Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Hypothesis driven single cell dual oscillator mathematical model of circadian rhythms

Abstract

Molecular mechanisms responsible for 24 h circadian oscillations, entrainment to external cues, encoding of day length and the time-of-day effects have been well studied experimentally. However, it is still debated from the molecular network point of view whether each cell in suprachiasmatic nuclei harbors two molecular oscillators, where one tracks dawn and the other tracks dusk activities. A single cell dual morning and evening oscillator was proposed by Daan et al., based on the molecular network that has two sets of similar non-redundant per1/cry1 and per2/cry2 circadian genes and each can independently maintain their endogenous oscillations. Understanding of dual oscillator dynamics in a single cell at molecular level may provide insight about the circadian mechanisms that encodes day length variations and its response to external zeitgebers. We present here a realistic dual oscillator model of circadian rhythms based on the series of hypotheses proposed by Daan et al., in which they conjectured that the circadian genes per1/cry1 track dawn while per2/cry2 tracks dusk and they together constitute the morning and evening oscillators (dual oscillator). Their hypothesis also provides explanations about the encoding of day length in terms of molecular mechanisms of per/cry expression. We frame a minimal mathematical model with the assumption that per1 acts a morning oscillator and per2 acts as an evening oscillator and to support and interpret this assumption we fit the model to the experimental data of per1/per2 circadian temporal dynamics, phase response curves (PRC's), and entrainment phenomena under various light-dark conditions. We also capture different patterns of splitting phenomena by coupling two single cell dual oscillators with neuropeptides vasoactive intestinal polypeptide (VIP) and arginine vasopressin (AVP) as the coupling agents and provide interpretation for the occurrence of splitting in terms of ME oscillators, though they are not required to explain the morning and evening oscillators. The proposed dual oscillator model based on Daan's hypothesis supports per1 and per2 playing the role of morning and evening oscillators respectively and this may be the first step towards the understanding of the core molecular mechanism responsible for encoding the day length.

Introduction

The circadian clock in the mammalian suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) is the master endogenous oscillator with a period close to 24 h oscillations that can persist even in the absence of external cues like light-dark (LD) cycles and temperatures. Maintaining a constant phase relationship with the external cues is important to keep the circadian clock synchronized with the local time. As a result, circadian clocks are flexible, adaptable and adjust its speed to the external zeitgeber. Alteration in circadian clock leads to neurological, metabolic, and mental disorders [1] and importantly, it is shown to adversely affect the cognitive functions like learning and memory [2]. With the advent of new biological techniques, the molecular mechanisms of gene expressions and the regulations responsible for the generation of circadian oscillations are well characterized and the time-of-day effects to the external cues are well studied in the models of fungal species Neurospora, fruit fly Drosophila, plant Arabidopsis thaliana and mammalian mice models [35]. Without any exceptions, circadian rhythms of all the species are tightly regulated by the interlinked multiple negative and positive feedback loops that guides circadian pacemakers to function under various conditions [6].

In mammals, the transcriptional-translation oscillator consists of positive and negative limbs with Bmal1, Clock genes and their proteins BMAL1 and CLOCK constitute the positive limbs. The CLOCK-BMAL1 heterodimer binds together to form a complex that positively regulates the negative limb genes per1/2/3, and cry1/2, and their protein products PER 1/2/3 and CRY1/2 [7] by binding to their E-box promoter regions [8]. The cytoplasmic proteins PER and CRY forms a heterodimer PER-CRY that translocates to the nucleus to repress their own transcription by binding to CLOCK-BMAL1 complex and thus establishing the negative feedback loop [9]. The orphan receptors REV-ERBα, and RORc represses and activates the transcription of Bmal1 respectively [10, 11]. There is also a positive feedback loop by which PER2 protein regulates Bmal1 positively [7, 12] by negatively regulating Rev-erbα [10] and this creates an asymmetry between per1 and per2 negative limbs.

One of the interesting problems that have attracted attention for a considerable period of time is to identify the molecular mechanisms responsible for encoding day length in the circadian pacemaker. It has been proposed that the encoding is done by the circadian pacemaker that consists of two distinct oscillators of which one is the morning (M) oscillator that locks on to dawn and controls the morning activities, while the other one is evening (E) oscillator that locks on to the dusk and controls the evening activities [1315]. The concept of ME oscillators was proposed a long time back when the hamster's locomotor activity under constant light splits into two distinct components [16] and the lesion in SCN abolished the two distinct components into a single locomotor activity [17]. Even though splitting has paved way to the concept ME oscillator, its occurrence is not due to the ME oscillator [18].

Inspired by the earlier works of Pittendrigh and Daan's dual oscillator model [13], Daan et al, [15] cogently put together the criteria for morning and evening oscillators in terms of the molecular mechanisms of circadian gene expression. We define here the genes responsible for the dual oscillator in single cell are per1/cry1 and per2/cry2 that can oscillate and function independently, yet they influence each other. The dual oscillator is also the ME oscillator in which we assume per1/cry1 act as M oscillator and per2/cry2 act as an E oscillator. We provide below the summary of Daan's hypotheses (H1-H5), which we verify by building coupled set of nonlinear differential equation model based on the molecular mechanisms of circadian gene expression.

(H1) The per1 peaks around early subjective day (CT 3–6 h) and per2 around the late subjective day (CT 10 h) with per1 leading per2. In the presence of light, per1/cry1 phase advances (accelerates), and hence they constitute M oscillator while per2/cry2 phase delays (decelerates) and thus constitutes the E oscillator. The peaking time of per1 and phase advances in the presence of light indicate that per1 may be the candidate for morning (M) oscillator, while the peaking time of per2 during the late subjective day and light induced phase delays indicates per2 may be the candidate for an evening (E) oscillator.

(H2) Under DD conditions, in comparison to wild type, the intrinsic period of both M and E oscillators is shorter. To translate this in terms of molecular mechanism, both per1 and per2 mutants under DD conditions should have a shorter period than the wild type. In the case of per1-per2 double mutant, oscillations are not possible and therefore, ME oscillator is dysfunctional.

(H3) Circadian pacemaker exhibits bidirectional phase response curve (PRC) under light pulse perturbation with per1 being primarily responsible for phase advances while per2 is responsible for phase delays. In other words, suppression of phase advance is expected to occur in per1 mutation while in per2 mutation, suppression of phase delay is expected to occur.

(H4) Under various light-dark (LD) conditions, the phase difference between M and E oscillator increases with increase in the photoperiod with M oscillator locks onto dawn and the E oscillator locks onto the dusk.

(H5) Under constant increasing light intensity (LL conditions), the period of circadian oscillation is expected to increase in per1 mutant mice (per2 limb intact) while in per2 mutant mice (per1 limb intact), the period is expected to decrease.

In the above hypothesis, we have not included the after effects of light. Besides these five hypotheses, we also explain the occurrence of splitting phenomena through coupling of the circadian oscillator models with neuropeptides as coupling agents. In free running wheel activity, during long days, phase separation between coupled oscillators increases and at certain time, the single rhythmic activity "splits" into two different bouts with the period of each bout is less than one circadian period. We explain these splitting phenomena in terms of molecular mechanisms of circadian gene expression.

Daan et al, [15] supported their hypothesis based on experimental data obtained from both wild and mutant types mice experimental data and these data are collated from various sources that includes phase response curves (PRC) [19], phase angle differences [12, 20], and entrainment curves under different light-dark conditions [21]. Therefore, the present aim of this work is to construct a mathematical model of circadian gene regulatory network (GRN) with the assumption that per1 and per2 will be taken as a candidate genes for morning and evening oscillators respectively and to verify whether the dynamics of per1/2 genes and PER1/2 proteins satisfy Daan's hypothesis H1-H5 to qualify as a dual ME oscillator in SCN.

Materials and methods

Mathematical model

Mathematical models for gene regulatory networks of circadian rhythms have been proposed for Neurospora [22, 23], Drosophila [2325] and mammals [2629] to understand the workings of circadian pacemaker under various conditions. However, till this date, there are hardly any attempts to understand the origin of ME dual oscillators from the point of view of GRN models. Previous models of ME oscillators [14, 30] are phenomenological in nature and are not based on the molecular basis of circadian oscillations and these models mostly accounted only for the splitting behavior. Existing models cannot be directly used because of various problems (see the supporting information (S1 Table) on the pros and cons of earlier models). So we develop a consensus model that is similar to most of the well known existing GRN models [2629] but in addition, we also include detailed per2 negative limb and add an explicit direct positive feedback loop between PER2 and Bmal1 in the network [7, 12]. We estimate the parameters of the model from the experimental data obtained from different sources.

In developing the mathematical model for circadian rhythms to explain Daan's hypothesis for the dual oscillator model, we made the following assumptions: We do not consider (i) cry genes separately because Okamura et al, [20] has shown that cry1/2 do not have distinguishable peaking rhythms as that of per1/2 (ii) the detailed phosphorylation reactions so that the number of variables are kept to a minimum (iii) the Rorc gene, as it is not a part of core circadian oscillators that explains dual oscillator hypothesis (iv) the clk genes in the model since the CLK protein levels are constant throughout the day [31]. Finally, we consider the direct influence of light on per1 and per2 expression and this is different from the customary introduction of light in the model through parameters that affects the transcription rates of per circadian genes [26].

