applied Physics, Eindhoven University of Technology and J.M. Burgers Centre for Fluid Dynamics, Postbus 513, 5600 MB Eindhoven, the Netherlands

  • Melvyn Roerdink,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Human Movement Sciences, Amsterdam Movement Science, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, van der Boechorststraat 9, 1081 BT Amsterdam, the Netherlands

  • Alja V. Huibers,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation

    Affiliation Department of Human Movement Sciences, Amsterdam Movement Science, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, van der Boechorststraat 9, 1081 BT Amsterdam, the Netherlands

  • Lotte L. W. Evers,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation

    Affiliation Department of Human Movement Sciences, Amsterdam Movement Science, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, van der Boechorststraat 9, 1081 BT Amsterdam, the Netherlands

  • Peter J. Beek

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Human Movement Sciences, Amsterdam Movement Science, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, van der Boechorststraat 9, 1081 BT Amsterdam, the Netherlands

Abstract

In swimming propelling efficiency is partly determined by intra-cyclic velocity fluctuations. The higher these fluctuations are at a given average swimming velocity, the less efficient is the propulsion. This study explored whether the leg-arm coordination (i.e. phase relation ϕ) within the breaststroke cycle can be influenced with acoustic pacing, and whether the so induced changes are accompanied by changes in intra-cyclic velocity fluctuations. Twenty-six participants were asked to couple their propulsive leg and arm movements to a double-tone metronome beat and to keep their average swimming velocity constant over trials. The metronome imposed five different phase relations ϕi (90, 135, 180, 225 and 270°) of leg-arm coordination. Swimmers adjusted their technique under the influence of the metronome, but failed to comply to the velocity requirement for ϕ = 90 and 135°. For imposed ϕ = 180, 225 and 270°, the intra-cyclic velocity fluctuations increased with increasing ϕ, while average swimming velocity did not differ. This suggests that acoustic pacing may be used to adjust ϕ and thereby performance of breaststroke swimming given the dependence of propelling efficiency on ϕ.

Introduction

Swimmers seek to improve their personal bests and set new records. Swimming more efficiently is one of the factors that may help to attain this goal. More efficient swimming implies converting a higher rate of power into forward speed, rather than into power losses. Efficiency may be studied in terms of the power balance: (1) where Ps represents the power generated by the swimmer, Pd represents the power losses to drag forces, and Pp represents the power losses related a moving point of push-off (i.e. kinematic losses to water set in motion). Pd can be divided in a component related to the average speed of a swimmer (effectively used power) and to a component related to the velocity fluctuations PΔv [1, 2]. In terms of Pd and drag force Fd (Pd = Fdvv2v) [2], velocity fluctuations are inefficient and should be suppressed. However, the propulsive actions in a stroke inevitably lead to velocity fluctuations. Previous studies concluded that at a certain speed higher velocity fluctuations resulted in a higher energy expenditure of the swimmer [1, 36]. Due to the sequential character of the leg and arm movements in the breaststroke (and butterfly), the velocity fluctuations are comparatively higher and the efficiency is lower than in other stroke types [1, 3, 7]. In this study, the focus will be on the breaststroke, because the velocity fluctuations are comparatively high and because of the clear sequential nature of the stroke. This implies that there might be more opportunities for improving swimming efficiency by reducing the velocity fluctuations, by changing the coordination between the legs and the arms for instance. In Fig 1 a typical velocity profile of a breaststroke cycle is shown.

thumbnail
Fig 1. An exemplary velocity profile of a breaststroke cycle.