The present mathematical model consists of (i) Hill's equation to describe the positive and negative regulations (ii) Michaelis-Menten equation to describe the degradation of mRNA's and proteins (iii) mass action kinetics to describe both the complexation and first order degradation reactions. In the model, we also consider the nuclear and cytosolic per1/2 and Bmal1 genes and their proteins separately. The terms that are used in the present model equations are similar to those of models [26, 28, 29], but it's different from the elaborate mass action kinetics model proposed by [27]. Therefore this can be considered as a consensus model. The variables Mp1, Mp2, P1c, P1n, P2c, P2n, Mb, Bc, BN, MR, R, PB1, and PB2, are per1 mRNA, per2 mRNA, PER1 protein (cytosol), PER1 protein (nucleus), PER2 protein (cytosol), PER2 protein (nucleus), Bmal1 mRNA, BMAL1 protein (cytosol), BMAL1 protein (nucleus), Rev-erbα mRNA, REV- ERBα protein, PER1—BMAL1 complex, and PER2 -BMAL1 complex, respectively. Rates are denoted by vsi (i = 1, 2…), Michaelis constants by kei's, degradation constants by kdi's, production and complexation rates by kpi's, m and w are Hill's coefficient to denote co-operativity, kai's denote activation constants, and kli denote inhibition constant. L is the light parameter, which when silenced (L = 0), is taken to be under DD conditions. There are overall 13 equations with 60 parameters. We estimate all the parameters for the wild type by genetic algorithms to fit the mRNA's and proteins got from the experiments. We also include the positive regulation of Bmal1 by PER2 protein that gives rise to the positive feedback loop [7,12]. This assumption is based on the experimental evidence performed invitro and invivo in NIH3T3 fibroblasts that PER2 coregulates Bmal1 by binding with the nuclear receptor PPARα [32]. However, presently there is no evidence that this happens in SCN and we only speculate this coregulation. This speculation is added as a new feedback in the model that was not hitherto considered in the earlier models of mammalian circadian rhythms. In summary, we assume that there is an additional indirect positive feedback between Bmal1 and PER2, where PER2 coactivates through a hitherto unknown nuclear receptor. We do not consider the indirect positive feedback of PER2 that regulates Rev-erbα, which in turn regulates Bmal1 through its protein REV-ERBα in the model. The biological network with interlinked feedback loops is shown in (Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. The Molecular network of circadian rhythms with feedback loops.

(A) The genes and RNA's are shown in italics, while the proteins and protein complexes are shown in capitals. The dynamical variables that are used in the model are given in the brackets. Light is the external zeitgeber. The protein BMAL1 positively regulates per1, per2, and Rev-erbα. per1 and per2 produce the corresponding proteins PER1 and PER2 and these proteins interact separately with BMAL1 to form a complex PER1-BMAL1 and PER2-BMAL1 to complete the negative feedback loop. Rev-erbα negatively regulates Bmal1 transcription to complete the second negative feedback loop. PER2 coregulates Bmal1 positively [32] and closes the only indirectpositive feedback loop. (B) Summary of the complete network shown in (A) that captures three negative and one positive feedback loops.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g001

The ODEs are (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) (11) (12) (13)

In the present work, CT is the circadian time with CT zero taken as the start of the subjective day under DD condition. ZT is the zeitgeber time with ZT zero taken as the start of the light phase under LD conditions. According to the convention proposed by Daan and Merrow [33], InT is the internal time given as [CT-18]mod 24 and ExT is the external time given as [ZT + half of the duration of the dark phase]mod24.

Results

Peaking times and phase relations of mRNA per's, Bmal1, and Rev-erbα under DD conditions, and the effects of light on late and early subjective night: Support of hypothesis-1 (H1)

Numerical simulation of the nonlinear model equations (Eqs 113) for the estimated parameters is shown in Fig 2A–2F. The period of free-running oscillations is 23.75 h, which is typical of mammalian circadian oscillator under constant darkness (DD) [13]. We take the light parameter L = 0 in the model to be the DD condition. In Table 1, we provide the simulated and experimental peaking times of all the clock components and they are in the expected range. To compare the phase relationships among the individual clock components, peaking time of per1 mRNA at CT 6 is taken as the reference under DD conditions. The mRNA's per1 and per2 show a reasonable phase difference in their peaking time as seen in the experiments [7]. PER1and PER2 proteins also maintain a right phase delay from their respective per1 and per2 mRNA's and their peaking time are also in good agreement with the reported experimental results [34]. In summary, our model fits the data well by maintaining the right temporal phase relationships among different clock components.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Free-running circadian oscillations under constant darkness and light pulse at certain circadian time.

Blue curves are from simulation, and red circles are the experimental data points. Simulation results were obtained by integrating the model equations (Eqs 113) with the estimated parameters, which are given in the S2 Table (parameters used for DD, LD). For comparison, the individual time series were normalized to maximum 1 and the minimum 0. Experimental data points of per1, per2, and Bmal1 mRNA were extracted from [7], Rev-erbα from [35] and PER1, PER2 protein from [34]. (G) per expression in response light. A 30 min light pulse with amplitude of 0.2 is applied at CT14 (early night), It shows a phase delay (green broken line). The same light pulse applied at CT 22 (late night), induce phase advance (green solid line), and agrees with the experimental results [36]. Inset shows perturbed time series came back into the original limit cycle with a delay ('D') or with an advance ('A') with respect to unperturbed time series.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g002

thumbnail
Table 1. Comparison of different peaking time of different clock variables in CT.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.t001

We also simulate the effects of light at CT14 (early subjective night) and at CT22 (late night). Albrecht et al, [36] applied light pulse at CT14 and at CT22 and found to induce phase delay and advances respectively. Similarly, in simulations, the light pulse applied at early subjective night (CT14) induces phase delay while at late subjective night (CT22) induces phase advance (Fig 2G). The model simulation captures the temporal dynamics and show proper phase differences between per1/2 mRNA's and their proteins PER1/2. Further, the model also captures phase acceleration and deceleration of light at the appropriate circadian time. This points to the fact that per1 may be the candidate gene for M oscillator and per2 for the E oscillator.

To consider further effects of light, we construct codimension-1 bifurcation diagram with light L as the bifurcation parameter (Fig 3A). For a very high value of L, the system remains in a stable steady state. As L decreases, the stable steady state becomes unstable through supercritical Hopf bifurcation, where the unstable steady state is surrounded by the stable limit cycle. Further decrease in L results in stable oscillations. However, the range of L for which the oscillations occur is very small. Unlike in many well-known models, we do not consider in our model the effects of light regulation through the parameters that affect the transcriptional production rate of per1, but as a stand-alone individual parameter that affects directly the rates of per1 and per2. With L as the bifurcation parameter, we obtain an inverted supercritical Hopf bifurcation (Fig 3A). We take L = 0 as the DD condition that oscillates with a period of 23.75 h and is taken as the reference period. For high L values, as observed in many circadian experiments [13, 38], the circadian oscillations are arrhythmic and this is observed in our simulations. For the range of L values that we obtain oscillations, the period of the oscillator varies marginally from 23.75 to 23.8 hrs (Fig 3B).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Bifurcation analysis with light L as the parameter.

(A) Oscillation amplitude is shown in green with L = 0 represents the DD conditions for the variable Mp1 (per1 mRNA). Black broken lines are unstable steady states. As light intensity increases, sustained oscillation disappeared via supercritical Hopf bifurcation (HB) and the system enters the stable steady state (red lines). (B) Period variation as a function of light intensity. Period increases very modestly with the increase in light intensity. We used Xppaut [39] for simulating bifurcation diagrams. Simulations are obtained by integrating the model equations (Eqs 113) with the estimated parameters, which are given in the S2 Table (parameters used for DD, LD simulation, WT).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g003

We also study the effect of positive feedback between PER2 and Bmal1 with parameter vs4 that contributes to the strength of feedback loop. We determine the bifurcation range and period variations with L as the bifurcation parameter for different vs4 values (Fig 4A). The increase in parameter vs4 increases the Hopf bifurcation regime and thereby also increases the period range with the increase of the light parameter in wild-type (Fig 4B). We also simulate the sensitivity of period to the variations of all the parameters and found that vs4 is highly sensitive as observed in simulations (S3 Fig).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Bifurcation analysis of the effect of Bmal1- PER2 positive feedback loop.

(A) Hopf bifurcation obtained for the range of vs4 that modulates the positive feedback loop. (B) The period of oscillation is shown to increase with the increase in vs4. per2 mutant mice showed period decrement in LL condition [38], here we observed that the positive regulation of Bmal1 mRNA by PER2 protein enhance the period of oscillation of the system under LL condition. Simulations are obtained by integrating the model equations (Eqs 113) with the estimated parameters, which are given in the S2 Table (parameters used for DD, LD simulation, WT).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g004

Dynamics of per1 and per2 mutants and per1-per2 double mutants (Hypothesis -H2)

In experiments, under DD conditions, the period of per1 mutant mice is shorter than the wild type [34, 38, 40], and in simulation we fit the experimental data [34] to capture this effect (Fig 5A–5C). We find that the per1 mutant for which per2 is functional, exhibits shorter period. In the case of per2 mutant, the dynamics of two different mutant phenotypes, namely per2Brdm1 and per2ldc are available in the literature and their dynamics are different. Under DD conditions, the oscillations are absent in per2Brdm1 mutant [12,34], whereas in the per2ldc mutant, rhythmic oscillations are observed [34, 40, 41]. We simulate the behavior of both per2Brdm1 (S1A Fig) and per2ldc mutants (Fig 5D–5F), but to account for hypothesis H2, we consider only per2ldc mutant that exhibits oscillations. The period of the per2ldc mutant in the simulation is 21.75 h and is shorter than the wild type (23.75 h), and this agrees well with the experiments [40, 34]. We also simulate per1-per2 double mutant by silencing the transcription rates of per1 and per2 mRNA (vs1 = 0, vs2 = 0). In experiments, per1- per2 mutant mice were found to be arrhythmic [34] and this behavior is accurately captured by our model (S1D Fig). According to hypothesis H2, this indicates that both morning and evening oscillators are dysfunctional. Besides per1/2 knockouts, our model also correctly captures (i) the arrhythmicity in Bmal1 mutation (S1B and S1C Fig)) as seen in the experiments in [42], (ii) a robust sustained oscillations of Rev-erbα as seen in the experiments [10].

thumbnail
Fig 5. Simulation of the rhythmic behavior of per1 and per2 mutant mice.