The profile clearly exhibits intra-cyclic velocity variations, i.e. pronounced deviations from the average swimming velocity , as represented by the horizontal line. The characteristic points from Eqs 3 and 2 are indicated as local maxima and minima in the velocity profile. The profile was obtained by averaging 7 consecutive breaststroke cycles from one individual measured with automated LED tracking on a normalized time-scale. The standard deviation is indicated by the dashed lines, reflecting inter-cyclic variations. Note that in this trial the leg-arm phase coordination was around 210°.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186160.g001

The velocity profile is bi-modal; the increase to the first local maximum is related to the propulsive phase of the legs and the increase to the second maximum is related to the propulsive phase of the arms. The strong reduction in speed is caused by the leg and arm recovery phase, which is accompanied by large drag forces. The slight decrease of the velocity after leg propulsion is associated with the glide phase of the stroke in which the swimmer extends the arms and legs [6, 811]. The velocity fluctuations of the breaststroke will be quantified by the intra-cyclic velocity variability IVV [4, 10, 11], given by: (2) with vmax, L representing the maximal velocity after the leg propulsion, vmin, L the minimal velocity before leg propulsion, vmax, A the maximal velocity after arm propulsion, vmax, T the local minimum in the velocity profile between leg and arm propulsion and the mean velocity. These characteristic velocities are indicated in the velocity profile shown in Fig 1. Previous studies on breaststroke have shown that at a given speed higher IVV impose higher energy costs [4] and that the IVV was greater for low speed swimming conditions [11]. The IVV seem to be dependent on technique, since the IVV for undulating styles are smaller compared to a flat style, due to (de)acceleration of body parts above the water [12]. Moreover, it has been observed that the minimum hip velocity is higher for elite swimmers while the maximum velocity is similar, which suggests lower IVV [13]. However, it has also been found that the IVV of elite swimmers are higher, due to higher peak velocities needed to perform at higher speed and similar minimum velocities compared to non-elite swimmers [10].

The leg-arm coordination is an important performance determining factor. Previous studies have shown that the timing of the leg- and arm-propulsive phases, the glide phase and the recovery phase differs for different swimming levels [10, 13, 14] and events [9, 13]. For example, it has been reported that recreational swimmers do not employ a glide phase at all, while competitive swimmers can switch from a gliding coordination towards an overlapping coordination with increasing speed [11, 13, 14]. In this study the leg-arm phasing is quantified as the ratio of time spent between the onset of leg and arm propulsion tLA and the total cycle time ttot: (3)

The tLA is indicated in Fig 1, corresponding to a ϕ of ≈ 210°. It is assumed that the onset of leg and arm propulsion coincides with the local minima in the velocity profile [10] (see also the video in the supplementary material, S1 Movie).

It appears that there should be a relation between the relative phasing of the leg and arm movements and the IVV. It has already been suggested that optimizing the stroke cycle might be important for efficient breaststroke swimming [15]. The purpose of this study is to explore whether one’s preferred stroke cycle can be changed by a simple acoustic intervention and to examine the effects of the thus induced changes in coordination on the IVV. It is not yet evident which phasing between the legs and arms is optimal in breaststroke. Considering that low IVV are more efficient and that elite swimmers generally have lower IVV at specified speeds, determining the phase relation with the lowest IVV at a given speed, and training swimmers to coordinate their leg-arm coordination correspondingly, can help in optimizing the breaststroke performance. The modification of the phase relation of the leg and arm movements within the breaststroke might be achieved by a double-tone acoustic pacing signal. Inspired by more basic coordination dynamics studies examining acoustic pacing as a means to stabilize coordination [16, 17] and modulate the execution of cyclic movements [1820], acoustic pacing for guiding cyclic movements has been employed to stabilize cadence in running and to effectively modulate stepping frequency and stepping phase of walking [21, 22]. Acoustic pacing in swimming has already been used as an accurate method to pace swim speed, in the sense that sounds at regular time intervals had to coincide with the swimmer progressing a certain distance [23]. It has also been used to constrain the stroke rate, in order to stabilize and slightly change the motor organization of swimmers in time to exhaustion tests [24, 25]. Furthermore, adding constraints (by instruction), for example about the glide duration, seemed a fruitful way to access the coordination adaptability. Finally, it has been concluded that the self-selected glide pattern showed less IVV and energy costs [26, 27]. Whether acoustic pacing can be used to modify the phase relation between leg and arm movements is still unknown.

The research questions for this study were thus twofold, namely:

  • Can the phase relation between leg and arm movements within the breaststroke cycle be influenced using acoustic pacing while keeping the average swimming speed constant?
  • And, if so, how do the imposed changes in the leg-arm coordination affect the intra-cyclic velocity variability?

Method

Participants

Twelve female and fourteen male swimmers, competing at the Dutch regional level or higher over at least the past four years, volunteered to participate in this study. Participants were 20.0 ± 3.3 years of age (mean ± standard deviation), 177.9 ± 8.5 cm tall, weighed 71.0 ± 10.6 kg, had 10.9 ± 3.8 years of experience and practised 5.4 ± 4.1 hours per week. Their personal bests on the 50 m breaststroke were 40.8 ± 3.8 s for the women and 35.1 ± 3.4 s for the men. All participants were healthy and had no injury, because this could have influenced swimming technique. Prior to the experiment, participants gave their written informed consent. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Faculty of Behavioural and Movement Sciences of the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam.

Apparatus

The experiment was performed in the 50 m indoor training pool of the Tongelreep in Eindhoven, the Netherlands. During all trials, participants wore an underwater MP3-player (FINIS, Neptune) attached to their swimming goggle, transmitting the sound through bone conduction. Via this MP3-player a double-toned metronome beat was presented to prescribe the stroke frequency and the required phase relation (ϕ) between leg and arm movements. The beat for the onset of arm propulsion and onset of leg propulsion was identified by a high and low tone, respectively. The time interval between similarly pitched beeps was kept constant, corresponding to one’s preferred stroke frequency. However, the time interval between the high and low pitched beeps was varied to impose the following arm-leg phase relations: ϕ = 90, 135, 180, 225 and 270°. These double-toned metronome beeps were generated in Matlab (Mathworks release 2012a) for each individual swimmer.

Three cameras (Basler, sc1400gc, 50 fps, resolution: 788 × 524) positioned at 35, 40 and 45 m from the beginning of the lane, at a depth of 0.55 m in the side wall of the pool, were used for recording the swimmer’s motion in the sagittal plane (see Fig 2). The total recording range at the level of the swimmer (in the middle of the second lane from the side wall, ≈ 3.75 m) measured 17 m, which proved to be sufficient for capturing approximately seven swimming cycles.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Setup for velocity tracking of the LED marker.

Top view of the swimming pool with the synchronized cameras placed in the side wall of the pool. The cameras (at 35, 40 and 45 m) recorded the swimmer’s motion, using a LED marker on the hip.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186160.g002

A white LED light, placed at the swimmer’s hip, was used as a marker to track the horizontal and vertical position and instantaneous velocity of the swimmer. Although there is a difference between the velocity profiles of the hip and the true swimming velocity (measured at the center of gravity), the similarities for breaststroke are high and we therefore considered the hip velocity as a good estimate of the forward velocity profile [28, 29]. The position and velocity data were obtained using an automated tracking algorithm (in-house code in Matlab: developed by J. van Houwelingen, A.P.C. Holten (TU Eindhoven)). Sometimes there were indications of marker occlusions at the edge of the camera view. Due to overlapping views of the camera, the data were often not interrupted at relevant points. If data were interrupted, the affected stroke could be skipped during the analysis. Innosportlab de Tongelreep made available the calibration of the cameras for post-processing the data. On average 7 ± 2 stroke cycles were averaged to obtain the mean velocity profile. An example of an averaged velocity profile of a single participant obtained with this tracking algorithm was provided in Fig 1. As the application of the calibration required a selection of the vertical plane of interest parallel to the side of the pool (here chosen half-way the second lane at 3.75 m), deviations of the LED marker from this plane resulted in a small error in the velocity signal. However, since the IVV is a normalized quantity the effect on the final result is very limited.

A stopwatch (Elite sports) was used to measure lap times.

Protocol

Prior to the experiment, participants did an individual warming up in the water. After the warm-up participants were instructed to swim 50 m breaststroke at maximal pace. 70% of this maximal velocity was determined as the exertion level for the remainder of the experiment, a pace that is commonly used in technique training. As a baseline test, participants were instructed to swim 50 m breaststroke at 70% of their maximal velocity. The preferred phase relation (ϕp) and stroke frequency could be determined from this trial, as well as an indication for the lap time at the associated exertion level. The preferred stroke frequency was used for generating the metronome beats for the experimental trials. Next, the participants were instructed to practice the synchronization of the leg and arm movements to the metronome beats for two times 50 m (ϕi = 160 and 230°) at 70% of their maximal speed. Specifically, they were asked to synchronize their arm propulsion with the high tone and their leg propulsion with the low tone. These trials were excluded from the analysis.