(A-C) are the simulations of per1 mutant. Sustained oscillation of per2 mRNA, PER2 protein, and Bmal1 mRNA are shown in blue lines. The simulated results are in good agreement with the experimental data (red circle) that are observed for per1ldc mutant mice [34]. (D-F) are the simulation shown for per2 mutant. Similar to per1 mutant, sustained oscillations of per1 mRNA, PER1 protein, and Bmal1 mRNA are observed in per2 mutant and are in good agreement with the experimental results that observed in per2ldc mutant mice [34]. For comparison, each time series were normalized between 0 and 1. For per1 mutant, parameter values adjusted are vs1 = 0 nMh-1, v4 = 0.43 nMh-1, and kp4 = 0.19 nMh-1. For per2 mutant, parameter values varied are vs1 = 0.7 nMh-1, vs2 = 0 nMh-1, vs3 = 4.5 nMh-1, vs5 = 0.5 nMh-1,v1 = 0.44 nMh-1, v2 = 1.38 nMh-1, v3 = 1.67 nMh-1,k1 = 1.44 h-1, kd3 = 0.08 h-1,kp1 = 0.11 nM-1h-1, and kp2 = 0.18 nM-1h-1. Remaining parameters are same as that of in Fig 2. per2 mRNA that peaks at CT8 is taken as the reference point in the case of per1 mutant, and PER1 protein that peaks at CT12 is taken as the reference point for per2 mutant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g005

In summary, our model accounts for most of the knockout phenotypes by capturing the rhythmic and the arrhythmic behaviors. In Table 2, we provide the summary of period changes seen in both simulations and experiments for wild type (WT) and the mutants. We also provide in Table 3, the peaking time of different molecular components in the mutants seen in both simulations and experiments. The shorter period in both per1 and per2 mutants under DD conditions and arrhythmicity in per1-per2 double mutant indicates that the model simulations support hypothesis H2. This also indicates per1 and per2 as the plausible candidates for M and E oscillator respectively.

thumbnail
Table 2. Period of molecular phenotype of mutants in mammalian SCN.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Peaking time of different oscillator components in mutants given in CT.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.t003

Phase response curves of wild and mutant types (Hypothesis-H3)

The circadian pacemaker in SCN responds differentially to light pulse at different phases of one circadian cycle. Application of light pulse during subjective day results mostly in dead zones, during early and middle of subjective night results in phase delay and during the late subjective night till the start of subjective day results in phase advance [41]. This selective bidirectional response of circadian pacemaker to light is mediated by multiple signaling pathways and it signifies that PRC depends on the time-of-day input and gated by different signaling pathways. Different scenarios on the effect of light on circadian rhythms are proposed: (i) Pittendrigh and Daan (1976) [13] postulated that the PRC of E oscillator has only phase delays while that of M oscillator exhibits only phase advances. (ii) On the other hand, Daan et al, [15] hypothesized that the PRC can be bidirectional in response to light and large phase delays will occur in the E-only oscillator, while large phase advance will occur in the M-only oscillator. These predictions are based on the observations made by Albrecht et al, [36] that in the per1 mutant, at ZT22 light pulse suppressed phase advance, whereas in the per2 mutant at ZT14, light pulse eliminated phase delay. (iii) Daan et al, [15] also proposed that under light pulse perturbation, phase advances of both per1 and per2 are suppressed during subjective day, but not during the early subjective day (see Fig 3A in [15]). Strong phase delays are possible only during the subjective night. Therefore, in this scenario, in comparison to WT, phase delays in per1 and per2 mutants are much larger and smaller respectively and this also encapsulates both scenarios (i) and (ii). We expect our model simulation to capture PRC of scenario (iii).

To simulate scenario (iii), we construct three PRC's, one for wild type, and the other two for per1 and per2 mutants separately. We apply light pulse for duration of 30 min at different phases of one circadian cycle. In the case of per2 mutant, we specifically consider per2ldc mutant that oscillates under DD conditions and not the arrhythmic per2Brdm1 mutant. We also project the PRC simulation curves on to the PRC of the experimental data constructed from the free-running activity of mice under normal and mutant conditions [41]. The experimental PRC curves of WT and mutants exhibits phase delays during the late subjective night, advances during the early subjective day. A large phase delay is seen between CT 15–19 (Fig 6E–6G). The only fact that distinguishes PRC of mutants from wild type is that in comparison to WT, phase delays are lesser in the per2 mutant, while in the per1 mutant, phase delays are larger. This is also expected in Daan's hypothesis (Fig 3A in [15])

thumbnail
Fig 6. Gating variables, phase response curves of wild type, per1, and per2 mutants with and without a gating variable.

In the left of the figure is shown the gating variable changes the light intensity at different phases. (A) Constant and varying gating variables are used for per1 (blue) and per2 (red) respectively. Gating variable for per1 is constant throughout the whole circadian period, whereas gating variable for per2 is changed with circadian time, and reaches the maximum value between CT15 and CT20. It is at this maximum value of gating variable, a maximum delay in PRC is observed experimentally [41]. (B) An example of single light pulse with unit amplitude applied at CT14 for 30 min duration is shown. (C) The light pulse for per1 and per2 after multiplying with the gating variable. Light pulse for per1 is scale down to half and the light pulse for per2 have different values at CT14. Simulated phase response curves without the gating variable for (E) wild- type (F) per1 mutant and (G) for per2 mutant. Experimental data points were extracted from [41] and they are shown in red circles and a continuous line was drawn for readability. The blue lines are simulated PRC curves. To simulate PRC, light pulse L in the model was applied for duration of 30 mins with an amplitude value 0.35, and phase difference is measured after 10 cycles. For WT,per1 mRNA (Mp1) peaking at CT 6 is taken as the reference point. per2 mRNA peaks at CT 8 is taken as the reference point when simulating per1 mutant, and PER1 protein (P1c) that peaks at CT 12 is taken as the reference point when simulating per2 mutant. For clarity, two horizontal lines are drawn; one to show the zero phases (in black) and the other magenta line in the bottom indicates the maximum phase delay of wild type. In all the experimental PRC, CT15 is the phase at which maximum delay occurs. (F) per1 mutant PRC for which the maximum phase shift occurs at CT 15 and it is much higher than the wild type. (G) per2 mutant PRC for which, the maximum phase shift occurs also at CT 15, but it is lower than the wild type. Simulated PRC with the gating variable for (H) wild- type (I) per1 mutant and (J) for per2 mutant. Compared to the PRC without a gating variable, per2 mutant shows suppressed phase delay. Parameters for WT are as in (Fig 2), and parameters for per1 mutants and per2 mutant are as in (Fig 5).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g006

Our model simulates bidirectional PRC as seen in the experiments, but WT and per2 mutant PRC show large phase delay than in the experiments. The maximum delay in the per1 mutant simulation is phase shifted by 2h from the experiments. To ameliorate this problem, we introduce two gating variables, one each for the light activation of per1 and per2. The idea of gating variables was previously used in the mammalian circadian models based on molecular mechanisms of gene expression [43, 44], and taking cue from there, we also similarly framed the gating expressions that act via the light variable L in per1 and per2 equations. The experimental results [45] show that photic inputs are phase gated that modifies the light responses at different phases of the oscillation. In our model, light L induces per1 and per2, and therefore we modulate this induction through the gating variables. In order to match with experimental data, we scaled down the light parameter L for per1 induction, and for per2, we made light parameter reach high during the early subjective night and low during both late subjective night and day. The mathematical expressions for gating variables are given below.

(14)(15)

In DD conditions, τ is the free-running period and in LD conditions it takes the entraining period of 24 h. The temporal dynamics of gating variables are shown in (Fig 6A). The gated PRC's are in good agreement with the experimental PRC's, but again the dead zones are not observed during subjective day (Fig 6H–6J). We modified again the gating variables and could simulate the dead zones during the subjective day besides the phase advances during the late subjective night and strong phase delays during subjective night (S2 Text, S2 Fig). These results are in agreement with Daan et al, [15] as well as Pittendrigh and Daan's [13] predictions where the dead zones, phase advances, and delays occurred at the appropriate circadian time for all the cases and we could capture the differential time-of-day effects for light pulse perturbations in the simulation. The hypothesis regarding PRC, experimental and simulated PRC results are summarized in Table 4, which strongly suggests per1 and per2 acts like M and E oscillator respectively.

Entrainment of wild type, per1 and per2 mutants to different photoperiods (Hypothesis -H4)

Entrainment of the endogenous circadian pacemaker to different external LD cycles (photoperiods) provides information about the encoding of day length. Specifically, external LD cycles of different ratios capture long or short days and endogenous circadian network should perceive and map this external cues. Therefore, circadian pacemakers should adapt and be plastic to entrain and encode variations in LD cycles. To evaluate the performance of our model, we consider different photoperiods for which we expect per1 tracks only dawn by peaking during or close to the light phase and per2 tracks dusk by peaking during the transition from light to dark phase.