For the experimental trials the participants were instructed to swim ten trials of 50 m breaststroke at a constant speed corresponding to 70% of the maximal velocity. Five different phase relations (ϕi), ranging from 90 to 270° in increments of 45°, were imposed in randomized order. Each ϕi was imposed twice in succession. The first trial associated with each pacing regime experienced was treated as a practice trial, which was not used in the analysis. The participants did not receive prior information about the ϕi during the trials. In the end, the participants were instructed to swim a final trial at 70% of their maximal velocity, without acoustic pacing.

Directly after each trial, the participants were asked to indicate how fatigued they felt, i.e. rate of perceived exertion (RPE), and how difficult the imposed phase relation (Dϕ) was, both assessed using a Borg scale [30]. Between trials they rested for a couple of minutes while they were informed about their lap time.

Data analysis

After capturing the position data in pixels for each camera using the tracking algorithm (see supplementary material for an exemplary video, S1 Movie), the data were converted to world coordinates in meters (m) using the calibration, differentiated to obtain the instantaneous velocity (in m/s) and combined into a single velocity array. Single stroke cycles were selected by manually selecting the area of characteristic peaks within the velocity profile. Peaks were then automatically picked using the local minimum. Stroke cycles were time-normalized in order to obtain a mean velocity profile per trial. The phase relation ϕ and the intra-cyclic velocity fluctuations IVV were determined from this mean velocity profile, by determining the characteristic points as indicated in Eqs (3) and (2) similar to the peak picking criteria for single stroke cycles. The intra-individual standard deviation σϕ was determined by taking the standard deviation over the ϕ over all strokes in a trial. The average velocity was calculated based on the position and time of the start of the first stroke cycle and finish of the last stroke within the camera view.

Statistical analysis

The effect of imposed phasing (5 levels: ϕi = 90, 135, 180, 225, 270°) on the mean executed phase relation (), the intra-individual standard deviation of the executed phase relation over the included swimming cycles (σϕ), intra-cyclic velocity fluctuations (IVV), mean swimming velocity (), rate of perceived exertion (RPE) and rate of perceive difficulty (Dϕ) was examined. All statistical tests were conducted in IBM SPSS statistics 23. Values are given as (inter-individual) mean ± standard deviation. The effects were considered significant at p < 0.05. The , σϕ, IVV and were examined using a 1 × 5 analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures. Effect sizes are represented as partial eta squared values () and were considered low, moderate and high for and 0.26 respectively. When the sphericity assumption was violated, the degrees of freedom were adjusted using the Greenhouse-Geisser adjustment (if ϵ < 0.75) or the Huyn-Feldt adjustment (if ϵ > 0.75). For analysing the pairwise comparison within different conditions a Bonferroni adjustment was applied. Because of their ordinal scale, the subjective ratings of the Borg scales (score 6–20) were examined using a non-parametric Friedman test. When the Borg scales varied significantly over the conditions, Wilcoxon-signed-rank tests were conducted to compare differences between conditions. A Bonferroni correction was applied to correct for the number of tests and for potential false-negative outcomes (p < 0.005). The outcomes of unpaced trials before and after the block of paced trials were compared with dependent t-tests.

Results

In Table 1 the statistical effects of the dependent variables are summarized.

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary of the statistical results.

On the left side the results of the ANOVAs and on the right side the results of the non-parametric Friedman tests executed on the dependent variables as function of the imposed phase relation ϕi.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186160.t001

In Table 2 the results of the comparison of the baseline and final trial are summarized.

thumbnail
Table 2. Results of the unpaced trials.

Mean values and standard deviations, as well as the outcome of the dependent t-test of the baseline and final trial are given.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186160.t002

All dependent variables were significantly affected by the imposed phase relation ϕi as shown in Table 1. The results of the dependent variables are presented later in this section.

In Fig 3a the means of the executed phase relation per ϕi are shown. The dashed horizontal line with shaded area shows the mean preferred phase relation resulting from the unpaced baseline trial. The dashed diagonal represents the line of identity, where ϕe equals ϕi. To illustrate the effect of imposing phase relations with acoustic pacing, Fig 3c shows the mean velocity profile of a participant for three conditions. The velocity profile is clearly affected by ϕi, and therewith tLA and ϕe.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Executed phase relation ϕe.

(a) Mean as a function of the imposed phase relation ϕi. The grey dashed lines are the line of identity and the mean preferred phase relation respectively. The shaded area represents the standard deviation from the . (b) Intra-individual standard deviation σϕ as a function of ϕi. Standard deviations are indicated by the vertical bars. (c) Typical mean velocity profiles for three different ϕi obtained from one participant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186160.g003

A post-hoc test showed all ’s differed significantly from each other, indicating a significant increase with ϕi. As can be appreciated from Fig 3a, the participants’ leg-arm coordination deviated from the imposed phase relations for all but ϕi = 180°, where was 182.4 ± 19.1°, with larger deviations for ϕi further away from 180°. Post-hoc analyses for the intra-individual standard deviation σϕ (a measure for the variation of ϕe over stroke cycles of an individual, see Fig 3b) revealed that σϕ was significantly larger for ϕi = 90° (14.3°, p < 0.05) compared to ϕi = 180°. Paired-samples t-tests for the two unpaced conditions showed that the preferred phase relation of the baseline trial (205.2 ± 27.0°) differed significantly from that of the final trial (195.9 ± 23.2°: t(25) = 4.065, p = 0.000).

Fig 4a shows the results of the mean velocity per ϕi. In the conditions with ϕi = 90° and 135° was significantly lower compared to the mean velocities in the other conditions p < 0.01 and p < 0.05, respectively, which were not significantly different from each other.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Effects of imposed phase relation ϕi on mean velocity (a), intra-cyclic velocity fluctuations IVV (b) rate of perceived exertion RPE (c) and rate of perceived difficulty Dϕ (d).

Vertical bars represent the standard deviation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186160.g004

In Fig 4b the results of the mean IVV are depicted. Post-hoc tests showed that IVV for ϕi = 225 and 270° were significantly higher than ϕi = 90° (p < 0.05 and p < 0.01, respectively), ϕi = 135° (p < 0.01 and p = 0.000, respectively) and ϕi = 180° (p = 0.000 and p = 0.000, respectively). All other pair-wise comparisons were not significant.

In Fig 4c the results of the rate of perceived exertion RPE, expressed in a mean Borg score, are shown. The post-hoc test showed that the RPE for the ϕi = 90° condition compared to 180° (p < 0.005) and 90° compared to 225° (p < 0.005) were significantly different from each other. Overall, the participants found the condition ϕi = 90° (12.12) the most exerting and the conditions ϕi = 180° (9.65) and 225° (9.96) the least exerting.

Lastly, the results of the rate of perceived difficulty Dϕ, expressed in a mean Borg score, are depicted in Fig 4d. Following the result of the post-hoc test, Dϕ for all conditions were significantly different from each other (p < 0.005), except for the comparisons between the conditions ϕi = 135° and 270°, between 180° and 225° and between 180° and 270°, indicative of a parabolic relation with a minimum close to ϕp. The participants found ϕi = 90° significantly more difficult (14.44) to perform than all other ϕi.

Discussion

The aim of this study was twofold. First of all, it was determined whether the participants were able to adjust their leg-arm coordination to acoustically imposed phase relations ϕi. Based on the results (Table 1 and Fig 3), it can be concluded that the executed phase relations were significantly affected by acoustic pacing, and that increased with increasing ϕi. Participants adhered to the ϕi = 180°, which was close to participants’ self-selected leg-arm coordination ϕp, as observed in the two unpaced trials.