To simulate different LD cycles, we modulate the light parameter L in the model as a square-wave function that goes from L = 0 during the dark phase to L = 0.1 during the light phase. Experimental results indicate that in wild-type, per1 and per2 mRNA levels in mammals rise up during the light phase and falls down during the dark phase [38, 4648]. However, the peaking time of per1 and per2 mRNA differs significantly under different photoperiods and their phase differences increase with increase in the photo periods [38, 46].

The simulation results of both wild and mutant types are shown in (Figs 79) for different photoperiods. In wild-type simulations (Fig 7A–7C), the rising portion of both per1 and per2 mRNA occurs during the light phase and starts to decrease during the dark phase as seen in the experiments [38, 4648]. Under short photoperiods, per1 peaking occurs near the light offset (Fig 7A), but as photoperiod increases, peaking occurs near the middle of the light phase (Fig 7A and 7B). On the other hand, peaking time of per2 mRNA, as observed in the experiments [38, 46], is always close to the transition between light and dark phases (dusk) (Fig 7A–7C). We also observe that, when photoperiod increases, our model show broadening of the maxima of both per1, per2 oscillations (Fig 7A–7C). In WT, under different photoperiods, the phase difference between per1 and per2 never went beyond 4 h (Table 5).

thumbnail
Fig 7. Wild type and mutant entrainment to different LD cycles with and without the gating variable.

(A-C) The model entrains to various external LD cycle without the gating variable and (D-F) with gating variable (Eqs 14 and 15, Fig 6A) for WT. Blue and red lines are the simulated per1 and per2 mRNA, whereas blue and red circles are the experimental per1, per2 mRNA respectively. Since an external cue forcing the oscillation, we consider the x axis as external time rather than circadian time. External time is defined as the middle of the light phase which in the present case is time 12. Model simulations of wild type show a maximal per1expression in the light phase whereas per2 gene peaks close to dusk (off set of the light phase) during the entrainment. In (G-L) the per1 mutant that expresses only per2, peaks following the dusk, and in (M-R), per2 mutant, per1 peak follow the dawn (on set of light phase).Under long photoperiods, per1 mutant with gating variable displays more delay in per2 peaking (L) than in without gating variable (I). Compared with WT, In per2 mutant, per1 peaks advanced both in without gating variable (M-O) and with gating variable (P-R), the magnitude of advance is more in with gating variable. Time series are normalized so that maximum value is 1 and minimum value is 0. L values changed in a square wave manner, during the light phase the value of L is 0.1 and in dark phase the value of L is 0. Experimental data points for LD 12:12 were extracted from [48], and all other data points were extracted from [38]. The dark bar in (M-R) is the dark phase, while the unfilled white bar is the light phase. This bar is also common for the entire figure and for clarity it is shown only in the last row. Parameters for WT are as in (Fig 2), and parameters for per1 mutant and per2 mutant are as in (Fig 5).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g007

thumbnail
Fig 8. Photoperiodic variation of the PER protein.

Experimental and simulated nuclear PER1 and PER2 protein under LD 8:16 (A), and LD 16:8 (B). Compared to the experimental data, phase difference between simulated PER1 and PER2 protein is higher. Experimental data points are extracted from [21]. Time series are normalized so that the maximum value is 1 and minimum value is 0.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g008

thumbnail
Fig 9. Peaking time of per1 and per2 mRNA under different photoperiods.

(A-D) Simulation without a gating variable. (A) In WT, under short photoperiod, per1 peaks near the dusk, photo periods increases its peaking time, shift towards the middle of the light phase. (B) per2 peaks near the light offset, follow the dawn. (C) In per2 mutant, per1 always peak near the middle of the light phase. (D) In per1 mutant, per2 follows dusk, as in the WT. (E-H) Simulation with gating variable. (E) In WT, per1 peaking time is shifted from middle of the light phase. (F) per2 peaks exactly follows the dusk. (G) In per2 mutant, per1 peaking time always follows the dawn or late night. (H) In per1 mutant, per2 peaking time follows the dusk. Taking together, it is clear that per1 is the part of M oscillator, follows the dawn and per2 is the part of E oscillator, follows the dusk.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g009

thumbnail
Table 5. Peaking time of per1 and per2 mRNA under different photoperiods.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.t005

To assess whether different photoperiods affects per1 and per2 mutants, we simulate mutants under different LD cycles. We observe that for different LD cycles, peaking time of per2 expression in per1 mutant show no significant variations in comparison to WT (Fig 7G–7I), and it peaks always at the transition between the light and dark phase. Similarly, for the per2 mutant, the peaking always occurs during the light phase, but we see phase advance of per1 gene expression in the per2 mutant, compared with WT. We also observe an increase in the phase difference between per1 and per2 peaking time with the increase in photoperiod (Table 5), therefore an uncoupling of per1 and per2 gene expression occurred in mutants.

We also simulate the entrainment curves with gating variable that is described in the previous section. We observe that in WT with gating variable, per1 and per2 peaking time is delayed under long days (Fig 7D–7F) than without the gating variable (Fig 7A–7C). Peaking time of per2 expression in the per1 mutant (Fig 7J–7L) did not show much variations in comparison to WT (Fig 7D–7F), but the peaking time of per1 expression in the per2 mutant is more advanced (Fig 7P–7R).

In the case of proteins, phase difference between the peaking time of PER1 and PER2 in the Siberian hamsters [21] is very small under different LD conditions (Fig 8, red and blue circles). However, peak duration of PER protein abundance increases with increase in the photoperiod. Based on these observations, Hastings et al, [18] indicated that Daan's hypothesis of per1/per2 may not be the right candidate for ME oscillators. However, we firmly believe that per1/per2 mRNA's and not their proteins PER1/PER2 may be the right candidate to look into as ME oscillators, because these proteins undergo various post-translational modifications as well as involve in diffusion process from cytoplasm to nucleus for regulation. So PER proteins may not be the right choice to explain ME oscillators. Further, Hastings et al, [18] have come to this conclusion from one case of photoperiod experiments [21]. We believe that PER protein peaking time may be different for different photoperiods and to verify this result, we simulate and plot the nuclear PER1(P1n) and PER2 (P2n) proteins time series for two different LD cycles (8:16 and 16:8) (Fig 8) and compare it with the experimental data points of Siberian hamsters [21]. Although the duration of PER1 peak increases with increase in photoperiods, our simulation results did not follow the experimental results. Besides LD cycle of 8:16 and 16:8, we also simulate for different photoperiods and found that the phase difference between PER1 and PER2 increases with increase in the photoperiods (S9 Fig). Our model simulation gives PER1 peaking time close to dusk and PER2 peaking close to midnight. There is a considerable phase difference in the peaking time of PER1 and PER2 in our simulation and this is different from what Hastings et al, [18] have proposed. Therefore, based on both mRNA and protein peaking time of per's our model supports Daan's hypothesis.

We also simulate the entrainment curves for the range of photoperiod (Fig 9). per1 peaking time in WT occur in the middle of the light phase under long photo period (Fig 9A), but for very short photoperiods our model fails to peak in the middle of light phase. In per2 mutant, per1 always follow the middle of the light phase (Fig 9C). In WT model, gating variable shifts the peaking time of per1 near to light offset, however it still peaks during the day time. However, in per2 mutant (Fig 9G), per1 peaking time follows the light onset rather than light offset. On the other hand, peaking time of per2 is near the light offset in both WT (Fig 9B) and in per1 mutant (Fig 9D). By using the gating variable, peaking time of per2 is close to dusk (Fig 9F and 9H). These simulation results clearly shows that per1 behaves like an M oscillator, and controlled by light onset (dawn), and per2 behaves more like an E oscillator controlled by the light offset (dusk).

Period length variations of wild type and mutants at constant light (Hypothesis-H5)

According to Aschoff's rule, for nocturnal animals, the period increases as a function of increasing LL, but the circadian activity decreases as a function of LL. The hypothesis is that under constant light conditions (LL) longer period is observed in the E oscillator than in the M oscillator [13, 14]. In per1 mutant mice, where per2 (E oscillator) is operational, it is known that the period increases with increase in light intensity [15]. In the case of per2 mutant mice, where per1 is operational (M oscillator), period decreases with increase in light intensity in comparison to DD [15]. The effect of light intensity on the period length under the constant light condition is shown in (Fig 10A) for the parameters that are used to fit the experimental data for DD and LD condition (as in Fig 7). The range of L values for which the period varies is very small and we didn't observe the trend seen in the experiments [38] in our simulations. To account for this effect in our model, we adjust the model parameters for both WT and mutants and the parameters are given in the S2 Table (parameters used for LL condition). Most of the parameters that we change to simulate the effects of constant light are the production and degradation rates of Bmal1, per1/2, and PER1/2. After the parameters are adjusted, the period increases with increase in light intensity in both wild-type and per1 mutant, and this follow Aschoff's rule [49]. In the case of per2 mutant, we observe a decrease in the period with the increase in light intensity (Fig 10B), and this did not follow the Aschoff's rule [15, 38, 50]. These results are in good agreement with the experimental results [38]. Taken together, we conclude that, under constant light condition, light accelerates per1 and hence period decreases (frequency increases) and in the case of per2, light decelerates and hence the period increases (frequency decreases). This simulation supports both hypothesis H1 and H5 for the effects of light and this points to the fact that per1 acts like an M oscillator, while per2 acts like an E oscillator.

thumbnail
Fig 10. Period length as a function of light intensity.