However, participants did not succeed in reproducing imposed leg-arm coordinations other than 180°, presumably due to a competition between following the imposed pattern and maintaining the participant’s preferred leg-arm coordination. Since the latter type of coordination stems from the interplay between neural (e.g., different central pattern generators) and biomechanical constraints (e.g., different eigenfrequencies of leg and arm movements), it is understandable that such a competition may occur, especially when large deviations are imposed. This was indeed the case for ϕi = 90°, with an imposed pattern deviating most from the participant’s self-selected leg-arm coordination, for which σϕ was significantly larger suggesting less stable leg-arm coordination. Correspondingly, the participants’ rating of perceived difficulty Dϕ (Fig 4d) was significantly higher for ϕi = 90° than for all other conditions.

The second aim was to examine if imposed changes in leg-arm coordination affected intra-cyclic velocity fluctuations (IVV). Since IVV is dependent on the average swimming velocity [11], it is imperative for a proper comparison of the data that remains constant across conditions. This requirement was not met for ϕi = 90 and 135°, where was significantly lower than in the three other conditions (Fig 4a). Therefore, the interpretation of the effect of ϕ on IVV will be restricted to the ϕi = 180, 225 and 270° conditions. For these conditions, the IVV for ϕi = 180° was significantly lower than those for ϕi = 225 and 270° (Fig 4b). Based on the theoretical assumption that propelling efficiency might be enhanced by reducing IVV at a given swimming velocity, it can be concluded that swimming with a ϕ around 180° is optimal (i.e. among the examined conditions complying to the velocity requirement).

Note that in the present study the most efficient imposed phase relation was close to the participants’ self-selected ϕp observed in the unpaced trials. This is consistent with the findings of Seifert et al. [26, 27], who observed that the self-selected glide pattern resulted in the lowest IVV. Does this imply that our participants already attained the most efficient leg-arm coordination? Although the results of the ratings of perceived exertion and difficulty (Fig 4) point in that direction, the significant difference in self-selected leg-arm coordination between the baseline and final unpaced trials is worth mentioning. That is, at baseline, participants’ self-selected leg-arm coordination was 205.2°. After executing different acoustically imposed leg-arm coordination patterns, participants changed their self-selected leg-arm coordination to 195.9°, that is, closer to the ϕi = 180° for which we observed the lowest IVV. However, no significant effect was observed for , IVV and RPE) (Table 2). This immediate learning effect may be related to the concept of differential learning [31]. According to this notion, motor learning benefits from invoking larger than usual variations in task execution during practice allowing the actor to experience the differences among operating in different parts of the coordinative work space in order to help discover optimal solutions. Such variations may be brought about through task instructions or by manipulating organismic and/or environmental constraints (e.g., fatigue, or the surface or medium). Likewise, acoustic pacing of the leg-arm phasing may be exploited to expose the swimmer to different leg-arm coordination patterns, providing essential information to a swimmer about the manner in which the breaststroke can be performed best. Given the emphasis on drills in current swim training, it would be interesting to explore this possibility in greater detail in future work, along with imposed variations in frequency and speed.

In this study, a fairly wide range of ϕ was explored. For a follow-up study it is recommended to vary ϕi in the direct vicinity of ϕp and to use smaller increments of ϕi to determine the optimal ϕ. Moreover, it is recommended to pace the velocity (using visual feedback) to gain a better control over the velocity throughout the different trials. In the present study the swimmers were examined while swimming at 70% of their maximal velocity, a pace that is commonly used in technique training. Note that IVV are highly dependent on v [10, 11]. In contrast to previous studies [11], we found that the IVV were lower for slower velocities (in the ϕi = 90 and 135° conditions). This might be explained by the particulars of our study, in which a technique variation was acoustically imposed, whereas previous studies allowed participants to swim at different velocities without stroke constraints. It would be of interest to examine the effects of acoustic pacing on propelling efficiency when swimming at different paces (e.g. race pace). The same holds for the level of expertise of the swimmer.