(A) Simulation results are shown for the period length variation with respect to light intensity under constant light condition for wild type (red dots), per1 mutant (black dots) and per2 mutants (green dots) with normal set of parameters (as in Fig 7). Oscillations are found only for the small range of values of L with the corresponding period variation also being very small. (B)) simulation results of period length variation with respect to light intensity under constant light condition with the modified set of parameters (given in the S2 Table, parameters used for LL condition). In wild type and per1 mutant, period increases with increase in light intensity and in per2 mutant, period decreases with increase in the light intensity, which agrees with the experimental results [38].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g010

Coupled oscillator model for splitting: Role of neuropeptides as coupling agents and construction of molecular actogram

Splitting in circadian rhythms is observed under constant light when single daily bout of locomotors activity exhibits two components, one during subjective day and the other during subjective night. It has been shown in hamsters that during splitting PER1 rhythms in core and shell regions of SCN are antiphase to each other [51]. These observations of splitting in experimental data paved way to ME oscillator concept, but it's argued that to explain ME concept splitting need not be invoked (See [18] for detailed discussion). We therefore explain splitting through coupled oscillator model within our framework of dual oscillator model by making assumptions that cluster of M oscillators are present in VL region, while cluster of E oscillators are present in DM regions and they are coupled by neuropeptides that controls the phase differences between the M and E oscillators. Many coupled oscillator models [14, 30, 5254] have been proposed to explain splitting phenomena, but they are all phenomenological in nature. For the single cell dual oscillator model that we described in earlier sections, the phase differences observed between per1 and per2 in WT under constant light condition (LL) was found to be very small and splitting was not seen in the simulations. Splitting appears to be a network property rather than that of a single oscillator. Therefore, taking cue from earlier works [14, 30, 5254], we built a coupled oscillator model with neuropeptides VIP and AVP as the coupling agents. We consider in our simulation ventrolateral (VL, core) and the dorsomedial (DM, shell) regions (Fig 11) and VIP is expressed in the neurons localized mostly in the VL part of SCN [55], where light is directly received [56]. This light information is transferred to the dorsomedial part [56], where AVP neurons are located [55].VIP is arrhythmic under DD condition but displays sustained circadian oscillations under LD conditions [57]. AVP oscillates under both DD and LD conditions [58]. The network of each single cell individual oscillator has the same structure described in the earlier sections except that the parameters are different in these two oscillators. We make a strong assumption that VL region has M oscillator that guides morning activities, since it is exposed to light while DM region harbors E oscillator that is not directly exposed to light and therefore guides evening activities. For M oscillator in VL region, we choose parameters for which per1 loop dominates based on hypothesis (H5), and here the period of the per1 oscillator decreases with increase in light intensity [15]. Again based on the hypothesis (H5), E oscillator in DM region, we choose parameters for which per2 loop dominates, and here the period increases with increase in light intensity [15]. Further, we assume that VIP and AVP are controlled byPER1/2 in VL and DM regions respectively and VIP induces the transcription of per1/2 mRNA in DM region while AVP induces per1/2 mRNA's in the VL region. The coupled oscillator model with full parameters is provided in the S4 Text and S2 Table.

thumbnail
Fig 11. Molecular network of coupled oscillator.

Coupled oscillator is constructed from two single cell dual oscillators. M oscillator is present in VL region that guides morning activities and E oscillator in DM region guides evening activities. These two oscillators respond to light differentially. Light accelerates M oscillator while light decelerates E oscillator. The two oscillators are coupled to each other by the neuropeptides AVP and VIP that acts as coupling agents for M and E oscillators. We assume here that VIP and AVP are controlled by PER1/2 in VL and DM regions respectively. Further, we assume that AVP induces per1 and per2 expression in the M oscillator, and VIP induces per1 and per2 expression in the E oscillator.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g011

To simulate splitting, we choose parameters for M (VL region) and E (DM region) oscillators that cycles with a period of 23.3 h and 23.4 h respectively under DD conditions. As it is known that period and phase of individual neurons in SCN maintain different period and phases [59], for simulation the periods of oscillators in VL and DM regions are taken differently. Light directly affects the VL region, and this light information is transferred to DM region through VIP (S4 Text Eqs 14 and 17). When VL and DM oscillators are coupled by the neuropeptides AVP and VIP, they synchronize to a common period of 23.6 h under DD conditions (Initial condition of VIP is considered as 0.1nM, and β = 0.01nM). We estimate the AVP and VIP model parameters by fitting to the experimental data of AVP and VIP [57, 58] under both DD and LD conditions (Fig 12). Like previous models [30,52], we generate different splitting patterns by varying the coupling strength under constant LL conditions with the assumption that the long exposure to light will gradually change both the coupling strengths and the expressions of both VIP and AVP neuropeptides. To construct the actogram based on molecular network simulation, we assume that PER1 protein from the VL region(P1nm) determines morning activities whereas the PER1 protein of the DM region(P1ne) determines the evening activities. We took PER1 protein in both VL and DM region to explain splitting because behaviorally split hamsters showed antiphase oscillations of PER1 expression in each side (core and shell) of the SCN [51]. It should be noted that presently there is no evidence to indicate a direct influence of per/PER on free running activities and this is only an assumption. To plot the actogram of molecular network model that is analogous to the actogram of free wheel running activity, we assume the normalized value of the variables P1nm or P1ne are above certain threshold value and these values are plotted as activities. The threshold value we define is 1.3 times the mean value of the normalized value of P1nm or P1ne.

thumbnail
Fig 12. Model fitting of AVP and VIP experimental data.

(A) Under DD condition only AVP shows circadian oscillation and VIP is arrhythmic. Red line indicates AVP simulation and red circle indicates AVP data from experiments [58]. (B) Under LD condition, shown in black dotted line, both VIP and AVP show sustained oscillations. Red and blue lines indicate AVP and VIP simulation respectively, while red and blue circles are the data from experiments AVP from [58] and VIP from [57] respectively. Time series are normalized with maximum and minimum as 1 and 0 respectively. Under DD condition, AVP mRNA that peaks at CT4 is taken as the reference point. Simulation results are obtained by integrating the equations given in the S4 Text (Eqs 1–28) with parameter set given in the S2 Table (ME oscillator).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g012

We reproduce many experimentally observed splitting patterns by changing the select parameters of the coupled model and one typical splitting pattern is shown in (Fig 13C) and the rest are shown in the supporting information (S7 Fig). In simulation, initially P1ne,the PER1 of evening oscillator (DM) phase leads over P1nm, the PER1 of the morning oscillator (VL) and the phase difference between PER1 of morning and evening oscillator is small (Fig 13A). After 30 days, when we decrease the coupling strength (vcm1) and production rate (kvs2) of AVP, we observe a big change in the phase difference and phase exchange takes place between P1ne and P1nm (Fig 13B). The model also fits the experimental data of PER1 [51] present in the core (VL) and shell (DM) regions fairly well. In experimentally observed actogram (Fig 2C in [13]), initially M and E activity components are in a fused state with E component leading over M component. After few days, when exposed to light, the two components split, and exchange of phase takes place with M component phase leads over E component. In our simulation, we capture this effect by changing the coupling parameters of the neuropeptides under constant light conditions. At the beginning of splitting, M oscillator has a shorter period than the E oscillator, after which both components maintains a stable phase relationship and oscillates with a same period. The simulated actogram (Fig 13C) is very much agrees well with the experimentally observed splitting pattern (Fig 2C in [13]).We also simulate different split and re-fused molecular actogram patterns that are observed in the free running wheel activity experiments by varying the coupling parameters and these patterns are shown in the supporting information (S7 Fig).

thumbnail
Fig 13. Stable split under LL condition.

(A) The time series of PER1 protein in VL region (P1nm) and DM region (P1ne) are shown in blue and red lines respectively under normal condition. Simulation results are obtained by integrating the equations given in the S4 Text (Eqs 1–28). Initially we set VIP and AVP coupling parametersto vcm1 = 0.35 nMh-1, vcm2 = 0.25 nMh-1, and the AVP and VIP production rate to kvs1 = 0.001 h-1, kvs2 = 0.015 h-1 for unsplit condition. Rest of the parameters is given in the S2 Table (ME oscillator).The phase difference between proteins PER1, P1nm of morning oscillator and P1ne of the evening oscillator, is very small (~30 min). After 30 days, we vary the coupling parameters to vcm1 = 0.28 nMh-1, vcm2 = 0.25 nMh-1, and the production rates AVP and VIP to kvs1 = 0.001h-1, kvs2 = 0.009 h-1. When parameters are varied, there is an increase in phase difference between P1nm (blue line) and P1ne (red line). (B) Blue circles are experimentally observed PER1 protein at the core region of SCN and red circles are experimentally observed PER1 protein at the shell region of SCN, at the time of splitting [51]. (C) Simulated actogram of the coupled oscillator model. In simulations, actogram is constructed when the normalized value of the variables P1nm or P1ne are above certain threshold value. The threshold we define here is 1.3 times the mean value of the normalized value of P1nm or P1ne. Blue horizontal lines represent M activities and red horizontal lines the E activities. Arrow on the right indicates the time at which parameter is varied. At the beginning of splitting, oscillator in VL region (M oscillator) has a shorter period than oscillator in DM region (E oscillator). After 30 days when parameters are varied, exchange of phase occurs between M and E components as seen in the experimental actogram (Fig 2C in [13]). Finally both the components maintained a stable phase relationship and oscillate with same period. Time series are normalized so that maximum value is 1 and minimum value is 0. We choose L = 0.02 for the simulation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.g013

In summary, we have shown that the splitting phenomena can be captured by coupling two individual ME dual oscillators. We believe that the splitting is caused by ME oscillators, but these oscillators are highly regiospecific, where in one that is present in VL region with per1/PER1 dominant has a signature of M oscillator while the other present in DM region with per2/PER2 dominant has a signature of E oscillator. As a proof of concept, we coupled these oscillators through neuropeptides that modulates these oscillators and for certain coupling parameters, splitting under constant LL conditions is achieved and anti-phase relationship is maintained between the two oscillators. We also showed that different splitting patterns can be obtained when the neuropeptides are modulated and these neuropeptides are themselves induced by constant light. Therefore, through simulations, we provided a speculative molecular interpretation for splitting. However, presently, we have taken a simplistic view of taking VIP and AVP as coupling agents to generate splitting patterns, and the contribution from other neurotransmitters like GABA, NMDA, and AMPAR are yet to be studied.

Discussion

In this work, we simulated the single cell dual oscillator model of mammalian circadian rhythms based on the hypothesis proposed by Daan et al, [15]. The hypothesis is strongly based on the collation of various experimental data that includes per1 and per2 peaking times, their PRC's, entrainment to different LD conditions, and importantly we also simulate the splitting behavior observed in the locomotor activities. The dual oscillator we considered in our work is that in a single cell, per1/cry1 and per2/cry2 are the two oscillators that can function independently, yet they influence each other. We also added a direct positive feedback from PER2 to Bmal1 based on the experimental evidence in NIH 3T3 cells and liver tissue [32] that PER2 coregulates Bmal1 through the nuclear receptor PPARα. However, in SCN this evidence is yet to be found. Due to lack of information, we added directly the Bmal1 regulation by PER2 and as we get new information, we need to refine this model. We have not included the indirect regulation of PER2 on Bmal1 through Rev-erbα, even though REV-ERBα regulation of Bmal1 is included. We are particularly interested in knowing the role of per1 and per2 in encoding day length. In the case of splitting, the coupled oscillator model that we considered is the two single-cell oscillators that interact through neuropeptides like VIP and AVP. The important contribution of this work is that we explained the entire hypothesis H1-H5 through the construction of realistic single cell mathematical model based on the known molecular mechanisms of mammalian circadian gene and protein regulatory networks. The present single cell mathematical model does satisfy many of the Daan's hypotheses (See S1 Table for comparison of our model with four well-known models). Based on our simulations, we find that per1 acts as an M oscillator, whereas per2 as the E oscillator. However, our simulations are based on single cell model and have not considered the network properties of SCN that harbors more than10000 neurons on each side of SCN. We have also not considered the role of other important genes like cry1/2 and Rorc in influencing per1/2 dynamics. We have also not simulated the aftereffects of light. In spite of these drawbacks, simulations of our minimal model construct points strongly that per1 and per2 act as ME oscillator and encodes day length.

It's also interesting to draw a comparison between mouse and drosophila experimentalworks for ME oscillators since many of the works on dual oscillators recently has been in drosophila where clearly two distinct peaks in activity rhythm have been observed [6063]. Helfrich-Förster et al, [60] have reviewed and compared dual oscillator model of mice and drosophila and in supporting information(S5 Table) we summarize their work and compare with our present mathematical model. Based on Helfrich-Förster et al, [60] work, it appears that presently it's too early to arrive at a consensus model of dual oscillator to describe ME activities atleast in mice and drosophila, and it's due to various factors like number of neurons and its anatomical location, differential light responses, the role of different neurotransmitters in different species, the wiring of molecular network etc. It will be interesting to model the molecular networks in drosophila for ME oscillator and compare with the mouse models for differences and similarities and the advantages/disadvantages that confer on drosophila with a fewer number of ME cells.

We also find that different splitting patterns cannot be obtained from single cell model and could simulate all the patterns only through the construction of coupled oscillator model with suitable changes in the parameters. Existing network models based on molecular interactions explain synchrony [64], and the encoding of seasonal variations [65], but these models have not separately considered the role of per1 and per2 in their network. It will be interesting to know whether the existing network models can explain the hypothesis if they are properly modified to include per1 and per2 separately. Presently it can only be speculated based on our simulations that per1 and per2 in a single cell act as an ME oscillator that tracks dawn and dusk respectively, but to counteract the seasonal and external variations, and noise, inter and intra cellular interactions of the population of oscillators in SCN may be necessary. In future, we intend to carry out simulations at a network level through coupling of multiple single-cell circadian oscillator models to verify Daan's hypothesis.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Validation of the model against the arrhythmic behavior of the mutants.

(A) Blue lines are the simulated per1 mRNA for the per2 arrhythmic mutant model (vs2 = 0 nMh-1). Red circles are the experimental data of per1 mRNA in per2Brdm mice [40]. (B) Blue lines are simulated per1 mRNA for the Bmal1 mutant model (vs3 = 0, vs4 = 0). Red circles are the experimental data of per1 mRNA in the Bmal1mutant mice [42]. (C) Blue lines are simulated per2 mRNA for the Bmal1 mutant model. Red circles are the experimental data of per2 mRNA in Bmal1 mutant mice [40]. (D) Blue lines are the simulated per1 mRNA for the per1-per2 double mutant model(vs1 = 0 nMh-1, vs2 = 0 nMh-1). Red circles are the experimental data of per1 mRNA in the per1-per2 double mutant mice [40]. For mutants, their transcription rate constants are made zero. Time series are normalized in such a way that maximum value is 1. Simulation results were obtained by integrating the model Eqs (113) in the main text. Parameters values, except transcription rate constants, are given in the S2 Table (parameters used for DD and LD).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s001

(TIF)

S2 Fig. PRC with a dead-zone.

(A-C) gating variable common to both per1 and per2 (for more details see S2 Text). Light input is regulated by a suitable clock variable, Bmal1 mRNA (MB). Simulated phase response curves with the gating for (D) wild- type (E) per1 mutant and (F) for per2 mutant. A dead zone is observed between CT3 to CT10 for WT, CT0 to CT10 for per1 mutant and CT2 to CT10 for per2 mutant. To simulate the PRC, light pulse L in the model was applied for a duration of 30 min with an amplitude value 0.2, and phase difference is measured after 10 cycle. The reference points for different phenotypes are similar to that of the previous PRC in the main text. Experimental data points extracted from [41] are shown in red circles and a continuous line was drawn for readability. The blue lines are simulated PRC curves from the model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s002

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Period sensitivity of the WT-DD-model parameters.

Period sensitivity of transcription rate (A), Michaelis constant (B), protein synthesis (C), enzymatic degradation (D), exponential degradation (E), activation constant (F), complex formation and dissociation (G), and Hill's coefficient (H). While considering transcription rate, Bmal1 loop shows higher sensitivity (vs3, vs4), and per2 loop slightly lesser (vs2). For all the other constants, the parameters of the per2 loop showed higher sensitivity (v4, k5, kd4, kp4, kp3, ka2, m). Red arrows indicate the parameters related to the positive feedback loop between Bmal1 and PER2 (vs4) which the period is shown to be highly sensitive to changes in the parameter.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s003

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Period sensitivity of the WT-LL model parameters.

Period sensitivity of transcription rate (A), Michaelis constant (B), protein synthesis (C), enzymatic degradation (D), exponential degradation (E), activation constant (F), complex formation and dissociation (G), and Hill's coefficient (H). While considering transcription rate, Bmal1 loop shows higher sensitivity (vs3, vs4), and per2 loop slightly lesser (vs2). Compared with DD parameter, sensitivity of per1 transcription rate (vs1) is higher than that of per2 (vs2). For all the other constants, the parameters of the per2 loop showed higher sensitivity (v4, k5, kd4, kp3, ka2, m). Red arrows indicate the parameters related to the positive feedback loop between Bmal1 and PER2 (vs4) which the period is shown to be highly sensitive to changes in the parameter.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s004

(TIF)

S5 Fig. Period sensitivity of the per1 mutant—LL model parameters.

Period sensitivity of transcription rate (A), Michaelis constant (B), protein synthesis (C), enzymatic degradation (D), exponential degradation (E), activation constant (F), complex formation and dissociation (G), and Hill's coefficient (H). Here all parameters related to per1 is completely insensitive. While considering transcription rate, Bmal1 loop shows higher sensitivity (vs3, vs4), and per2 loop slightly lesser (vs2). For all the other constants, the parameters of the per2 loop showed higher sensitivity (v4, k5, kd4, kp3, ka2, m). Red arrows indicate the parameters related to the positive feedback loop between Bmal1 and PER2 (vs4) which the period is shown to be highly sensitive to changes in the parameter.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s005

(TIF)

S6 Fig. Effect of Bmal1-PER2 positive feedback loop on period length variation.

(A) When vs4 increases, period of the per1 mutant also increase, but slope of the period variation with respect light intensity do not change much. vs4 for both WT and per1 mutant increased from 0.78 to 1(B), and 1.2 (C). In both the case period should increase, but the slope of increment with respect to light intensity remain the same. Since the per2 feedback loop is absent in per2 mutant, it doesn't show any change in the period variation. Except vs4, remaining parameters are LL parameter set, that provided in the S2 Table.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s006

(TIF)

S7 Fig. Split and re-fuse under LL.