The velocity profile is a result of the propulsive actions of the legs and arms. Throughout this study it is assumed that the onset of the propulsive actions of the legs and arms coincides with the minima indicated in Fig 1, on the basis of which we determined the executed leg-arm coordination (Eq 2). We did not quantify auditory-motor coordination [21], as this requires knowledge about beat onsets in relation to the velocity profile, which was not available since the measurement devices were not synchronized. Nevertheless, the key finding of our study stands, i.e. leg-arm coordination and thereby IVV can be readily modulated using acoustic pacing.

Conclusion

Within certain margins, breaststroke swimmers can adjust the phase relation between their leg and arm movements to an acoustically prescribed phase relation ϕ. It was not possible to draw a clear-cut conclusion about the optimal ϕ regarding intra-cyclic velocity fluctuations, because the average velocity varied significantly over pacing conditions, with slower velocities for imposed phase relations of 90 and 135°. The average swimming velocity did not differ for imposed phase relations of 180, 225 and 270°, with the former being the most efficient coordination among the studied patterns, yielding significantly lower IVV. Exposure to different leg-arm coordination with pacing resulted in a change in participants self-selected leg-arm coordination towards the most efficient pattern in a subsequent unpaced condition, suggesting that pacing the phasing may be a fruitful entry point for coaches in discovering optimal swimming techniques.

Supporting information

S1 Movie. An exemplary movie of the automated tracking algorithm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186160.s001

(MP4)

S1 File. Datasheet used for the statistical analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186160.s002

(SAV)

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by the STW (‘Stichting Technische Wetenschappen’) research program Sport (project #12868). Thanks go to Innosportlab de Tongelreep for providing the facilities for the experiments.