(A) Simulated actogram of the model. Arrows indicate the point at which the parameter change occur. Initially set the parameters as vcm1 = 0.35 nMh-1,vcm2 = 0.25 nMh-1, kvs1 = 0.001h-1, kvs2 = 0.015 h-1 for unsplit condition, then changed the parameters (day 6-black arrow) to vcm1 = 0.01nMh-1, vcm2 = 0.25 nMh-1, kvs1 = 0.001 h-1, kvs2 = 0.01 h-1 for splitting. When splitting occurs M oscillate with a period lesser than E and finally M regains its original phase. Then we change the parameters to initial value for unsplit condition (day 44—green arrow). The simulated actogram is very much agrees well with the experimentally observed splitting pattern (Fig 2B in [13]). Here we proposed that splitting may be occur due to the transient change in the coupling strength between M and E oscillators, after the transient, the system will retains its original strength and re-fuse the splitting component. (B) Initially set the parameters as vcm1 = 0.35 nMh-1, vcm2 = 0.25 nMh-1, kvs1 = 0.001 h-1, kvs2 = 0.015 h-1 for unsplit condition, then changed the parameters (day 8-black arrow) to vcm1 = 0.28 nMh-1, vcm2 = 0.25 nMh-1, kvs1 = 0.001h-1, kvs2 = 0.009 h-1 for splitting and again change the parameter (day 41- green arrow) to vcm1 = 0.35 nMh-1, vcm2 = 0.25 nMh-1, kvs1 = 0.004h-1, kvs2 = 0.025h-1 for re-fuse. The simulated actogram is very much agrees well with the experimentally observed splitting pattern (Fig 2D in [13]).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s007

(TIF)

S8 Fig. Phase difference between P1nm and P1ne under parameter change.

(A) The coupling term vcm1 changed from a lower value to higher value under LL condition (L = 0.02). It is observed that at lower values of vcm1, phase difference between P1nm and P1ne is higher, splitting will happen. However, if the coupling strength increases, phase difference decreases, unsplit condition arises. (B) The similar result observed with the parameter kvs2, production rate of AVP. At higher values of kvs2, P1nm lag behind P1ne, and when kvs2 decreases, phase lead between P1ne and P1nm exchange and eventually splitting occurs. These results indicates that, at constant light condition some internal process take place that reduce the coupling strength between M and E oscillator, and that will lead to splitting behavior.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s008

(TIF)

S9 Fig. Peaking time of PER1 and PER2 protein under different photoperiods.

PER1 peaking time in WT (A) and per2 mutant (C) is near the light offset, as photo period increases it moves towards light phase. PER2 peaks near the midnight in both WT (B) and per1 mutant (D). The phase difference between PER1 and PER2 increases with increase in photo period.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s009

(TIF)

S1 Table. Comparison of new model predictions with experimental data and previous model predictions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s011

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Model parameters.

The model parameters to simulate wild type and mutants under both DD and LD conditions are provided in the excel file. The file contains two sheets. In the sheet named 'single cell DD and LL', the parameters all the estimated and modified parameters are provided. In the second sheet named 'ME oscillator', we provide all the parameters that are used to simulate splitting for ME oscillators. This also includes the parameter for coupling agents VIP and AVP.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s012

(XLSX)

S3 Table. Comparison of previous model predictions on splitting.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s013

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Comparison of ME oscillator in mammals and drosophila.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s015

(DOCX)

S2 Text. modified gating variable for dead zone in PRC.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s017

(DOCX)

S4 Text. Coupled oscillator model for morning and evening oscillators.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177197.s019

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

This work is supported by the DST cognitive neuroscience grant, SR/CSI/299/2012 from the Department of Science and Technology, INDIA.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: KS SS.
  2. Data curation: SS.
  3. Formal analysis: SS KS.
  4. Funding acquisition: KS.
  5. Investigation: SS KS.
  6. Methodology: SS KS.
  7. Project administration: KS.
  8. Resources: KS SS.
  9. Software: SS.
  10. Supervision: KS.
  11. Validation: SS KS.
  12. Visualization: SS KS.
  13. Writing – original draft: SS KS.
  14. Writing – review & editing: SS KS.

References

  1. 1. Maury E, Ramsey KM, Bass J. Circadian rhythms and metabolic syndrome. Circ Res. 2010 Feb 19;106(3):447–62. pmid:20167942
  2. 2. Ruby NF, Hwang CE, Wessells C, Fernandez F, Zhang P, Sapolsky R, et al. Hippocampal-dependent learning requires a functional circadian system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008 Oct 7;105(40):15593–8. pmid:18832172
  3. 3. Loros JJ, Dunlap JC. Genetic and molecular analysis of circadian rhythms in Neurospora. Annu Rev Physiol. 2001 Mar;63(1):757–94.
  4. 4. Young MW, Kay SA. Time zones: a comparative genetics of circadian clocks. Nature Rev Genet. 2001 Sep 1;2(9):702–15. pmid:11533719
  5. 5. Reppert SM, Weaver DR. Coordination of circadian timing in mammals. Nature. 2002 Aug 29;418(6901):935–41. pmid:12198538
  6. 6. Dunlap JC. Molecular bases for circadian clocks. Cell. 1999 Jan 22;96(2):271–90. pmid:9988221
  7. 7. Reppert SM, Weaver DR. Molecular analysis of mammalian circadian rhythms. Ann Rev Phys. 2001 Mar;63(1):647–76.
  8. 8. Gekakis N, Staknis D, Nguyen HB, Davis FC, Wilsbacher LD, King DP, et al. Role of the CLOCK protein in the mammalian circadian mechanism. Science. 1998 Jun 5;280(5369):1564–9. pmid:9616112
  9. 9. Lee C, Etchegaray JP, Cagampang FR, Loudon AS, Reppert SM. Posttranslational mechanisms regulate the mammalian circadian clock. Cell. 2001 Dec 28;107(7):855–67. pmid:11779462
  10. 10. Preitner N, Damiola F, Zakany J, Duboule D, Albrecht U, Schibler U. The orphan nuclear receptor REV-ERBα controls circadian transcription within the positive limb of the mammalian circadian oscillator. Cell. 2002 Jul 26;110(2):251–60. pmid:12150932
  11. 11. Akashi M, Takumi T. The orphan nuclear receptor RORα regulates circadian transcription of the mammalian core-clock Bmal1. Nat Struct Biol. 2005 May 1;12(5):441–8.
  12. 12. Shearman LP, Sriram S, Weaver DR, Maywood ES, Chaves I, Zheng B, et al. Interacting molecular loops in the mammalian circadian clock. Science. 2000 May 12;288(5468):1013–9. pmid:10807566
  13. 13. Pittendrigh CS, Daan S. A functional analysis of circadian pacemakers in nocturnal rodents. J Comp Physiol. 1976 Oct 1;106(3):333–355.
  14. 14. Daan S, Berde C. Two coupled oscillators: simulations of the circadian pacemaker in mammalian activity rhythms. J Theor Biol. 1978 Feb 6;70(3):297–313. pmid:633922
  15. 15. Daan S, Albrecht U, Van der Horst GT, Illnerova H, Roenneberg T, Wehr TA, et al. Assembling a clock for all seasons: are there M and E oscillators in the genes?. J Biol Rhythms. 2001 Apr;16(2):105–16. pmid:11302553
  16. 16. Pittendrigh CS. Circadian rhythms, space research and manned space flight. Life SCi Space Res. 1966 Dec;5:122–34.
  17. 17. Pickard GE, Turek FW. Splitting of the circadian rhythm of activity is abolished by unilateral lesions of the suprachiasmatic nuclei. Science. 1982 Feb 26;215(4536):1119–21. pmid:7063843
  18. 18. Hastings M. Modeling the molecular calendar. J Biol Rhythms. 2001 Apr;16(2):117–23. pmid:11302554
  19. 19. Illnerová H, Vaneček J. Dynamics of discrete entrainment of the circadian rhythm in the rat pineal N-acetyltransferase activity during transient cycles. J Biol Rhythms. 1987 Jun 1;2(2):95–108. pmid:2979657
  20. 20. Okamura H, Miyake S, Sumi Y, Yamaguchi S, Yasui A, Muijtjens M, et al. Photic induction of mPer1 and mPer2 in cry-deficient mice lacking a biological clock. Science. 1999 Dec 24;286(5449):2531–4. pmid:10617474
  21. 21. Nuesslein‐Hildesheim B, O'brien JA, Ebling FJ, Maywood ES, Hastings MH. The circadian cycle of mPER clock gene products in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of the Siberian hamster encodes both daily and seasonal time. Eur J Neurosci. 2000 Aug 1;12(8):2856–64. pmid:10971628
  22. 22. Ruoff P, Vinsjevik M, Monnerjahn C, Rensing L. The Goodwin model: simulating the effect of light pulses on the circadian sporulation rhythm of Neurospora crassa. J Theor Biol. 2001 Mar 7;209(1):29–42. pmid:11237568
  23. 23. Leloup JC, Gonze D, Goldbeter A. Limit cycle models for circadian rhythms based on transcriptional regulation in Drosophila and Neurospora. J Biol Rhythms. 1999 Dec 1;14(6):433–48. pmid:10643740
  24. 24. Kulasiri D, Xie Z. Modelling circadian rhythms in Drosophila and investigation of VRI and PDP1 feedback loops using a new mathematical model. Math Model Nat Phenom. 2008;3(2):1–26.
  25. 25. Ruoff P, Christensen MK, Sharma VK. PER/TIM-mediated amplification, gene dosage effects and temperature compensation in an interlocking-feedback loop model of the Drosophila circadian clock. J Theor Biol. 2005 Nov 7;237(1):41–57. pmid:15935389
  26. 26. Leloup JC, Goldbeter A. Toward a detailed computational model for the mammalian circadian clock. Proc Natl Acad SciU S A. 2003 Jun 10;100(12):7051–6.
  27. 27. Forger DB, Peskin CS. A detailed predictive model of the mammalian circadian clock. Proc Natl Acad.Sci U S A. 2003 Dec 9;100(25):14806–11. pmid:14657377
  28. 28. Becker-Weimann S, Wolf J, Herzel H, Kramer A. Modeling feedback loops of the mammalian circadian oscillator. Biophys J. 2004 Nov 30;87(5):3023–34. pmid:15347590
  29. 29. Mirsky HP, Liu AC, Welsh DK, Kay SA, Doyle FJ. A model of the cell-autonomous mammalian circadian clock. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009 Jul 7;106(27):11107–12. pmid:19549830
  30. 30. Oda GA, Friesen WO. A model for “splitting” of running-wheel activity in hamsters. J Biol Rhythms. 2002 Feb;17(1):76–88. pmid:11837951
  31. 31. Maywood ES, O'brien JA, Hastings MH. Expression of mCLOCK and other circadian clock‐relevant proteins in the mouse suprachiasmatic nuclei. J Neuroendocrino. 2003 Apr 1;15(4):329–34.
  32. 32. Schmutz I, Ripperger JA, Baeriswyl-Aebischer S, Albrecht U. The mammalian clock component PERIOD2 coordinates circadian output by interaction with nuclear receptors. Genes Dev. 2010 Feb 15;24(4):345–57. pmid:20159955
  33. 33. Daan S, Merrow M. External time internal time. J Biol Rhythms. 2002 Apr; 17(2):107–9. pmid:12002157
  34. 34. Bae K, Jin X, Maywood ES, Hastings MH, Reppert SM, Weaver DR. Differential functions of mPer1, mPer2, and mPer3 in the SCN circadian clock. Neuron. 2001 May 31;30(2):525–36. pmid:11395012
  35. 35. Punia S, Rumery KK, Elizabeth AY, Lambert CM, Notkins AL, Weaver DR. Disruption of gene expression rhythms in mice lacking secretory vesicle proteins IA-2 and IA-2β. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2012 Sep 15;303(6):E762–76. pmid:22785238
  36. 36. Albrecht U, Zheng B, Larkin D, Sun ZS, Lee CC. MPer1 and mper2 are essential for normal resetting of the circadian clock. J Biol Rhythms. 2001 Apr;16(2):100–4. pmid:11302552
  37. 37. DeWoskin D, Geng W, Stinchcombe AR, Forger DB. It is not the parts, but how they interact that determines the behaviour of circadian rhythms across scales and organisms. Interface Focus. 2014 Jun 6;4(3):20130076. pmid:24904739
  38. 38. Steinlechner S, Jacobmeier B, Scherbarth F, Dernbach H, Kruse F, Albrecht U. Robust circadian rhythmicity of Per1 and Per2 mutant mice in constant light, and dynamics of Per1 and Per2 gene expression under long and short photoperiods. J Biol Rhythms. 2002 Jun;17(3):202–9. pmid:12054191
  39. 39. Ermentrout B. Simulating, analyzing, and animating dynamical systems: a guide to XPPAUT for researchers and students. Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics; 2002 Jan 1.
  40. 40. Zheng B, Albrecht U, Kaasik K, Sage M, Lu W, Vaishnav S, et al. Nonredundant roles of the mPer1 and mPer2 genes in the mammalian circadian clock. Cell. 2001 Jun 1;105(5):683–94. pmid:11389837
  41. 41. Pendergast JS, Friday RC, Yamazaki S. Photic entrainment of period mutant mice is predicted from their phase response curves. J Neurosci. 2010 Sep 8;30(36):12179–84. pmid:20826680
  42. 42. Bunger MK, Wilsbacher LD, Moran SM, Clendenin C, Radcliffe LA, Hogenesch JB, et al. Mop3 is an essential component of the master circadian pacemaker in mammals. Cell. 2000 Dec 22;103(7):1009–17. pmid:11163178
  43. 43. Geier F, Becker-Weimann S, Kramer A, Herzel H. Entrainment in a model of the mammalian circadian oscillator. J Biol Rhythms. 2005 Feb 1;20(1):83–93. pmid:15654073
  44. 44. Forger DB, Peskin CS. Model based conjectures on mammalian clock controversies. J Theor Biol. 2004 Oct 21;230(4):533–9. pmid:15363674
  45. 45. Hughes AT, Fahey B, Cutler DJ, Coogan AN, Piggins HD. Aberrant gating of photic input to the suprachiasmatic circadian pacemaker of mice lacking the VPAC2 receptor. J Neurosci. 2004 Apr 7;24(14):3522–6. pmid:15071099
  46. 46. De la Iglesia HO, Meyer J, Schwartz WJ. Using Per gene expression to search for photoperiodic oscillators in the hamster suprachiasmatic nucleus. Mol Brain Res. 2004 Aug 23;127(1):121–7.
  47. 47. Carr AJ, Johnston JD, Semikhodskii AG, Nolan T, Cagampang FR, Stirland JA, et al. Photoperiod differentially regulates circadian oscillators in central and peripheral tissues of the Syrian hamster. Curr Biol. 2003 Sep 2;13(17):1543–8. pmid:12956958
  48. 48. Challet E, Poirel VJ, Malan A, Pévet P. Light exposure during daytime modulates expression of Per1 and Per2 clock genes in the suprachiasmatic nuclei of mice. J Neurosci Res. 2003 Jun 1;72(5):629–37. pmid:12749028
  49. 49. Aschoff J. Circadian rhythms: influences of internal and external factors on the period measured in constant conditions. Ethology. 1979 Jan 12;49(3):225–49.
  50. 50. Oster H, Maronde E, Albrecht U. The circadian clock as a molecular calendar.Chronobiol Int. 2002 Jan 1;19(3):507–16. pmid:12069034
  51. 51. Yan L, Foley NC, Bobula JM, Kriegsfeld LJ, Silver R. Two antiphase oscillations occur in each suprachiasmatic nucleus of behaviorally split hamsters. J Neurosci. 2005 Sep 28;25(39):9017–26. pmid:16192393
  52. 52. Oda GA, Menaker M, Friesen WO. Modeling the dual pacemaker system of the tau mutant hamster. J Biol Rhythms. 2000 Jun;15(3):246–64. pmid:10885879
  53. 53. Indic P, Schwartz WJ, Paydarfar D. Design principles for phase-splitting behaviour of coupled cellular oscillators: clues from hamsters with ‘split’circadian rhythms. J R Soc Interface. 2008 Aug 6;5(25):873–83. pmid:18077247
  54. 54. Schroder S, Herzog ED, Kiss IZ. Transcription-based oscillator model for light-induced splitting as antiphase circadian gene expression in the suprachiasmatic nuclei. J Biol Rhythms. 2012 Feb;27(1):79–90. pmid:22306976
  55. 55. Van den Pol AN. The hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nucleus of rat: intrinsic anatomy. J Comp Neurol. 1980 Jun 15;191(4):661–702. pmid:6158529
  56. 56. Romijn HJ, Sluiter AA, Pool CW, Wortel J, Buijs RM. Evidence from Confocal Fluorescence Microscopy for a Dense, Reciprocal Innervation Between AVP‐, somatostatin‐, VIP/PHI‐, GRP‐and VIP/PHI/GRP‐immunoreactive Neurons in the Rat Suprachiasmatic Nucleus. Eur J Neurosci. 1997 Dec 1;9(12):2613–23. pmid:9517467
  57. 57. Shinohara K, Tominaga K, Isobe Y, Inouye ST. Photic regulation of peptides located in the ventrolateral subdivision of the suprachiasmatic nucleus of the rat: daily variations of vasoactive intestinal polypeptide, gastrin-releasing peptide, and neuropeptide Y. J Neurosci. 1993 Feb 1;13(2):793–800. pmid:8426236
  58. 58. Tominaga K, Shinohara K, Otori Y, Fukuhara C, Inouye SI. Circadian rhythms of vasopressin content in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of the rat. Neuroreport. 1992 Sep 1;3(9):809–12. pmid:1421139
  59. 59. Quintero JE, Kuhlman SJ, McMahon DG. The biological clock nucleus: a multiphasic oscillator network regulated by light. J Neurosci. 2003 Sep 3;23(22):8070–6. pmid:12954869
  60. 60. Helfrich-Förster C. Does the morning and evening oscillator model fit better for flies or mice?. J Biol Rhythms. 2009 Aug 1;24(4):259–70. pmid:19625728
  61. 61. Kalsbeek A, Merrow M, Roenneberg T, Foster R. Two clocks in the brain: an update of the morning and evening oscillator model in Drosophila. The Neurobiology of Circadian Timing. 2012 Dec 6;199:5.
  62. 62. Stoleru D, Peng Y, Agosto J, Rosbash M. Coupled oscillators control morning and evening locomotor behaviour of Drosophila. Nature. 2004 Oct 14;431(7010):862–8. pmid:15483615
  63. 63. Grima B, Chélot E, Xia R, Rouyer F. Morning and evening peaks of activity rely on different clock neurons of the Drosophila brain. Nature. 2004 Oct 14;431(7010):869–73. pmid:15483616
  64. 64. Gonze D, Bernard S, Waltermann C, Kramer A, Herzel H. Spontaneous synchronization of coupled circadian oscillators. Biophys J. 2005 Jul 31;89(1):120–9. pmid:15849258
  65. 65. Bodenstein C, Gosak M, Schuster S, Marhl M, Perc M. Modeling the seasonal adaptation of circadian clocks by changes in the network structure of the suprachiasmatic nucleus. PLoS Comput Biol. 2012 Sep 20;8(9):e1002697. pmid:23028293