References

  1. 1. Barbosa TM, Bragada JA, Reis VM, Marinho DA, Carvalho C, Silva AJ. Energetics and biomechanics as determining factors of swimming performance: updating the state of the art. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2010;13(2):262–269. pmid:19409842
  2. 2. Toussaint HM, Beek PJ. Biomechanics of competitive front crawl swimming. Sports medicine. 1992;13(1):8–24. pmid:1553457
  3. 3. Nigg B. Selected methodology in biomechanics with respect to swimming. Biomechanics and Medicine in Swimming. 1983; p. 72–80.
  4. 4. Vilas-Boas JP. Speed fluctuations and energy cost of different breaststroke kinematic variables in national and elite male and female 100-m and 200-m breaststroke swimmers. Journal of Sports Sciences. 1996;18(6):421–443.
  5. 5. Barbosa TM, Keskinen KL, Fernandes R, Colaço P, Lima AB, Vilas-Boas JP. Energy cost and intracyclic variation of the velocity of the centre of mass in butterfly stroke. European Journal of Applied Physiology. 2005;93(5–6):519–523. pmid:15605282
  6. 6. Barbosa TM, Marinho DA, Costa MJ, Silva AJ. Biomechanics of competitive swimming strokes. InTech; 2011.
  7. 7. Barbosa TM, Fernandes RJ, Keskinen KL, Colaço P, Cardoso C, Silva J, et al. Evaluation of the energy expenditure in competitive swimming strokes. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 2006;27(11):894–899. pmid:16612740
  8. 8. Holmér I. Analysis of acceleration as a measure of swimming proficiency. Swimming III. 1979; p. 118–124.
  9. 9. Chollet D, Seifert L, Leblanc H, Boulesteix L, Carter M. Evaluation of arm-leg coordination in flat breaststroke. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 2004;25(7):486–495. pmid:15459828
  10. 10. Leblanc H, Seifert L, Trouny-Chollet C, Chollet D. Intra-cyclic distance per stroke phase, velocity fluctuations and acceleration time ratio of a breaststroker’s hip: A comparison between elite and nonelite swimmers at different race paces. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 2007;28:140–147. pmid:16835822
  11. 11. Komar J, Sanders RH, Chollet D, Seifert L. Do qualitative changes in inter-limb coordination lead to effectiveness of aquatic locomotion rather than efficiency. Journal of Applied Biomechanics. 2014;30(2):189–196. pmid:23878208
  12. 12. Colman V, Persyn U, Daly D, Stijnen V. A comparison of the intra-cyclic velocity variation in breaststroke swimmers with flat and undulating styles. Journal of Sports Sciences. 1998;16(7):653–665.
  13. 13. Takagi H, Sugimoto S, Nishijima N, Wilson B. Swimming: Differences in stroke phases, arm-leg coordination and velocity fluctuation due to event, gender and performance level in breaststroke. Sports Biomechanics. 2004;3(1):15–27.
  14. 14. Leblanc H, Seifert L, Chollet D. Arm-leg coordination in recreational and competitive breaststroke swimmers. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2009;12(3):352–356. pmid:18358780
  15. 15. Manley PK, Atha J. Intra-stroke velocity fluctuations in paced breaststroke swimming. Biomechanics and Medicine in Swimming Vi. 1992; p. 151–160.
  16. 16. Fink PW, Foo P, Jirsa VK, Kelso JAS. Local and global stabilization of coordination by sensory information. Experimental Brain Research. 2000;134:9–20. pmid:11026721
  17. 17. Kudo K, Park H, Kay BA, Turvey MT. Environmental coupling modulates the attractors of rhythmic coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2006;23(3):599–609.
  18. 18. Balasubramaniam R, Wing DA A M. Keeping with the beat: movement trajectories contribute to movement timing. Experimental Brain Research. 2004;159:129–134. pmid:15365663
  19. 19. Kelso JAS, DelColle JD, Schöner G. Action-perception as a pattern formation process. In: Attention and Performance XIII: Motor Representation and Control, edited by Jeannerod M., N.J. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1990. p. 139–169.
  20. 20. Roerdink M, Ridderikhoff A, Peper CE, Beek PJ. Informational and neuromuscular contributions to anchoring in rhythmic wrist cycling. Annuals of Biomedical Engineering. 2013;41(8):1726–1739.
  21. 21. Roerdink M, Bank PJM, Peper CLE, Beek PJ. Walking to the beat of different drums: Practical implications for the use of acoustic rhythms in gait rehabilitation. Gait & Posture. 2011;33(4):690–694.
  22. 22. Bood RJ, Nijssen M, van der Kamp J, Roerdink M. The power of auditory-motor synchronization in Sports: Enhancing running performance by coupling cadence with the right beats. PloS One. 2013;8(8):e70758. pmid:23951000
  23. 23. Thompson KG, MacLaren DPM, Lees A, Atkinson G. Accuracy of pacing during breaststroke swimming using a novel pacing device, the Aquapacer. Journal of Sports Sciences. 2002;20(7):537–546. pmid:12166880
  24. 24. Alberty MR, Potdevin FP, Dekerle J, Pelayo PP, Gorce P, Sidney MC. Changes in swimming technique during time to exhaustion at freely chosen and controlled stroke rates. Journal of Sports Sciences. 2008;26(11):1191–1200. pmid:18608844
  25. 25. Alberty MR, Potdevin FP, Dekerle J, Pelayo PP, Sidney MC. Effect of stroke rate reduction on swimming technique during paced exercise. The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research. 2011;25(2):392–397.
  26. 26. Seifert L, Komar J, Crettenand F, Dadashi F, Aminian K, Millet GP. Inter-limb coordination and energy cost in swimming. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2014;17(4):439–444. pmid:23932428
  27. 27. Seifert L, Komar J, Crettenand F, Millet GP. Coordination Pattern Adaptability: Energy Cost of Degenerate Behaviors. PloS One. 2014;9(9):e107839. pmid:25255016
  28. 28. Maglischo CW, Maglischo EW, Santos TR. The relationship between the forward velocity of the center of gravity and the forward velocity of the hip in the four competitive strokes. Journal of Swimming Research. 1987;3(2):11–17.
  29. 29. Costill DL, Lee G, D’Acquisto L. Video-computer assisted analysis of swimming technique. Journal of Swimming Research. 1987;3(2):5–9.
  30. 30. Borg G. Borg’s perceived exertion and pain scales. Human kinetics; 1998.
  31. 31. Schollhorn W, Beckmann H, Davids KW. Exploiting system fluctuations. Differential training in physical prevention and rehabilitation programs for health and exercise. Medicina (Kaunas). 2010;46(6):365–373.